12.07.2015 Views

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

36 PATSY M. LIGHTBOWN AND NINA SPADAto learn new material. Teachers can use this <strong>in</strong>formation to help learners expand theirrepertoire of learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies and thus develop greater flexibility <strong>in</strong> their way ofapproach<strong>in</strong>g language learn<strong>in</strong>g.Age of acquisitionWe now turn to a learner characteristic of a different type: age.This characteristic is easierto def<strong>in</strong>e and measure than personality, aptitude, or motivation. Nevertheless, the relationshipbetween a learner’s age and his or her potential for success <strong>in</strong> second languageacquisition is the subject of much lively debate.It has been widely observed that children from immigrant families eventually speak thelanguage of their new community with native like fluency, but their parents rarely achievesuch high levels of mastery of the spoken language. To be sure, there are cases where adultsecond language learners have dist<strong>in</strong>guished themselvcs by their exceptional performance.For example, one often sees reference to Joseph Conrad, a native speaker of Polish whobecame a major writer <strong>in</strong> the <strong>English</strong> language. Many adult second language learners becomecapable of communicat<strong>in</strong>g very successfully <strong>in</strong> the language but, for most, differences ofaccent, word choice, or grammatical features dist<strong>in</strong>guish them from native speakers andfrom second language speakers who began learn<strong>in</strong>g the language while they were very young.One explanation for this difference is that, as <strong>in</strong> first language acquisition, there is a criticalperiod for second language acquisition.The Critical Period Hypothesis suggests that there isa time <strong>in</strong> human development when the bra<strong>in</strong> is predisposed for success <strong>in</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g.Developmental changes <strong>in</strong> the bra<strong>in</strong>, it is argued, affect the nature of language acquisition.Accord<strong>in</strong>g to this view, language learn<strong>in</strong>g which occurs after the end of the critical period maynot be based on the <strong>in</strong>nate biological structures believed to contribute to first languageacquisition or second language acquisition <strong>in</strong> early childhood. Rather, older learners dependon more general learn<strong>in</strong>g abilities ~ the samc one7 they might use to lcarn other k<strong>in</strong>ds of skillsor <strong>in</strong>formation. It is argued that these general learn<strong>in</strong>g abilities are not as successful forlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g as the more specific, <strong>in</strong>nate capacities which are available to the young child.It is most often claimed that the critical period ends somewhere around puberty, but someresearchers suggest it could be even earlier.Of course, it is difficult to compare children and adults as second language learners. Inaddition to the possible biological differences suggested by the Critical Period Hypothesis,the conditions for language learn<strong>in</strong>g are often very different. Younger learners <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>formallanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g environments usually have more time to devote to learn<strong>in</strong>g 1anguage.Theyoften have more opportunities to hear and use the language <strong>in</strong> environments where they donot experience strong pressure to speak fluently and accurately from the very beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g.Furthermore, their early imperfect efforts arc often praised or, at least, accepted. On theother hand, older learners are often <strong>in</strong> situations which demand much more complexlanguage and the expression of much more complicated ideas. Adults are often embarrassedby their lack of mastery of the language and they may develop a sense of <strong>in</strong>adequacy afterexperiences of frustration <strong>in</strong> try<strong>in</strong>g to say exactly what they mean.The Critical Period Hypothesis has been challenged <strong>in</strong> recent years from several differentpo<strong>in</strong>ts of view. Some studies of the second language development of older and youngerlearners who are learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> similar circumstances have shown that, at least <strong>in</strong> the early stagesof second language devclopment, older learners are more efficient than younger learners.In educational research, it has been reported that learners who began learn<strong>in</strong>g a secondlanguage at the primary school level did not fare better <strong>in</strong> the long run than those who began<strong>in</strong> early adolescence. Furthermore, there are countless anecdotes about older learners

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!