12.07.2015 Views

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

22 ROSAMOND MITCHELL AND FLORENCE MYLESreceiv<strong>in</strong>g.This ensures that the <strong>in</strong>put is well adapted to thcir own <strong>in</strong>ternal needs (i.e. to thepresent state of development of their L2 knowledge). What this means is that learnersneed the chance to talk with native speakers <strong>in</strong> a fairly open-ended way, to ask questions, andto clarify mean<strong>in</strong>gs when they do not immctliately understand. Under these conditions, itis believed that the utterances that result will be at the right level of difficulty to promotelearn<strong>in</strong>g; <strong>in</strong> Krashen’s terms, they will provide true ‘comprehensible <strong>in</strong>put’. Conversationalepisodes <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g the regular negotiation of mean<strong>in</strong>g have been <strong>in</strong>tensively studied by manyof the Krashen-<strong>in</strong>fluenced researchers.Interaction is also <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g to l<strong>in</strong>guistic theorists, because of recent controversies overwhether the provision of negative evidence is necessary or helpful for L2 development. By‘negative evidence’ is meant some k<strong>in</strong>d of <strong>in</strong>put which lets the learner know that a particularform is not acceptable accord<strong>in</strong>g to target language norms. In L2 <strong>in</strong>teraction this might takethe shape of a formal correction offered by a teacher, say, or a more <strong>in</strong>formal rephras<strong>in</strong>g ofa learner’s L2 utterance, offered by a native-speak<strong>in</strong>g conversational partner.Why is there a controversy about negative evidence <strong>in</strong> L2 learn<strong>in</strong>g? The problem is thatcorrection often seems <strong>in</strong>effective ~- and not only because L2 learners are lazy. It seems thatlearners often cannot benefit from correction, but cont<strong>in</strong>ue to makc the same mistakcshowever much fcedback is offered. For some current theorists, any natural language mustbe learnable from positive evidence alone, and corrective feedback is largely irrelevant. Otherscont<strong>in</strong>ue to see value <strong>in</strong> corrections and negative evidence, though it is generally acceptedthat these will be useful only when they relate to ‘hot spots’ currently be<strong>in</strong>g restructured <strong>in</strong>the learner’s emerg<strong>in</strong>g L2 system.These different (psycho)l<strong>in</strong>guistic views have one th<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> common, however; they viewthe learner as operat<strong>in</strong>g and develop<strong>in</strong>g a relatively autonomous L2 system, and see <strong>in</strong>teractionas a way of feed<strong>in</strong>g that system with more or less f<strong>in</strong>e-tuned <strong>in</strong>put data, whethcr positive ornegative. Sociol<strong>in</strong>guistic vicws of <strong>in</strong>teraction are very different. Here, the language learn<strong>in</strong>gprocess is viewed as essentially social; both thc identity of the learncr, and their languagcknowledge, are collaboratively constructed and reconstructed <strong>in</strong> the course of <strong>in</strong>teraction.Some theorists strcss a broad view ofthc second language learn<strong>in</strong>g process as an apprenticeship<strong>in</strong>to a range of new discourse practices (e.g. Hall 1995); others arc more concerned withanalys<strong>in</strong>g the detail of <strong>in</strong>teraction between more expert and less expert speakers, to determ<strong>in</strong>ehow the learner is scaJolded <strong>in</strong>to us<strong>in</strong>g (and presumably learn<strong>in</strong>g) new L2 forms.Views of the language learnerWho is the second language learner, and how are they <strong>in</strong>troduced to us, <strong>in</strong> current SLLresearch? ‘Second language’ research generally deals with learners who embark on thelearn<strong>in</strong>g of an additional language, at least some years after they have started to acquire theirfirst 1anguage.This learn<strong>in</strong>g may take place formally and systematically, <strong>in</strong> a classroom sett<strong>in</strong>g;or it may take place through <strong>in</strong>formal social contact, through work, through migration, orother social forces which br<strong>in</strong>g speakers of different languages <strong>in</strong>to contact, and makecommunication a necessity.So, second language learners may be children, or thcy may be adults; they may belearn<strong>in</strong>g the target languagc formally <strong>in</strong> school or college, or ‘pick<strong>in</strong>g it up’ <strong>in</strong> thc playgroundor the workplacc.They may be learn<strong>in</strong>g a highly localized language, which will helpthem to become <strong>in</strong>siders <strong>in</strong> a local speech community; or the targct language may be alanguage of widcr communication relevant to thcir region, which givcs access to economicdevelopment and public life.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!