12.07.2015 Views

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

RECORDING AND TRANSCRIBING TALI< 341Quantitative and qualitative approaches to the analysis of spokenlanguageA quantitative approach allows you to represent your data <strong>in</strong> terms of numbers.You canmake a numerical comparison between talk produced by different people or dur<strong>in</strong>gdifferent events.When represent<strong>in</strong>g data that has been analysed us<strong>in</strong>g quantitative methods it is usualto display this <strong>in</strong> a table. Alternative forms of representation such as histograms or barcharts may be used to po<strong>in</strong>t up comparisons between people or events.Data may be analysed us<strong>in</strong>g prespecified categories of talk. Alternatively, as <strong>in</strong>Jayalakshmi’s research, categories may emerge from close scrut<strong>in</strong>y of data, e.g. fromplay<strong>in</strong>g, and replay<strong>in</strong>g, an audio or video record<strong>in</strong>g, or work<strong>in</strong>g slowly through atranscript. Such categories are not ‘naturally’ present <strong>in</strong> the data, but will depend uponyour own research <strong>in</strong>terests.Represent<strong>in</strong>g talk <strong>in</strong> terms of numbers has the disadvantage that it is necessarily areductive exercise: talk is reduced to a set of categories; it is abstracted from <strong>its</strong> orig<strong>in</strong>alcontext; it is unambiguously pigeon-holed, mask<strong>in</strong>g the rather fluid, uncerta<strong>in</strong> andnegotiated mean<strong>in</strong>gs that are evident when talk is exam<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> context.Talk may be recorded and analysed <strong>in</strong> a more open-ended way. Researchers adopt<strong>in</strong>ga qualitative approach to record<strong>in</strong>g can note down and explore any <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g aspectsof their data. What count as <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g aspects will depend upon the questions theresearcher is concerned to <strong>in</strong>vestigate, but sometimes po<strong>in</strong>ts emerge that are quiteunexpected.Aspects of the data may only beg<strong>in</strong> to make sense when mulled over and comparedwith other <strong>in</strong>formation, or perhaps discussed with speakers. Sometimes <strong>in</strong>terpretationsmay change, or you may want to allow for a number of different <strong>in</strong>terpretations.When present<strong>in</strong>g and discuss<strong>in</strong>g data that has been recorded and analysed us<strong>in</strong>g aqualitative approach, researchers frequently quote selectively from field-notes ortranscripts to support po<strong>in</strong>ts they wish to make. Transcripts may be supported by adetailed commentary, as <strong>in</strong> Chapters 18 and 19.Such ways of analys<strong>in</strong>g and present<strong>in</strong>g data allow the researcher to preserveimportant contextual <strong>in</strong>formation that affects the mean<strong>in</strong>gs of utterances, and also topreserve the ambiguity and fluidity of these mean<strong>in</strong>gs. The approach is selective <strong>in</strong> thattwo researchers may (legitimately) notice different th<strong>in</strong>gs about a stretch of talk orprovide different <strong>in</strong>terpretations of utterances. There is also a danger of un<strong>in</strong>tended bias,<strong>in</strong> that researchers may notice features of talk that support a po<strong>in</strong>t they wish to makeand ignore counter evidence.to identify stylistic differences between different (literary) authors or different types of text.Wegerif and Mercer illustrate how corpus-based methods may be used with smaller amountsof data, and <strong>in</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ation with a qualitative exploration of language.Wegerif and Mercer drew on this comb<strong>in</strong>ation of methods as part of an ongo<strong>in</strong>g studyof exploratory talk <strong>in</strong> the classroom. They found that primary school children performedbetter on a standardised test of reason<strong>in</strong>g after they had been ‘coached’ <strong>in</strong> the use ofexploratory talk. They also looked at transcript evidence of the quality of children’s talkdur<strong>in</strong>g problem solv<strong>in</strong>g activities carried out before and after the coach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tervention.Extracts from transcripts are used to show that, after the <strong>in</strong>tervention children spent moretime discuss<strong>in</strong>g problems, considered alternative solutions and eventually reached

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!