12.07.2015 Views

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

312 MICHAEL P. BREENor explicit discursive work while devot<strong>in</strong>g their discursive energies to keep<strong>in</strong>g track of theteacher’s text and be<strong>in</strong>g alert to the moments when they have to contribute to it and to theteacher’s reactions to their contributions.Learners’ discursive practices <strong>in</strong> the classroomSo far, on the basis of language classroom research, I have suggested that the discourse oflessons is significantly shapcd by the teacher, that learners are positioned <strong>in</strong> particular waysby this, that the discourse manifests a shift<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>ter-textuality, and that learners are obligedto undertake pragmatic navigation with<strong>in</strong> this <strong>in</strong>ter-textuality if they arc to f<strong>in</strong>d their waythrough it <strong>in</strong> order to make scnse of it. For a fuller picture, however, we need to focus uponvariations <strong>in</strong> the overt participation of learners <strong>in</strong> the discourse which may be sccn as furthercontributory factors <strong>in</strong> their differential achievement <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g.There arc fcw studics of learner <strong>in</strong>put <strong>in</strong> the classroom apart from the body of workon controversial modifications dur<strong>in</strong>g group or dyad work on tasks, some of which hav<strong>in</strong>gbeen undertaken <strong>in</strong> classroom sett<strong>in</strong>gs. Perhaps this is not surpris<strong>in</strong>g when, if we exam<strong>in</strong>ethe research on learner participation and, by implication, their contributions to the text oflessons as discursive practitioners, we f<strong>in</strong>d that learners are most often positioned by thediscourse <strong>in</strong> a responsive role (Polit7er et al. 1981). Generally, it seems that, through theircontrol of the discursive practices of lessons, through their use of questions, explanations,procedural <strong>in</strong>structions, and, crucially, their evaluation of much of the language producedby learners immediately after it is uttcred, teachers construct learners as primarilyresponsive and scem<strong>in</strong>gly fairly passive participants <strong>in</strong> the discourse. In offer<strong>in</strong>g anexplanation for the failure of French immersion students to fully atta<strong>in</strong> native-speaker likelevels <strong>in</strong> their own speech despite years of exposure to content-based and comprehensiblelanguage <strong>in</strong>put result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> very high levels of receptive understand<strong>in</strong>g, Swa<strong>in</strong> (1 985)suggests that this failure may bc partially due to the relative lack of opportunities for themto participate overtly <strong>in</strong> classroom discourse through their own speech production.Howcvcr, cvcn responsive discursivc practices appear to lead to variation <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g.In <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g whether greater learner participation had an cffcct upon lcarn<strong>in</strong>g, Strong(1 983 and 1984) discovered that a high response rate among certa<strong>in</strong> learners corrclatcdwith thcir achievement <strong>in</strong> tests based upon the grammar, pronunciation and vocabulary ofclassroom speech. Scligcr (1977) suggcstcd that those learners which he identified as “high<strong>in</strong>put gcncrators” performed better on an aural comprehension task than did lessparticipat<strong>in</strong>g learners. In their classic study of the good language learner, Naiman et al.(1 978) found that lcarners who raised their hands more and more often responded toteacher elicitations did better on tests than other learners.Studies by Larsen-Freeman (1 976a and 1976b), Hamayan and Tucker (1 980),Lightbown (1 983), and Long (1 980), all suggest that the frequency of occurrence of certa<strong>in</strong>l<strong>in</strong>guistic forms <strong>in</strong> classroom text is likely to correlate with thc accurate production of theseforms by learners. More significantly, studics by Lightbown (1980 and 1991), Snow andHoefnagel Hohle (1 982), and White et al. (1991) not only confirm this but also show highretention rates of question forms. Given the regular occurrcncc of questions <strong>in</strong> the text oflessons, this may not be surpris<strong>in</strong>g. Learners are obliged to be alert to questions <strong>in</strong> case theyare directed to them <strong>in</strong>dividually.These studies also found that, not only questions, but otherk<strong>in</strong>ds of utterances directed specifically at <strong>in</strong>dividual learners correlated with higher ga<strong>in</strong>scores <strong>in</strong> tests taken by those <strong>in</strong>dividuals. It appears that, while it may not be surpris<strong>in</strong>g thatfrequent occurrence of certa<strong>in</strong> features <strong>in</strong> the text of lessons render them more accessible,

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!