12.07.2015 Views

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

NAVIGATING THE DISCOURSE 307psychological processes such as attention or memory, and through affective <strong>in</strong>volvement <strong>in</strong>the process, to strategic behaviour which may rcnder the process more manageable andunthreaten<strong>in</strong>g; (2) the nature of the actual language learn<strong>in</strong>g process; and (3) the outcomesfrom the process <strong>in</strong> terms of l<strong>in</strong>guistic or, more broadly, communicative competence <strong>in</strong> thetarget language.In explor<strong>in</strong>g this rclationship, SLA research to date has primarily focused upon the<strong>in</strong>teraction between what learners contribute, particularly their <strong>in</strong>nate template for languageor their cognitive processes, and the language data made available to them. In a recent reviewof SLA research, I argued that the research appears to favour particular paradigms of learn<strong>in</strong>gand, thereby, constructs the learner <strong>in</strong> particular ways (Brcen, 1996). Summaris<strong>in</strong>g verybriefly, SLA research tells us a great deal about the learner as be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terpretative,accommodat<strong>in</strong>g, and strategic. That is, the <strong>in</strong>terpretation of mean<strong>in</strong>gful <strong>in</strong>put and thc effortto express mean<strong>in</strong>g appear to be thc catalysts for language 1earn<strong>in</strong>g.The accommodation bythe lcarner of language data is typified by the learner’s creative construction of<strong>in</strong>tcrlanguages which represent gradual approximations to the target language. And bothlearn<strong>in</strong>g strategies and communicative strategies are adopted by learners <strong>in</strong> order to maketheir <strong>in</strong>terpretative and accommodat<strong>in</strong>g work much more manageable. These threeconstructs of the learner which we can deduce from the research contribute significantlyto an explanation of how language is learned.However, this explanation will rema<strong>in</strong> partial if much of SLA research persists <strong>in</strong>decontextualis<strong>in</strong>g learner contributions, the learn<strong>in</strong>g process, and learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes fromthe location <strong>in</strong> which these three factors are realised. Ma<strong>in</strong>stream SLA research, <strong>in</strong> focus<strong>in</strong>gupon the relationship between the learner and language data, is conducted and reported on<strong>in</strong> ways that appear to overlook the social reality <strong>in</strong> which the research is actually conducted.Dyadic encounters between caretakers and young learners or between native spcakerresearchers and non-native speak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formants, experimental situations us<strong>in</strong>g elicitationtechniques, quasi-experimental negotiation tasks undertaken by non-native speakers, orobserved <strong>in</strong>teractions dur<strong>in</strong>g lessons are never socially neutral activities. To reduce the datafrom such events to a psychol<strong>in</strong>guistic objectivity of <strong>in</strong>puts and outputs is to dislocate themfrom their <strong>in</strong>tersubjectivc nature. The evidence we obta<strong>in</strong> from any learn<strong>in</strong>g evcnt, even <strong>in</strong>a quasi experimental sett<strong>in</strong>g, is significantly shaped by thc social situation and the socialrelations with<strong>in</strong> that event.If we used Ellis’s recent very comprehensive review of SLA research (Ellis, 1994) as an<strong>in</strong>dicator of the major focus of SLA researchers to the present time, we f<strong>in</strong>d that more thantwo thirds of the chapters <strong>in</strong> his account refer to work which assumes that the <strong>in</strong>teractionbetween the learner’s mental resources and features of l<strong>in</strong>guistic <strong>in</strong>put will provide asufficiently adequate explanation for language learn<strong>in</strong>g Ellis fairly reflects current SLAresearch <strong>in</strong> devot<strong>in</strong>g just over a quarter of his review to more recent studies which locatethe <strong>in</strong>teraction between learner and language <strong>in</strong> the context of <strong>in</strong>terpersonal or socialsituations. His account reveals that context has been def<strong>in</strong>ed or framed <strong>in</strong> particular waysby SLA research. It is addressed <strong>in</strong> a fragmentary way as a diversity of“socia1 factors”- fromidentification by the learncr with the target language group to the possible effects of differenttypes of language programs ~ or as the specific features of classroom <strong>in</strong>teraction, or as thepossible impact of formal <strong>in</strong>struction. Ellis himself concludes that “the relationship betweensocial factors and L2 achievemcnt is an <strong>in</strong>direct rather than a direct one” (1994: 239). Inreferr<strong>in</strong>g to classroom <strong>in</strong>teraction studies, he concludes that they have “contributed littleto our understand<strong>in</strong>g of how <strong>in</strong>teraction affects acquisition” (1994: 607). And he deducesthat formal <strong>in</strong>ytruction can, at most, be credited with “facilitat<strong>in</strong>g natural languagedeve1opment”<strong>in</strong> terms of <strong>in</strong>creased accuracy and accelerated progress (1994: 659).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!