12.07.2015 Views

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

EVALUATION OF CLASSROOM INTERACTION 297this does not require the use of metalanguage, <strong>in</strong> fact, a close perusal of the learners’ uptakecharts demonstrates that the <strong>in</strong>formants were perfectly capable of report<strong>in</strong>g what went ondur<strong>in</strong>g the course of the lessons <strong>in</strong> terms of grammar. By and large, learners succeeded <strong>in</strong>account<strong>in</strong>g for the teacher’s structural <strong>in</strong>tentions by report<strong>in</strong>g the title if not writ<strong>in</strong>g thema<strong>in</strong> po<strong>in</strong>ts of the sessions. Some even illustrated the teacher’s focus of <strong>in</strong>struction byprovid<strong>in</strong>g examples of sentences to show their comprehension or at least familiarity withwhat was taught. This suggests that the <strong>in</strong>formants did not lack the means of express<strong>in</strong>g thestructural objectives.It is believed that one of the reasons why learners did not report as many structuralfeatures as lexical ones is that several of these features were already familiar to the class. Infact, it is not surpris<strong>in</strong>g that most of the structural features emphasised dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>structionwere not reported as newly learned because most of them, if not all of them, were part ofthe syllabus <strong>in</strong> high school. For <strong>in</strong>stance, only one <strong>in</strong>formant claimed to have seen andlearned the passive and active voices for the first time dur<strong>in</strong>g the observed events. In fact,these affirmations are confirmed by the 20 per cent of topicalised episodes <strong>in</strong> the lessonwhich were claimed to be part of the learners’ prior knowledge. One could add that aftera few hours of teach<strong>in</strong>g, second language <strong>in</strong>struction becomes very much remedial asstructural features are presented and represented for a review.It looks as if the learners’ claims are somewhat different from what the teacher hasplanned for them. His <strong>in</strong>tentions might have helped learners to rehearse already encountered(if not mastered) structural features. However, <strong>in</strong> the process of carry<strong>in</strong>g out the plan, the<strong>in</strong>teractive work has lent <strong>its</strong>elf to the creation of a whole range of perhaps unexpected andbeneficial events (at least, to some learners if not to all).The learners’ claims (44 per centonTable 18.2) rema<strong>in</strong> a comb<strong>in</strong>ation of the teacher’s objectives but also their by-productas well as the by-product of the classroom <strong>in</strong>teraction. For these reasons, therefore, attemptsto evaluate the learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes aga<strong>in</strong>st the teacher’s plan can be mislead<strong>in</strong>g if one doesnot take <strong>in</strong>to account the mediat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>teractive processes which characterise classroom<strong>in</strong>teraction.In view of the data expressed <strong>in</strong> the table, therefore, the teacher’s <strong>in</strong>fluence over thesubjects’ learn<strong>in</strong>g did not reveal <strong>its</strong>elf to be as strong as suggested earlier s<strong>in</strong>ce approximately56 per cent of what has been focused upon did not apparently bear any immediate fruit: 20per cent were claimed to be already familiar and 36 per cent were not, <strong>in</strong> any way, mentionedby the learners.It should lie po<strong>in</strong>ted out that about 77.45 per cent of the topicalisation was effected bythe teacher. This is not particularly surpris<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> view of the fact that the discourse wasunidirectionally controlled by the teacher, who did 45 per cent of the talk<strong>in</strong>g. What appearsto be strik<strong>in</strong>gly <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g though is that a further analysis of the effect of the teacher’sversus the learners’ scarce opportunities (22.54 per cent) for topicalisation showed that thelatter offered much higher chances for items to be uptaken. Learners benefited much morefrom their peers’ rare <strong>in</strong>stances of topicalisation than from the teacher’s.A close scrut<strong>in</strong>y of the theme of topicalisation reveals that topics <strong>in</strong>itiated by learnersattracted more claims from the learners than the ones <strong>in</strong>itiated by the teacher. The analysi sshows that out of 46 items <strong>in</strong>itiated by the learners, 34 (73.9 per cent) were claimed,whereas only 78 (49.4 per cent) out of 158 were claimed when topicalised by the <strong>in</strong>structor.Thus, the chances for claims are much higher when items are triggered by classmates. Afurther emphasis on the profitability of the learners’ <strong>in</strong>itiation is that it attracts morereporters than when topics are brought up by the teacher.By limit<strong>in</strong>g to himself the <strong>in</strong>itiative of topicalis<strong>in</strong>g most items for <strong>in</strong>struction, the teacherdoes not give the learners much opportunity to dist<strong>in</strong>guish between items which are

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!