12.07.2015 Views

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

184 MICHAEL H. LONGdcvelopmcnt now completed, not one shows either tutored or naturalistic learnersdevelop<strong>in</strong>g proficiency one l<strong>in</strong>guistic item at a time. On the contrary, all reveal complex,gradual and <strong>in</strong>ter-related developmental paths for grammatical subsystems, such as auxiliaryand negation <strong>in</strong> ESL (Stauble 1981 ; Kelley 1983), and copula and word ordcr <strong>in</strong> GSL(Meisel, Clahsen and Pienemann 1981). Moreover, development is not unidirectional;omission/suppliance of forms fluctuates, as does accuracy of suppliance.Although most syllabi and methods assume the opposite, learncrs do not move fromignorance of a form to mastery of it <strong>in</strong> one step, as is attested by the very existence ofdevelopmental scqucnces like that for ESL negation. Typically, when a form first appears <strong>in</strong>a learner’s IL, it is used <strong>in</strong> a non-target-like manner, and only gradually improves <strong>in</strong> accuracyof use. It sometimes shifts <strong>in</strong> function over time as other new (target-like and non-targetlike)forms enter (Huebner 1983). It quite often decl<strong>in</strong>es <strong>in</strong> accuracy or even temporarilydisappears altogether due to a change elsewhere <strong>in</strong> the IL (see, e.g. Meisel, Clahsen andPienemann 1981 ; Huebner 1983; Lightbown 1983; Neumann 1977), a phenomenonsometimes describable as U-shaped behavior (Kellcrman 1985). Further, attempts to teachisolated items one at a timc fail unless the structure happens to be one the learner can processand so is psychol<strong>in</strong>guistically ready to acquire. In Pienemann’s (1984) term<strong>in</strong>ology,learnability determ<strong>in</strong>es teachability. F<strong>in</strong>ally, as language teachers, employers and learnersalike will attest, there is a great difference between structural knowledge of a language,when that is achieved, and ability to usc that knowledge to communicative effect.As noted earlier, facts about IL development like these have led some to advocate thatteachers abandon not just a focus onforms, but a focus onform, i.e. any attention to languageas object, as well. Flaws <strong>in</strong> this reason<strong>in</strong>g are obvious. Further, reviews of studies of the effectsof <strong>in</strong>struction on IL dcvclopment (Harley 1988; Long 1988) f<strong>in</strong>d clear evidence of somebeneficial effects of a focus onform, and suggestive evidence of others. Briefly, while it is truethat <strong>in</strong>struction does not seem capable of altcr<strong>in</strong>g sequences of developmcnt, it does appcarto offer three other advantages over either naturalistic SLA or classroom <strong>in</strong>struction with nofocus on form. (1) It speeds up the rate of learn<strong>in</strong>g (for review, see Long 1983). (2) It affectsacquisition processes <strong>in</strong> ways possibly beneficial to long-term accuracy (Lightbown 1983; Pica1983). And most crucially, on the basis of prelim<strong>in</strong>ary data, (3) it appears to raise the ultimatelevel ofatta<strong>in</strong>ment. Further, as White (1987, 1989) has argued, <strong>in</strong>comprehensible <strong>in</strong>put anddraw<strong>in</strong>g learners’ attention to <strong>in</strong>admissable constructions <strong>in</strong> thc L2 (two k<strong>in</strong>ds of negativeevidence) may be necessary when learn<strong>in</strong>g from positive evidence alone will be <strong>in</strong>adequate.To illustrate, an L1 may allow placement of adverbs of manner more flexibly than an L2. “Hedr<strong>in</strong>ks every day coffee” and “He dr<strong>in</strong>ks coffee every day” arc both acceptable <strong>in</strong> French, forexample, but not <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong>. Both will be communicatively effective <strong>in</strong> <strong>English</strong>, however,with the result that the French learner of <strong>English</strong> (hut not the <strong>English</strong> learner of French) willneed negative <strong>in</strong>put (e.g. error correction) on this po<strong>in</strong>t.Whereas the content of lessons with a focus onforms is theforms themselves, a syllabuswith a focus onform teaches someth<strong>in</strong>g else ~ biology, mathematics, workshop practice,automobile repair, the geography of a country where the foreign language is spoken, thecultures of <strong>its</strong> speakers, and so on ~ and overtly draws students’ attention to l<strong>in</strong>guisticelements as they arise <strong>in</strong>cidentally <strong>in</strong> lessons whose overrid<strong>in</strong>g focus is on mean<strong>in</strong>g, orcommunication. Views about how to achieve this vary. One proposal is for lessons to bebriefly “<strong>in</strong>terrupted” by teachers when they notice students mak<strong>in</strong>g errors which are (1)systematic, (2) pervasive and (3) remediable. The l<strong>in</strong>guistic feature is brought to learners’attention <strong>in</strong> any way appropriate to the students’ age, proficiency level, etc. before the classreturns to whatever pedagogic task they were work<strong>in</strong>g on when the <strong>in</strong>terruption occurred.(For details and a rationalc, see Crookes and Long 1987; Long, <strong>in</strong> press).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!