12.07.2015 Views

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

~ uponTHE SOCIAL CONTEXT FOR LANGUAGE LEARNING 127def<strong>in</strong>itions, conventions, and procedures which enable a work<strong>in</strong>g together <strong>in</strong> a crowd. Ofcourse, the discourse of a classroom may provide a w<strong>in</strong>dow onto the surface expressionof the <strong>in</strong>tersubjective experience and even onto momentary expressions of subjectiveexperiences, for these two dimensions of experience must <strong>in</strong>terrelate and <strong>in</strong>fluence oneanother. However, classroom discourse alone allows us a partial view from which we areobliged to dcscribc others’ experiences as if “through a glass darkly.”Classroom-oriented research shares with SLA studies the tendency to reduce or avoidconsideration of certa<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terven<strong>in</strong>g variables which <strong>in</strong>evitably <strong>in</strong>fluence how and whylearners may <strong>in</strong>ternalise <strong>in</strong>put and how and why learners <strong>in</strong>teract with a teacher <strong>in</strong> the waysthey do.Ths reductionism is characterised by an emphatic focus upon l<strong>in</strong>guistic performanceobservable features of language and discourse. To be fair, neither research traditionmay <strong>in</strong>tend to undcrstand or even expla<strong>in</strong> language learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the classroom situation.However, any researcher who tries to correlate features of l<strong>in</strong>guistic Performance data <strong>in</strong>terms of classroom <strong>in</strong>put with some learn<strong>in</strong>g outcome is, at lcast implicitly, seek<strong>in</strong>g apossible explanation of that learn<strong>in</strong>g outcome. And such an cxplanation can only be causal.Classroom rcscarch is not asocial like SLA research, but it does share a non-cognitive viewof learner comprehension and reconstruction of <strong>in</strong>put despite <strong>its</strong> potentially richer view of<strong>in</strong>put as discourse rather than merely grammatical data. Classroom-oriented researchperceives the learner as actively contribut<strong>in</strong>g to the discourse. But how can we relate suchcontributions or even non contributions to language learn<strong>in</strong>g? Learners and tcachers arenot dualities of social be<strong>in</strong>g and mental be<strong>in</strong>g ~ an idea apparcntly unfortunately supportedby the very separateness of SLA and classroom-oriented research priorities. It is <strong>in</strong>cumbentupon classroom-based <strong>in</strong>vestigations of language learn<strong>in</strong>g to account for those socialpsychological forces which gcnerate classroom discourse and for those socio-cognitiveeffects of the discourse even f<strong>its</strong> objective is primarily to describe social phenomena. If thesubjective and <strong>in</strong>tersubjective experiences of and from classroom discourse are reduced towhat we can f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>in</strong> the discourse alone, then we are allowed to deduce that classroomlanguage learn<strong>in</strong>g results from discoursal condition<strong>in</strong>g ~ no more nor less than socialdeterm<strong>in</strong>ism!It appears that the two metaphors for the classroom which we have availablc to u3 atprcsent offer def<strong>in</strong>itions of the classroom situation which seem to neglect the social realityof language learn<strong>in</strong>g as I t IS experienced and created by teachers and learners. Both metaphorsunfortunately constra<strong>in</strong> our understand<strong>in</strong>g of language learn<strong>in</strong>g because each takes forgranted crucial <strong>in</strong>terven<strong>in</strong>g psychological and social variables which arc the fulcra uponwhich language learn<strong>in</strong>g is balanced. The reconstructive cognition of learners and the socialand psychological forces which permeate the processes of teach<strong>in</strong>g and learn<strong>in</strong>g must residewith<strong>in</strong> any explanation conccrn<strong>in</strong>g how and why people do what thcy do when they worktogether on a new language. More seriously, perhaps, both contemporary metaphorsimplicitly reduce human action and <strong>in</strong>teraction to classical condition<strong>in</strong>g, where<strong>in</strong> learnersthough superficially participat<strong>in</strong>g are essentially passive respondents to observable l<strong>in</strong>guisticand discoursal stimuli. It thcrefore appears nccessary that research has still to adopt adef<strong>in</strong>ition of the classroom which will encompass both cognitive and socd variables 70 thattheir mutual <strong>in</strong>fluence can be better understood. More precisely, we need a metaphor forthe classroom through which teacher and earners can be viewed as th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g social actorsand not reduced to generators of <strong>in</strong>put-output nor analyzed as dualities of either conceptualor social be<strong>in</strong>gs. Perhaps the metaphor we require can providc a basis for the synthesis ofSLA and classroom-oriented research endeavours whilst necessarily be<strong>in</strong>g more comprehensivethan both. These deductions lead me to propose a third metaphor for theclassroom <strong>in</strong> the hope that it might further facilitate our understand<strong>in</strong>g of classroom

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!