12.07.2015 Views

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

English Language Teaching in its Social Context

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

100 LEO VAN LIERSpeaker 2, an ESL learner, is the same person <strong>in</strong> both <strong>in</strong>teractions, but <strong>in</strong> the first her<strong>in</strong>terlocutor is of roughly equal proficiency and <strong>in</strong> the second her <strong>in</strong>terlocutor is a nativelikebil<strong>in</strong>gual speaker. The first extract illustrates symmetry, and all utterances exhibit a highdegree of cont<strong>in</strong>gency. The second extract is more like an <strong>in</strong>terview <strong>in</strong> which speaker 1encourages speaker 2 to speak. Relations of cont<strong>in</strong>gency are weaker, and symmetry isreduced. If cont<strong>in</strong>gency could be visualized as bundles of str<strong>in</strong>gs connect<strong>in</strong>g utterances,then the str<strong>in</strong>gs would be thicker and more numerous <strong>in</strong> the first conversation and moresparse and sp<strong>in</strong>dly <strong>in</strong> the second.Many sorts of devices can be used to create cont<strong>in</strong>gency: empathy markers (“Wow!”),repetitions of parts of each other’s utterances (“two bedroom ~ two bedroom”), <strong>in</strong>tonationpatterns, gestures, and so on. The devices come from a stock of resources similar toGumperz’s (1992) “contextualization cues” (<strong>in</strong>deed, as I suggested above, the creation ofcont<strong>in</strong>gencies overlaps significantly with the process of contextualization), though any<strong>in</strong>teractional marker that can be used to make a cont<strong>in</strong>gent l<strong>in</strong>k can also be used for otherpurposes, and this makes tabulat<strong>in</strong>g and quantify<strong>in</strong>g cont<strong>in</strong>gency impossible.Cont<strong>in</strong>gency, negotiation, and language learn<strong>in</strong>gThe dynamics of <strong>in</strong>teraction have been studied <strong>in</strong> most detail by Teresa Pica and hercolleagues (Pica 1987,1992; Pica and Doughty 1985; Pica,Young, and Doughty 1987).Thisresearch, which focuses on opportunities for learners to carry out repair strategies follow<strong>in</strong>gcommunicative problems, has revealed various conditions that favor or disfavor such<strong>in</strong>teractional modification and has shown how it benef<strong>its</strong> comprehension. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Pica(1 987), “What enables learners to move beyond their current <strong>in</strong>terlanguage receptive andexpressive capacities when they need to understand unfamiliar l<strong>in</strong>guistic <strong>in</strong>put or whenrequired to produce a comprchcnsiblc message are opportunities to modify and restructuretheir <strong>in</strong>teraction with their <strong>in</strong>terlocutor until mutual comprehension is reached”.By resolv<strong>in</strong>g communicative problems through the use of <strong>in</strong>teractional modifications(requests for clarification or confirmation, comprehension checks, recasts, and other suchrepair<strong>in</strong>g moves), the learner obta<strong>in</strong>s comprehensible <strong>in</strong>put or makes new <strong>in</strong>put availablefor learn<strong>in</strong>g. Research has shown how learners actively work on the language to <strong>in</strong>creasetheir knowledge and proficiency.The follow<strong>in</strong>g observations, based on these analyses of repair <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>ter-language talk,might help to place repair<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the overall context of <strong>in</strong>teractional language use.First, as Guy Aston has po<strong>in</strong>ted out, repair work and adjustments of various k<strong>in</strong>ds canbe used to express convergence of perspectives among participants or to “seek closure ona problem” (Rudduck 1991), not necessarily to make someth<strong>in</strong>g comprehensible. GeorgeYule (1 990) found that more-proficient <strong>in</strong>terlocutors sometimes simply decide to give upon certa<strong>in</strong> problematic items <strong>in</strong> a task and move on.Therefore repair may have results otherthan <strong>in</strong>creased comprehension, though <strong>in</strong>crcascd comprehension can reasonably be regardedas <strong>its</strong> chief aim.Second, the preponderance of repair (<strong>in</strong> the highly visible form of <strong>in</strong>teractionalmodifications) may be the result of the type of discourse <strong>in</strong>vestigated. In much of the workof Pica and associatcs (Pica,Young, and Doughty 1987; Pica 1992), the activity types <strong>in</strong>question are communication tasks <strong>in</strong> which participants (often a native speaker and anonnative speaker) need to exchange <strong>in</strong>formation. This need leads to <strong>in</strong>teraction thatis usually both asymmetrical and unequal, an environment <strong>in</strong> which explicit repair,with imbalances of the k<strong>in</strong>d illustrated by Yule, tends to be salient. A similar focus on

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!