12.07.2015 Views

metacognitive awareness of reading strategies of turkish - Ozean ...

metacognitive awareness of reading strategies of turkish - Ozean ...

metacognitive awareness of reading strategies of turkish - Ozean ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS
  • No tags were found...

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

European Journal <strong>of</strong> Educational Studies 3(2), 2011European Journal <strong>of</strong> Educational Studies 3(2), 2011© 2011 <strong>Ozean</strong> PublicationMETACOGNITIVE AWARENESS OF READING STRATEGIES OF TURKISHLEARNERS WHO LEARN ENGLISH AS A FOREIGN LANGUAGETURAN TEMUR* AND OZGE BAHAR,Dumlupınar University, Education Faculty, Kutahya, Turkey.*E-mail adress for correspondence: turtemur@yahoo.comINTRODUCTIONCurrently, there is an interest in understanding second/foreign language readers’ <strong>reading</strong> <strong>strategies</strong>. They are <strong>of</strong>interest to many researchers as they show how readers interact with the written material and in what way these<strong>strategies</strong> are associated with text comprehension (Carrell, 1989, 121).Traditionally, as <strong>reading</strong> has been viewed as a cognitive task, regarding research has generally centered on suchcognitive processes as language, memory, and attention, and their influence on <strong>reading</strong> skills. The restricted view<strong>of</strong> <strong>reading</strong> as a combination <strong>of</strong> decoding and comprehension has been replaced by a view that also takes readers’<strong>awareness</strong> and control <strong>of</strong> their cognitive activities into account (Schneider, 1988, 53). In other words, recentresearch has begun to center upon metacognition, “<strong>metacognitive</strong> <strong>awareness</strong> <strong>of</strong>, or perceptions about, <strong>strategies</strong>and the relationships among <strong>awareness</strong> or perception <strong>of</strong> <strong>strategies</strong>, strategy use, and <strong>reading</strong> comprehension”(Waxman & Pardon, 1987; Pardon & Waxman, 1988; Barnett, 1988; Carrell, 1989a; cited in Carrell et al., 1989,648).J. H. Flavell was the first to use the term metacognition. In his own words, metacognition:refers to one’s knowledge concerning one’s own cognitive processes and products or anythingrelated to them, e.g., the learning-relevant properties <strong>of</strong> information or data. For example, I amengaging in metacognition (metamemory, metalearning, metaattention, metalanguage, orwhatever) if I notice that I am having more trouble learning A than B; if it strikes me that Ishould double check C before accepting it as a fact; if it occurs to me that I had better scrutinizeeach and every alternative in any multiple-choice type task situation before deciding which isthe best one; if I sense that I had better make a note <strong>of</strong> D because I may forget it. (Flavell, 1976,232; cited in Sanacore, 1984, 706).Literally, metacognition means “cognition <strong>of</strong> cognition” (Carrell et al., 1989, 648) or as Anderson (2002) definesit, it is “thinking about thinking”. Similarly, Shimamura states that (2000, 142), “knowing about what you know”lies in the center <strong>of</strong> metacognition. It is contingent on the ability to monitor or evaluate one’s own cognitiveactivities to be able to make a proper assessment about his/her future performance and decide, accordingly, howmuch further processing is needed. In sum, while cognition refers to knowledge and skills one has and their use,metacognition refers to <strong>awareness</strong> and conscious control over those.421


European Journal <strong>of</strong> Educational Studies 3(2), 2011Metacognitively aware learners “know what to do when they don’t know what to do”. In other words, they have<strong>strategies</strong> for discovering or working out what needs to be done. The use <strong>of</strong> <strong>metacognitive</strong> <strong>strategies</strong> ignites one’sthinking and can lead to better learning and higher performance, especially among learners who strive (Anderson,2002).The union <strong>of</strong> the art <strong>of</strong> teaching and the science <strong>of</strong> how the brain reacts to stimuli helps educators to find the bestways in preparing lessons and using <strong>strategies</strong> to increase students’ success as they learn to read in an effectuousand joyous manner (Willis, 2009, 334). As <strong>reading</strong> skill is not just the decoding <strong>of</strong> letters, the <strong>strategies</strong> thatstudents use while <strong>reading</strong>, especially in a language other than mother tongue, becomes very important incomprehension and development <strong>of</strong> this skill. In the last two decades, great attention has been given tocomprehending what pr<strong>of</strong>icient, skilled readers commonly do when <strong>reading</strong>, identifying the <strong>strategies</strong> they adoptand determining how and when they use them (Sheorey & Mokhtari, 2001, 432). As Sheorey and Mokhtari (2001,432) put forward, strategic <strong>awareness</strong> and checking <strong>of</strong> the comprehension process are very important components<strong>of</strong> skilled <strong>reading</strong>, which is <strong>of</strong>ten referred to as metacognition that can be regarded as the knowledge <strong>of</strong> thereaders’ cognition concerning the <strong>reading</strong> process and the self-control mechanisms employed to monitor andincrease understanding.Metacognition is an important feature <strong>of</strong> effective <strong>reading</strong> and <strong>reading</strong> instruction (Israel, 2007, 1), so is<strong>metacognitive</strong> <strong>awareness</strong> <strong>of</strong> the <strong>reading</strong> <strong>strategies</strong> one uses. In particular, <strong>reading</strong> <strong>strategies</strong> employed by readers,their <strong>metacognitive</strong> <strong>awareness</strong>, and <strong>reading</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>iciency are closely related. Essentially, successful readers usemore <strong>strategies</strong> compared to less successful students and use them more frequently. Moreover, better readers havebetter <strong>metacognitive</strong> <strong>awareness</strong> <strong>of</strong> their own strategy use, which leads to an enhanced <strong>reading</strong> ability (Baker &Brown, 1984; Garner, 1987; Afflerbach, 2002 as cited in Hamdan et al., 2010, 135). The research findings suggestthat good readers are better at monitoring their understanding than poor readers, are more aware <strong>of</strong> thecharacteristics <strong>of</strong> the text and the <strong>strategies</strong> they use while <strong>reading</strong> (Block, 1986, 465; Presley, Wharton-McDonald, 1997; Brown, Armbruster, & Baker, 1986, 49 as cited in Carrell, Pharis, & Liberto, 1989, 650; Baker,& Brown, 1984; Kletzien, 1991 as cited in Sheorey,&Mokhtari, 2001, 445; Duffy et al., 1987, 348 ) In addition,students who have effective <strong>reading</strong> <strong>strategies</strong> can engage in higher thinking skills about texts and their relationsto those texts (National Rading Panel, 2000; cited in Hernàndez-Laboy, 2009, 4). In other words, strategic readersare very <strong>metacognitive</strong> and they are aware <strong>of</strong> the ongoing <strong>of</strong> the <strong>reading</strong> process and how a text can be difficult(Pressley, Warthon&Mc Donald, 1997).Sheorey and Baboczky (161) define <strong>reading</strong> <strong>strategies</strong> as purposeful and intended steps taken to understand whatis being read. Reading <strong>strategies</strong> are an important part <strong>of</strong> a skilled reader’s occupation with text and his or herfollowing success with that text. For the skilled reader, strategic <strong>reading</strong> is an automatic process before, duringand after <strong>reading</strong> (Brushaber, 2003, 3). Reading <strong>strategies</strong> have to do with understanding the point <strong>of</strong> <strong>reading</strong>,activating the related background knowledge one has, paying attention to key concepts, monitoringcomprehension, and consequencing (Brown, Palincar, Armbruster, 1984; cited in Magno, 2008). Good readers,being able to read strategically, overview, read selectively, summarize, and recite information that they want toremember (Pressley& Wharton-McDonald, 1997). They are <strong>metacognitive</strong>ly aware while <strong>reading</strong>. That is, goodreaders attend the characteristics <strong>of</strong> the text (e.g. relevance <strong>of</strong> the text to their <strong>reading</strong> goals, how the differentcomponents <strong>of</strong> the text are connected) (Pressley, 296). They also monitor when they are having problems while<strong>reading</strong>. Thanks to the <strong>awareness</strong> <strong>of</strong> the difficulties they encounter, they can adjust their <strong>reading</strong> such as speedingup, slowing down, or stopping to read another text to get some background information about the text at hand(Pressley & Gaskins, 2006, 101). However, as poor readers lack these critical <strong>reading</strong> <strong>strategies</strong>, they putstrenuous effort into the text to be able to comprehend it. If instructed how to employ and apply the <strong>reading</strong><strong>strategies</strong>, poor readers, too, can become strategic readers.Instruction in <strong>metacognitive</strong> <strong>strategies</strong> may lead to an improvement in <strong>reading</strong> comprehension(http://wik.ed.uiuc.edu/index.php/Metacognitive_knowledge). It is because, in that way students become aware <strong>of</strong>what they already know about the text, what they expect to get out <strong>of</strong> it, and how to read it effectively helping themidentifying and remembering the necessary information later on. As <strong>reading</strong> is not just a cognitive task and cannotbe developed just by an individual practice, it is the teacher’s duty to help his/her students be aware <strong>of</strong> their thinkingprocesses and develop their <strong>reading</strong> comprehension consequently. Of these <strong>reading</strong> <strong>strategies</strong>, some can be veryeffective for some learners while being ineffective for some others. The important thing is that the learner canchoose the <strong>strategies</strong> efficient for his/her <strong>reading</strong> or use several <strong>of</strong> them simultaneously (Güngör, 2005,102) and that422


European Journal <strong>of</strong> Educational Studies 3(2), 2011<strong>reading</strong> teachers expand average and poor readers’ <strong>awareness</strong> and their knowledge in comprehension <strong>strategies</strong> toincrease students’ performances in <strong>reading</strong> comprehension tasks (Ahmad&Asraf, 2004, 34).METHODThe present study which aims to analyze the <strong>metacognitive</strong> <strong>awareness</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>reading</strong> <strong>strategies</strong> <strong>of</strong> Turkish learners <strong>of</strong>English as a foreign language is a descriptive one.The Study GroupThe study group was composed <strong>of</strong> freshmen, sophomore and junior students studying English Language Educationat a state university. The descriptive data for the students participated in the study is shown in Table 1 below.Table 1: The descriptive data for the study groupGenderTotalFemale MaleGrade 1 42 10 522 43 7 503 21 9 30Total 106 26 132Data Gathering Process and AnalysisSurvey <strong>of</strong> Reading Strategies (SORS) developed by Kouider and Sheorey (2002) was used for gathering the data.The scale has three sub-dimensions: global <strong>strategies</strong>, supporting <strong>strategies</strong> and problem solving <strong>strategies</strong>, and thereare 30 items in these sub-dimensions in total. Students’ agreeing level with the statements in each item wasdetermined and then the <strong>reading</strong> <strong>strategies</strong> total points for the sub-dimensions and the scale in general wascalculated. The scale was pre-applied to another group before it was applied to the study group <strong>of</strong> the present studyand the reliability coefficient was calculated then (Alpha= 86.6).Nonparametric statistics were used with the data gathered taking the range in the number <strong>of</strong> the participants intoconsideration. So, Mann-Whitney U was used for the difference resulting from the gender, Kruskall-Wallis Test forthe grade level difference and students’ views regarding the items were analyzed using the mean and standarddeviation.RESULTIn this part <strong>of</strong> the study, descriptive data for the use <strong>of</strong> <strong>reading</strong> <strong>strategies</strong> and the difference between the use <strong>of</strong> these<strong>strategies</strong> depending on gender and grade level variables was tested.Table 2: Descriptive data for the students’ use <strong>of</strong> <strong>reading</strong> <strong>strategies</strong>Statements n M Sd1. I have a purpose in mind when I read. 132 3,9 1,092. I take notes while <strong>reading</strong> to help me understand what I read. 130 3,5 1,083. I think about what I know to help me understand what I read. 129 3,8 0,984. I take an overall view <strong>of</strong> the text to see what it is about before <strong>reading</strong> it 127 3,7 1,16423


European Journal <strong>of</strong> Educational Studies 3(2), 2011Based on the answers that the participants gave to the scale items, it can be said that the <strong>metacognitive</strong> <strong>awareness</strong> <strong>of</strong><strong>reading</strong> <strong>strategies</strong> differ depending on gender. When the mean rank is taken into consideration, it is seen thatfemales’ mean scores are higher than those <strong>of</strong> males in GLOB, SUP and PROB <strong>strategies</strong>. However, the differencebetween the means is not statistically significant (U Glob = 863.5, p> 0.05; U Sub = 861.5, p> 0.05; U Prob = 860.0, p>0.05).Table 4: Kruskal Wallis Test results <strong>of</strong> the <strong>metacognitive</strong> <strong>awareness</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>reading</strong><strong>strategies</strong> depending on the grade levelsinif N Mean Rank Chi-Square pGlob 1,00 49 61,052,00 38 53,923,00 27 56,09Sup 1,00 51 71,572,00 46 56,083,00 27 56,31Prob 1,00 49 74,522,00 44 46,953,00 27 57,131,063 ,5885,548 ,06214,977 ,001University students’ <strong>metacognitive</strong> <strong>awareness</strong> <strong>of</strong> the <strong>reading</strong> <strong>strategies</strong> differ according to the grade level. As thegrade increases, the use <strong>of</strong> GLOB, SUB and PROB <strong>strategies</strong> decreases. However, the difference between the gradelevels is not statistically significant in GLOB and SUB (X 2 Glob= 1,063, p> 0.05; X 2 Sub = 5.548, p> 0.05) <strong>strategies</strong>while it is statistically significant in PROB <strong>strategies</strong> (X 2 Prob= 14.977, p< 0.05). So, it can be said that problemsolving <strong>strategies</strong> for <strong>reading</strong> comprehension is used more by the freshmen compared to the sophomores and juniors.CONCLUSIONThe present study which aimed at finding out the <strong>metacognitive</strong> <strong>awareness</strong> <strong>of</strong> the <strong>reading</strong> <strong>strategies</strong> <strong>of</strong> universitystudents who learn English as a foreign language came up with the findings below:University students use PROB, GLOB and SUB <strong>strategies</strong> respectively. While the strategy used most is the problemsolving <strong>strategies</strong>, the least used one is the supporting <strong>strategies</strong>. It can be said that the university students use theproblem solving <strong>strategies</strong> to solve the comprehension problems while <strong>reading</strong> a text in English. So, the students usetechniques as adjusting their <strong>reading</strong> speed to understand better, re-<strong>reading</strong> at times, paying closer attention whenthey have difficulty in understanding the text and guessing the unknown words.Other <strong>strategies</strong> used by university students while <strong>reading</strong> a text in English are global <strong>reading</strong> <strong>strategies</strong>. They usethese <strong>strategies</strong> usually in the pre-<strong>reading</strong> session to understand the text better. Students use such techniques asdetermining the purpose for <strong>reading</strong>, taking an overall view <strong>of</strong> the text, seeing how the text is organized, usingvisuals like pictures, figures or tables and paying attention to the words or sentences in bold face and italic.The <strong>strategies</strong> that university students use less are the supporting <strong>reading</strong> <strong>strategies</strong>, which are used while <strong>reading</strong> tobetter understand and increase remembering. While <strong>reading</strong>, students use the techniques like taking notes,underlining the important points, using a dictionary, <strong>reading</strong> aloud, paraphrasing, and asking questions andanswering them.425


European Journal <strong>of</strong> Educational Studies 3(2), 2011Freshmen use GLOB, SUB and PROB <strong>strategies</strong> more than the sophomores and juniors. Thus, it can be said that thelevel <strong>of</strong> using <strong>reading</strong> <strong>strategies</strong> decreases as the grade level increases. In other words, as the grade level increases,students’ pr<strong>of</strong>iciency in the foreign language they are learning also increases and this language development makestheir <strong>reading</strong> easier.Although there is a difference in the university students’ <strong>metacognitive</strong> <strong>awareness</strong> <strong>of</strong> the <strong>reading</strong> <strong>strategies</strong> in favor<strong>of</strong> females, this difference is not statistically significant.REFERENCESAhmad, I. S. & Asraf, R. M. (2004). Making sense <strong>of</strong> text: Strategies used by good and average readers. TheReading Matrix, 4 (1), 26-37. Retrieved January 2, 2011, fromhttp://www.<strong>reading</strong>matrix.com/articles/asraf_ahmad/article.pdf.Anderson, N. J. (2002). The role <strong>of</strong> metacognition in second language teaching and learning. Washington, DC:ERIC Clearinghouse on Languages and Linguistics, Center for Applied Linguistics. Retrieved December 3,2010, from http://www.cal.org/resources/digest/digest_pdfs/0110_Anderson.pdf.Block, E. (1986). The comprehension <strong>strategies</strong> <strong>of</strong> second language readers. TESOL Quarterly, 20 (3). RetrievedDecember 3, 2010, from http://www.jstor.org/stable/3586295Brushaber, T. (2003). Teaching comprehension through a comprehension strategy framework. Southwest TexasState University, 1-19. Retrieved December 3, 2010, from http://www.eric.ed.gov/PDFS/ED477160.pdfCarrell, P. L. (1989). Metacognitive Awareness and Second Language Reading. The Modern Language Journal, 73(2), 121-134.Carrell, P. L., Pharis, B. G., Liberto, J. C. (1989). Metacognitive strategy training for ESL <strong>reading</strong>. TESOLQuarterly, 23 (4), 647-678.Duffy, G. G., Roehler, L. R., Sivan, E., Rackliffe, G., Book, C., Meloth, M. S., Vavrus, L. G., Wesselman, R.,Putnam, J., Bassiri, D. (1987). Effects <strong>of</strong> explaining the reasoning associated with using <strong>reading</strong> <strong>strategies</strong>.Reading Research Quarterly, 22(3), 347-368.Ghonsooly, B., Eghtesadee, A-R. (2006). Role <strong>of</strong> cognitive style <strong>of</strong> field-dependence/indepence in using<strong>metacognitive</strong> and cognitive <strong>reading</strong> <strong>strategies</strong> by a group <strong>of</strong> skilled and novice Iranian students <strong>of</strong> Englishliterature. Asian EFL Journal, 8(4), 119-150.Gil, A., Osiecki, N., Juarez, A. (2001). Students reflecting on what they know. Paper presented at the AnnualMeeting <strong>of</strong> the International Council for Innovation in Higher Education (19 th , Rome, Italy).Güngör, A. (2005). Altıncı, yedinci ve sekizinci sınıf öğrencilerinin okuduğunu anlama stratejilerini kullanmadüzeyleri (Sixth, seventh and eighth graders’ level <strong>of</strong> <strong>reading</strong> comprehension strategy use). HacettepeÜniversitesi Eğitim Fakültesi Dergisi, 28, 1001-108.Hamdan, A. R., Ghafar, M. N., Sihes, A. J., Atan, S. B. (2010). The cognitive and metacognition <strong>reading</strong> <strong>strategies</strong><strong>of</strong> foundation course students in Teacher Education Institute in Malaysia. European Journal <strong>of</strong> SocialSciences, 13(1), 133-144.426


European Journal <strong>of</strong> Educational Studies 3(2), 2011Hernàndez-Laboy, O.I. (2009). Reading <strong>strategies</strong> ESL teachers utilize to enhance comprehension in students:Implications for classroom practices. Dissertation presented to the Department <strong>of</strong> Graduate Studies College<strong>of</strong> Education University <strong>of</strong> Puerto Rico.Israel, S. E. (2007). Thinking <strong>metacognitive</strong>ly. In Using <strong>metacognitive</strong> assessments to create individualized <strong>reading</strong>instruction (pp.1-13). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.Magno, C. (2008). Reading strategy, amount <strong>of</strong> writing, metacognition, metamemory, and apprehension aspredictors <strong>of</strong> English written pr<strong>of</strong>iciency. Asian EFL Journal, 29. Retrieved January 3, 2011, fromhttp://www.asian-efl-journal.com/pta_July_08_cm.php.Presley, M. (2002). Metacognition and self-regulated comprehension. In A. E. Farstrup & S.J. Samuels (Eds.), WhatResearch Has to Say About Reading Instruction (pp. 291-309). Newark, DE: International ReadingAssociation.Pressley, M. & Gaskins, I. W. (2006). Metacognitively competent <strong>reading</strong> comprehension is constructivelyresponsive <strong>reading</strong>: How can such <strong>reading</strong> be developed in students? Metacognition Learning, 1, 99-113.Pressley, M., Wharton-McDonald, R. (1997). Skilled comprehension and its development through instruction.School Psychology Review, 26(3), 448-466.Sanacore, J. (1984). Metacognition and the improvement <strong>of</strong> <strong>reading</strong>: Some important links. Journal <strong>of</strong> Reading,27(8), 706-712.Schneider, W. (1988). Review: Cognition, metacognition, and <strong>reading</strong>. Educational Researcher, 17(3), 53-55.Sheorey, R.,&Baboczky, E. S. Metacognitive <strong>awareness</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>reading</strong> <strong>strategies</strong> among Hungarian college students. InK. Mokhtari & R. Sheorey (Eds.), Reading Strategies <strong>of</strong> First- and Second-Language Learners. See HowThey Read (pp. 161-173). Massachusetts: Cristopher-Gordon Publishers, Inc.Sheorey, R.,& Mokhtari, K. (2001). Differences in the <strong>metacognitive</strong> <strong>awareness</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>reading</strong> <strong>strategies</strong> among nativeand non-native readers. System, 29, 431-449.Shimamura, A. P. (2000). Review: What is metacognition? The brain knows (book review). The American Journal<strong>of</strong> Psychology, 113(1), 142-146.Willis, J. (2009). What brain research suggests for teaching <strong>reading</strong> <strong>strategies</strong>. The Educational Forum, 73(4), 333-346.http://wik.ed.uiuc.edu/index.php/Metacognitive_knowledge.427

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!