12.07.2015 Views

The Relationship between Sex Guilt and Sexual Desire in a ...

The Relationship between Sex Guilt and Sexual Desire in a ...

The Relationship between Sex Guilt and Sexual Desire in a ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

JOURNAL OF SEX RESEARCH, 49(2–3), 290–298, 2012Copyright # <strong>The</strong> Society for the Scientific Study of <strong>Sex</strong>ualityISSN: 0022-4499 pr<strong>in</strong>t=1559-8519 onl<strong>in</strong>eDOI: 10.1080/00224499.2010.551792<strong>The</strong> <strong>Relationship</strong> <strong>between</strong> <strong>Sex</strong> <strong>Guilt</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Sex</strong>ual <strong>Desire</strong> <strong>in</strong> a CommunitySample of Ch<strong>in</strong>ese <strong>and</strong> Euro-Canadian WomenDownloaded by [<strong>The</strong> University of British Columbia] at 10:24 13 March 2012Jane S. T. WooDepartment of Psychology, University of British ColumbiaLori A. BrottoDepartment of Obstetrics <strong>and</strong> Gynecology, University of British ColumbiaBoris B. GorzalkaDepartment of Psychology, University of British ColumbiaMany studies have documented significant differences <strong>in</strong> sexual desire <strong>between</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividuals ofEuropean <strong>and</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese descent, but few have exam<strong>in</strong>ed the mechanisms that underlie thesedifferences. A recent study of university students found that sex guilt is one mechanism bywhich culture <strong>in</strong>fluences sexual desire among Ch<strong>in</strong>ese <strong>and</strong> Euro-Canadian women. <strong>The</strong> goalof this study was to exam<strong>in</strong>e whether sex guilt also mediates the relationship <strong>between</strong>ethnicity <strong>and</strong> sexual desire <strong>in</strong> a sample that is more representative of women <strong>in</strong> the generalpopulation. Euro-Canadian (n ¼ 78; mean age ¼ 42.1 years) <strong>and</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese (n ¼ 87; meanage ¼ 42.8 years) women were recruited from the community. Euro-Canadian womenreported greater sexual desire <strong>and</strong> less sex guilt. In the entire sample, sex guilt mediatedthe relationship <strong>between</strong> ethnicity <strong>and</strong> sexual desire such that the Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women reportedgreater sex guilt, which, <strong>in</strong> turn, was associated with lower sexual desire. Among the Ch<strong>in</strong>esewomen, sex guilt mediated the relationship <strong>between</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>stream acculturation (degree ofWesternization) <strong>and</strong> sexual desire such that more Westernized Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women reported lesssex guilt, which, <strong>in</strong> turn, was associated with greater sexual desire. <strong>The</strong>se results supportrecent f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong> further suggest that sex guilt may be one mechanism by which ethnicityaffects sexual desire.Research compar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividuals of European <strong>and</strong> Asi<strong>and</strong>escent on sexuality-related variables has found significantgroup differences across almost every doma<strong>in</strong> ofsexuality that has been exam<strong>in</strong>ed. However, there hasbeen a remarkable paucity of research on the causes ofthese cultural differences <strong>in</strong> sexuality, with few studiesthat have exam<strong>in</strong>ed variables that may mediate thesecultural effects. A better underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of such mediat<strong>in</strong>gvariables has important implications for treatmentof low desire, the most prevalent of women’s sexualfunction<strong>in</strong>g concerns (Ca<strong>in</strong> et al., 2003; Laumann et al.,This research was supported by a Hampton Fund Research Grantfrom the University of British Columbia awarded to Lori A. Brotto.Jane S. T. Woo was funded by a Predoctoral Fellowship from theCanadian Institutes of Health Research <strong>and</strong> a Predoctoral ResearchTra<strong>in</strong>ee Award from the Michael Smith Foundation for HealthResearch. We acknowledge Dr. Del Paulhus for his help with thestatistical analyses.Correspondence should be addressed to Lori A. Brotto, Departmentof Obstetrics <strong>and</strong> Gynecology, University of British Columbia,2775 Laurel St., 6th Floor, Vancouver, British Columbia V5Z 1M9,Canada. E-mail: Lori.Brotto@vch.ca2005) but a condition with few evidence-based optionsavailable. <strong>The</strong> goal of the current study is to clarifythe role of sex guilt as a possible underly<strong>in</strong>g mechanismby which culture <strong>in</strong>fluences sexual desire <strong>in</strong> Euro-Canadian <strong>and</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women. Rapid growth <strong>in</strong> theAsian population <strong>in</strong> North America has promptedgreater research <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>fluence of culture ondoma<strong>in</strong>s of sexuality rang<strong>in</strong>g from sexual knowledgeto sexual attitudes, sexual dysfunctions, sexual behaviors,<strong>and</strong> reproductive health. Much of the researchefforts <strong>in</strong> this area have focused on compar<strong>in</strong>g Asianwith Western sexuality. A review of the literature revealsthat there are notable differences <strong>between</strong> the two culturesthat span the various dimensions of sexuality thathave been studied, with <strong>in</strong>dividuals of Asian descentconsistently report<strong>in</strong>g more sexually conservative tendenciesthan their European descent counterparts.Research <strong>in</strong> university samples has found that comparedto North American norms, Asian <strong>in</strong>dividuals possessless accurate sexual knowledge (e.g., Brotto, Chik,Ryder, Gorzalka, & Seal, 2005; Meston, Trapnell, &


SEX GUILT AND SEXUAL DESIREDownloaded by [<strong>The</strong> University of British Columbia] at 10:24 13 March 2012Gorzalka, 1998; Woo, Brotto, & Gorzalka, 2009) <strong>and</strong>report more conservative sexual attitudes (e.g., Ahrold& Meston, 2010; Higg<strong>in</strong>s & Sun, 2007; Meston,Trapnell, & Gorzalka, 1996; Woo, Brotto, & Gorzalka,2010a). Asian <strong>in</strong>dividuals also report higher rates ofsexual difficulties (e.g., Brotto et al., 2005; Woo et al.,2010a), <strong>and</strong> more acceptance of sexual harassment(Kennedy & Gorzalka, 2002), demonstrate a restrictedrange of sexual behaviors (e.g., Meston & Ahrold,2010; Meston et al., 1998; Tang, Lai, & Chung, 1997;Woo et al., 2009), <strong>and</strong> exhibit lower rates of participation<strong>in</strong> preventive screen<strong>in</strong>g procedures <strong>in</strong> reproductivehealth (e.g., Woo et al., 2009). Similar f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gshave been reported <strong>in</strong> community-based samples (e.g.,Ca<strong>in</strong> et al., 2003; Hislop et al., 2004; Kagawa-S<strong>in</strong>ger &Pourat, 2000; Laumann et al., 2005; Taylor et al.,2002; Tu et al., 2005; Yu, Wu, & Mood, 2005).In addition to study<strong>in</strong>g ethnic differences as one <strong>in</strong>dicatorof cultural effects, some researchers have alsoexam<strong>in</strong>ed how acculturation with<strong>in</strong> Asian <strong>in</strong>dividualsaffects various doma<strong>in</strong>s of sexuality. Acculturationrefers to the process by which an <strong>in</strong>dividual who belongsto an ethnic m<strong>in</strong>ority group assimilates aspects of thedom<strong>in</strong>ant culture’s values, attitudes, <strong>and</strong> behaviors <strong>in</strong>totheir self-identity (Ryder, Alden, & Paulhus, 2000). <strong>The</strong><strong>in</strong>dividual’s self-identity <strong>and</strong> extent of affiliation withboth the culture of orig<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> the dom<strong>in</strong>ant cultureare modified to accommodate <strong>in</strong>formation about <strong>and</strong>experiences with the new culture. Thus, study<strong>in</strong>g acculturationis important as a richer underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of howculture affects sexuality can be ga<strong>in</strong>ed than by study<strong>in</strong>gthe effects of ethnic group alone. Both the degree ofaffiliation with the culture of orig<strong>in</strong> (heritage culture)<strong>and</strong> the degree of affiliation with the dom<strong>in</strong>ant culture(ma<strong>in</strong>stream culture) will vary among members of anethnic group, <strong>and</strong> exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g these two dimensions ofacculturation separately (i.e., bidimensionally) enablesus to study the effects of culture <strong>between</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividualsof the same ethnic group.Among Asian <strong>in</strong>dividuals liv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> North America,significant associations have been found <strong>between</strong> acculturation<strong>and</strong> a number of sexuality-related variables. Infemale university students, for <strong>in</strong>stance, greater ma<strong>in</strong>streamacculturation has been found to be significantlyassociated with higher sexual desire <strong>and</strong> arousal (Brottoet al., 2005), higher sexual arousability (Brotto et al.,2005), more accurate sexual knowledge (Woo et al.,2009), less sex guilt (Abramson & Imai-Marquez, 1982;Woo et al., 2010a), <strong>and</strong> higher participation rates <strong>in</strong>sexual psychophysiological research (Woo, Brotto, &Yule, 2010). Similarly, greater heritage acculturationhas been found to be associated with lower rates of cervicalcancer screen<strong>in</strong>g (Woo et al., 2009), more conservativeattitudes toward casual sex <strong>and</strong> gender role traditionality(Ahrold & Meston, 2010), less experience <strong>in</strong> masturbation<strong>and</strong> oral sex (Meston & Ahrold, 2010), <strong>and</strong> olderage at sexual debut (Meston & Ahrold, 2010). Similarf<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs have been reported among male universitystudents <strong>in</strong> North America. Greater ma<strong>in</strong>stream acculturationhas been found to be associated with greatersexual openness <strong>and</strong> higher sexual function<strong>in</strong>g (Brotto,Woo, & Ryder, 2007), less sex guilt (Abramson &Imai-Marquez, 1982; Woo, Brotto, & Gorzalka,2010b), <strong>and</strong> higher rates of conduct<strong>in</strong>g testicular selfexam<strong>in</strong>ations(Woo et al., 2010b). Greater heritage acculturationsignificantly predicted greater conservatism <strong>in</strong>attitudes toward casual sex, homosexuality, <strong>and</strong> genderrole traditionality (Ahrold & Meston, 2010). Older ageat sexual debut has also been associated with greater heritageacculturation <strong>in</strong> men (Meston & Ahrold, 2010).Overall, higher ma<strong>in</strong>stream acculturation is associatedwith greater sexual openness, whereas greater heritageacculturation is l<strong>in</strong>ked to greater sexual conservatism.Thus, there are several studies that have explored ethnicdifferences <strong>between</strong> Asian <strong>and</strong> Western sexuality, aswell as a number of studies that have exam<strong>in</strong>ed theeffects of acculturation on sexuality among Asian <strong>in</strong>dividuals.In contrast, research on the underly<strong>in</strong>g mechanismsof these cultural differences has lagged beh<strong>in</strong>d.Given that cultural differences (both ethnic group <strong>and</strong>acculturation effects) have been observed <strong>and</strong> documented<strong>in</strong> myriad facets of sexuality, <strong>and</strong> that these differenceshave been remarkably consistent <strong>in</strong> nature, acloser exploration of the mechanisms by which cultureaffects sexuality may improve the underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g ofcultural <strong>in</strong>fluences on sexuality.Among the research on cultural differences <strong>in</strong> sexualitythat has explored possible underly<strong>in</strong>g mechanisms,one recent study exam<strong>in</strong>ed the <strong>in</strong>fluence of acculturation<strong>and</strong> religiosity on sexual attitudes <strong>in</strong> Euro-American,Asian American, <strong>and</strong> Hispanic American <strong>in</strong>dividuals(Ahrold & Meston, 2010). No mediat<strong>in</strong>g effect of acculturation<strong>in</strong> the relationship <strong>between</strong> religiosity <strong>and</strong>sexual attitudes was found.<strong>The</strong> role of sex guilt has been explored as a possibleunderly<strong>in</strong>g mechanism of culture-l<strong>in</strong>ked differences <strong>in</strong>sexual desire among East Asian (Ch<strong>in</strong>ese, Japanese,<strong>and</strong> Korean) <strong>and</strong> Euro-Canadian female university students(Woo et al., 2010a). Woo et al. (2010a) drew onthe def<strong>in</strong>ition of sex guilt offered by Mosher <strong>and</strong> Cross(1971, p. 27): ‘‘a general expectancy for self-mediatedpunishment for violat<strong>in</strong>g or for anticipat<strong>in</strong>g violat<strong>in</strong>gst<strong>and</strong>ards of proper sexual conduct. Such a dispositionmight be manifested by resistance to sexual temptation,by <strong>in</strong>hibited sexual behavior, or by the disruption ofcognitive processes <strong>in</strong> sex-related situations.’’ Interest<strong>in</strong>gly,Woo <strong>and</strong> colleagues (2010a) found that sex guiltmediated the relationship <strong>between</strong> ethnicity <strong>and</strong> sexualdesire, with East Asian ethnicity be<strong>in</strong>g significantly associatedwith greater sex guilt, which, <strong>in</strong> turn, was associatedwith less sexual desire. <strong>Sex</strong> guilt, but not sexualconservatism, was found to mediate the relationship<strong>between</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>stream acculturation <strong>and</strong> sexual desireamong the East Asian women, such that greater291


WOO, BROTTO, AND GORZALKADownloaded by [<strong>The</strong> University of British Columbia] at 10:24 13 March 2012ma<strong>in</strong>stream acculturation was predictive of less sexguilt, which, <strong>in</strong> turn, was associated with greatersexual desire. No mediat<strong>in</strong>g effect of sex guilt was found<strong>in</strong> the relationship <strong>between</strong> heritage acculturation <strong>and</strong>sexual desire.With East Asian women consistently report<strong>in</strong>g lowersexual desire than women of European descent, a greaterunderst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of the role of sex guilt <strong>in</strong> sexual desiremay represent an important new avenue by which compla<strong>in</strong>tsof low sexual desire may be addressed <strong>in</strong> psychologicaltreatment. However, one of the limitations ofthe study by Woo et al. (2010a) was the use of a universityconvenience sample <strong>in</strong> which participants were, onaverage, significantly younger than women <strong>in</strong> the generalpopulation (mean age ¼ 20 years). Thus, the vastmajority of the women were unmarried, render<strong>in</strong>g itimpossible to ascerta<strong>in</strong> unequivocally the relationshipsamong culture, sex guilt, <strong>and</strong> sexual desire among EastAsian women <strong>in</strong> the general population. This is of particularsignificance <strong>in</strong> study<strong>in</strong>g East Asian sexualitybecause of the traditional East Asian view that sexualactivity is acceptable only <strong>in</strong> the context of marriage.Parents are highly <strong>in</strong>tolerant of premarital sexualactivity, especially <strong>in</strong> daughters, <strong>and</strong> children arebrought up with strong parental <strong>and</strong> societal expectationsthat expressions of sexuality will be reserved formarriage (Lee & Chang, 1999; Youn, 2001).<strong>The</strong> purpose of this study was to determ<strong>in</strong>e outcomes<strong>in</strong> the general population by exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g sex guilt <strong>and</strong>sexual desire <strong>in</strong> a sample of Euro-Canadian <strong>and</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>esewomen recruited from the community, <strong>and</strong> to exam<strong>in</strong>ethe possible protective effect of be<strong>in</strong>g married. Basedon the results of population-based studies that foundsignificant ethnic differences <strong>in</strong> sex guilt <strong>and</strong> sexualdesire (Abramson & Imai-Marquez, 1982; Ca<strong>in</strong> et al.,2003; Laumann et al., 2005), we hypothesized that theCh<strong>in</strong>ese women would report greater sex guilt <strong>and</strong> lowersexual desire than their Euro-Canadian counterparts.Among the Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women, we hypothesized thatgreater ma<strong>in</strong>stream acculturation <strong>and</strong> lower heritageacculturation would be associated with greater sexualdesire <strong>and</strong> less sex guilt. Furthermore, we hypothesizedthat sex guilt would mediate the relationship <strong>between</strong>ethnicity <strong>and</strong> sexual desire <strong>in</strong> the entire sample, as wellas the relationship <strong>between</strong> acculturation <strong>and</strong> sexualdesire among the Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women.MethodParticipantsCh<strong>in</strong>ese <strong>and</strong> Euro-Canadian women 20 years of age<strong>and</strong> older were eligible to participate <strong>in</strong> this study. Atotal of 165 women participated. Of these, 87self-identified as Ch<strong>in</strong>ese, <strong>and</strong> 78 self-identified asEuro-Canadian. Participants were recruited from the292community us<strong>in</strong>g a variety of methods, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g a lettermail campaign <strong>and</strong> the snowball method. Flyers werealso posted <strong>in</strong> public areas describ<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>clusion criteria<strong>and</strong> provid<strong>in</strong>g a brief description of the study. Ofthe participants <strong>in</strong> this study, 51 were recruited via a lettermail campaign, 64 were recruited through flyersposted <strong>in</strong> public places, <strong>and</strong> 50 were recruited by thesnowball method. About 200 letters were mailed outto Euro-Canadian <strong>and</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women as part of theletter mail campaign.<strong>The</strong>re were significant ethnic differences <strong>in</strong> the proportionof participants recruited by the variousmethods, v 2 (2, N ¼ 165) ¼ 29.15, p < .001. Of the Euro-Canadian women, 51% were recruited by the letter mailcampaign, 19% by the snowball method, <strong>and</strong> 30% bysee<strong>in</strong>g the posted flyers. Of the Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women, 13%were recruited by the letter mail campaign, 40% by thesnowball method, <strong>and</strong> 47% by see<strong>in</strong>g the posted flyers.<strong>The</strong>re were no significant ethnic differences <strong>in</strong> age,but the Euro-Canadian women reported liv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>Canada for a significantly longer period of time thanthe Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women. Demographic data are presented<strong>in</strong> Table 1.ProcedureInterested participants telephoned the laboratory<strong>and</strong> were screened to ensure that they self-identified aseither Ch<strong>in</strong>ese or Euro-Canadian <strong>and</strong> met the m<strong>in</strong>imumage requirement. Participants were <strong>in</strong>terviewed <strong>in</strong> personby tra<strong>in</strong>ed female psychology students at a time<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> a location of the participants’ choice. Locationsthat were commonly selected <strong>in</strong>cluded the researchTable 1. Demographic Characteristics of Euro-Canadian(n ¼ 78) <strong>and</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese (n ¼ 87) ParticipantsVariable Euro-Canadian Ch<strong>in</strong>eseMean age <strong>in</strong> years (SD) 42.1 (17.2) 42.8 (13.2)Place of birth (%) Canada or United States 82.1 9.2Ch<strong>in</strong>a=Hong Kong=Taiwan 1.3 86.2Southeast Asia 0.0 2.3Europe 11.5 0.0Other 5.1 2.3Years of residency <strong>in</strong> Canada (SD) 36.4 (18.6) 12.0 (10.4)Marital status a (%) Unmarried 51.4 23.5Common-law 8.1 2.9Married 24.3 58.8Divorced 13.5 10.3Widowed 2.7 4.4Mean acculturation score b (SD)Ma<strong>in</strong>stream NA 59.0 (12.7)Heritage NA 68.4 (12.8)a Figures reported are for the 142 participants who responded to thisquestion.b Scale ranges from 20–180. p < .001.


SEX GUILT AND SEXUAL DESIREDownloaded by [<strong>The</strong> University of British Columbia] at 10:24 13 March 2012laboratory, participants’ homes, cafes, <strong>and</strong> libraries.All of the Euro-Canadian women were <strong>in</strong>terviewed <strong>in</strong>English. <strong>The</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women were <strong>in</strong>terviewed <strong>in</strong>the language of their choice (English, Cantonese, orM<strong>and</strong>ar<strong>in</strong>). <strong>The</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview consisted of a number ofvalidated measures that were verbally adm<strong>in</strong>istered.Measures conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g items of a more personal nature,such as those relat<strong>in</strong>g to sexual function<strong>in</strong>g, werecompleted by the participants themselves if they chose.Participants received a monetary honorarium for theirparticipation <strong>in</strong> this study. All procedures were approvedby the university’s Behavioural Research Ethics Board.MeasuresVancouver Index of Acculturation (VIA). <strong>The</strong> VIA(Ryder et al., 2000) is a self-report questionnaire thatassesses heritage <strong>and</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>stream acculturation on twoseparate dimensions. ‘‘Heritage culture’’ refers to the<strong>in</strong>dividual’s culture of birth or upbr<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>g, whereas‘‘ma<strong>in</strong>stream culture’’ refers to the predom<strong>in</strong>ant culture<strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>dividual’s new environment. In the context ofthis study, heritage culture refers to Ch<strong>in</strong>ese culture,<strong>and</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>stream culture refers to Western culture. <strong>The</strong>VIA consists of 20 items, with two items associated witheach of 10 doma<strong>in</strong>s, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g enterta<strong>in</strong>ment preferences<strong>and</strong> cultural traditions. Higher scores on the ma<strong>in</strong>streamdimension <strong>in</strong>dicate greater Westernization, <strong>and</strong> higherscores on the heritage dimension demonstrate greaterma<strong>in</strong>tenance of Ch<strong>in</strong>ese culture <strong>and</strong> traditions. Examplesof items that comprise the heritage dimension<strong>in</strong>clude, ‘‘I would be will<strong>in</strong>g to marry a person frommy heritage culture,’’ <strong>and</strong> ‘‘I enjoy social activitieswith people from the same heritage culture as myself.’’Examples of items that comprise the ma<strong>in</strong>streamdimension <strong>in</strong>clude, ‘‘I am comfortable work<strong>in</strong>g withtypical North American people,’’ <strong>and</strong> ‘‘I enjoy NorthAmerican enterta<strong>in</strong>ment.’’ Both dimensions werefound to have good <strong>in</strong>ternal consistency <strong>in</strong> this sample(Cronbach’s a ¼ .90 for heritage acculturation <strong>and</strong> .89for ma<strong>in</strong>stream acculturation).Female <strong>Sex</strong>ual Function Index (FSFI). <strong>The</strong> FSFI(Rosen et al., 2000) is a 19-item, self-report questionnairethat assesses six doma<strong>in</strong>s of sexual function. In thisstudy, we exam<strong>in</strong>ed only the desire subscale of the FSFI<strong>in</strong> which higher scores <strong>in</strong>dicated higher levels of sexualdesire over the previous four weeks. <strong>The</strong> two questionsthat comprise the desire subscale are, ‘‘Over the past 4weeks, how often did you feel sexual desire or <strong>in</strong>terest?,’’<strong>and</strong> ‘‘Over the past 4 weeks, how would you rate yourlevel (degree) of sexual desire or <strong>in</strong>terest?’’ Internal consistency<strong>in</strong> this sample was high (Cronbach’s a ¼ .91).Women with hypoactive sexual desire disorder (HSDD)may be differentiated from those without us<strong>in</strong>g the FSFI(Wiegel, Meston, & Rosen, 2005). <strong>The</strong> FSFI desiredoma<strong>in</strong> is relevant for women who have been sexuallyactive over the previous four weeks, as well as thosewho have not (Meyer-Bahlburg & Dolezal, 2007).Revised Mosher <strong>Guilt</strong> Inventory (RMGI). <strong>The</strong>RMGI (Mosher, 1988) is a self-report questionnairethat measures three dimensions of guilt: sex guilt, hostilityguilt, <strong>and</strong> guilty conscience. It consists of 114items <strong>in</strong> a limited comparison format. In this format,items are presented <strong>in</strong> pairs, <strong>and</strong> participants are askedto rate their responses on a seven-po<strong>in</strong>t Likert scalethat ranges from 0 (not at all true for me) to 6(extremely true for me) while compar<strong>in</strong>g the degree oftrueness with<strong>in</strong> each item pair. As the variable of <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong> this study was sex guilt, only the 50 items thatcomprise the sex guilt subscale were adm<strong>in</strong>istered:‘‘When I have sexual desires, I enjoy it like all healthyhuman be<strong>in</strong>gs,’’ <strong>and</strong> ‘‘When I have sexual desires, Ifight them because I must have complete control ofmy body.’’ <strong>The</strong> total score on this subscale ranges from0 to 300, with higher scores signify<strong>in</strong>g greater sex guilt.Internal consistency for the sex guilt subscale <strong>in</strong> thissample was high (Cronbach’s a ¼ .95). <strong>The</strong> construct,convergent, <strong>and</strong> discrim<strong>in</strong>ant validity of earlier versionsof the Mosher <strong>Guilt</strong> Inventory have been establishedby various studies (e.g., Abramson & Mosher, 1979;Ruma & Mosher, 1967).Demographics questionnaire. An <strong>in</strong>vestigatorderivedquestionnaire assess<strong>in</strong>g demographic variables,<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g relationship <strong>and</strong> sexual function<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong>experience with various types of sexual activities wasalso adm<strong>in</strong>istered.Statistical AnalysesSPSS Version 13 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, Ill<strong>in</strong>ois) wasused for all statistical analyses. T tests were used <strong>in</strong> comparisonsof the two ethnic groups on self-reported sexguilt <strong>and</strong> sexual desire. In analyses of the association<strong>between</strong> ethnicity <strong>and</strong> sexual variables, the po<strong>in</strong>t-biserialstatistic was used.<strong>The</strong> bootstrap method for mediation analysis recommendedby Shrout <strong>and</strong> Bolger (2002) was used for allmediation analyses. Three thous<strong>and</strong> samples, withreplacement, were used <strong>in</strong> each mediation analysis.Mediation analyses were conducted us<strong>in</strong>g an SPSSmacro developed by Preacher <strong>and</strong> Hayes (2004).ResultsEthnic Group Comparisons on Basic DemographicCharacteristics<strong>The</strong>re was no significant ethnic difference <strong>in</strong> age,t(144) ¼ 0.31, p > .05 (mean age of Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women ¼293


WOO, BROTTO, AND GORZALKADownloaded by [<strong>The</strong> University of British Columbia] at 10:24 13 March 201242.84 years, SD ¼ 13.21; mean age of Euro-Canadianwomen ¼ 42.10 years, SD ¼ 17.15). <strong>The</strong>re was also nosignificant ethnic difference <strong>in</strong> the proportion of womenwho reported be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a relationship, v 2 (1, N ¼ 165) ¼1.46, p > .05. However, a significantly larger proportionof the Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women reported hav<strong>in</strong>g been married(<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g those who reported be<strong>in</strong>g married, divorced,or widowed) than the Euro-Canadian women,v 2 (1, N ¼ 142) ¼ p < .01. <strong>The</strong>re was also a significantethnic difference <strong>in</strong> years of education, with the Euro-Canadian women report<strong>in</strong>g more years of education,t(163) ¼ 3.73, p < .001.Ethnic Group Comparisons on <strong>Sex</strong>uality Measures<strong>The</strong> two groups did not significantly differ <strong>in</strong> theproportion of women who had engaged <strong>in</strong> kiss<strong>in</strong>g,hugg<strong>in</strong>g, or hold<strong>in</strong>g h<strong>and</strong>s (p > .05). However, theEuro-Canadian women were significantly more likelyto have engaged <strong>in</strong> touch<strong>in</strong>g with cloth<strong>in</strong>g removed,v 2 (1, N ¼ 89) ¼ 11.31, p < .01; touch<strong>in</strong>g partner’s genitals,v 2 (1, N ¼ 89) ¼ 14.47, p < .001; perform<strong>in</strong>g oralsex on their partner, v 2 (1, N ¼) ¼ 13.14, p < .001; receiv<strong>in</strong>gof oral sex from their partner, v 2 (1, N ¼ 88) ¼ 6.56,p < .05; <strong>and</strong> sexual <strong>in</strong>tercourse, v 2 (1, N ¼ 91) ¼ 6.23,p < .05. All proportions are presented <strong>in</strong> Table 2.We adjusted for large family-wise error rate, apply<strong>in</strong>ga Bonferroni correction by divid<strong>in</strong>g the conventionalalpha level of .05 by two (the number ofcomparisons be<strong>in</strong>g made). Thus, ethnic differenceswere considered statistically significant only if p < .025Table 2.Ethnic Group Differences on <strong>Sex</strong>ual Activity(.05=2 comparisons). Compared to the Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women,the Euro-Canadian women scored significantly higheron the desire subscale of the FSFI, <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g thatthe Euro-Canadian women reported significantlygreater sexual desire than the Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women <strong>in</strong> theprevious four weeks, t(155) ¼ 4.21, p < .001. <strong>The</strong>Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women scored significantly higher on theRMGI compared with the Euro-Canadian women,t(137) ¼ 13.00, p < .001, demonstrat<strong>in</strong>g that the Ch<strong>in</strong>esewomen reported significantly higher levels of sex guilt(see Table 3).Scores on the RMGI <strong>and</strong> FSFI desire subscale weresignificantly <strong>and</strong> negatively correlated <strong>in</strong> both theEuro-Canadian, r(63) ¼ .47, p < .001; <strong>and</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>esewomen, r(72) ¼ .53, p < .001, <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that more sexguilt was associated with lower sexual desire.<strong>The</strong> Mediat<strong>in</strong>g Role of <strong>Sex</strong> <strong>Guilt</strong> <strong>in</strong> the <strong>Relationship</strong>Between Ethnicity <strong>and</strong> <strong>Sex</strong>ual <strong>Desire</strong><strong>The</strong> effect of ethnicity on sex guilt was demonstrated(B ¼ 75.25, p < .001), <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that Ch<strong>in</strong>ese ethnicitywas associated with significantly greater sex guilt. <strong>The</strong>effect of sex guilt on sexual desire, hold<strong>in</strong>g ethnicity constant,was also significant (b ¼ 0.02, p < .001), show<strong>in</strong>gthat <strong>in</strong>creased sex guilt was l<strong>in</strong>ked to less sexual desire.<strong>The</strong> <strong>in</strong>direct effect of ethnicity on sexual desire, computedby multiply<strong>in</strong>g the effect of ethnicity on sex guiltwith the effect of sex guilt on sexual desire while controll<strong>in</strong>gfor ethnicity, was also significant (ab ¼ 1.29;SE ¼ 0.22; 95% confidence <strong>in</strong>terval [CI] ¼ 0.74, 1.89).Variable Euro-Canadian Ch<strong>in</strong>eseWho have engaged <strong>in</strong> hugg<strong>in</strong>g, kiss<strong>in</strong>g, or hold<strong>in</strong>g h<strong>and</strong>s 97.9% 88.9%Who have engaged <strong>in</strong> touch<strong>in</strong>g with cloth<strong>in</strong>g removed 95.6% 68.2%Who have touched their partner’s genitals 93.3% 59.1%Who have performed oral sex on their partner 68.2% 29.5%Who have had oral sex performed on them by their partner 61.4% 34.1%Who have engaged <strong>in</strong> vag<strong>in</strong>al–penile <strong>in</strong>tercourse 93.3% 73.9% p < .05. p < .01. p < .001.Table 3. Ethnic Group Differences on Scores from the <strong>Desire</strong> Subscale of the Female <strong>Sex</strong>ual Function Index (FSFI) <strong>and</strong> the RevisedMosher <strong>Guilt</strong> Inventory (RMGI)Euro-CanadianCh<strong>in</strong>eseVariable M SD M SD t (df) p Effect Size cFSFI desire a 3.64 1.16 2.82 1.26 4.21 (155)


SEX GUILT AND SEXUAL DESIREFigure 1. <strong>The</strong> mediat<strong>in</strong>g role of sex guilt <strong>in</strong> the relationship <strong>between</strong>ethnicity <strong>and</strong> sexual desire <strong>in</strong> Euro-Canadian <strong>and</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women.Note. a represents the effect of ethnicity on sex guilt; b represents theeffect of sex guilt on sexual desire after controll<strong>in</strong>g for the effect of ethnicity;c represents the direct effect of ethnicity on sexual desire; <strong>and</strong> c 0represents the effect of ethnicity on sexual desire after controll<strong>in</strong>g forsex guilt. p < .001.Figure 2. <strong>The</strong> mediat<strong>in</strong>g role of sex guilt <strong>in</strong> the relationship <strong>between</strong>ma<strong>in</strong>stream acculturation <strong>and</strong> sexual desire <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women. Note. arepresents the effect of ma<strong>in</strong>stream acculturation on sex guilt; b representsthe effect of sex guilt on sexual desire after controll<strong>in</strong>g for theeffect of ma<strong>in</strong>stream acculturation; c represents the direct effect ofma<strong>in</strong>stream acculturation on sexual desire; <strong>and</strong> c 0 represents the effectof ma<strong>in</strong>stream acculturation on sexual desire after controll<strong>in</strong>g for sexguilt. p < .001.Downloaded by [<strong>The</strong> University of British Columbia] at 10:24 13 March 2012Hence, sex guilt mediated the ethnic group difference <strong>in</strong>sexual desire (see Figure 1).Effects of Acculturation (Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Women Only) onMeasures of <strong>Sex</strong>ualityMa<strong>in</strong>stream acculturation was not significantly correlatedwith FSFI desire scores, r(81) ¼ .10, p > .05; butwas significantly correlated with RMGI scores,r(75) ¼ .24, p < .05, <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that more WesternizedCh<strong>in</strong>ese women reported significantly less sex guilt.Heritage acculturation was not correlated with eitherFSFI desire or RMGI scores (all ps > .05).<strong>The</strong> Mediat<strong>in</strong>g Role of <strong>Sex</strong> <strong>Guilt</strong> <strong>in</strong> the <strong>Relationship</strong><strong>between</strong> Ma<strong>in</strong>stream Acculturation <strong>and</strong> <strong>Sex</strong>ual <strong>Desire</strong>A mediation analysis to exam<strong>in</strong>e whether sex guiltwould mediate the relationship <strong>between</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>streamacculturation <strong>and</strong> sexual desire was conducted. <strong>The</strong>effect of ma<strong>in</strong>stream acculturation on sex guilt was notsignificant at conventional alpha levels (b ¼ 0.67,p ¼ .10), <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that ma<strong>in</strong>stream acculturation wasnot significantly associated with sex guilt. On the otherh<strong>and</strong>, the effect of sex guilt on sexual desire, hold<strong>in</strong>gma<strong>in</strong>stream acculturation constant, was significant(b ¼ 0.02, p < .001), show<strong>in</strong>g that greater sex guiltwas l<strong>in</strong>ked to less sexual desire after controll<strong>in</strong>g forthe effect of ma<strong>in</strong>stream acculturation. <strong>The</strong> <strong>in</strong>directeffect of ma<strong>in</strong>stream acculturation on sexual desire wassignificant (ab ¼ 0.01; SE ¼ 0.006; 95% CI ¼ 0.0004,0.02). Hence, sex guilt mediated the relationship<strong>between</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>stream acculturation <strong>and</strong> sexual desire <strong>in</strong>the Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women (see Figure 2).DiscussionEthnicity <strong>and</strong> <strong>Sex</strong>ualityIn ethnic group comparisons, the Euro-Canadianwomen reported significantly greater sexual desirethan the Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women. This result supported ourhypothesis <strong>and</strong> is consistent with research show<strong>in</strong>gethnic differences <strong>in</strong> general sexual function <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>sexual desire particularly (e.g., Brotto et al., 2005; Ca<strong>in</strong>et al., 2003; Laumann et al., 2005; Woo et al., 2009;Woo et al., 2010a).Analyses of ethnic differences <strong>in</strong> sex guilt revealedthat the Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women reported significantly greatersex guilt compared to their Euro-Canadian counterparts.This f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g fits with both the results of Wooet al. (2010a), who found less sex guilt among Euro-Canadian <strong>in</strong>dividuals, <strong>and</strong> the results of Abramson<strong>and</strong> Imai-Marquez (1982), who found significantlyhigher levels of sex guilt <strong>in</strong> Japanese American thanEuro-American <strong>in</strong>dividuals.Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, mediation analyses revealed that sexguilt mediated the relationship <strong>between</strong> ethnicity <strong>and</strong>sexual desire, such that higher levels of sex guilt <strong>in</strong> theCh<strong>in</strong>ese women accounted for the ethnic disparity <strong>in</strong>sexual desire. This f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g is consistent with the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gsof Woo et al. (2010a) <strong>in</strong> university students, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicatesthat sex guilt is a mediator of the relationship<strong>between</strong> ethnicity <strong>and</strong> sexual desire even <strong>in</strong> this samplethat was more diverse than the university conveniencesample that was studied by Woo et al. (2010a).<strong>The</strong>se results raise the question of how the Ch<strong>in</strong>esewomen came to experience higher levels of sex guiltcompared to the Euro-Canadian women. <strong>The</strong>re havebeen three major <strong>in</strong>fluences on Ch<strong>in</strong>ese thought <strong>and</strong> culture,with Confucianism hav<strong>in</strong>g the most profound <strong>and</strong>widespread impact by far. Confucius was a Ch<strong>in</strong>eseth<strong>in</strong>ker <strong>and</strong> philosopher who lived about 2,500 yearsago <strong>and</strong> whose teach<strong>in</strong>gs were perpetuated follow<strong>in</strong>ghis death by his disciples <strong>and</strong> eventually declared theofficial state doctr<strong>in</strong>e. Confucius himself viewed sex aspositive as long as it did not disrupt <strong>in</strong>terpersonalrelationships or cause social <strong>in</strong>stability. Moreover, heconsidered sexual desire to be beneficial <strong>in</strong> fulfill<strong>in</strong>gthe filial obligation of produc<strong>in</strong>g children to carry onthe family l<strong>in</strong>e. However, Neo-Confucian scholarsdur<strong>in</strong>g the Song Dynasty (960–1276 A.D.) gave theclassical Confucian teach<strong>in</strong>gs strict <strong>in</strong>terpretations <strong>and</strong>precipitated the more repressive view of sexuality as295


WOO, BROTTO, AND GORZALKADownloaded by [<strong>The</strong> University of British Columbia] at 10:24 13 March 2012be<strong>in</strong>g reserved for marriage <strong>and</strong> procreation (Ng & Lau,1990). This is the po<strong>in</strong>t of view that has persisted untilthe present day, with parents convey<strong>in</strong>g their clearexpectation to their children, especially daughters, thatsexuality is not to be expressed outside of marriage.Acculturation <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese Women <strong>and</strong> <strong>Sex</strong>ualityAs another <strong>in</strong>dex of cultural <strong>in</strong>fluences, we exam<strong>in</strong>edthe effect of bidimensional acculturation on sex guilt<strong>and</strong> sexual desire. <strong>The</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g that ma<strong>in</strong>stream acculturationwas not significantly correlated with sexualdesire stood <strong>in</strong> contrast to our hypothesis that greaterWesternization would be associated with higher levelsof sexual desire <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women. This f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g is also<strong>in</strong>congruent with other studies that have found a significantassociation <strong>between</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>stream acculturation <strong>and</strong>sexual desire (e.g., Brotto et al., 2005; Woo et al.,2010a). One possible explanation for this f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g is thatthis study may have been <strong>in</strong>sufficiently powered, limit<strong>in</strong>gour ability to detect a significant effect of ma<strong>in</strong>streamacculturation on sexual desire. We note that this sampleof Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women is much smaller than that studied byWoo et al. (2010a). On the other h<strong>and</strong>, ma<strong>in</strong>streamacculturation was found to be significantly correlatedwith sex guilt such that greater Westernization was associatedwith less sex guilt—a f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g that is consistentwith the literature on the effects of acculturation onCh<strong>in</strong>ese sexuality (Woo et al., 2010a).<strong>The</strong> results of mediation analyses supported ourhypothesis that sex guilt would mediate the relationship<strong>between</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>stream acculturation <strong>and</strong> sexual desireamong the Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women <strong>and</strong> are <strong>in</strong> accord with theresults of Woo et al. (2010a), who found a similar mediat<strong>in</strong>geffect <strong>in</strong> a sample of female university students.Although ma<strong>in</strong>stream acculturation was not significantlyassociated with sex guilt, a crucial step <strong>in</strong> the traditionalapproach to mediation popularized by Baron<strong>and</strong> Kenny (1986), the bootstrap approach used <strong>in</strong> thisstudy <strong>in</strong>dicated that the <strong>in</strong>direct effect of ma<strong>in</strong>streamacculturation on sexual desire, through the <strong>in</strong>terven<strong>in</strong>gvariable sex guilt, was significant. <strong>The</strong> bootstrap methodwas used because of the temporally distal nature of thecausal process <strong>between</strong> culture <strong>and</strong> sexual desire. However,simulation research reveals that this method alsohas the advantages of greater power <strong>and</strong> better Type 1error control compared to the Baron <strong>and</strong> Kennyapproach (Hayes, 2009).In contrast to the mediat<strong>in</strong>g role that sex guilt plays<strong>in</strong> the relationship <strong>between</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>stream acculturation<strong>and</strong> sexual desire, mediation analyses revealed that sexguilt did not mediate the relationship <strong>between</strong> heritageacculturation <strong>and</strong> sexual desire among the Ch<strong>in</strong>esewomen. This f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g did not support our hypothesis thatsex guilt would underlie the effect of heritage acculturationon sexual desire, but is consistent with the results ofWoo et al. (2010a), who found no mediation effect <strong>in</strong> the296relationship <strong>between</strong> heritage acculturation <strong>and</strong> sexualdesire. Jo<strong>in</strong>tly, these f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs suggest that, althoughsex guilt is not associated with heritage acculturation<strong>and</strong> changes <strong>in</strong> sexual desire, sex guilt plays an importantrole <strong>in</strong> account<strong>in</strong>g for the augment<strong>in</strong>g effect ofWesternization on sexual desire.Implications<strong>The</strong>se results were consistent with those of Woo et al.(2010a) <strong>in</strong> that sex guilt mediated both the relationship<strong>between</strong> ethnicity <strong>and</strong> sexual desire <strong>in</strong> the entire sample,as well as that <strong>between</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>stream acculturation <strong>and</strong>sexual desire <strong>in</strong> the Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women. It is <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g tonote, however, that the effect of ethnicity on sex guilt isstronger <strong>in</strong> this study than <strong>in</strong> the study by Woo et al.(2010a), as <strong>in</strong>dicated by the relevant effect sizes (Cohen’sd ¼ 2.17 <strong>in</strong> this study vs. 1.48 <strong>in</strong> the study by Woo et al.,2010a). Although both effect sizes are large (Cohen,1992), the relative magnitude of the relationship <strong>between</strong>ethnicity <strong>and</strong> sex guilt <strong>in</strong> the two studies suggests that sexguilt may vary with sample characteristics—that is,younger age <strong>and</strong> more education may be factors thatprotect <strong>in</strong>dividuals from experienc<strong>in</strong>g greater guilt <strong>in</strong>sex-related situations. Support<strong>in</strong>g this proposition,additional data analyses revealed that age was significantlycorrelated with sex guilt, such that older age wasassociated with greater sex guilt <strong>in</strong> both the Euro-Canadian <strong>and</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women—a f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g that may nothave been detected <strong>in</strong> a university sample composed ofparticipants who are largely homogeneous <strong>in</strong> age. Thus,conclusions about culture <strong>and</strong> sexuality that are based onresearch with university samples must be made tentatively<strong>and</strong> researchers might strive to <strong>in</strong>clude a moresocioeconomically <strong>and</strong> demographically diverse sample.Moreover, that the mediat<strong>in</strong>g effect of sex guilt <strong>in</strong> therelationship <strong>between</strong> culture <strong>and</strong> sexual desire was found<strong>in</strong> this community-based sample may have implicationsfor the psychological treatment of sexual desire difficulties<strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women. <strong>The</strong> limited research on psychologicaltreatments for female HSDD <strong>in</strong>dicate that these<strong>in</strong>terventions are generally effective (Brotto, Basson, &Luria, 2008; Hawton, Catalan, & Fagg, 1991; Hurlbert,1993; McCabe, 2001; Schover & LoPiccolo, 1982; Trudelet al., 2001), comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g elements such as sensate focus,sexual skills tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, sexual psychoeducation, m<strong>in</strong>dfulnesstra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, cognitive restructur<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> behaviorchange. However, the extent to which these <strong>in</strong>terventionsare effective <strong>in</strong> non-European descent groups is unclear,as this has not been studied. Given the significantdifferences <strong>between</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese <strong>and</strong> Euro-Canadian <strong>in</strong>dividualsacross practically every doma<strong>in</strong> of sexuality thathas been studied, <strong>and</strong> the grow<strong>in</strong>g evidence that sex guiltplays an important role <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women’s level ofsexual desire, <strong>in</strong>corporat<strong>in</strong>g a specific focus on reduc<strong>in</strong>gsex guilt <strong>in</strong> psychological <strong>in</strong>terventions may be especiallybeneficial for Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women with compla<strong>in</strong>ts of low


SEX GUILT AND SEXUAL DESIREDownloaded by [<strong>The</strong> University of British Columbia] at 10:24 13 March 2012sexual desire. For example, thoughts <strong>and</strong> beliefs thatare l<strong>in</strong>ked to sex guilt may be identified, challenged,<strong>and</strong> substituted with more balanced thoughts. Examplesof thoughts that may be associated with sex guilt<strong>in</strong>clude, ‘‘<strong>Sex</strong> should happen only if a man <strong>in</strong>itiates,’’‘‘A good mother must control her sexual urges,’’ <strong>and</strong>‘‘Experienc<strong>in</strong>g pleasure dur<strong>in</strong>g sexual activity is notacceptable <strong>in</strong> a virtuous woman’’ (Nobre, Gouveia, &Gomes, 2003).LimitationsThis study has some limitations that must beaddressed. First, although this study exam<strong>in</strong>ed a sampleof women who were recruited from the community <strong>and</strong>who were, therefore, more heterogeneous <strong>in</strong> their backgroundscompared to a university convenience sample,the sample was not nationally representative, whichmay limit the generalizability of the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs. Second,it is well-established that volunteer bias—the tendencyfor those who choose to take part <strong>in</strong> research studiesto differ from those who choose not to take part—isan ongo<strong>in</strong>g concern <strong>in</strong> sex research (e.g., Plaud, Gaither,Hegstad, Rowan, & Devitt, 1999; Strassberg & Lowe,1995; Wiederman, 1993; Woo et al., 2010). Thus, theCh<strong>in</strong>ese women who participated <strong>in</strong> this study may havediffered from those who did not participate <strong>in</strong> their levelof acculturation. It is also possible that the women whochose to participate <strong>in</strong> this study may differ from women<strong>in</strong> the general population with regard to their level of sexguilt. Given that both acculturation <strong>and</strong> sex guilt werevariables of <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> this study, volunteer bias <strong>in</strong> theseareas may affect the conclusions that can be drawnabout the role of sex guilt <strong>in</strong> the relationship <strong>between</strong>culture <strong>and</strong> sexual desire.Conclusion<strong>The</strong> results of this study were consistent with priorresearch that exam<strong>in</strong>ed differences <strong>in</strong> sexual desire <strong>and</strong>sex guilt <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese <strong>and</strong> Euro-Canadian women. <strong>The</strong>seresults were also consistent with those of Woo et al.(2010a), who found that sex guilt is one mechanismby which culture <strong>in</strong>fluences sexual desire among Ch<strong>in</strong>esewomen. Given that sex guilt has been found to play animportant role <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women’s low sexual desire<strong>in</strong> both a university sample <strong>and</strong> a community-basedsample, an <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g extension of this researchwould be to <strong>in</strong>vestigate whether an <strong>in</strong>terventiondesigned to target sex guilt may be effective <strong>in</strong> treat<strong>in</strong>gCh<strong>in</strong>ese women seek<strong>in</strong>g treatment for compla<strong>in</strong>ts oflow sexual desire. Furthermore, it is plausible that sexguilt may represent a concern for <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong>other cultural groups. Susta<strong>in</strong>ed empirical study of the<strong>in</strong>terrelationships among sex guilt, sexual desire, <strong>and</strong>culture may further our knowledge of how thesevariables <strong>in</strong>teract with one another, as well as providean evidence base upon which <strong>in</strong>terventions for compla<strong>in</strong>tsof low sexual desire may be developed <strong>and</strong> improved.ReferencesAbramson, P. R., & Imai-Marquez, J. (1982). <strong>The</strong> Japanese-American:A cross-cultural, cross-sectional study of sex guilt. Journal ofResearch <strong>in</strong> Personality, 16, 227–237.Abramson, P. R., & Mosher, D. L. (1979). An empirical <strong>in</strong>vestigationof experimentally <strong>in</strong>duced masturbatory fantasies. Archives of<strong>Sex</strong>ual Behavior, 8, 27–39.Ahrold, T. K., & Meston, C. M. (2010). Ethnic differences <strong>in</strong> sexualattitudes of U.S. college students: Gender, acculturation, <strong>and</strong>religiosity factors. Archives of <strong>Sex</strong>ual Behavior, 39, 190–202.Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). <strong>The</strong> moderator–mediatorvariable dist<strong>in</strong>ction <strong>in</strong> social psychological research: Conceptual,strategic, <strong>and</strong> statistical considerations. Journal of Personality<strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.Brotto, L. A., Basson, R., & Luria, M. (2008). A m<strong>in</strong>dfulness-basedgroup psychoeducational <strong>in</strong>tervention target<strong>in</strong>g sexual arousaldisorder <strong>in</strong> women. Journal of <strong>Sex</strong>ual Medic<strong>in</strong>e, 5, 1646–1659.Brotto, L. A., Chik, H. M., Ryder, A. G., Gorzalka, B. B., & Seal, B.N. (2005). Acculturation <strong>and</strong> sexual function <strong>in</strong> Asian women.Archives of <strong>Sex</strong>ual Behavior, 34, 613–626.Brotto, L. A., Woo, J. S. T., & Ryder, A. G. (2007). Acculturation <strong>and</strong>sexual function <strong>in</strong> Canadian East Asian men. Journal of <strong>Sex</strong>ualMedic<strong>in</strong>e, 4, 72–82.Ca<strong>in</strong>, V. S., Johannes, C. B., Avis, N. E., Mohr, B., Schocken, M.,Skurnick, J., et al. (2003). <strong>Sex</strong>ual function<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> practices <strong>in</strong> amulti-ethnic study of midlife women: Basel<strong>in</strong>e results fromSWAN. Journal of <strong>Sex</strong> Research, 40, 266–276.Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bullet<strong>in</strong>, 112,155–159.Hawton, K., Catalan, J., & Fagg, J. (1991). Low sexual desire: <strong>Sex</strong>therapy results <strong>and</strong> prognostic factors. Behaviour Research <strong>and</strong><strong>The</strong>rapy, 29, 217–224.Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron <strong>and</strong> Kenny: Statistical mediationanalysis <strong>in</strong> the new millenium. Communication Monographs, 76,408–420.Higg<strong>in</strong>s, L. T., & Sun, C. H. (2007). Gender, social background <strong>and</strong>sexual attitudes among Ch<strong>in</strong>ese students. Culture, Health &<strong>Sex</strong>uality, 9, 31–42.Hislop, T. G., Teh, C., Lai, A., Ralston, J. D., Shu, J., & Taylor, V. M.(2004). Pap screen<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> knowledge of risk factors for cervicalcancer <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women <strong>in</strong> British Columbia, Canada. Ethnicity& Health, 9, 267–281.Hurlbert, D. F. (1993). A comparative study us<strong>in</strong>g orgasm consistencytra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the treatment of women report<strong>in</strong>g hypoactive sexualdesire. Journal of <strong>Sex</strong> & Marital <strong>The</strong>rapy, 19, 41–55.Kagawa-S<strong>in</strong>ger, M., & Pourat, N. (2000). Asian American <strong>and</strong> PacificIsl<strong>and</strong>er breast <strong>and</strong> cervical carc<strong>in</strong>oma screen<strong>in</strong>g rates <strong>and</strong>Healthy People 2000 objectives. Cancer, 89, 696–705.Kennedy, M. A., & Gorzalka, B. B. (2002). Asian <strong>and</strong> non-Asian attitudestoward rape, sexual harassment <strong>and</strong> sexuality. <strong>Sex</strong> Roles,46, 227–238.Laumann, E. O., Nicolosi, A., Glasser, D. B., Paik, A., G<strong>in</strong>gell, C.,Moreira, E., et al. (2005). <strong>Sex</strong>ual problems among women <strong>and</strong>men aged 40–80y: Prevalence <strong>and</strong> correlates identified <strong>in</strong> theGlobal Study of <strong>Sex</strong>ual Attitudes <strong>and</strong> Behaviors. InternationalJournal of Impotence Research, 17, 39–57.Lee, O., & Chang, J. (1999). Women, family, <strong>and</strong> society. Seoul, Korea:Seyoungsa.McCabe, M. P. (2001). Evaluation of a cognitive behavior therapyprogram for people with sexual dysfunction. Journal of <strong>Sex</strong> &Marital <strong>The</strong>rapy, 27, 259–271.297


WOO, BROTTO, AND GORZALKADownloaded by [<strong>The</strong> University of British Columbia] at 10:24 13 March 2012Meston, C. M., & Ahrold, T. K. (2010). Ethnic, gender, <strong>and</strong> acculturation<strong>in</strong>fluences on sexual behaviors. Archives of <strong>Sex</strong>ual Behavior,39, 179–189.Meston, C. M., Trapnell, P. D., & Gorzalka, B. B. (1996). Ethnic <strong>and</strong>gender differences <strong>in</strong> sexuality: Variations <strong>in</strong> sexual behavior<strong>between</strong> Asian <strong>and</strong> non-Asian university students. Archives of<strong>Sex</strong>ual Behavior, 25, 33–72.Meston, C. M., Trapnell, P. D., & Gorzalka, B. B. (1998). Ethnic, gender,<strong>and</strong> length-of-residency <strong>in</strong>fluences on sexual knowledge <strong>and</strong>attitudes. Journal of <strong>Sex</strong> Research, 35, 176–188.Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F., & Dolezal, C. (2007). <strong>The</strong> Female <strong>Sex</strong>ualFunction Index: A methodological critique <strong>and</strong> suggestions forimprovement. Journal of <strong>Sex</strong> & Marital <strong>The</strong>rapy, 33, 217–224.Mosher, D. L. (1988). Revised Mosher <strong>Guilt</strong> Inventory. In C. M.Davis, W. L. Yarber & S. L. Davis (Eds.), <strong>Sex</strong>uality-relatedmeasures (pp. 152–155). Lake Mills, IA: Graphic Publish<strong>in</strong>g.Mosher, D. L., & Cross, H. J. (1971). <strong>Sex</strong> guilt <strong>and</strong> premarital sexualexperiences of college students. Journal of Consult<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Cl<strong>in</strong>icalPsychology, 36, 27–32.Ng, M. L., & Lau, M. P. (1990). <strong>Sex</strong>ual attitudes <strong>in</strong> the Ch<strong>in</strong>ese.Archives of <strong>Sex</strong>ual Behavior, 19, 373–388.Nobre, P. J., Gouveia, J. P., & Gomes, F. A. (2003). <strong>Sex</strong>ual DysfunctionalBeliefs Questionnaire: An <strong>in</strong>strument to assess sexualdysfunctional beliefs as vulnerability factors to sexual problems.<strong>Sex</strong>ual <strong>and</strong> <strong>Relationship</strong> <strong>The</strong>rapy, 18, 171–204.Plaud, J. J., Gaither, G. A., Hegstad, H. J., Rowan, L., & Devitt, M.K. (1999). Volunteer bias <strong>in</strong> human psychophysiological sexualarousal research: To whom do our research results apply? Journalof <strong>Sex</strong> Research, 36, 171–179.Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS <strong>and</strong> SAS procedures forestimat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>direct effects <strong>in</strong> simple mediation models. BehaviorResearch Methods, Instruments, <strong>and</strong> Computers, 36, 717–731.Rosen, R., Brown, C., Heiman, J., Leiblum, S., Meston, C., Shabsigh,R., et al. (2000). <strong>The</strong> Female <strong>Sex</strong>ual Function Index (FSFI): Multidimensionalself-report <strong>in</strong>strument for the assessment of femalesexual function. Journal of <strong>Sex</strong> & Marital <strong>The</strong>rapy, 26, 191–208.Ruma, E. H., & Mosher, D. L. (1967). <strong>Relationship</strong> <strong>between</strong> moraljudgment <strong>and</strong> guilt <strong>in</strong> del<strong>in</strong>quent boys. Journal of AbnormalPsychology, 72, 122–127.Ryder, A. G., Alden, L. E., & Paulhus, D. L. (2000). Is acculturationunidimensional or bidimensional? A head-to-head comparison <strong>in</strong>the prediction of personality, self-identity, <strong>and</strong> adjustment.Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 79, 49–65.Schover, L. R., & LoPiccolo, J. (1982). Treatment effectiveness fordysfunctions of sexual desire. Journal of <strong>Sex</strong> & Marital <strong>The</strong>rapy,8, 179–197.Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation <strong>in</strong> experimental <strong>and</strong>nonexperimental studies: New procedures <strong>and</strong> recommendations.Psychological Methods, 7, 422–445.Strassberg, D. S., & Lowe, K. (1995). Volunteer bias <strong>in</strong> sexualityresearch. Archives of <strong>Sex</strong>ual Behavior, 24, 369–382.Tang, C. S., Lai, F. D., & Chung, T. K. H. (1997). Assessment of sexualfunction<strong>in</strong>g for Ch<strong>in</strong>ese college students. Archives of <strong>Sex</strong>ualBehavior, 26, 79–90.Taylor, V. M., Hislop, T. G., Jackson, J. C., Tu, S. P., Yasui, Y.,Schwartz, S. M., et al. (2002). A r<strong>and</strong>omized controlled trial of<strong>in</strong>terventions to promote cervical cancer screen<strong>in</strong>g among Ch<strong>in</strong>esewomen <strong>in</strong> North America. Journal of the National Cancer Institute,94, 670–677.Trudel, G., March<strong>and</strong>, A., Ravart, M., Aub<strong>in</strong>, S., Turgeon, L., &Fortier, P. (2001). <strong>The</strong> effect of a cognitive-behavioural grouptreatment program on hypoactive sexual desire <strong>in</strong> women. <strong>Sex</strong>ual<strong>and</strong> <strong>Relationship</strong> <strong>The</strong>rapy, 16, 145–164.Tu, S. P., Jackson, S. L., Yasui, Y., Deschamps, M., Hislop, T. G., &Taylor, V. M. (2005). Cancer preventive screen<strong>in</strong>g: A cross-bordercomparison of United States <strong>and</strong> Canadian Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women.Preventive Medic<strong>in</strong>e, 41, 36–46.Wiederman, M. W. (1993). Demographic <strong>and</strong> sexual characteristics ofnonresponders to sexual experience items <strong>in</strong> a national survey.Journal of <strong>Sex</strong> Research, 30, 27–35.Wiegel, M., Meston, C., & Rosen, R. (2005). <strong>The</strong> Female <strong>Sex</strong>ual FunctionIndex (FSFI): Cross-validation <strong>and</strong> development of cl<strong>in</strong>icalcutoff scores. Journal of <strong>Sex</strong> & Marital <strong>The</strong>rapy, 31, 1–20.Woo, J. S. T., Brotto, L. A., & Gorzalka, B. B. (2009). <strong>The</strong> role ofsexuality <strong>in</strong> cervical cancer screen<strong>in</strong>g among Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women.Health Psychology, 28, 598–604.Woo, J. S. T., Brotto, L. A., & Gorzalka, B. B. (2010a). <strong>The</strong> role of sexguilt <strong>in</strong> the relationship <strong>between</strong> culture <strong>and</strong> women’s sexual desire.Archives of <strong>Sex</strong>ual Behavior. Advance onl<strong>in</strong>e publication. doi:10.1007=s10508-010-9609-0Woo, J. S. T., Brotto, L. A., & Gorzalka, B. B. (2010b). <strong>Sex</strong> guilt <strong>and</strong>culture-l<strong>in</strong>ked barriers to testicular exam<strong>in</strong>ations. InternationalJournal of <strong>Sex</strong>ual Health, 22, 144–154.Woo, J. S. T., Brotto, L. A., & Yule, M. (2010). Do East Asian <strong>and</strong>Caucasian women differ <strong>in</strong> sexual psychophysiology researchparticipation? Journal of <strong>Sex</strong> Research, 47, 345–354. doi: 10.1080=00224490902999294Youn, G. (2001). Perceptions of peer sexual activities <strong>in</strong> Koreanadolescents. Journal of <strong>Sex</strong> Research, 38, 352–360.Yu, M. Y., Wu, T. Y., & Mood, D. W. (2005). Cultural affiliation <strong>and</strong>mammography screen<strong>in</strong>g of Ch<strong>in</strong>ese women <strong>in</strong> an urban countyof Michigan. Journal of Transcultural Nurs<strong>in</strong>g, 16, 107–116.298

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!