27.11.2012 Views

Introduction to Sports Biomechanics: Analysing Human Movement ...

Introduction to Sports Biomechanics: Analysing Human Movement ...

Introduction to Sports Biomechanics: Analysing Human Movement ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

BOX 3.2 A CAUTIONARY TALE OF UNRELIABLE DATA<br />

MORE ON MOVEMENT PATTERNS – THE GEOMETRY OF MOTION<br />

As we have noted many times in this chapter, one of the major tasks for movement analysts is <strong>to</strong><br />

be able <strong>to</strong> analyse coordination patterns in sport. Of particular interest <strong>to</strong> some movement<br />

analysts, including me, is the variability that we find in coordination patterns such as angle–angle<br />

diagrams, when even a highly skilled performer repeats a movement – this variability is also<br />

observed in movement patterns seen as a function of time. Of considerable interest <strong>to</strong> applied<br />

researchers is whether we can accurately and reliably assess such movement variability in<br />

competition.<br />

To answer this question, some of my colleagues at the University of Otago and I carried out a<br />

study <strong>to</strong> compare the reliability of estimating movement patterns in labora<strong>to</strong>ry and simulated<br />

field conditions. Both conditions were recorded using a digital video camera viewing perpendicular<br />

<strong>to</strong> the sagittal plane of the runner in our labora<strong>to</strong>ry, with good participant–clothing<br />

and clothing–background contrasts. The difference between the two conditions was as follows.<br />

In the ‘labora<strong>to</strong>ry’ condition, the participant had reflective markers attached <strong>to</strong> his clothing <strong>to</strong><br />

improve identification of joint axes of rotation; in the ‘simulated field’ condition, no markers<br />

were used because they cannot be in sports competitions. Although this is not the only difference<br />

between labora<strong>to</strong>ry and real field conditions, it is usually the most important one by far.<br />

The participant ran five trials in each condition at the same speed on a treadmill with equal rest<br />

periods between trials. From each trial, we selected three strides, from <strong>to</strong>e-off <strong>to</strong> <strong>to</strong>e-off, <strong>to</strong> be<br />

digitised – this means that a human opera<strong>to</strong>r identified each marker on each video frame,<br />

effectively manually tracking the markers or estimating the positions of the joints in the nomarker<br />

condition. We also tracked the markers au<strong>to</strong>matically using SIMI software (SIMI Reality<br />

Motion Systems GmbH, Unterschliessheim, Germany; http://www.simi.com). Five marker trials<br />

were au<strong>to</strong>-tracked using SIMI and manually tracked by four experienced movement analysts<br />

on five consecutive days. The four human opera<strong>to</strong>rs then digitised the five no-marker trials on<br />

consecutive days.<br />

Several sources of variability, or variance, are present in this design. The one of interest <strong>to</strong><br />

movement analysts is movement variability – the variance among trials. The next two sources<br />

are due <strong>to</strong> variability within and among the human opera<strong>to</strong>rs, respectively known as intraopera<strong>to</strong>r<br />

variability and inter-opera<strong>to</strong>r variability. The first of these, in our study, was the variance<br />

across days and the second that across the four opera<strong>to</strong>rs. There are other sources of<br />

variance from the three two-fac<strong>to</strong>r and the one three-fac<strong>to</strong>r interactions between our three<br />

main fac<strong>to</strong>rs – trials, days and people. We then used a statistical technique known as analysis of<br />

variance (ANOVA) <strong>to</strong> partition the variances between the three main effects and the several<br />

interactions. The results for the marker and the no-marker conditions are summarised in the pie<br />

charts of Figures 3.21(a) and (c) for the marker condition and Figures 3.21(b) and (d) for no<br />

markers.<br />

Small variances across repeated attempts by the same person (across days in our study) show<br />

good intra-opera<strong>to</strong>r reliability; small variances across opera<strong>to</strong>rs (across people) show good interopera<strong>to</strong>r<br />

reliability, sometimes known as objectivity. It should be clear from Figures 3.21(a) and (c)<br />

that, when markers are used, movement variability (variance across trials) is by far the<br />

107

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!