Free fall misconceptions: A comparison between science and non ...

Free fall misconceptions: A comparison between science and non ... Free fall misconceptions: A comparison between science and non ...

11.07.2015 Views

Free fall misconceptions: A comparison betweenscience and non-science university majorsEleanor Alma D. Jugueta 1 , Clark Kendrick C. Go 2 , Johanna Mae M. Indias 11 Department of Physics, Ateneo de Manila University, Katipunan Avenue, Quezon City,The Philippines.2 Department of Mathematics, Ateneo de Manila University, Katipunan Avenue, QuezonCity, The Philippines.E-mail: clarkkendrickcgo@yahoo.com(Received 25 July 2011; accepted 17 November 2011)AbstractThe study was conducted to find out the conceptual understanding and misconceptions of students in objectsundergoing freely falling motion. Open-ended questions are utilized and are given to one science and three non-scienceclasses to assess students’ understanding of the most basic concepts in free-fall. Results showed that majority of thestudents in both classes were able to correctly describe the motion of an object in free-fall in terms of velocity. Similarto previous studies, misconceptions were mostly on the acceleration of the object when it is at the highest point of itsflight. Further analysis showed that there is no significant difference between the answers of science and non-sciencemajors. Regardless of their concentration, students have the same general misconceptions on free fall. Science and nonsciencemajors equally share the 17% of the population who described the acceleration and velocity in the samemanner. However, only 12% of the entire student population answered perfectly and contrary to expectation, only 4%of this are science majors.Keywords: Misconceptions, Free-fall, Velocity, Acceleration.ResumenEl estudio fue conducido a descubre la comprensión y las ideas falsas conceptuales de estudiantes en los objetos queexperimentaban el movimiento libre-que caía. Las preguntas ampliables se utilizan y se dan a una ciencia y a tres clasesde la no-ciencia para determinar entender a los estudiantes el' de los conceptos más básicos de la caída libre. Losresultados demostraron que la mayoría de los estudiantes en ambas clases podía describir correctamente el movimientode un objeto en caída libre en términos de velocidad. Similar a los estudios anteriores, ideas falsas estaban sobre todoen la aceleración del objeto cuando está en el punto más alto de su vuelo. El análisis adicional demostró que no haydiferencia significativa entre las respuestas de la ciencia y los comandantes de la no-ciencia. Sin importar suconcentración, los estudiantes tienen las mismas ideas falsas generales en caída libre. Los comandantes de la ciencia yde la no-ciencia comparten igualmente el 17% de la población que describió la aceleración y la velocidad de manerasemejante. Sin embargo, los solamente 12% de la población entera estudiantil contesta perfectamente y contrariamentea la expectativa, el solamente 4% de esto son comandantes de la cienciaPalabras clave: Ideas falsas, caída libre, velocidad, aceleración.PACS: 01.40.-d, 01.40.Fk, 01.55.+b ISSN 1870-9095I. INTRODUCTIONThe concept of free-falling motion is often introduced tostudents as part of natural science courses in universitylevel Physics classes. Physics classes traditionally beginwith classical mechanics, with freely falling bodiesdiscussed in both high school and universities. The range,breadth and depth of the topics discussed in the entirecourse vary according to instructors’ area of expertise.Undergraduate students, who took part in this study, areclassified into science (SC) and non-science (NSC)students. Almost all students are familiar with the conceptof free-fall, as required by the Basic Education Curriculum.The non-science students, mainly first year collegestudents, have taken Physics classes in the previous year, asit is part of the Basic Education Curriculum. Sciencestudents, who are graduating non-Physics students, hadtheir last Physics classes at least four years prior. They arerequired to take Physics classes in preparation for medicalschool. The non-science classes are made up of studentsfrom various disciplines with class size of at least 30students while the science class is a large class with a classsize of 60 students.Lat. Am. J. Phys. Educ. Vol. 6, Suppl. I, August 2012 145 http://www.lajpe.org

<strong>Free</strong> <strong>fall</strong> <strong>misconceptions</strong>: A <strong>comparison</strong> <strong>between</strong><strong>science</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>non</strong>-<strong>science</strong> university majorsEleanor Alma D. Jugueta 1 , Clark Kendrick C. Go 2 , Johanna Mae M. Indias 11 Department of Physics, Ateneo de Manila University, Katipunan Avenue, Quezon City,The Philippines.2 Department of Mathematics, Ateneo de Manila University, Katipunan Avenue, QuezonCity, The Philippines.E-mail: clarkkendrickcgo@yahoo.com(Received 25 July 2011; accepted 17 November 2011)AbstractThe study was conducted to find out the conceptual underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>and</strong> <strong>misconceptions</strong> of students in objectsundergoing freely <strong>fall</strong>ing motion. Open-ended questions are utilized <strong>and</strong> are given to one <strong>science</strong> <strong>and</strong> three <strong>non</strong>-<strong>science</strong>classes to assess students’ underst<strong>and</strong>ing of the most basic concepts in free-<strong>fall</strong>. Results showed that majority of thestudents in both classes were able to correctly describe the motion of an object in free-<strong>fall</strong> in terms of velocity. Similarto previous studies, <strong>misconceptions</strong> were mostly on the acceleration of the object when it is at the highest point of itsflight. Further analysis showed that there is no significant difference <strong>between</strong> the answers of <strong>science</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>non</strong>-<strong>science</strong>majors. Regardless of their concentration, students have the same general <strong>misconceptions</strong> on free <strong>fall</strong>. Science <strong>and</strong> <strong>non</strong><strong>science</strong>majors equally share the 17% of the population who described the acceleration <strong>and</strong> velocity in the samemanner. However, only 12% of the entire student population answered perfectly <strong>and</strong> contrary to expectation, only 4%of this are <strong>science</strong> majors.Keywords: Misconceptions, <strong>Free</strong>-<strong>fall</strong>, Velocity, Acceleration.ResumenEl estudio fue conducido a descubre la comprensión y las ideas falsas conceptuales de estudiantes en los objetos queexperimentaban el movimiento libre-que caía. Las preguntas ampliables se utilizan y se dan a una ciencia y a tres clasesde la no-ciencia para determinar entender a los estudiantes el' de los conceptos más básicos de la caída libre. Losresultados demostraron que la mayoría de los estudiantes en ambas clases podía describir correctamente el movimientode un objeto en caída libre en términos de velocidad. Similar a los estudios anteriores, ideas falsas estaban sobre todoen la aceleración del objeto cu<strong>and</strong>o está en el punto más alto de su vuelo. El análisis adicional demostró que no haydiferencia significativa entre las respuestas de la ciencia y los com<strong>and</strong>antes de la no-ciencia. Sin importar suconcentración, los estudiantes tienen las mismas ideas falsas generales en caída libre. Los com<strong>and</strong>antes de la ciencia yde la no-ciencia comparten igualmente el 17% de la población que describió la aceleración y la velocidad de manerasemejante. Sin embargo, los solamente 12% de la población entera estudiantil contesta perfectamente y contrariamentea la expectativa, el solamente 4% de esto son com<strong>and</strong>antes de la cienciaPalabras clave: Ideas falsas, caída libre, velocidad, aceleración.PACS: 01.40.-d, 01.40.Fk, 01.55.+b ISSN 1870-9095I. INTRODUCTIONThe concept of free-<strong>fall</strong>ing motion is often introduced tostudents as part of natural <strong>science</strong> courses in universitylevel Physics classes. Physics classes traditionally beginwith classical mechanics, with freely <strong>fall</strong>ing bodiesdiscussed in both high school <strong>and</strong> universities. The range,breadth <strong>and</strong> depth of the topics discussed in the entirecourse vary according to instructors’ area of expertise.Undergraduate students, who took part in this study, areclassified into <strong>science</strong> (SC) <strong>and</strong> <strong>non</strong>-<strong>science</strong> (NSC)students. Almost all students are familiar with the conceptof free-<strong>fall</strong>, as required by the Basic Education Curriculum.The <strong>non</strong>-<strong>science</strong> students, mainly first year collegestudents, have taken Physics classes in the previous year, asit is part of the Basic Education Curriculum. Sciencestudents, who are graduating <strong>non</strong>-Physics students, hadtheir last Physics classes at least four years prior. They arerequired to take Physics classes in preparation for medicalschool. The <strong>non</strong>-<strong>science</strong> classes are made up of studentsfrom various disciplines with class size of at least 30students while the <strong>science</strong> class is a large class with a classsize of 60 students.Lat. Am. J. Phys. Educ. Vol. 6, Suppl. I, August 2012 145 http://www.lajpe.org


Eleanor Alma D. Jugueta, Clark Kendrick C. Go, Johanna Mae M. IndiasII. METHODOLOGYTo establish the level of conceptual underst<strong>and</strong>ing of thestudents, a test was given prior to classroom instruction onthe concepts of free-<strong>fall</strong>. Six open-ended questions weregiven with specific instructions that explanations given bythe students should not exceed 3 sentences. Results wereclassified <strong>and</strong> tabulated based on the responses of students.Each correct response was given 1 point while incorrectresponses were given 0 point. The incorrect responses werealso classified <strong>and</strong> tabulated to get the commonmisconception of students about the questions.The test was administered to 158 students in fourIntroductory Physics classes to determine their level ofunderst<strong>and</strong>ing; 60 are from the <strong>science</strong> class <strong>and</strong> 98 arefrom the <strong>non</strong>-<strong>science</strong> classes.It is important to note that the students being examinedare senior Biology majors who took their high schoolphysics four years ago. On the other h<strong>and</strong>, the freshmen<strong>non</strong>-<strong>science</strong> majors from various disciplines have takentheir high school physics just a year ago.respectively. As for the acceleration of the ball at the top ofits flight, the correct answer for both classes is alarminglymuch lower; 17% for <strong>non</strong>-<strong>science</strong> majors <strong>and</strong> 9% for<strong>science</strong> majors.III. RESULTS AND DISCUSSIONThe test administered describes the motion of a ball thrownupward with an initial velocity <strong>and</strong> with specificinstructions that air resistance is to be neglected. TheFIGURE 1. The chart shows the distribution of the answers of SCtrajectory of the ball’s path was divided into three<strong>and</strong> NSC students for the velocity of the ball moving upward.segments: (1) Motion of a ball moving upward, (2) Motionof the ball at the top of its flight <strong>and</strong> (3) The motion of theball as it moves downward. The students were instructed todescribe the motion of the ball in each segment in not morethan 3 sentences in terms of its velocity <strong>and</strong> acceleration.The distribution of the students’ answers in the threesegments of the ball shows that both classes can correctlydescribe the motion of the object with regards to velocity. Acareful analysis of the students’ velocity responses for eachsegment also shows that the percentage of correct answersfrom NSC students is higher than that of the SC students.In Q1, 83% of the NSC students correctly answered thatthe velocity decreases as the ball moves upward comparedto the 75% of the SC students as shown in Fig. 1.As for the velocity of the ball at the top of its flight(Q3), 99% of NSC students correctly answered that velocityis zero compared to the 92% of the SC students.It was only on the third segment of the motion (Q5) didthe <strong>science</strong> students score better than the <strong>non</strong>-<strong>science</strong>students as shown in Fig. 2. These results show that thestudents, whether <strong>science</strong> or <strong>non</strong>-<strong>science</strong> majors, possess agood conceptual underst<strong>and</strong>ing of the velocity of an objectundergoing free <strong>fall</strong>.However, in terms of the acceleration of the ball, theFIGURE 2. The chart shows the distribution of the answers of SCpercentage of correct answers for both classes decreased<strong>and</strong> NSC students for the velocity of the ball moving downward.significantly. For the first part of the motion (Q2), thepercentage of correct answers for NSC students is only 30%while 38% for SC students. Similarly, for the downward Some answers of students that acceleration is – 9.8m/s 2motion of the ball (Q6), the percentage of correct answers is were not considered to be correct as it does not depict whatonly 30% <strong>and</strong> 35% for <strong>non</strong>-<strong>science</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>science</strong> majors’, happens to the acceleration as an object moves upward.Lat. Am. J. Phys. Educ. Vol. 6, Suppl. I, August 2012 146 http://www.lajpe.org


Some students associate <strong>and</strong> limit the description ofacceleration to just –9.8m/s 2 without truly underst<strong>and</strong> whatit means. Another basis for not considering their answer isthat after a close inspection of student responses for eachsegment of the motion, most of the students believe that theacceleration vector changes from moment to moment asevident in their responses in Q2, Q4 <strong>and</strong> Q6. Theacceleration varies from -9.8m/s 2 to zero <strong>and</strong> back to -9.8m/s 2 .These results show that even after a year ofcomprehensive instruction in Physics during high school,the proper conceptual underst<strong>and</strong>ing of acceleration is stilldifficult <strong>and</strong> ambiguous for the students.It is interesting to note that in each segment of the ball’smotion, the next popular answer to the question ofacceleration is similar to the description of the motion ofthe velocity. For instance, in the first part of the motion(Q2), about 29% <strong>and</strong> 27% of <strong>non</strong>-<strong>science</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>science</strong>students, respectively, answered that acceleration decreasesas the velocity decreases. It also shows that the students arerelating the decrease in the velocity of the ball with anegative acceleration. Another interesting response is thatacceleration is + 9.8m/s 2 as shown in Fig. 3. It proves thatstudents’ conceptual underst<strong>and</strong>ing of acceleration isdependent on the motion of the object. Since motion of theball is moving upward, acceleration must also be moving inthe same direction, thus, + 9.8m/s 2 .<strong>Free</strong> <strong>fall</strong> <strong>misconceptions</strong>: A <strong>comparison</strong> <strong>between</strong> <strong>science</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>non</strong>-<strong>science</strong> university majorsFIGURE 4. The chart shows the distribution of answers of SC<strong>and</strong> NSC students for the acceleration of the ball at the top of itsflight.The same trend is observed in the downward motion of theball (Q6) where 27% <strong>and</strong> 39% of NSC <strong>and</strong> SC students,respectively, answered that acceleration increases as thevelocity increases. It is evident that students’ underst<strong>and</strong>ingof acceleration is sy<strong>non</strong>ymous to velocity. Similar to theresults in Q2, a significant increase is observed in thenumber of students who answered that acceleration is +9.8m/s 2 as shown in Fig. 5. The students are relating that anincrease in the velocity is accompanied by a positive valueof the acceleration. For any object speeding up, it musthave a positive acceleration regardless of its direction.FIGURE 3. The chart shows the distribution of answers of SC<strong>and</strong> NSC students for the acceleration of the ball moving upward.As for the acceleration of the ball at the top of its flight(Q4), about 59% of NSC students answered thatacceleration is also zero like its velocity while 80% of SCFIGURE 5. The chart shows the distribution of answers of SC<strong>and</strong> NSC students for the acceleration of the moving downward.students answered similarly as shown in Fig. 4.Lat. Am. J. Phys. Educ. Vol. 6, Suppl. I, August 2012 147 http://www.lajpe.org


Eleanor Alma D. Jugueta, Clark Kendrick C. Go, Johanna Mae M. IndiasA careful study of the responses for the entire motion of theball revealed that 17% of all the students answered velocity<strong>and</strong> acceleration behaves similarly, while only 12% of thestudents correctly answered both the velocity <strong>and</strong>acceleration for each segment. Of these correct answers,contrary to expectations, only 4% are <strong>science</strong> majors.IV. CONCLUSIONAn open-ended, free response pre-test was used todetermine the conceptual underst<strong>and</strong>ing of <strong>science</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>non</strong><strong>science</strong>students on the concept of free <strong>fall</strong>. It is noted in themethodology that the freshmen NSC students had their lastphysics class only a year ago, while the senior biologystudents had theirs 4 years ago. With their physics class stillfresh in their minds, freshmen NSC students performedslightly better than the SC students, contrary to what isexpected; that <strong>science</strong> students should know more about the<strong>science</strong>s.However, regardless of the differences in the number ofyears when they took their last physics class, the resultsshow that most students still fail to have the properconceptual underst<strong>and</strong>ing with regards to freely <strong>fall</strong>ingobjects. Students tend to resort to just memorizing the valueof acceleration <strong>and</strong> not truly underst<strong>and</strong>ing its meaning.Most of the students equate whatever is happening to theacceleration is similar to whatever is happening to itsvelocity. However, since the velocity vector of freely<strong>fall</strong>ing object changes from segment to segment, studentsmistakenly assume that the acceleration will behave in thesame manner. This observation really shows theirmisconception about acceleration. To correct this behavior,the vector nature of both velocity <strong>and</strong> acceleration must bereviewed to reiterate that acceleration can either speed up orslow down the motion of an object depending on thedirection of the velocity, <strong>and</strong> not behave similar to velocity.ACKNOWLEDGMENTSThe authors are grateful for the support of the Departmentof Physics <strong>and</strong> Department of Mathematics, Office of theDean of the School of Science <strong>and</strong> Engineering, <strong>and</strong> theOffice of the Vice President for the Loyola Schools.REFERENCES[1] Palmer, D., Student’s alternative conceptions <strong>and</strong>scientifically acceptable conceptions about gravity,International Journal of Science Education 23, 691–706(2001).[2] Jagger, J. M., Students’ underst<strong>and</strong>ing of acceleration,Mathematics in School 16, 24–25 (1987).[3] Jones, A. T., Investigation of students’ underst<strong>and</strong>ing ofspeed, velocity <strong>and</strong> acceleration, Research in ScienceEducation 13, 95–104 (1983).[4] Tasar, M. F., What part of the concept of acceleration isdifficult to underst<strong>and</strong>: The mathematics, the physics, orboth?, ZDM Mathematics Education 42, 469–482 (2010).[5] Serway <strong>and</strong> Jett., Physics for Scientists <strong>and</strong> Engineers,6th Ed. (Thomson Brooks, Belmont, CA, 2004), pp. 40-44.[6] Cutnell, J. D. <strong>and</strong> Johnson, K. W., Physics, 6th Ed.(Wiley, New Jersey, 2004), p.p. 40-43.Lat. Am. J. Phys. Educ. Vol. 6, Suppl. I, August 2012 148 http://www.lajpe.org

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!