11.07.2015 Views

Masked phonological priming effects in English - Center for Reading ...

Masked phonological priming effects in English - Center for Reading ...

Masked phonological priming effects in English - Center for Reading ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

142 K. Rastle, M. Brysbaert / Cognitive Psychology 53 (2006) 97–145Appendix B (cont<strong>in</strong>ued)Relationship Condition E1: target E1: prime E2: target E2: primeGraphemic SDS kive korve tole tuilGraphemic SDS zorgue zaigue nise naisGraphemic SSD dobe dode yel yedGraphemic SSD coob coom dait daichGraphemic SSD vig vib rhed rhellGraphemic SSD paim paith boath boamGraphemic SSD vove vope froe frieGraphemic SSD nuck nutch harve hargeGraphemic SSD shace shafe bumb buthGraphemic SSD thutch thunn sead seaphReferencesAndrews, S. (1996). Lexical retrieval and selection processes: Effects of transposed-letter confusability. Journal ofMemory and Language, 35, 775–800.Baddeley, A. (1986). Work<strong>in</strong>g memory. Ox<strong>for</strong>d: Ox<strong>for</strong>d University Press.Berent, I. (1997). Phonological <strong>prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>in</strong> the lexical decision task: Regularity <strong>effects</strong> are not necessarily evidence<strong>for</strong> assembly. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Per<strong>for</strong>mance, 23, 1727–1742.Berent, I., & Perfetti, C. A. (1995). A rose is a REEZ: The two-cycles model of <strong>phonological</strong> assembly <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g<strong>English</strong>. Psychological Review, 102, 146–184.Bibi, U., Tzelgov, J., & Henik, A. (2000). Stroop effect <strong>in</strong> words that differ from color words <strong>in</strong> one letter only.Psychonomic Bullet<strong>in</strong> & Review, 7, 678–683.Booth, J. R., Perfetti, C. A., & MacWh<strong>in</strong>ney, B. (1999). Quick, automatic, and general activation of orthographicand <strong>phonological</strong> representations <strong>in</strong> young readers. Developmental Psychology, 35, 3–19.Borowsky, R. & Besner, D. (<strong>in</strong> press). Parallel distributed process<strong>in</strong>g and lexical-semantic <strong>effects</strong> <strong>in</strong> visual wordrecognition: Are a few stages necessary? To appear <strong>in</strong> Psychological Review.Bowers, J. S. (2002). Challeng<strong>in</strong>g the widespread assumption that connectionism and distributed representationsgo hand-<strong>in</strong>-hand. Cognitive Psychology, 45, 413–445.Bowers, J. S., Vigliocco, G., & Haan, R. (1998). Orthographic, <strong>phonological</strong> and articulatory contributions tomasked letter and word <strong>prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong>. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Per<strong>for</strong>mance, 24,1705–1719.Brysbaert, M. (2001). Prelexical <strong>phonological</strong> cod<strong>in</strong>g of visual words <strong>in</strong> Dutch: Automatic after all. Memory &Cognition, 29, 765–773.Brysbaert, M., Grondelaers, S., & Rat<strong>in</strong>ckx, E. (2000). Sentence read<strong>in</strong>g: Do we make use of orthographic cueson homophones? Acta Psychologica, 105, 31–56.Brysbaert, M., & Praet, C. (1992). Read<strong>in</strong>g isolated words: No evidence <strong>for</strong> automatic <strong>in</strong>corporation of thephonetic code. Psychological Research, 54, 91–102.Coltheart, M. (2004). Are there lexicons? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 1153–1171.Coltheart, M., Davelaar, E., Jonasson, J. T., & Besner, D. (1977). Access to the <strong>in</strong>ternal lexicon. In S. Dornic(Ed.), Attention and per<strong>for</strong>mance VI. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Coltheart, M., & Rastle, K. (1994). Serial process<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g aloud: Evidence <strong>for</strong> dual-route models of read<strong>in</strong>g.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Per<strong>for</strong>mance, 20, 1197–1211.Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). The DRC model: A model of visual wordrecognition and read<strong>in</strong>g aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204–256.Coltheart, M., Woollams, A., K<strong>in</strong>oshita, S., & Perry, C. (1999). A position-sensitive Stroop effect: Furtherevidence <strong>for</strong> a left-to-right component <strong>in</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>t-to-speech conversion. Psychonomic Bullet<strong>in</strong> & Review, 6,456–463.Davis, C., Castles, A., & Iakovidis, E. (1998). <strong>Masked</strong> homophone and pseudohomophone <strong>prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>in</strong> adults andchildren. Language and Cognitive Processes, 13, 625–651.Davis, C. J. (1999). The Self-Organis<strong>in</strong>g Lexical Acquisition and Recognition (SOLAR) model of visual wordrecognition. Unpublished doctoral dissertation.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!