11.07.2015 Views

Masked phonological priming effects in English - Center for Reading ...

Masked phonological priming effects in English - Center for Reading ...

Masked phonological priming effects in English - Center for Reading ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

K. Rastle, M. Brysbaert / Cognitive Psychology 53 (2006) 97–145 1296.7.1. Parameter setSimulation 6 compared the per<strong>for</strong>mance of two models: One controlled by the set ofparameters developed <strong>in</strong> Simulation 5 and one controlled by the standard set of parametersused <strong>for</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g aloud (Coltheart et al., 2001; Rastle & Coltheart, 1999).6.7.2. ResultsFig. 2 depicts the <strong>in</strong>fluence of target type (word or nonword) on the activation of <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mationused to make a lexical decision when the model is runn<strong>in</strong>g under (a) the parametersused <strong>in</strong> Simulation 5; and (b) the standard parameters <strong>for</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g aloud.A statistical analysis is not necessary to demonstrate that the DRC model’s ability todiscrim<strong>in</strong>ate between words and nonwords is seriously compromised under the parameterset adopted <strong>in</strong> Simulation 5.6.7.3. DiscussionSimulation 5 yielded a parameter set that shows scope <strong>for</strong> produc<strong>in</strong>g a fast <strong>phonological</strong><strong>prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong> effect. However, the DRC model runn<strong>in</strong>g under that parameter set was totallyunable to read aloud exception words correctly. In Simulation 6, we asked whether wecould justify us<strong>in</strong>g the standard set of parameters <strong>for</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g aloud (Coltheart et al.,2001; Rastle & Coltheart, 1999) and the set of parameters developed <strong>in</strong> Simulation 5<strong>for</strong> lexical decision. This simulation was unambiguous. It revealed that the discrim<strong>in</strong>ationbetween word and nonword stimuli is far more difficult when the model is runn<strong>in</strong>g underthe parameters used <strong>in</strong> Simulation 5 than when it is runn<strong>in</strong>g under the standard parameters<strong>for</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g aloud. We there<strong>for</strong>e consider that adopt<strong>in</strong>g the parameters used <strong>in</strong> Simulation5 could not be justified <strong>in</strong> terms of a strategic variation aris<strong>in</strong>g due to the demands ofthe lexical decision task.A1.2B1.2Maximum Activation10.80.60.40.201 9 17 25 33 41 49 57 65 73 81 89 97CyclesWordsNonwordsTotal Activation10.80.60.40.201 9 17 25 33 41 49 57 65 73 81 89 97CyclesWordsNonwordsCMaximum Activation1.210.80.60.40.201 9 17 25 33 41 49 57 65 73 81 89 97CyclesWordsNonwordsDTotal Activation1098765432101 9 17 25 33 41 49 57 65 73 81 89 97CyclesWordsNonwordsFig. 2. Maximum orthographic activation (A and C) and total orthographic activation (B and D) produced bywords and nonwords. The top panels (A and B) reflect the DRC model’s per<strong>for</strong>mance under the standardparameters <strong>for</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g aloud, and the bottom panels (C and D) reflect the DRC model’s per<strong>for</strong>mance under theparameters used <strong>in</strong> Simulation 5.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!