11.07.2015 Views

Masked phonological priming effects in English - Center for Reading ...

Masked phonological priming effects in English - Center for Reading ...

Masked phonological priming effects in English - Center for Reading ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

120 K. Rastle, M. Brysbaert / Cognitive Psychology 53 (2006) 97–145pr<strong>in</strong>tFeatureAnalysisLetter AnalysisOrthographic <strong>in</strong>putlexiconPhonological outputlexiconGPCtranslationPhoneme systemspeechFig. 1. The DRC model of Coltheart et al. (2001).is <strong>in</strong>fluenced by <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation (i.e., excitation and <strong>in</strong>hibition) from both letter units and<strong>phonological</strong> units. Phonological units can be activated via feedback from the phonemeunits, which are activated via nonlexical grapheme-to-phoneme (GPC) translation ofthe stimulus. It is this <strong>in</strong>put from the <strong>phonological</strong> assembly procedure to orthographiclexical entries that may provide an avenue <strong>for</strong> simulat<strong>in</strong>g fast <strong>phonological</strong> <strong>prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>effects</strong>on lexical decision (Coltheart & Rastle, 1994).On the lexical decision rule described by Coltheart et al. (2001), the ‘‘YES’’ response <strong>in</strong>the lexical decision task is made if (a) the activation of any s<strong>in</strong>gle unit <strong>in</strong> the orthographiclexicon reaches a critical value, A; or (b) if the summed activation of every unit <strong>in</strong> that lexiconreaches a critical value, S (i.e., the ‘‘fast guess’’ mechanism of Gra<strong>in</strong>ger & Jacobs,1996). In a <strong>prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong> situation, there<strong>for</strong>e, primes can affect target lexical decisions by (a)<strong>in</strong>fluenc<strong>in</strong>g the activation of any s<strong>in</strong>gle unit <strong>in</strong> the orthographic lexicon (e.g., the unit represent<strong>in</strong>gthe target); or (b) <strong>in</strong>fluenc<strong>in</strong>g the total activation of the orthographic lexicon.In an ideal world, DRC simulations would thus comprise the follow<strong>in</strong>g approach:Primes would be presented <strong>for</strong> some number of cycles consistent with the 58-ms exposureduration used <strong>in</strong> the experiments; targets would then be presented (without resett<strong>in</strong>g activations<strong>in</strong> the model) until a lexical decision was made; lexical decision latencies would becompared across <strong>phonological</strong> <strong>prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong> and graphemic control <strong>prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong> conditions; andf<strong>in</strong>ally, <strong>prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong> data from the model would be compared with <strong>prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong> data from humanreaders. However, two ma<strong>in</strong> issues preclude this approach. First, the account of masked<strong>prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong> offered by the DRC model is not well developed. Coltheart, Woollams, K<strong>in</strong>oshita,and Perry (1999) conducted a prelim<strong>in</strong>ary exploration of masked <strong>prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g a 15-cycleprime exposure duration followed immediately by presentation of the target. However, theauthors of the DRC model (Coltheart et al., 2001, p. 250) emphasise that the work ofColtheart et al. (1999) provides only a very coarse approximation to an adequate

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!