11.07.2015 Views

Masked phonological priming effects in English - Center for Reading ...

Masked phonological priming effects in English - Center for Reading ...

Masked phonological priming effects in English - Center for Reading ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

K. Rastle, M. Brysbaert / Cognitive Psychology 53 (2006) 97–145 115corrected-to-normal vision. Participants received either course credit or payment of $10Australian dollars (approximately $4 US dollars) <strong>for</strong> their participation.4.1.2. Stimuli and apparatusOne hundred and twelve pseudohomophone primes, each compris<strong>in</strong>g three phonemes,were selected from the ARC Nonword Database (Rastle, Harr<strong>in</strong>gton, & Coltheart, 2002).Phonologically identical <strong>English</strong> word targets were derived from these pseudohomophoneprimes by chang<strong>in</strong>g one, two or all three of the graphemes <strong>in</strong> the pseudohomophone. Forexample, the target RAISE was derived by chang<strong>in</strong>g all three of the graphemes <strong>in</strong> thepseudohomophone WRAZE (WR fi R, A.E fi AI.E, and Z fi S), whereas the targetFARM was derived by chang<strong>in</strong>g only one of the graphemes <strong>in</strong> the pseudohomophonePHARM (PH fi F). Sixteen <strong>English</strong> word targets were derived by chang<strong>in</strong>g all three graphemeswith<strong>in</strong> the pseudohomophone prime; 48 <strong>English</strong> word targets were derived bychang<strong>in</strong>g two graphemes with<strong>in</strong> the pseudohomophone prime; and 48 <strong>English</strong> word targetswere derived by chang<strong>in</strong>g one grapheme with<strong>in</strong> the pseudohomophone prime.With<strong>in</strong> the sets of prime–target pairs derived by a one- or two-grapheme change, wefurther varied the position at which the graphemic change was made. With<strong>in</strong> the prime–target pairs derived by a two-grapheme change, the graphemic similarity structure betweenprime and target could be ‘different-different-same’ (e.g., koan–CONE), ‘different-samedifferent’(e.g., writch–RICH), or ‘same-different-different’ (e.g., beize–BAYS), with 16prime–target pairs fall<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>to each similarity-structure type. With<strong>in</strong> the prime–target pairsderived by a one-grapheme change, the graphemic similarity structure could be ‘differentsame-same’(e.g., pharm–FARM), ‘same-different-same’ (e.g., nurve–NERVE), or‘same-same-different’ (e.g., skee–SKI), with 16 prime–target pairs fall<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>to each similarity-structuretype.Graphemic controls were generated <strong>for</strong> each prime–target pair. All shared letters <strong>in</strong>shared positions by primes and targets were preserved <strong>in</strong> graphemic controls (e.g., the pairgroe–GROW shares the GRO component, and this is preserved <strong>in</strong> the graphemic control,groy). Other letters shared by primes and targets, but not <strong>in</strong> shared positions, were also<strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> controls (e.g., pharm/gharm–FARM) except <strong>in</strong> very rare cases <strong>in</strong> which thiswas impossible (<strong>in</strong> such <strong>in</strong>stances, we compensated <strong>for</strong> this on another item of the same type).Whenever possible, shared onsets, nuclei, and codae between <strong>phonological</strong> primes and targetswere preserved <strong>in</strong> graphemic controls. Graphemic controls and pseudohomophoneprimes always conta<strong>in</strong>ed the same number of letters. All stimuli are listed <strong>in</strong> Appendix A.Triplets from each of the seven similarity-structure types were divided <strong>in</strong>to two equallists <strong>for</strong> counterbalanc<strong>in</strong>g purposes. Each subject saw each target, participated <strong>in</strong> both levelsof the <strong>prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong> variable (related and unrelated), but saw each target only once. An unrelatedcontrol condition (present <strong>in</strong> many of the studies reviewed <strong>in</strong> Table 4) does not reveal<strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation with respect to the issue of <strong>phonological</strong> <strong>prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong>; as such, it was not <strong>in</strong>cluded<strong>in</strong> the present experiments s<strong>in</strong>ce it would have reduced the number of useful observationsper participant.One hundred and twelve nonword targets, each with three phonemes and with similarorthographic characteristics to the word targets, were selected from the ARC NonwordDatabase (Rastle et al., 2002). Nonword targets had a similar number of letters to wordtargets (words: mean 4.25 letters, range 3–6; nonwords: mean 4.22 letters, range 3–6,t(222) < 1), as well as similar neighborhood properties (words: mean 8.39 neighbors, range0–21; nonwords: 8.29 neighbors, range 0–20, t(222) < 1).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!