11.07.2015 Views

Masked phonological priming effects in English - Center for Reading ...

Masked phonological priming effects in English - Center for Reading ...

Masked phonological priming effects in English - Center for Reading ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

114 K. Rastle, M. Brysbaert / Cognitive Psychology 53 (2006) 97–145orthographic and <strong>phonological</strong> pathways, a prime like ‘‘yuice’’ will activate its orthographictarget ‘‘USE’’ only via the <strong>phonological</strong> pathway. Further, at least <strong>in</strong> the DRC model, theprime ‘‘yuice’’ will actively <strong>in</strong>hibit the activation of the orthographic unit ‘‘USE.’’ Simulationsus<strong>in</strong>g these models have shown a pattern of per<strong>for</strong>mance consistent with this verbal account.For example, Coltheart and Rastle (1994) demonstrated that the activation of the orthographicunit COAT is <strong>in</strong>fluenced far more strongly by the visually similar stimulus ‘‘koat’’than by the visually dist<strong>in</strong>ct stimulus ‘‘kote’’. Similarly, Harm and Seidenberg (2004) demonstratedthat pseudohomophones which are visually similar to their base words (e.g., ‘‘nat’’)activate appropriate semantic features (e.g., <strong>in</strong>sect) along the orthography-semantics pathwayto a far greater degree than do pseudohomophones which are visually dissimilar to theirbase words (e.g., ‘‘nox’’). Thus, each of these models predicts <strong>phonological</strong> <strong>prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>effects</strong> tobe most evident <strong>in</strong> situations <strong>in</strong> which orthographic overlap between prime and target is high.Just as <strong>phonological</strong> primes and their targets can vary <strong>in</strong> the extent to which they <strong>in</strong>volveorthographic change, they also vary <strong>in</strong> terms of the position at which this change occurs (e.g.,klip–CLIP versus groe–GROW). While many theories (e.g., Frost, 1998; Harm &Seidenberg, 2004) predict that <strong>phonological</strong> cod<strong>in</strong>g occurs <strong>in</strong> parallel <strong>for</strong> all letters of the<strong>in</strong>put and would thus predict no <strong>in</strong>fluence of overlap position, some theories predict that thisfactor may have an <strong>in</strong>fluence on <strong>phonological</strong> <strong>prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong> (e.g., Berent & Perfetti, 1995; Coltheart& Rastle, 1994; Coltheart et al., 2001; Perry & Ziegler, 2002; Rastle & Coltheart,1999). In the DRC model (Coltheart et al., 2001), <strong>for</strong> example, nonlexical letter-to-soundconversion procedures operate serially, from left-to-right, across the <strong>in</strong>put str<strong>in</strong>g. Phonological<strong>prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>effects</strong> might there<strong>for</strong>e be most evident <strong>in</strong> situations <strong>in</strong> which the orthographicchange between primes and targets is early (e.g., klip–CLIP versus plip–CLIP), though thisprediction would need to be verified through simulation.In our experiments, we wished to ensure that the presence or absence of a masked <strong>phonological</strong><strong>prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong> effect did not rest <strong>in</strong>advertently on uncontrolled aspects of the degree andlocation of orthographic overlap between primes and targets (see e.g., Lukatela et al.,1998, who used only a s<strong>in</strong>gle type of orthographic change, e.g., klip–CLIP, <strong>in</strong> their <strong>in</strong>vestigation).We there<strong>for</strong>e took care <strong>in</strong> design<strong>in</strong>g the stimulus set used <strong>in</strong> Experiments 1 and 2to <strong>in</strong>clude prime–target pairs represent<strong>in</strong>g the full range of orthographic overlap and positionof that overlap. Given the humble effect sizes revealed <strong>in</strong> our meta-analysis, however,our purpose was not to test <strong>for</strong> differences across these types of overlap. For readers <strong>in</strong>terested<strong>in</strong> specific post hoc contrasts, we have <strong>in</strong>cluded complete item data <strong>for</strong> each experiment <strong>in</strong>Appendix A.4. Experiment 1For Experiment 1, we conducted a ‘‘typical’’ <strong>for</strong>ward-masked lexical decision experiment<strong>in</strong> which pseudohomophones primes always mapped onto a ‘‘YES’’ response. Giventhese circumstances, we expected an overall <strong>phonological</strong> <strong>prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong> effect of approximately10 ms, <strong>in</strong> l<strong>in</strong>e with those studies reviewed <strong>in</strong> Table 4.4.1. Method4.1.1. ParticipantsParticipants were 42 students from Macquarie University. All participants werenative speakers of Australian or New Zealand <strong>English</strong>, and reported normal or

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!