11.07.2015 Views

Masked phonological priming effects in English - Center for Reading ...

Masked phonological priming effects in English - Center for Reading ...

Masked phonological priming effects in English - Center for Reading ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

K. Rastle, M. Brysbaert / Cognitive Psychology 53 (2006) 97–145 1133.3. Are all <strong>phonological</strong> primes equal?Phonological <strong>prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>effects</strong> are thought to reflect sav<strong>in</strong>gs on target process<strong>in</strong>g due to<strong>phonological</strong> overlap between prime and target, once the sav<strong>in</strong>gs from orthographic overlapbetween prime and target has been elim<strong>in</strong>ated. At first sight, this seems to suggest thatthe smaller the orthographic overlap between <strong>phonological</strong> prime and target, the moreroom there is <strong>for</strong> <strong>phonological</strong> sav<strong>in</strong>gs. Consider the <strong>phonological</strong> <strong>prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>effects</strong> thatmight be obta<strong>in</strong>ed with the items ‘‘yuice–USE’’ and ‘‘klip–CLIP.’’ Primes and targets compris<strong>in</strong>gthese pairs are <strong>phonological</strong>ly identical, yet the pairs vary considerably <strong>in</strong> terms oftheir orthographic overlap. Will this difference have an effect on the magnitude of the <strong>phonological</strong><strong>prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong> effect, obta<strong>in</strong>ed by compar<strong>in</strong>g the above primes with their graphemiccontrols ‘‘douke–USE’’ and ‘‘plip–CLIP’’? The theories under consideration may makedifferent predictions concern<strong>in</strong>g this issue.Frost’s (1998; Frost et al., 2003) strong <strong>phonological</strong> theory claims that the <strong>phonological</strong>code upon which lexical identification is based is underspecified, mak<strong>in</strong>g it difficult toobserve a reliable <strong>phonological</strong> <strong>prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong> effect when homophonic and graphemic controlprimes are only m<strong>in</strong>imally (<strong>phonological</strong>ly) different (e.g., klip/plip–CLIP). On this theory,<strong>phonological</strong> <strong>prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>effects</strong> would be most evident <strong>in</strong> situations <strong>in</strong> which the graphemiccontrol can be made maximally (<strong>phonological</strong>ly) different from the target (e.g., yuice–USEversus douke–USE). Empirical <strong>effects</strong> support<strong>in</strong>g the underspecification claim have beenobserved <strong>in</strong> Hebrew perceptual identification (Gronau & Frost, 1997) and <strong>for</strong>wardmaskedlexical decision (Frost et al., 2003). In both cases, the advantage conferred by ahomophone prime on target recognition (e.g., QPIT–KPIT; /kapit/-/kapit/) is greaterwhen it is compared with the advantage conferred by an orthographically equivalentbut <strong>phonological</strong>ly dissimilar prime (e.g., KPZT–KPIT; /kapezet/-/kapit/) than the advantageconferred by an orthographically equivalent but <strong>phonological</strong>ly similar prime (e.g.,KPIZ–KPIT; /kapiz/-/kapit/). It is important to appreciate, however, that the Hebrewcase described here is very different from the <strong>English</strong> yuice/douke–USE case. In theHebrew case, a large <strong>phonological</strong> alteration is <strong>in</strong>duced by a small orthographic alteration,whereas <strong>in</strong> the yuice/douke–USE case, the large <strong>phonological</strong> alteration between controland target is accompanied by a large orthographic alteration between <strong>phonological</strong> primeand target. Effects of this nature (i.e., more <strong>phonological</strong> <strong>prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong> when there is low orthographicsimilarity between prime and target) on <strong>English</strong> word process<strong>in</strong>g have so far beenlimited to <strong>for</strong>ward-masked perceptual identification <strong>in</strong> young readers (Booth et al., 1999;see also Brysbaert, 2001, who made a similar claim on the basis of a near-significant posthoc analysis of perceptual identification data <strong>in</strong> Dutch).In contrast to the strong <strong>phonological</strong> theory, current weak <strong>phonological</strong> theories(Coltheart et al., 2001; Harm & Seidenberg, 2004) predict that <strong>phonological</strong> <strong>prim<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>effects</strong>should be greatest when orthographic overlap between prime and target is high (e.g., the klip/plip–CLIP case). In each of these models, the activation of the orthographic and/or semanticunits monitored <strong>in</strong> lexical decision 3 is determ<strong>in</strong>ed jo<strong>in</strong>tly by orthographic and <strong>phonological</strong><strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation. There<strong>for</strong>e, while a prime like ‘‘klip’’ will activate its target ‘‘CLIP’’ via both3 Lexical decisions <strong>in</strong> the DRC model (Coltheart et al., 2001) are made on the basis of two sources of<strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation: total activation <strong>in</strong> the orthographic lexicon and maximum activation <strong>in</strong> the orthographic lexicon.The simulations of Harm and Seidenberg (2004) trialled a number of potential orthographic and semantic sourcesof <strong>in</strong><strong>for</strong>mation <strong>for</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g lexical decisions, but did not make a firm commitment to any of these.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!