11.07.2015 Views

Sex Differences in Sex Drive, Sociosexuality, and Height across 53 ...

Sex Differences in Sex Drive, Sociosexuality, and Height across 53 ...

Sex Differences in Sex Drive, Sociosexuality, and Height across 53 ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Arch <strong>Sex</strong> BehavDOI 10.1007/s10508-007-9242-8ORIGINAL PAPER<strong>Sex</strong> <strong>Differences</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Sex</strong> <strong>Drive</strong>, <strong>Sociosexuality</strong>, <strong>and</strong> <strong>Height</strong> <strong>across</strong><strong>53</strong> Nations: Test<strong>in</strong>g Evolutionary <strong>and</strong> Social Structural TheoriesRichard A. LippaReceived: 4 September 2006 / Revised: 14 June 2007 / Accepted: 14 June 2007Ó Spr<strong>in</strong>ger Science+Bus<strong>in</strong>ess Media, LLC 2007Abstract By analyz<strong>in</strong>g cross-cultural patterns <strong>in</strong> fiveparameters—sex differences, male <strong>and</strong> female trait means,male <strong>and</strong> female trait st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations—researchers canbetter test evolutionary <strong>and</strong> social structural models of sexdifferences. Five models of biological <strong>and</strong> social structural<strong>in</strong>fluence are presented that illustrate this proposal. Us<strong>in</strong>gdata from <strong>53</strong> nations <strong>and</strong> from over 200,000 participantssurveyed <strong>in</strong> a recent BBC Internet survey, I exam<strong>in</strong>edcross-cultural patterns <strong>in</strong> these five parameters for twosexual traits—sex drive <strong>and</strong> sociosexuality—<strong>and</strong> forheight, a physical trait with a biologically based sex difference.<strong>Sex</strong> drive, sociosexuality, <strong>and</strong> height all showedconsistent sex differences <strong>across</strong> nations (mean ds = .62,.74, <strong>and</strong> 1.63). Women were consistently more variable thanmen <strong>in</strong> sex drive (mean female to male variance ratio =1.64). Gender equality <strong>and</strong> economic development tendedto predict, <strong>across</strong> nations, sex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality,but not sex differences <strong>in</strong> sex drive or height. Parameters forsociosexuality tended to vary <strong>across</strong> nations more thanparameters for sex drive <strong>and</strong> height did. The results forsociosexuality were most consistent with a hybrid model—thatboth biological <strong>and</strong> social structural <strong>in</strong>fluencescontribute to sex differences, whereas the results for sexdrive <strong>and</strong> height were most consistent with a biologicalmodel—that evolved biological factors are the primarycause of sex differences. The model test<strong>in</strong>g proposed hereencourages evolutionary <strong>and</strong> social structural theorists tomake more precise <strong>and</strong> nuanced predictions about the pattern<strong>in</strong>gof sex differences <strong>across</strong> cultures.R. A. Lippa (&)Department of Psychology, California State University,Fullerton, Fullerton, CA 92834, USAe-mail: rlippa@fullerton.eduKeywords Evolutionary theory BBC Internet study <strong>Sex</strong> differences <strong>Sex</strong> drive <strong>Sociosexuality</strong> Socialstructural theoryIntroductionCross-cultural research provides an important test<strong>in</strong>gground for both evolutionary <strong>and</strong> social structural theoriesof sex differences <strong>in</strong> sexuality. Proponents of evolutionarytheories argue that cross-culturally consistent sex differences<strong>in</strong> traits such as sociosexuality (i.e., restricted versusunrestricted sexual attitudes <strong>and</strong> behavior), sexual activitylevels, <strong>and</strong> the characteristics preferred <strong>in</strong> mates (e.g.,physical attractiveness) constitute strong evidence thatevolved biological dispositions underlie these sex differences—dispositionsthat show through the ‘‘noise’’ ofcultural variations (Buss, 1989, 2005; Schmitt, 2005). Inresponse, proponents of social structural theories note thatthere are strong associations, <strong>across</strong> societies, between<strong>in</strong>dices of gender equality <strong>and</strong> sex differences <strong>in</strong> sexuality,<strong>and</strong> these associations constitute powerful evidence thatsocial <strong>and</strong> cultural factors contribute to these sex differences(Eagly & Wood, 1999, 2005; Wood & Eagly, 2002).Despite the either-or tone that often colors the debatebetween evolutionary <strong>and</strong> social structural theorists, thesetwo approaches are not necessarily mutually exclusive, <strong>and</strong>it is likely that both have valid doma<strong>in</strong>s of application(Lippa, 2007; Schmitt, 2005). Cross-cultural consistencies<strong>in</strong> sex differences (e.g., <strong>in</strong> all countries <strong>and</strong> cultures studiedto date, men score higher than women on sociosexuality)may <strong>in</strong>deed constitute evidence that there are biologicalcomponents to these sex differences. At the same time, thepower of <strong>in</strong>dices of gender equality to predict sex differences<strong>in</strong> sexuality <strong>across</strong> cultures suggests that some sex123


Arch <strong>Sex</strong> Behavdifferences are highly sensitive to cultural ideologies <strong>and</strong>social ecologies. In a sense, what constitutes the ‘‘figure’’to one theoretical approach (e.g., expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g systematiccross-cultural variation <strong>in</strong> sex differences is of primary<strong>in</strong>terest to social structural theory) is background noise tothe other (cross-cultural variation is sometimes regarded byevolutionary theory as error variance mask<strong>in</strong>g the ‘‘ma<strong>in</strong>effect’’ of evolved sex differences).Recent attempts to test the relative power of evolutionary<strong>and</strong> social structural theories to account for crossculturaldata on sex differences have been <strong>in</strong>conclusive, <strong>in</strong>part because the data to be expla<strong>in</strong>ed do not decisivelydist<strong>in</strong>guish between the two theoretical approaches. To usea statistical metaphor, the models proposed by the twoapproaches are under-identified by the data that have beenused to test them. For example, evolutionary theoristsargue that the consistent f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g that men score higher thanwomen on sociosexuality <strong>in</strong> all cultures surveyed to date isstrong evidence for evolved sex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality(Buss, 2005; Schmitt, 2005). Social structural theoristscounter that all modern societies display patriarchalsocial structures, albeit to vary<strong>in</strong>g degrees, <strong>and</strong> thus it is notsurpris<strong>in</strong>g that all societies show unidirectional sex differences<strong>in</strong> traits such as sociosexuality, sexual activitylevels, <strong>and</strong> preferences for particular traits <strong>in</strong> mates (Eagly& Wood, 2005). Thus, the very same f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs—unidirectionalsex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality that vary <strong>across</strong>cultures—can be expla<strong>in</strong>ed, post-hoc, by both evolutionary<strong>and</strong> social structural theories.One goal of the current research was to generate betteridentified models that could more clearly dist<strong>in</strong>guishbetween the predictions of evolutionary <strong>and</strong> social structuraltheories. One route to achiev<strong>in</strong>g this goal is to providemore ‘‘data po<strong>in</strong>ts’’ to be fitted to the theories’ proposedmodels. To accomplish this goal, <strong>in</strong> the research reportedhere I focused not only on the ability of evolutionary <strong>and</strong>social structural theories to predict sex differences <strong>across</strong>cultures, but also on their ability to predict the pattern ofmale <strong>and</strong> female mean trait levels <strong>and</strong> the pattern of male<strong>and</strong> female trait st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations (SDs) <strong>across</strong> cultures.By attempt<strong>in</strong>g to predict the patterns of these additionalparameters, researchers may be able to generate betteridentified models that can discrim<strong>in</strong>ate more conclusivelybetween evolutionary <strong>and</strong> social structural theories.Researchers can also evaluate the predictive power ofhybrid models that assume both biological <strong>and</strong> socialstructural contributions to sex differences.To illustrate the promise of these methods, I analyzed alarge cross-cultural dataset generated by a recent BBCInternet survey of over 200,000 participants, <strong>and</strong> I focusedparticularly on two sexual traits—sex drive <strong>and</strong> sociosexuality.My goal was to demonstrate that cross-culturalpatterns of sex differences, male <strong>and</strong> female trait means,<strong>and</strong> male <strong>and</strong> female trait SDs were different for these twotraits. I argue, based on these f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, that different modelsof biological <strong>and</strong> social structural <strong>in</strong>fluence are needed toaccount for the cross-cultural patterns of results for thesetwo sexual traits. Thus, <strong>in</strong> addition to apply<strong>in</strong>g newmethods to the cross-cultural study of sex differences, Ialso exam<strong>in</strong>e the possibility that the relative power ofevolutionary <strong>and</strong> social structural theories to expla<strong>in</strong> sexdifferences may vary depend<strong>in</strong>g on the trait underconsideration.In addition to analyz<strong>in</strong>g data on sex drive <strong>and</strong> sociosexuality,I also conducted parallel analyses on a third,nonsexual trait—self-reported height. Because it seemsnoncontroversial to assume that sex differences <strong>in</strong> heightare largely determ<strong>in</strong>ed by evolved genetic differencesbetween the sexes (see Gaul<strong>in</strong> & Boster, 1985), results forheight provide a pure example of the pattern of results thatmight be expected for a sex difference that is largely biological<strong>in</strong> orig<strong>in</strong>. This pattern of results can then be comparedwith the correspond<strong>in</strong>g patterns of results for sexdrive <strong>and</strong> sociosexuality.Five Hypothetical Cross-National Patterns of <strong>Sex</strong><strong>Differences</strong>, Male <strong>and</strong> Female Means, <strong>and</strong> Male<strong>and</strong> Female SDsThe follow<strong>in</strong>g sections present five models of how biological<strong>and</strong> cultural factors might contribute to sex differences,trait means, <strong>and</strong> trait SDs <strong>in</strong> psychological traits. Itis important to note that these models are not exhaustive.Rather, they constitute a small subset of a large universe ofpossible models.The Pure Biological Causation ModelSome sex differences result largely from biological causes.Possible examples are sex differences <strong>in</strong> height, fundamentalvoice frequency, waist-to-hip ratios, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividuals’degree of sexual attraction to men <strong>and</strong> to women. Inthe pure biological model, sex differences are constant<strong>across</strong> cultures <strong>and</strong> countries, <strong>and</strong> male <strong>and</strong> female traitSDs are assumed not to vary <strong>across</strong> cultures. Male <strong>and</strong>female trait means may vary <strong>across</strong> cultures—for example,men’s <strong>and</strong> women’s mean heights could vary <strong>across</strong> culturesas a result of biological factors (e.g., genetic differencesbetween ethnic groups) <strong>and</strong> environmental factors(national <strong>and</strong> cultural differences <strong>in</strong> diet <strong>and</strong> health care).However, if men’s <strong>and</strong> women’s means vary <strong>across</strong> cultures,they are assumed to do so <strong>in</strong> a parallel fashion (e.g.,genetic or nutritional factors <strong>in</strong> given cultures wouldequally depress or elevate male <strong>and</strong> female mean heights).Because men’s <strong>and</strong> women’s means vary <strong>in</strong> a parallelfashion <strong>and</strong> men’s <strong>and</strong> women’s SDs are constant <strong>across</strong>123


Arch <strong>Sex</strong> Behavparticularly low variability <strong>in</strong> unrestricted societies <strong>and</strong> particularlyhigh variability <strong>in</strong> restricted societies, whose culturalpressures conflict with their <strong>in</strong>nate dispositions. Similarly,women may show particularly low variability <strong>in</strong> restrictedsocieties <strong>and</strong> particularly high variability <strong>in</strong> unrestrictedsocieties, whose cultural pressures conflict with their <strong>in</strong>natedispositions (or, alternately, whose liberal ideologies allowwomen freer re<strong>in</strong> to express <strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality).When we superimpose the pattern <strong>in</strong> Fig. 3 (greatercultural <strong>in</strong>fluence on women’s than on men’s SDs) with thebiology-culture <strong>in</strong>teraction just hypothesized, the net resultis the follow<strong>in</strong>g: Women show strongly decreased SDs<strong>in</strong> sociosexuality <strong>in</strong> restrictive cultures, because they aregenerally more <strong>in</strong>fluenced by cultural pressures than menare <strong>and</strong>, simultaneously, the cultural press <strong>in</strong> restrictivecultures is congruent with their <strong>in</strong>nate disposition, so thetwo <strong>in</strong>fluences superimpose <strong>and</strong> re<strong>in</strong>force one another. Effectsfor men should be weaker, because the effect of culture (restrictedcultures reduce variance <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality) opposes thehypothesized biology-culture <strong>in</strong>teraction (men will showmore variable sociosexuality <strong>in</strong> restrictive cultures, because <strong>in</strong>these cultures they are subject to the countervail<strong>in</strong>g pushes <strong>and</strong>pulls of cultural pressures <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>nate dispositions).Previous Research on <strong>Sex</strong> <strong>Drive</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Sociosexuality</strong>To place the current research on sex differences <strong>in</strong> sex drive<strong>and</strong> sociosexuality <strong>in</strong> a broader context, <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>gsections I briefly review previous work on the assessment ofthese traits <strong>and</strong> exist<strong>in</strong>g evidence on sex differences <strong>in</strong> sexdrive <strong>and</strong> sociosexuality.Measurement of <strong>Sex</strong> <strong>Drive</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Sociosexuality</strong>A number of self-report measures of sex drive have appeared<strong>in</strong> recent years (Lippa, 2006; Ostovich & Sab<strong>in</strong>i, 2004;Spector, Carey, & Ste<strong>in</strong>berg, 1996).Thesescalestypicallyassess respondents’ desire for sex, their frequency of sexualactivity, their degree of th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g about <strong>and</strong> fantasiz<strong>in</strong>g aboutsex, <strong>and</strong> their evaluation of the reward<strong>in</strong>gness of sex. Forexample, <strong>in</strong> a recent study of the relation between sex drive<strong>and</strong> same-sex <strong>and</strong> other-sex attractions (Lippa, 2006), I useda five-item sex drive scale that asked participants to rate howmuch they agreed with the follow<strong>in</strong>g statements: (1) ‘‘I havea strong sex drive;’’ (2) ‘‘I frequently th<strong>in</strong>k about sex;’’ (3) ‘‘Itdoesn’t take much to get me sexually excited;’’ (4) ‘‘I th<strong>in</strong>kabout sex almost every day;’’ (5) ‘‘<strong>Sex</strong>ual pleasure is themost <strong>in</strong>tense pleasure a person can have.’’The most commonly used <strong>in</strong>strument used to assesssociosexuality is the <strong>Sociosexuality</strong> Orientation Inventory(SOI; Simpson & Gangestad, 1991), a 7-item scale that<strong>in</strong>cludes three overt behavior items (e.g., ‘‘With how manydifferent partners have you had sex with<strong>in</strong> the past year?),one covert behavior item (‘‘How often do [did] you fantasizeabout hav<strong>in</strong>g sex with someone other than yourcurrent [most recent] dat<strong>in</strong>g partner?’’), <strong>and</strong> three attitud<strong>in</strong>alitems (e.g., ‘‘<strong>Sex</strong> without love is ok.’’). Because thebehavior items have different response formats from attitud<strong>in</strong>al<strong>and</strong> covert behavioral items, Simpson <strong>and</strong> Gangestadproposed a complex weight<strong>in</strong>g system to comb<strong>in</strong>eSOI items (see Simpson & Gangestad, 1991, Appendix A).Although the SOI shows good <strong>in</strong>ternal consistency,highly skewed response distributions for some items <strong>and</strong>the complex weight<strong>in</strong>g system proposed by Simpson <strong>and</strong>Gangestad for scor<strong>in</strong>g the SOI have led some researchersto question its psychometric soundness (Voracek, 2005).Another concern is that responses to some items (e.g., thosethat assess number of sexual partners <strong>in</strong> the past year <strong>and</strong>number of one-night st<strong>and</strong>s) depend too much on participants’relationship status. One item (fantasiz<strong>in</strong>g about sexwith someone other than one’s current or most recentdat<strong>in</strong>g partner) does not readily apply to married people orto people <strong>in</strong> committed relationships, <strong>and</strong> thus it may notbe appropriate for studies that assess non-college students.F<strong>in</strong>ally, behavioral items may tend to confound the constructsof sex drive <strong>and</strong> sociosexuality.<strong>Sex</strong> <strong>Differences</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Sex</strong> <strong>Drive</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Sociosexuality</strong>In a comprehensive review, Baumeister, Catanese, <strong>and</strong> Vohs(2001) surveyed a broad range of research results <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>gthat, on average, men’s sex drive is higher than women’s. Insummariz<strong>in</strong>g their f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, they observed that ‘‘[t]here wereno measures that showed women hav<strong>in</strong>g stronger drives thanmen’’ (p. 264). As part of their review, Baumeister et al.exam<strong>in</strong>ed research results from several non-western societies,<strong>and</strong> they concluded that these studies were consistent withother studies show<strong>in</strong>g that men have higher sex drive thanwomen. However, they acknowledged that ‘‘[r]igorous datafrom other cultures are difficult to f<strong>in</strong>d…’’ (p. 268).There is little quantitative <strong>in</strong>formation on the magnitudeof sex differences <strong>in</strong> sex drive. In a recent study (Lippa,2006, Study 3), I computed a reliable (a = .82) multi-itemself-report measure of sex drive, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> a sample of over1,700 participants, heterosexual men <strong>and</strong> women showed alarge difference <strong>in</strong> their self-reported sex drive (d = .82).Furthermore, women were more variable than men <strong>in</strong> theirsex drive (female to male variance ratios were 1.43 forheterosexual participants). Us<strong>in</strong>g a less reliable one-itemmeasure of sex drive to assess more than 1,700 college123


Arch <strong>Sex</strong> Behavstudents (Lippa, 2006, Study 1), I found a smaller but stillsubstantial difference <strong>in</strong> heterosexual men’s <strong>and</strong> women’ssex drives (d = .58) <strong>and</strong>, once aga<strong>in</strong>, women’s self-reportedsex drive was significantly more variable than men’s(female to male variance ratio = 1.30). Ostavich <strong>and</strong> Sab<strong>in</strong>i(2004) used a four-item scale to assess sex drive <strong>in</strong> 129college men <strong>and</strong> 148 college women, <strong>and</strong> their statisticstranslated to a very large sex difference (d = 1.17). Theirdata also showed women to be more variable than men <strong>in</strong>sex drive (female to male variance ratio = 1.60).An ambitious study by Schmitt (2005) has provided thecurrent gold st<strong>and</strong>ard for estimates of sex differences <strong>in</strong>sociosexuality. Us<strong>in</strong>g SOI data collected <strong>in</strong> 48 nations,Schmitt found d values for sex differences that ranged from.30 to 1.24 <strong>in</strong> various nations, with an overall mean valueof .74. Schmitt concluded that ‘‘[t]hese results…place sexdifferences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality…among the largest <strong>and</strong> culturallymost robust ever documented <strong>in</strong> the doma<strong>in</strong> of sex<strong>and</strong> human mat<strong>in</strong>g’’ (p. 262). Schmitt further noted that‘‘[w]ith<strong>in</strong> the constra<strong>in</strong>ts of the current methodology <strong>and</strong>sampl<strong>in</strong>g limitations, it can be concluded from these resultsthat sex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality are a cultural universal,support<strong>in</strong>g the basic tenets of parental <strong>in</strong>vestmenttheory…’’ (p. 265).Social Structural Factors that Correlate with <strong>Sex</strong><strong>Differences</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Sociosexuality</strong> <strong>across</strong> CulturesIn addition to document<strong>in</strong>g cross-culturally consistent sexdifferences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality, Schmitt (2005) identified anumber of cultural, economic, <strong>and</strong> demographic factors thatwere associated, <strong>across</strong> nations, with men’s <strong>and</strong> women’slevels of sociosexuality <strong>and</strong> with sex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality.For example, <strong>in</strong>dices of nations’ degree of genderequality tended to be associated with women’s levels ofsociosexuality <strong>and</strong> with sex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality,with greater gender equality associated with higher levelsof sociosexuality <strong>in</strong> women <strong>and</strong> smaller sex differences.Measures of economic <strong>and</strong> environmental stress (e.g., asassessed by <strong>in</strong>fant mortality rates, life expectancy, <strong>and</strong> grossdomestic product) were associated, <strong>across</strong> nations, withlower levels of sociosexuality <strong>in</strong> both men <strong>and</strong> women.Schmitt viewed these f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs as provid<strong>in</strong>g strong supportfor strategic pluralism theory, which proposes that humanshave evolved a menu of mat<strong>in</strong>g strategies, which are triggeredby environmental cont<strong>in</strong>gencies (Gangestad &Simpson, 2000). F<strong>in</strong>ally, higher sex ratios (more men thanwomen) were associated with lower national levels ofsociosexuality, support<strong>in</strong>g some of the predictions of sexratio theory (Guttentag & Secord, 1983; Lazarus, 2002;Pedersen, 1991). The results that follow will, among otherth<strong>in</strong>gs, test whether Schmitt’s f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs replicate <strong>in</strong> anotherlarge <strong>in</strong>ternational sample.MethodParticipants <strong>and</strong> ProcedureFrom February through May 2005, the British Broadcast<strong>in</strong>gCorporation (BBC) conducted an English-languageInternet survey, which focused on human sex differences <strong>in</strong>cognition, motivation, personality, <strong>and</strong> sexuality. The survey,designed to produce data for use <strong>in</strong> the BBC documentarySecrets of the <strong>Sex</strong>es, was advertised on the BBCwebsite <strong>and</strong> participants responded onl<strong>in</strong>e. Because of thebroad reach of the BBC as an <strong>in</strong>ternational news source,survey participants came from all over the world. Participantscould complete a variety of psychological tests <strong>and</strong>questionnaires, which were arranged <strong>in</strong> six modules, eachof which took about five m<strong>in</strong>utes to complete. A total of255,114 people responded to at least some items <strong>in</strong> everymodule. In the course of respond<strong>in</strong>g to approximately200 questions, most participants reported their sex <strong>and</strong>height <strong>and</strong> completed a two-item sex drive scale <strong>and</strong> athree-item scale that assessed the attitud<strong>in</strong>al component ofsociosexuality.For the entire BBC sample, men’s median age was 30(M = 32.26, SD = 11.26), <strong>and</strong> women’s median age was 28(M = 31.11, SD = 10.83). The current analyses focused onparticipants whose ages were ‡18 <strong>and</strong> £80 years. Thebreakdown of participants by reported relationship statuswas: 29% married, 29% s<strong>in</strong>gle, 16% liv<strong>in</strong>g together <strong>in</strong> aserious relationship, 15% liv<strong>in</strong>g apart <strong>in</strong> a serious relationship,8% <strong>in</strong> a causal relationship, 3% divorced, <strong>and</strong> under 1%widowed. Participants came from countries <strong>across</strong> the world,but the largest numbers were from the United K<strong>in</strong>gdom(45%), the United States (29%), Canada (5%), <strong>and</strong> Australia(4%). Participants from cont<strong>in</strong>ental Western Europe madeup about 6% of the sample.Fifty-three nations <strong>in</strong> the BBC dataset had samples of 90or more participants. In many of the national samples, therewere roughly equal numbers of men <strong>and</strong> women, althoughthere was a tendency for men to outnumber women. Inall national samples but two, both male <strong>and</strong> female samplesizes were larger than 40. The two exceptions wereVenezuela (female n = 32 for sociosexuality <strong>and</strong> sex drivescores) <strong>and</strong> Saudi Arabia (female n = 20 for sociosexuality<strong>and</strong> sex drive scores). In his 48-nation study of sociosexuality,Schmitt (2005) noted that ‘‘[a]t least 25 men <strong>and</strong> 25women were needed to achieve the necessary statisticalpower for evaluat<strong>in</strong>g sex differences…(when sett<strong>in</strong>gb = .80, a = .05, <strong>and</strong> look<strong>in</strong>g for an effect moderate tolarge <strong>in</strong> size…)’’ (p. 2<strong>53</strong>). All of the male <strong>and</strong> femalesamples <strong>in</strong> the current study achieved this criterion, exceptfor the sample of Saudi women. For additional demographic<strong>in</strong>formation about the BBC sample, see Lippa(2007) <strong>and</strong>Reimers(2007).123


Arch <strong>Sex</strong> BehavMeasuresDemographicDemographic <strong>in</strong>formation collected by the BBC Internetsurvey <strong>in</strong>cluded age, relationship status, country of residence,ethnicity, education level, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>come level. For anoverview of the BBC Internet survey, its questions, <strong>and</strong>their response formats, see Reimers (2007).Nations’ Dom<strong>in</strong>ant ReligionsMost of the <strong>53</strong> nations studied <strong>in</strong> the current study wereclassified, based on their majority religions, as Buddhist,Catholic, Eastern Orthodox, H<strong>in</strong>du, Jewish, Moslem, Protestant,or Taoist. Information about dom<strong>in</strong>ant religions wasobta<strong>in</strong>ed from the US Department of State InternationalReligious Freedom Report 2004. If Catholics or Protestantsdid not constitute a majority <strong>in</strong> a given country, but Christiansnonetheless formed a majority of the population, thennations were classified as ‘‘mixed Christian.’’ The five religiousgroup<strong>in</strong>gs that <strong>in</strong>cluded the most nations were: Catholic(Argent<strong>in</strong>a, Austria, Belgium, Brazil, Chile, Croatia,France, Hungary, Italy, Irel<strong>and</strong>, Lithuania, Malta, Mexico,the Philipp<strong>in</strong>es, Pol<strong>and</strong>, Portugal, Slovenia, Spa<strong>in</strong>, <strong>and</strong>Venezuela); Eastern Orthodox (Bulgaria, Cyprus, Greece,Romania, <strong>and</strong> Russia); Moslem (Egypt, Pakistan, Turkey,Saudi Arabia, <strong>and</strong> the United Arab Emirates); Protestant(Denmark, F<strong>in</strong>l<strong>and</strong>, Icel<strong>and</strong>, Norway, South Africa, Sweden,the United K<strong>in</strong>gdom, <strong>and</strong> the United States); <strong>and</strong> mixedChristian (Australia, Canada, Germany, the Netherl<strong>and</strong>s,New Zeal<strong>and</strong>, Switzerl<strong>and</strong>, <strong>and</strong> Tr<strong>in</strong>idad <strong>and</strong> Tobago).<strong>Sex</strong> <strong>Drive</strong>, <strong>Sociosexuality</strong>, <strong>and</strong> <strong>Height</strong>In one section of the BBC survey, participants were askedabout their romantic <strong>and</strong> sexual relationships, <strong>and</strong> there weretwo items that assessed sex drive: ‘‘I have a strong sex drive’’<strong>and</strong> ‘‘It doesn’t take much to get me sexually excited.’’Participants responded to these items us<strong>in</strong>g a 7-po<strong>in</strong>t scalethat ranged from ‘‘disagree’’ to ‘‘agree.’’ This section also<strong>in</strong>cluded three items that assessed the attitud<strong>in</strong>al componentof sociosexuality: ‘‘<strong>Sex</strong> without love is OK.’’ ‘‘I can imag<strong>in</strong>emyself be<strong>in</strong>g comfortable <strong>and</strong> enjoy<strong>in</strong>g casual sex withmultiple partners.’’ ‘‘I would have to be closely attached(emotionally <strong>and</strong> psychologically) to someone before I’dfeel comfortable <strong>and</strong> fully enjoy hav<strong>in</strong>g sex with him/her.’’Response formats were the same as for sex drive items. <strong>Sex</strong>drive <strong>and</strong> sociosexuality scores were computed from theseitems, with appropriate items reversed.The decision to use just attitud<strong>in</strong>al sociosexuality itemswas based on three considerations. First, although a numberof behavioral SOI items were <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the BBC survey,their word<strong>in</strong>g was sometimes different from that <strong>in</strong> theorig<strong>in</strong>al SOI <strong>and</strong> some were targeted at only a subset of theBBC participants (e.g., s<strong>in</strong>gle participants). Second, comput<strong>in</strong>gsociosexuality from just the attitud<strong>in</strong>al items sidesteppedsome of the psychometric problems that affect thefull sociosexuality scale. Third, the current research soughtto compare <strong>and</strong> contrast results for sex drive <strong>and</strong> sociosexuality.Because sex drive <strong>and</strong> attitud<strong>in</strong>al sociosexualityitems <strong>in</strong> the BBC survey used exactly the same responseformat <strong>and</strong> because the sex drive <strong>and</strong> sociosexuality scaleswere both short (a two-item <strong>and</strong> a three-item scale), the twomeasures were psychometrically similar—a major virtue <strong>in</strong>the current analyses.The BBC survey assessed participants’ height by ask<strong>in</strong>gthem to type <strong>in</strong>, as str<strong>in</strong>g variables, their height <strong>in</strong> feet <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>ches or <strong>in</strong> meters. In the f<strong>in</strong>al data files, heights wereconverted to feet <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>ches. To exclude implausibly lowor high values for height, I <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> my analyses onlyheights that ranged from 56 to 84 <strong>in</strong>ches (four feet eight<strong>in</strong>ches to seven feet). The number of participants who haduseable height <strong>in</strong>formation was somewhat smaller than thenumber who had valid sex drive <strong>and</strong> sociosexuality scores.Gender Equality, Economic Development, <strong>and</strong> <strong>Sex</strong> RatiosStatistics for United Nations gender-related development<strong>and</strong> gender empowerment <strong>in</strong>dices were taken from theUnited Nations 2005 <strong>and</strong> 2001 Human Development Reports(available at: http://www.hdr.undp.org/statistics/data/, see the section on ‘‘data by <strong>in</strong>dicator’’). The UN genderrelateddevelopment <strong>in</strong>dex assesses nations’ gender equityon three dimensions: health <strong>and</strong> longevity, st<strong>and</strong>ard ofliv<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> knowledge <strong>and</strong> education. The UN genderempowerment measure assesses nations’ gender equity onthree power dimensions: power over economic resources,participation <strong>in</strong> economic decision mak<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> participation<strong>in</strong> political decision mak<strong>in</strong>g. In several cases, when2005 statistics were not available for given nations, I used2001 statistics <strong>in</strong>stead. United Nations gender empowermentstatistics were not available for six of the <strong>53</strong> nationsstudied here.Indices of economic development were also obta<strong>in</strong>edfrom UN Human Development reports. These <strong>in</strong>cludednations’ per capita <strong>in</strong>come <strong>in</strong> U.S. dollars, life expectancy,<strong>in</strong>fant mortality per thous<strong>and</strong> live births, fertility rates, <strong>and</strong>rates of contraception use. A f<strong>in</strong>al variable, national sexratios for people aged 15–64 years of age, was obta<strong>in</strong>edfrom the World Fact Book 2005, a publication of the UnitedStates Central Intelligence Agency (http://www.odci.gov/cia/publications/factbook/<strong>in</strong>dex.html).123


Arch <strong>Sex</strong> BehavResultsReliability of <strong>Sex</strong> <strong>Drive</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Sociosexuality</strong>Across Nations <strong>and</strong> the Correlation of <strong>Sex</strong><strong>Drive</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Sociosexuality</strong>Mean reliabilities (a) over all <strong>53</strong> nations for sex drive <strong>and</strong>sociosexuality were .72 <strong>and</strong> .78, respectively. Reliabilities forsex drive <strong>and</strong> sociosexuality computed over all participantswere .79 <strong>and</strong> .81, respectively. Over all participants, sex drive<strong>and</strong> sociosexuality were modestly correlated, r(210277) =.28, p < .001. The correlation of sex drive <strong>and</strong> sociosexualitywas lower when computed separately for men, r(115923)=.21,p < .001, <strong>and</strong> for women, r(942<strong>53</strong>) = .20, p < .001,suggest<strong>in</strong>g that the association between sex drive <strong>and</strong>sociosexuality <strong>in</strong> the entire sample was partly due to thefact that both variables were correlated with sex.Intercorrelation of Indices of Gender Equality,Economic Development, <strong>and</strong> <strong>Sex</strong> RatiosTable 1 presents <strong>in</strong>tercorrelations of <strong>in</strong>dices of gender equality,economic development, <strong>and</strong> sex ratios. The two UN<strong>in</strong>dices (gender development <strong>and</strong> gender empowerment) werehighly correlated (r = .82) <strong>and</strong> they were also substantiallycorrelated with <strong>in</strong>dices of economic development. In general,the correlations <strong>in</strong> Table 1 show that countries that weregender egalitarian also tended to be high on economicdevelopment, whereas countries that were gender nonegalitariantended to be low on economic development. The onlymeasure that was relatively <strong>in</strong>dependent of other measureswas that of national sex ratios.<strong>Sex</strong> <strong>Drive</strong>The follow<strong>in</strong>g sections present the core analyses of thecurrent study, with one section devoted to each of the threetraits under study: sex drive, sociosexuality, <strong>and</strong> height. Ineach section, I first exam<strong>in</strong>e the <strong>in</strong>tercorrelations of male<strong>and</strong> female means, male <strong>and</strong> female SDs, <strong>and</strong> sex differencesfor the trait under study. The goal was to see whichfactor contributed most to sex differences: male means,female means, male SDs, or female SDs. A second goalwas to see whether there were national traits of sex drive,sociosexuality, <strong>and</strong> height. To the extent that male <strong>and</strong>female means correlated strongly <strong>across</strong> nations, then thisprovided evidence for national traits (e.g., if male <strong>and</strong>female heights strongly correlated <strong>across</strong> nations, then thiswould <strong>in</strong>dicate there were consistently ‘‘shorter’’ <strong>and</strong>‘‘taller’’ nations, on average).To <strong>in</strong>vestigate the power of cultural <strong>and</strong> social structuralfactors to predict sex differences <strong>and</strong> other parameters, ITable 1 Intercorrelations <strong>across</strong> nations of United Nations <strong>in</strong>dices of gender development <strong>and</strong> gender empowerment, <strong>in</strong>dices of economic development, <strong>and</strong> sex ratiosUN gender development UN gender empowerment Per capital <strong>in</strong>come Life expectancy Infant mortality Fertility Contraception <strong>Sex</strong> ratiosUN gender development –UN gender empowerment .82*** (47) –Per capital <strong>in</strong>come .78*** (52) .79*** (47) –Life expectancy .84*** (<strong>53</strong>) .69*** (47) .68*** (52) –Infant mortality –.91*** (<strong>53</strong>) –.60*** (47) –.57*** (52) –.82*** (<strong>53</strong>) –Fertility –.67*** (<strong>53</strong>) –.49** (47) –.34* (52) –.46*** (<strong>53</strong>) .70*** (<strong>53</strong>) –Contraception .59*** (45) .59** (39) .50** (44) .39** (45) –.43** (45) –.51*** (45) –<strong>Sex</strong> ratios –.13 (<strong>53</strong>) –.19 (47) .09 (52) .10 (<strong>53</strong>) .03 (<strong>53</strong>) .34 (<strong>53</strong>) –.46** (45) –Note:*p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001. Numbers <strong>in</strong> parentheses are sample sizes. All <strong>in</strong>dices are taken from 2005 <strong>and</strong> 2001 United Nations Human Development Reports, except for measuresof sex ratios, which come from the CIA World Fact Book, 2005123


Arch <strong>Sex</strong> Behavpresent for each trait the correlations between social <strong>in</strong>dices<strong>and</strong> the five parameters of <strong>in</strong>terest: male means, femalemeans, male SDs, female SDs, <strong>and</strong> sex differences. To theextent that social <strong>in</strong>dices predict parameters strongly (e.g.,UN <strong>in</strong>dices of gender equality predict sex differences <strong>in</strong>sociosexuality), then this provides evidence that there issystematic variation <strong>across</strong> nations <strong>in</strong> the parameters. However,if social <strong>in</strong>dices do not predict parameters, then there islittle evidence for systematic national <strong>and</strong> cultural variation,<strong>and</strong> the likelihood <strong>in</strong>creases that sex differences—particularlythose that are consistent <strong>across</strong> nations—have biologicalcauses.To elucidate the observed patterns of correlations, I presentfor each trait graphs of the five parameters of <strong>in</strong>terest: malemeans, female means, male SDs, female SDs, <strong>and</strong> sex differences.In these graphs, nations were arrayed along the x-axis <strong>in</strong> the order of the magnitude of their sex differences.These graphs correspond closely to the graphs presented forthe hypothetical models presented <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>troduction. In af<strong>in</strong>al set of analyses, I use regression analyses to compare thepower of sex, gender equality as assessed by UN <strong>in</strong>dices, <strong>and</strong>their <strong>in</strong>teraction to predict male <strong>and</strong> female trait means <strong>across</strong><strong>53</strong> nations.Intercorrelations of Male <strong>and</strong> Female means, Male<strong>and</strong> Female SDs, <strong>and</strong> <strong>Sex</strong> <strong>Differences</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Sex</strong> <strong>Drive</strong>For sex drive, sex differences as assessed by the raw differencebetween male <strong>and</strong> female means correlated .97(p < .001) with d scores, <strong>and</strong> thus these two measures ofsex differences were essentially the same. Because d valuesare the conventional way to assess sex differences, I usedthem <strong>in</strong> the current analyses.Male <strong>and</strong> female SDs correlated relatively weakly with dstatistics (r =–.36,p


Arch <strong>Sex</strong> Behav<strong>Sex</strong> <strong>Drive</strong> Means <strong>and</strong> SDs <strong>and</strong> <strong>Sex</strong> <strong>Differences</strong> (d's)6.005.004.003.002.001.000.00male meansfemale meansmale SDsfemale SDsd'sVenezuelaCyprusIndiaMaltaSaudi ArabiaTr<strong>in</strong>idad, Tobag.PakistanPortugalThail<strong>and</strong>S<strong>in</strong>gaporeHungaryPhilipp<strong>in</strong>esNew Zeal<strong>and</strong>SloveniaAustraliaMalaysiaUnited Arab Em.BelgiumIcel<strong>and</strong>Irel<strong>and</strong>GermanyUKIsraelNetherl<strong>and</strong>sLithuaniaMexicoRomaniaSouth AfricaUSACanadaF<strong>in</strong>l<strong>and</strong>AustriaFranceCroatiaJapanSpa<strong>in</strong>ItalySwitzerl<strong>and</strong>Pol<strong>and</strong>SwedenBulgariaTurkeyNorwayGreeceEgyptCh<strong>in</strong>aChileDenmarkArgent<strong>in</strong>aBrazilEstoniaCzech Rep.RussiaCountriesFig. 5 <strong>Sex</strong> drive parameters <strong>across</strong> <strong>53</strong> nations presented <strong>in</strong> order of their sex differences <strong>in</strong> sex drived = 4.24. The very large d value may seem puzzl<strong>in</strong>g, giventhat the mean d for sex drive, <strong>across</strong> nations, was .62.However, d values for nations were computed with <strong>in</strong>dividualsas the unit of analysis, whereas <strong>in</strong> the paired-datat-test, nations was the unit of analysis <strong>and</strong> the data po<strong>in</strong>tswere national means. Thus, at the level of nations, men’smean sex drive levels were consistently much higher thanwomen’s mean sex drive levels.Exam<strong>in</strong>ation of Fig. 5 also reveals a second dramaticsex difference: Across nations, female SDs <strong>in</strong> sex drivewere consistently higher than male SDs. This differencewas also significant <strong>and</strong> strong, paired-data t(52) = –20.75,p < .001, d = –2.85, mean female-to-male variance ratio<strong>across</strong> nations = 1.64.As noted earlier, nations were arranged along the x-axis<strong>in</strong> order of their sex differences <strong>in</strong> sex drive <strong>in</strong> Fig. 5.Because the graph of sex differences <strong>in</strong>creased <strong>in</strong> such anorderly fashion, it is tempt<strong>in</strong>g to see this <strong>in</strong>crease asreflect<strong>in</strong>g systematic factors. However, this was not thecase. The order<strong>in</strong>g of nations <strong>in</strong> Fig. 5 did not show anyculturally or demographically coherent pattern. Furthermore,the largest-sample nations (e.g., the United K<strong>in</strong>gdom,the United States, Canada, Australia, <strong>and</strong> a number ofEuropean nations) tended to be <strong>in</strong> the middle of the list,whereas many small-sample nations were to the left <strong>and</strong>right sides of the array. This suggests that variations <strong>in</strong>effect sizes resulted from sampl<strong>in</strong>g error, not from systematicfactors. Figure 6 makes this po<strong>in</strong>t more apparentby plott<strong>in</strong>g effect sizes as a function of the log of samplesizes.The scatter plot shows that, for large-sample nations,effect sizes were stable, cluster<strong>in</strong>g around .60 whereas, forsmall-sample nations, effect size values scattered considerablyabout this mean level. Indeed, when effect sizes wereregressed on the log of sample sizes, the regression l<strong>in</strong>e wasessentially flat (b = .05, ns), <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that mean effect sizesdid not vary <strong>across</strong> samples of various sizes. However,log sample size correlated significantly with the absolutevalue of residuals, r = –.46, p = .001—that is, larger samplesizes were associated with less variable effect sizes. Thus,much of the variation <strong>in</strong> d values, <strong>across</strong> nations, appeared toresult from sampl<strong>in</strong>g error, not from systematic differences<strong>across</strong> nations <strong>and</strong> cultures.<strong>Sex</strong> <strong>and</strong> Gender Equality as Predictors of Mean Levelsof <strong>Sex</strong> <strong>Drive</strong>Us<strong>in</strong>g regression analysis, I exam<strong>in</strong>ed the power of sex <strong>and</strong>gender equality to predict male <strong>and</strong> female means <strong>across</strong>cultures. The data po<strong>in</strong>ts were the <strong>53</strong> male means <strong>and</strong> the <strong>53</strong>female means for sex drive. The predictor variables weresex, coded as ‘‘1’’ for males <strong>and</strong> ‘‘2’’ for females, <strong>and</strong> genderequality (the mean of the two highly correlated UN <strong>in</strong>dices).An <strong>in</strong>teraction term was also entered <strong>in</strong>to the regression—theproduct of the st<strong>and</strong>ardized sex <strong>and</strong> genderequality variables.These three orthogonal variables significantly predictedmale <strong>and</strong> female sex drive means, multiple r =.93,r 2 = .86,p < .001, with sex the strongest predictor: b for sex = –.92,p


Arch <strong>Sex</strong> Behav<strong>Sex</strong> <strong>Differences</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Sex</strong> <strong>Drive</strong>Sizes forEffect1.000.800.600.400.20the <strong>in</strong>teraction = .01, ns. These results are consistent with thegraphs of male <strong>and</strong> female sex drive means <strong>in</strong> Fig. 5, whichshow a highly consistent sex difference <strong>across</strong> nations, notmuch variation <strong>in</strong> men’s <strong>and</strong> women’s sex drive levels <strong>across</strong>nations, <strong>and</strong> quite parallel graph l<strong>in</strong>es for men <strong>and</strong> women(i.e., no <strong>in</strong>teraction).When a correspond<strong>in</strong>g regression was run on men’s <strong>and</strong>women’s SDs, the prediction was aga<strong>in</strong> strong, multipler =.88,r 2 =.78,p < .001, <strong>and</strong> aga<strong>in</strong> sex proved to be thestrongest predictor: b for sex = .88, p


Arch <strong>Sex</strong> BehavTable 3 Correlations, <strong>across</strong> nations, between social <strong>in</strong>dices <strong>and</strong> sociosexuality parametersMen’s means,sociosexualityWomen’s means,sociosexualityMen’s SDs Women’s SDs <strong>Sex</strong> differences (ds)UN gender development .64*** (<strong>53</strong>) .76*** (<strong>53</strong>) –.19 (<strong>53</strong>) .67*** (<strong>53</strong>) –.58*** (<strong>53</strong>)UN gender empowerment .48** (47) .71** (47) .04 (47) .62*** (47) –.67*** (47)Per capita <strong>in</strong>come .32* (52) .58*** (52) –.02 (52) .48*** (52) –.63*** (52)Life expectancy .38** (<strong>53</strong>) .55*** (<strong>53</strong>) –.21 (<strong>53</strong>) .47*** (<strong>53</strong>) –.50*** (<strong>53</strong>)Infant mortality –.55*** (<strong>53</strong>) –.62*** (<strong>53</strong>) .24 (<strong>53</strong> –.57*** (<strong>53</strong>) .42** (<strong>53</strong>)Fertility –.63*** (<strong>53</strong>) –.62*** (<strong>53</strong>) .33* (<strong>53</strong>) –.55*** (<strong>53</strong>) .32* (<strong>53</strong>)Contraception .55*** (45) .71*** (45) –.15 (45) .64*** (45) –.60*** (45)<strong>Sex</strong> ratios –.25 (<strong>53</strong>) –.27 (<strong>53</strong>) .24 (<strong>53</strong>) –.25 (<strong>53</strong>) .14 (<strong>53</strong>)Note:p < .1, * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001. Numbers <strong>in</strong> parentheses are sample sizes<strong>Sociosexuality</strong> Means <strong>and</strong> SDs <strong>and</strong> <strong>Sex</strong> <strong>Differences</strong> (d's)5.004.003.002.001.000.00male meansfemale meansmale SDsfemale SDsd's for sex diffPhilipp<strong>in</strong>esTr<strong>in</strong>idad, Tobag.PakistanSaudi ArabiaMalaysiaThail<strong>and</strong>TurkeyVenezuelaHungaryMaltaIndiaRussiaRomaniaIrel<strong>and</strong>United Arab Em.S<strong>in</strong>gaporeLithuaniaJapanSloveniaEstoniaPol<strong>and</strong>Argent<strong>in</strong>aCyprusPortugalGreeceBulgariaSouth AfricaBrazilBelgiumAustraliaCroatiaCzech Rep.UKCh<strong>in</strong>aUSACanadaSpa<strong>in</strong>MexicoChileEgyptIsraelGermanyItalyNew Zeal<strong>and</strong>NorwayNetherl<strong>and</strong>sFranceF<strong>in</strong>l<strong>and</strong>SwedenSwitzerl<strong>and</strong>DenmarkAustriaIcel<strong>and</strong>CountriesFig. 7 <strong>Sociosexuality</strong> parameters <strong>across</strong> <strong>53</strong> nations presented <strong>in</strong> order of their sex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexualitylevels decrease as we move from left to right, from small tolarge sex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality. Superimposed on thedownward slope of male <strong>and</strong> female means was a consistentsex difference <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality, with men higher than women.A paired-data t-test showed that this sex difference wassignificant <strong>and</strong> strong, t(52) = 27.60, p < .001, d = 3.79.The mean d-value for sex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality<strong>across</strong> <strong>53</strong> nations was .74.Women’s SDs <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality decreased as we movefrom left to right, whereas men’s SDs were more stable. Theresult was a crossover <strong>in</strong> female <strong>and</strong> male SDs. To the leftside of the graph women’s SDs tended to be larger thanmen’s, whereas to the right side of the graph men’s SDstended to be larger than women’s. This crossover effect wassignificant: <strong>across</strong> nations, the difference between women’s<strong>and</strong> men’s SDs correlated significantly with sex differences<strong>in</strong> sociosexuality, r = –.70, p < .001.There were systematic differences between the low-sexdifferencenations to the left <strong>and</strong> the high-sex-differencenations to the right of Fig. 7. On the left side are Icel<strong>and</strong>,Austria, Denmark, Switzerl<strong>and</strong>, Sweden, F<strong>in</strong>l<strong>and</strong>, France,<strong>and</strong> the Netherl<strong>and</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> on the right side are the Philipp<strong>in</strong>es,Tr<strong>in</strong>idad <strong>and</strong> Tobago, Pakistan, Saudi Arabia, Malaysia,Thail<strong>and</strong>, Turkey, <strong>and</strong> Venezuela. As the correlations <strong>in</strong>Table 6 suggest, nations to the left side of the x-axis tendedto be high on gender equality <strong>and</strong> economic development,whereas nations to the right side of the x-axis tended to below on gender equality <strong>and</strong> economic development.123


Arch <strong>Sex</strong> Behav<strong>Sex</strong> <strong>and</strong> Gender Equality as Predictors of Mean Levelsof <strong>Sociosexuality</strong>As <strong>in</strong> the analyses of sex drive, I exam<strong>in</strong>ed the power ofsex <strong>and</strong> gender equality to predict male <strong>and</strong> female means<strong>across</strong> cultures. The data po<strong>in</strong>ts were the <strong>53</strong> male means<strong>and</strong> the <strong>53</strong> female means for sociosexuality The predictorvariables were sex, gender equality (the mean of the twoUN <strong>in</strong>dices), <strong>and</strong> an <strong>in</strong>teraction term.These three orthogonal variables significantly predictedmale <strong>and</strong> female sociosexuality means, multiple r = .90,r 2 = .81, p < .001: b for sex = –.79, p < .001; b for genderequality = .41, p < .001; <strong>and</strong> b for the <strong>in</strong>teraction = .14,p = .002. These results are consistent with the graph of male<strong>and</strong> female means for sociosexuality presented <strong>in</strong> Fig. 7,which shows a highly consistent sex difference <strong>across</strong> nations,a systematic decl<strong>in</strong>e <strong>across</strong> nations <strong>in</strong> both men’s <strong>and</strong>women’s mean sociosexuality levels, <strong>and</strong> a steeper decl<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong>women’s than <strong>in</strong> men’s sociosexuality <strong>across</strong> nations (i.e., an<strong>in</strong>teraction effect).Note that the <strong>in</strong>teraction between sex <strong>and</strong> genderequality <strong>in</strong> the current analysis tapped the same phenomenathat the correlations between UN <strong>in</strong>dices <strong>and</strong> sex differencesdid <strong>in</strong> Table 3. However, the current analysis rem<strong>in</strong>dsus that although the power of culture (i.e., structural measuresof gender equality) to moderate sex differences wasreal <strong>and</strong> substantial for sociosexuality, when one predictsthe overall pattern of male <strong>and</strong> female means, the ma<strong>in</strong>effects for sex (b 2 =.62)<strong>and</strong>forculture(b 2 =.17)weremuch stronger than the moderat<strong>in</strong>g effect of culture on sexdifferences (b 2 = .02 for the <strong>in</strong>teraction). Stated anotherway, although culture moderated the magnitude of sexdifferences, it was never the case that culture elim<strong>in</strong>atedthese sex differences, which rema<strong>in</strong>ed quite powerfuloverall, despite the presence of significant cultural ma<strong>in</strong>effects <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>teractions.When a similar regression was conducted on men’s <strong>and</strong>women’s SDs for sociosexuality, the prediction was not asstrong, but still substantial, multiple r = .68, r 2 = .47,p < .001: b for sex = –.28, p < .001; b for gender equality= .43, p < .001; <strong>and</strong> b for the <strong>in</strong>teraction = .45,p < .001. The <strong>in</strong>teraction effect once aga<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicated thatthere was a crossover of women’s <strong>and</strong> men’s SDs as wemove from gender egalitarian to gender nonegalitarian nations.The ma<strong>in</strong> effect for sex reflected the fact that, <strong>in</strong>general, men’s SDs were a bit higher than women’s, paireddatat(52) = 2.84, p = .006, d = .39, <strong>and</strong> the ma<strong>in</strong> effect forculture reflected the fact that SDs <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality tended tobe smaller <strong>in</strong> gender nonegalitarian than <strong>in</strong> gender egalitariansocieties. It is <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g to note that the sex difference<strong>in</strong> sociosexuality SDs, to the extent it was present, wasopposite <strong>in</strong> direction to the sex difference <strong>in</strong> sex drive SDs.<strong>Height</strong>In the follow<strong>in</strong>g sections, I report analyses for height thatcorrespond to the analyses reported <strong>in</strong> the previous sectionsfor sex drive <strong>and</strong> sociosexuality. Recall that I assumed atthe start that sex differences <strong>in</strong> height are largely caused bybiological factors, <strong>and</strong> thus the results that follow shouldillustrate the patterns of results expected for a trait with abiologically caused sex difference.Intercorrelations of Male <strong>and</strong> Female means, Male<strong>and</strong> Female SDs, <strong>and</strong> <strong>Sex</strong> <strong>Differences</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Height</strong>Unlike the correspond<strong>in</strong>g results for sex drive <strong>and</strong> sociosexuality,difference scores <strong>and</strong> d statistics for height wereonly moderately correlated (r = .51, p < .001). The reasonfor this relatively weak correlation was that d statistics forheight correlated strongly with men’s <strong>and</strong> women’s SDs(r = –.79 <strong>and</strong> –.57, respectively), but only weakly withmen’s <strong>and</strong> women’s mean heights (r = –.04 <strong>and</strong> –.27,respectively), <strong>and</strong> thus variations <strong>in</strong> d scores for height weremuch more a function of SDs than of means. Variations <strong>in</strong>men’s <strong>and</strong> women’s SDs <strong>in</strong> height were, <strong>in</strong> turn, largely afunction of sample size, as illustrated by Fig. 8, whichpresents a scatter plot of male <strong>and</strong> female SDs as a functionof the logarithm of male <strong>and</strong> female sample sizes. In thisplot, SDs clustered narrowly around a value of three <strong>in</strong>ches<strong>in</strong> large samples, but they were much more scattered <strong>in</strong> smallsamples. The net result was that sex differences <strong>in</strong> height, asSt<strong>and</strong>ard Deviations <strong>in</strong> <strong>Height</strong> for Men <strong>and</strong> Wome n5.004.003.002.001.002.00 4.00 6.00 8.00 10.00 12.00Log Sample SizeFig. 8 SDs of men’s <strong>and</strong> women’s height <strong>in</strong> <strong>53</strong> nations as a functionof the logarithm of sample sizes123


Arch <strong>Sex</strong> Behavassessed by d statistics, were greatly <strong>in</strong>fluenced by r<strong>and</strong>omerror <strong>in</strong> the estimation of SDs. Consequently, raw differencescores provide a better measure of sex differences <strong>in</strong> heightthan d statistics do <strong>and</strong>, therefore, raw difference scoreswere used <strong>in</strong> the analyses that follow.Men’s <strong>and</strong> women’s mean heights were strongly correlated,<strong>across</strong> nations (r = .93, p < .001), <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g thatthere were consistent national levels of heights—i.e., therewere ‘‘shorter’’ nations (e.g., India, Pakistan, the Philipp<strong>in</strong>es,S<strong>in</strong>gapore) <strong>and</strong> there were ‘‘taller’’ nations (Estonia,Lithuania, the Netherl<strong>and</strong>s, Norway).Correlations between Social Indices <strong>and</strong> <strong>Height</strong>ParametersWere sex differences <strong>in</strong> height predicted, <strong>across</strong> nations, bycultural <strong>and</strong> social structural factors? Table 4 presentscorrelations between societal <strong>in</strong>dices <strong>and</strong> the five parametersof <strong>in</strong>terest: men’s means, women’s means, men’s SDs,women’s SDs, <strong>and</strong> sex differences <strong>in</strong> height. The rightmostcolumn of Table 4 shows that sex differences <strong>in</strong>height did not correlate significantly with any <strong>in</strong>dex. Therewere, however, significant correlations between <strong>in</strong>dices <strong>and</strong>men’s <strong>and</strong> women’s mean heights <strong>across</strong> nations. In general,gender equality <strong>and</strong> economic development wereassociated with greater height. This may seem surpris<strong>in</strong>g atfirst glance, but upon reflection the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g makes sense.Countries high on gender equality tend to be affluentcountries with good nutrition <strong>and</strong> good health care, both ofwhich may serve to <strong>in</strong>crease the average height of citizens.In general, <strong>in</strong>dices of gender equality <strong>and</strong> economicdevelopment were not associated with men’s <strong>and</strong> women’sSDs <strong>in</strong> height.The pattern of correlations <strong>in</strong> Table 4 is clarified byFig. 9, which graphically portrays the five parameters of<strong>in</strong>terest <strong>across</strong> the <strong>53</strong> nations, arrayed <strong>in</strong> order of themagnitude of their sex differences. The most strik<strong>in</strong>g featureof Fig. 9 is the consistency of sex differences <strong>in</strong> height<strong>across</strong> nations. The graphs of men’s means <strong>and</strong> women’smeans tended to be quite parallel <strong>across</strong> the <strong>53</strong> nations, withmen’s means always higher than women’s means. A paireddatat-test showed that sex differences <strong>in</strong> height were highlysignificant <strong>across</strong> nations <strong>and</strong> very strong, t(52) = 101.08,p < .001, d = 13.88, mean sex difference = 5.26 <strong>in</strong>ches,mean of <strong>53</strong> national ds = 1.63.Exam<strong>in</strong>ation of Fig. 9 also shows that men’s <strong>and</strong> women’sSDs appear not to differ much—all SDs were clusteredaround 3 <strong>in</strong>ches. Although it is not visually apparent, men’sSDs (M = 3.37) were slightly larger than women’s SDs(M = 3.03), paired-data t(52) = 4.42, p


Arch <strong>Sex</strong> Behav<strong>Height</strong> Means <strong>and</strong> SDs (<strong>in</strong> Feet)<strong>and</strong> <strong>Sex</strong> <strong>Differences</strong> (Raw <strong>Differences</strong>)7.006.005.004.003.002.001.00male meansfemale meansraw differencesmale SDsfemale SDs0.00GreeceRomaniaNetherl<strong>and</strong>sCh<strong>in</strong>aSaudi ArabiaArgent<strong>in</strong>aIcel<strong>and</strong>USAJapanHungaryUKLithuaniaRussiaPakistanF<strong>in</strong>l<strong>and</strong>Pol<strong>and</strong>BelgiumTr<strong>in</strong>idad, TobagoCanadaSwedenDenmarkIrel<strong>and</strong>SloveniaSouth AfricaTurkeyAustraliaCroatiaNew Zeal<strong>and</strong>IndiaNorwayAustriaPortugalGermanyCzech RepBulgariaEstoniaFranceBrazilCyprusItalyChileS<strong>in</strong>gaporeSpa<strong>in</strong>MalaysiaSwitzerl<strong>and</strong>United Arab EmiratesMaltaEgyptMexicoVenezuelaIsraelPhilipp<strong>in</strong>esThail<strong>and</strong>CountriesFig. 9 <strong>Height</strong> parameters <strong>across</strong> <strong>53</strong> nations presented <strong>in</strong> order of their sex differences <strong>in</strong> heightFig. 9, which show a highly consistent sex difference <strong>across</strong>nations, not much variation <strong>in</strong> men’s <strong>and</strong> women’s height<strong>across</strong> nations, <strong>and</strong> quite parallel graph l<strong>in</strong>es for men <strong>and</strong>women (i.e., no <strong>in</strong>teraction).When a correspond<strong>in</strong>g regression was run on men’s <strong>and</strong>women’s SDs <strong>in</strong> height, the prediction was weak, multipler = .36, r 2 = .13, p = .003, <strong>and</strong> there was only one significantpredictor: b for sex = –.33, p = .001; b for genderequality = .07, ns; <strong>and</strong> b for the <strong>in</strong>teraction = .12, ns. Thesignificant effect for sex showed aga<strong>in</strong> that men’s SDswere significantly larger than women’s, which I hypothesizedmight be due to a scal<strong>in</strong>g effect. The low multipler was consistent with earlier evidence suggest<strong>in</strong>g thatvariation <strong>in</strong> height SDs resulted largely from sampl<strong>in</strong>gerror, not from systematic factors.Religion, <strong>Sex</strong> <strong>Drive</strong>, <strong>and</strong> <strong>Sociosexuality</strong>As noted earlier, the five religious group<strong>in</strong>gs that comprisedthe most nations <strong>in</strong> the total set of <strong>53</strong> nations were: Catholicnations (n = 18), Eastern Orthodox nations (n = 5), Moslemnations (n = 5), Protestant nations (n = 8), <strong>and</strong> mixedChristian nations (n = 7). To probe the effect of religion onsex differences <strong>in</strong> sex drive <strong>and</strong> sociosexuality, I conductedtwo one-way ANOVAs—the first on sex differences <strong>in</strong> sexdrive <strong>and</strong> the second on sex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality.The groups of nations compared <strong>in</strong> the ANOVAs were thefive religious group<strong>in</strong>gs just described. The ANOVA on sexdifferences <strong>in</strong> sex drive did not show a significant difference<strong>across</strong> religions, F(4, 39) = 1.03, ns. However, the ANOVAon sex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality did show a significantdifference, F(4, 39) = 4.34, p = .005. Group means, orderedfrom smallest to largest sex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality,were: Protestant nations (mean d = .<strong>53</strong>), mixed Christiannations (mean d = .64), Catholic nations (mean d = .76),Eastern Orthodox nations (mean d = .83), <strong>and</strong> Moslem nations(mean d = .92). This spectrum could be characterizedas rang<strong>in</strong>g from more liberal, <strong>in</strong>dividualistic, <strong>and</strong> ‘‘western’’religions (although it should be noted that Protestantdenom<strong>in</strong>ations vary considerably on the ‘‘liberal’’ dimension)to more conservative, collectivist, <strong>and</strong> ‘‘eastern’’religions.Religions might <strong>in</strong>fluence sex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexualityvia the gender ideologies <strong>and</strong> social structures theypromote. To test this hypothesis, I ran the one-way ANOVAon sex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality aga<strong>in</strong>, this time treat<strong>in</strong>gsocietal gender equality (as assessed by the mean of the twoUN <strong>in</strong>dices) as a covariate. After controll<strong>in</strong>g for genderequality, the five groups of nations no longer differed significantly,F(4, 38) = .81, ns. This result was consistent withthe hypothesis that religions had their impact via themediat<strong>in</strong>g route of gender ideologies <strong>and</strong> social structure,although it did not prove the hypothesis.DiscussionIn the current research, measures of sex drive, sociosexuality,<strong>and</strong> height served as useful controls to one another. In123


Arch <strong>Sex</strong> Behavparticular, sex drive <strong>and</strong> sociosexuality measures were wellmatched <strong>in</strong> terms of their response formats <strong>and</strong> scalelengths. Because of this, it is unlikely that the reportedf<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs resulted from response sets or from the idiosyncraticways that people <strong>in</strong> different countries used rat<strong>in</strong>gscales. If this were so, then the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs for sex drive <strong>and</strong>sociosexuality would have shown similarities cross-culturally,which they did not. Indeed, if there is a centraltake-home message that emerges from the current results, itis how different the results were for sex drive <strong>and</strong> sociosexuality.These two traits showed different patterns of sexdifferences, different patterns of with<strong>in</strong>-sex variability,different patterns of cross-cultural consistency, <strong>and</strong> differentpatterns of association with outside criteria. Thus, onone level, the current research could be viewed as a complexdemonstration of the discrim<strong>in</strong>ant validity of theconstructs of sex drive <strong>and</strong> sociosexuality (see Ostovich &Sab<strong>in</strong>i, 2004; Simpson & Gangestad, 1991).At the start of this article, I offered five hypotheticalmodels of how male <strong>and</strong> female trait means, male <strong>and</strong>female trait SDs, <strong>and</strong> sex differences might vary <strong>across</strong>nations, depend<strong>in</strong>g on various assumptions about biological<strong>and</strong> socio-cultural <strong>in</strong>fluences on men’s <strong>and</strong> women’sbehavior. The results presented <strong>in</strong> the previous sections canbe summarized succ<strong>in</strong>ctly <strong>in</strong> terms of these models. Thef<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs for sex drive <strong>and</strong> height were most consistent withthe pure biological causation model, whereas the results forsociosexuality were most consistent with the hybrid model,which assumes cultural <strong>in</strong>fluences superimposed on biologicalpredispositions.For both sex drive <strong>and</strong> height, sex differences wereextremely consistent <strong>across</strong> nations <strong>and</strong> they were notmoderated by gender equality or associated with economicdevelopment. Regression analyses that probed the power ofsex, gender equality, <strong>and</strong> their <strong>in</strong>teraction to predict male<strong>and</strong> female trait means yielded very similar results for sexdrive <strong>and</strong> height: bs for sex were –.92 <strong>and</strong> –.94, respectively;bs for gender equality were –.11 <strong>and</strong> .17, respectively,<strong>and</strong> bs for <strong>in</strong>teractions were .01 <strong>and</strong> –.01,respectively. Thus, for these two traits, sex accounted for85–88% of the variance <strong>in</strong> male <strong>and</strong> female trait means,gender equality accounted for one to three percent of thevariance, <strong>and</strong> sex-by-culture <strong>in</strong>teractions accounted for<strong>in</strong>significant amounts of variance.At the same time, there were several noteworthy differencesbetween the results for sex drive <strong>and</strong> height. Forone, height was more trait-like <strong>across</strong> nations than sexdrive was, <strong>and</strong> this probably resulted from both geneticfactors (ethnic differences <strong>in</strong> stature) <strong>and</strong> biologically activeenvironmental factors (national differences <strong>in</strong> nutrition<strong>and</strong> health care). A second important f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g that dist<strong>in</strong>guishedsex drive from height (<strong>and</strong> from sociosexuality aswell) was that, <strong>across</strong> nations, women were consistentlymore variable than men <strong>in</strong> sex drive. <strong>Sex</strong> drive was theonly trait to show such a powerful sex difference <strong>in</strong> traitvariability. Although there was a weak sex difference <strong>in</strong>SDs for height, this may have resulted from scal<strong>in</strong>g effects,<strong>and</strong> the weak sex difference <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality SDs wasopposite <strong>in</strong> direction to the sex difference <strong>in</strong> sex drive SDs.While many of the results for sex drive <strong>and</strong> height weresimilar, the results for sociosexuality, <strong>in</strong> contrast, differed<strong>in</strong> many ways from correspond<strong>in</strong>g results for sex drive <strong>and</strong>height. Although men were consistently higher <strong>in</strong> sociosexualitythan women, this sex difference was moderatelymoderated by nations’ levels of gender equality <strong>and</strong> economicdevelopment. <strong>Sex</strong> differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexualityproved to be smallest <strong>in</strong> nations that were high on genderequality <strong>and</strong> economic development <strong>and</strong> largest <strong>in</strong> nationsthat were low on gender equality <strong>and</strong> economic development.Both men’s <strong>and</strong> women’s mean levels of sociosexualitydecreased as nations’ levels of gender equalitydecreased, but the decrease was steeper for women thanfor men.Furthermore, women’s, but not men’s, variability <strong>in</strong> sociosexualitydecreased as levels of gender equality decreased<strong>across</strong> nations. This resulted <strong>in</strong> a crossover effect for men’s<strong>and</strong> women’s SDs <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality: <strong>in</strong> gender egalitariancountries, women tended to be more variable than men, but <strong>in</strong>gender nonegalitarian countries men tended to be more variablethan women. This crossover effect is an important pieceof evidence support<strong>in</strong>g the hybrid model. Social structuraltheorists have argued that all modern societies are patriarchalto vary<strong>in</strong>g degrees, <strong>and</strong> this expla<strong>in</strong>s why women’s sociosexualityis lower than men’s <strong>in</strong> all modern societies.Extend<strong>in</strong>g this argument to men’s <strong>and</strong> women’s variability <strong>in</strong>sociosexuality, social structural theorists might be expected tosimilarly argue that if patriarchal culture restricts the variabilityof women’s sociosexuality compared to men’s, thenthis effect should be consistent <strong>in</strong> direction <strong>across</strong> cultures, butvariable <strong>in</strong> magnitude. However, this was not the case. Theobserved crossover effect was consistent with the hybridmodel’s prediction that men’s <strong>and</strong> women’s biological predispositions<strong>in</strong>teract with ‘‘cultural presses’’ to <strong>in</strong>fluence thevariability of men’s <strong>and</strong> women’s sociosexuality differently<strong>across</strong> cultures.The Current Results <strong>and</strong> Those of Schmitt (2005)Thirty of the <strong>53</strong> nations assessed <strong>in</strong> the current study werealso assessed <strong>in</strong> Schmitt’s (2005) 48-nation study of sociosexuality.Across these 30 nations, Schmitt’s estimatedeffect sizes for sex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality correlatedmoderately with the current study’s estimates, r = .49,p = .006. The relatively modest size of this correlation mayhave resulted from the different scales used to assess123


Arch <strong>Sex</strong> Behavsociosexuality <strong>in</strong> the two studies <strong>and</strong> from the differentsamples obta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> the two studies.In summariz<strong>in</strong>g the results of his study, Schmitt (2005)wrote, ‘‘the most consistent f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g was that men scoredhigher on sociosexuality than women <strong>across</strong> cultures’’ (p.273). Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Schmitt, this cross-cultural consistencyprovided strong support for sexual selection theory <strong>and</strong>parental <strong>in</strong>vestment theory, which imply that the sex that<strong>in</strong>vests more <strong>in</strong> offspr<strong>in</strong>g (i.e., women), will show morerestricted sociosexuality than the sex that <strong>in</strong>vests less <strong>in</strong>offspr<strong>in</strong>g (men). The current results constituted a strongreplication of Schmitt’s f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs on the strength <strong>and</strong> crossculturalconsistency of sex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality.Indeed, the overall mean effect size for sex differences <strong>in</strong>sociosexuality was identical <strong>in</strong> the two studies, d = .74.After correct<strong>in</strong>g the effect size <strong>in</strong> the current study forattenuation due the unreliability of the three-item sociosexualityscale, the estimate was even larger, d = .85, <strong>and</strong>because scale reliability represents just a portion of measurementerror <strong>and</strong> does not address other sources ofattenuation, such as restrictions <strong>in</strong> range, this was verylikely an under-correction (see Baugh, 2002; Bobko, Roth,& Bobko, 2001).The current research identified two additional sex differencesthat were remarkably consistent <strong>across</strong> cultures. Inall nations, men reported hav<strong>in</strong>g higher sex drives thanwomen (mean d = .62; when corrected for unreliability,d = .74), <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> all nations women had more variable sexdrives than men did (<strong>in</strong> a paired t-test with nations as theunits of analysis, d = 2.85; mean female-to-male varianceratio = 1.64). These f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs also seem <strong>in</strong>terpretable <strong>in</strong>terms of sexual selection <strong>and</strong> parental <strong>in</strong>vestment theory.Because women are the ‘‘choosier’’ sex <strong>and</strong> becausewomen often offer mat<strong>in</strong>g privileges to men <strong>in</strong> exchangefor resources, protection, <strong>and</strong> commitment, it seems reasonableto hypothesize that sexual selection would endowwomen, on average, with less urgent sex drives that aremore subject to rational control. As the limit<strong>in</strong>g resource <strong>in</strong>reproduction, women typically do not have to worry aboutf<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g men to impregnate them, although they may haveto worry about <strong>in</strong>duc<strong>in</strong>g men to stay around after impregnation.In contrast, men’s success at mat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> reproductiontends to be more variable than women’s, <strong>and</strong> thehighest levels of male success (def<strong>in</strong>ed here <strong>in</strong> terms oftransmission of genes to future generations) may sometimesresult from promiscuous sexual activity with multiplepartners. Thus, sexual selection likely has led men, onaverage, to have stronger <strong>and</strong> more consistently ‘‘turnedon’’ sex drives than women.The current research replicated a number of additionalf<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs reported <strong>in</strong> Schmitt (2005), <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> several casesthe current f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs were stronger than Schmitt’s. Forexample, stressed reproductive environments were associatedwith lower levels of sociosexuality <strong>in</strong> both men <strong>and</strong>women, <strong>and</strong> these associations tended to be stronger forwomen than men. Associations between societal stress <strong>and</strong>sociosexuality were generally stronger <strong>in</strong> the current studythan <strong>in</strong> Schmitt’s study. Schmitt reported only weak evidencethat societal stress <strong>and</strong> low economic developmentwere l<strong>in</strong>ked to sex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality. In contrast,the current study found stronger evidence, with socialstress associated with larger sex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality.Schmitt reported fairly strong associations betweenUN <strong>in</strong>dices of gender equality <strong>and</strong> women’s levels ofsociosexuality. The current research replicated these f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs<strong>and</strong> further documented substantial associationsbetween UN <strong>in</strong>dices <strong>and</strong> men’s sociosexuality <strong>and</strong> betweenUN <strong>in</strong>dices <strong>and</strong> sex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality.Why were results <strong>in</strong> the current study often stronger thancorrespond<strong>in</strong>g results <strong>in</strong> Schmitt’s (2005) study? One possibleanswer is that the current study used a shortsociosexuality scale consist<strong>in</strong>g of just attitud<strong>in</strong>al items,whereas Schmitt’s research used the full SOI. Apropos ofthis possibility, one reason why men consistently showedgreater variability <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality than women did <strong>in</strong>Schmitt’s study (see commentary by Eagly & Wood, 2005,p. 283) may be that the behavioral items <strong>in</strong> the SOI acted to<strong>in</strong>flate male variability, because of the extreme behavioralresponses of a relatively small subset of men. A secondreason for stronger results <strong>in</strong> the current study is that Schmitt’snational samples may have been, on average, smaller<strong>and</strong> more restricted than the samples obta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> the currentresearch (see below).There was one f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the current study that proved tobe weaker than the correspond<strong>in</strong>g f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Schmitt’sreport—namely, the association between national sex ratios<strong>and</strong> sociosexuality. Table 3 showed that correlations betweensex ratios <strong>and</strong> men’s sociosexuality (r = –.25) <strong>and</strong>between sex ratios <strong>and</strong> women’s sociosexuality (r = –.27)were only marg<strong>in</strong>ally significant. When I computed theaverage of men’s <strong>and</strong> women’s sociosexuality for eachnation (a measure of overall national sociosexuality levels),this average, unlike its components, did correlate significantlywith national sex ratios, r(52) = –.27, p < .05.However, the correspond<strong>in</strong>g correlation from Schmitt’sstudy was larger, r(46) = –.45, p < .001. As others havenoted, local sex ratios are likely to be more importantdeterm<strong>in</strong>ants of men’s <strong>and</strong> women’s mat<strong>in</strong>g strategies thannational sex ratios (Bond, 2005), <strong>and</strong> this is particularlytrue for geographically large <strong>and</strong> diverse nations. Given thecrudeness of the sex ratio measures used <strong>in</strong> both Schmitt’sstudy <strong>and</strong> the current one, it is remarkable that there wereany significant correlations between sex ratios <strong>and</strong> sociosexuality.To the extent that these correlations were significant,they tended to support the predictions of sex ratiotheory (Guttentag & Secord, 1983; Pedersen, 1991), that123


Arch <strong>Sex</strong> Behavhigh sex ratios (more men than women) are associated withreduced levels of sociosexuality, because <strong>in</strong> societies withsurplus males the sexual ‘‘marketplace’’ is driven more bywomen’s than by men’s desires.The current results extended Schmitt’s f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs onsociosexuality by <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g the impact of nations’dom<strong>in</strong>ant religion on sex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality (<strong>and</strong>on sex drive as well). The results <strong>in</strong>dicated that religioncovaried with national sex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality, butnot with sex differences <strong>in</strong> sex drive. In general, nations withmore conservative <strong>and</strong> collectivist religions tended to showlarger sex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality. These differencesdisappeared, however, when UN gender equality was enteredas a covariate, <strong>and</strong> this provided at least circumstantialevidence that religion had its impact via the mediat<strong>in</strong>g routeof gender ideologies <strong>and</strong> social structure.Limitations <strong>and</strong> Strengths of the BBC DataThe BBC data had a number of limitations that are worthnot<strong>in</strong>g. For one, they were not drawn from a probabilitysample. As described <strong>in</strong> the method section, BBC Internetsurvey participants were self-selected—they tended to berelatively young, well educated, affluent, <strong>and</strong> Internet savvy.Thus, on average, BBC participants may have representedtheir nations’ educated elites. Furthermore, the BBC surveywas adm<strong>in</strong>istered <strong>in</strong> English, <strong>and</strong> this undoubtedly restrictedwho could participate <strong>in</strong> the survey, particularly <strong>in</strong> non-English-speak<strong>in</strong>g countries. As Schmitt (2005) has observed,it is difficult to obta<strong>in</strong> large, representative crossculturalsamples. A limitation of the BBC sex drive <strong>and</strong>sociosexuality measures was their brevity, which may haveresulted <strong>in</strong> reduced reliability. F<strong>in</strong>ally, the BBC data werecollected by an Internet survey, <strong>and</strong> such data may be subjectto unique k<strong>in</strong>ds of error variance (see Gosl<strong>in</strong>g, Vazire,Srivastava, & John, 2004).The BBC data also had strengths. One was the size <strong>and</strong>diversity of the BBC sample. It is virtually unheard of forresearchers to have access to self-report data on sex drive<strong>and</strong> sociosexuality from an <strong>in</strong>ternational sample compris<strong>in</strong>gover 200,000 people. The only study comparable <strong>in</strong>scope to the current one was Schmitt’s (2005) ambitiousanalysis of International <strong>Sex</strong>uality Description Project data.In a number of ways, however, the BBC data are likelysuperior <strong>in</strong> quality to ISDP data. Schmitt’s data came largelyfrom college students, who were young <strong>and</strong> often<strong>in</strong>experienced with long-term relationships. In contrast, themajority of BBC participants were not college students; theaverage age of BBC participants ensured that most BBCparticipants were older than the typical college student <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong> their prime reproductive years. Furthermore, becausemost BBC participants were either married or <strong>in</strong> seriousrelationships, they likely responded to sexuality-relatedquestions based on more extensive <strong>and</strong> varied real-lifeexperience than the average college student would. The factthat the BBC data were collected via an Internet surveyvirtually guaranteed that samples from various nations weremore geographically diverse, with<strong>in</strong> each nation, than Schmitt’ssamples, which often consisted of college studentssurveyed at particular universities. F<strong>in</strong>ally, many of the nationalsamples <strong>in</strong> the BBC dataset were larger than correspond<strong>in</strong>gsamples <strong>in</strong> Schmitt’s study. All of these factorstaken together suggest that parameter estimates <strong>in</strong> the currentresearch (e.g., estimates of national means <strong>and</strong> national SDs<strong>in</strong> sociosexuality) may have been more reliable <strong>and</strong> morerepresentative of their respective nations than correspond<strong>in</strong>gestimates from Schmitt’s study.Sett<strong>in</strong>g a More Dem<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g St<strong>and</strong>ard of Predictionfor Both Evolutionary <strong>and</strong> Social Structural TheoriesSpecific results <strong>in</strong> this article are likely to please both socialstructural <strong>and</strong> evolutionary theorists. Social structural theorywas successful <strong>in</strong> predict<strong>in</strong>g that sex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexualitywould be moderated by societies’ levels of genderequality <strong>and</strong> economic development. Consistent with socialstructural theory, societies with higher levels of genderequality <strong>and</strong> economic development tended to display weakersex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality. Furthermore, <strong>across</strong> nations,gender egalitarianism was associated with higher levels ofsociosexuality <strong>in</strong> both men <strong>and</strong> women, though the associationwas stronger for women than for men. The greater associationbetween ‘‘culture’’ <strong>and</strong> female sociosexuality provides newevidence support<strong>in</strong>g Baumeister’s (2000) claim that women’ssexuality is, <strong>in</strong> some ways, more <strong>in</strong>fluenced than men’s bysocial <strong>and</strong> cultural factors.In its most strongly stated form, Eagly <strong>and</strong> Wood’s(1999, 2005) social structural theory proposes that sex differences<strong>in</strong> psychological traits <strong>and</strong> behaviors do not at allresult from evolved psychological dispositions <strong>in</strong> men <strong>and</strong>women. Rather, they result from the social roles that haveevolved <strong>in</strong> some societies—particularly modern agricultural<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>dustrial societies—as a result of physical differencesbetween the sexes (e.g., greater male upper bodystrength, female gestation <strong>and</strong> lactation), <strong>and</strong> the economicdivisions of labor <strong>and</strong> social structures that followed fromthese physical differences. Thus, accord<strong>in</strong>g to Eagly <strong>and</strong>Wood’s theory, the proximate causes of sex differences aresocial structure (e.g., greater male than female power, patriarchy),social roles (men as workers, women as housekeepers),<strong>and</strong> the gender ideologies that accompany <strong>and</strong>susta<strong>in</strong> these patriarchal social structures <strong>and</strong> social roles.The current results offer at best partial support for thisstrong version of social structural theory, <strong>and</strong> this partial123


Arch <strong>Sex</strong> Behavsupport was present for only one trait—sociosexuality. Theresults offered no support for social structural theory <strong>in</strong>relation to sex differences <strong>in</strong> sex drive. Even <strong>in</strong> the case ofsociosexuality, it is important to note that regressionanalyses of male <strong>and</strong> female national means <strong>in</strong>dicated thatsex differences were much more powerful than culturema<strong>in</strong> effects or sex-by-culture <strong>in</strong>teractions. Although sexdifferences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality could plausibly result fromboth biological <strong>and</strong> cultural factors, it is important to notethat the ‘‘ma<strong>in</strong> effect’’ of gender equality on men’s <strong>and</strong>women’s levels of sociosexuality (b 2 = .17), was muchstronger than the <strong>in</strong>teraction between gender equality <strong>and</strong>sex (b 2 = .02). In other words, both men’s <strong>and</strong> women’smean sociosexuality levels tended to covary <strong>in</strong> a parallelfashion with nations’ levels of gender equality, <strong>and</strong> genderequality did not strongly moderate sex differences <strong>in</strong>sociosexuality. The overall pattern of results for sociosexuality,i.e., the cross-cultural patterns of sex differences,male <strong>and</strong> female means, <strong>and</strong> male <strong>and</strong> female SDs, seemsmost consistent with the hybrid model portrayed <strong>in</strong> Fig. 4.Although social role theory has not much addressed theissue of with<strong>in</strong>-sex variation, it seems to imply that societiescharacterized by strong gender roles <strong>and</strong> nonegalitariangender ideologies will show less with<strong>in</strong>-sex variation<strong>in</strong> gender-l<strong>in</strong>ked traits <strong>and</strong> behaviors (e.g., occupationalchoices) than societies characterized by weak gender roles<strong>and</strong> egalitarian gender ideologies (Lippa, 2005; see thepure social structural model presented <strong>in</strong> Fig. 1). In thecurrent results, this prediction held for women’s sociosexuality,but it did not hold for men’s sociosexuality, nordid it hold for women’s or men’s sex drive. Aga<strong>in</strong>, thisprovides at best partial support for social structural theory.Furthermore, social role theory does not offer an obviousexplanation for the <strong>in</strong>teraction between gender equality <strong>and</strong>men’s <strong>and</strong> women’s SDs <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality. This <strong>in</strong>teractioncan, however, be readily expla<strong>in</strong>ed by assum<strong>in</strong>g that biologicalpredispositions <strong>in</strong>teract with cultural ‘‘presses.’’A major weakness of social structural theory is that itoften seems to make a generic ‘‘strong gender roles lead tolarge sex differences’’ prediction for psychological traitsthat show sex differences. Although this prediction appearsto be true for sex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality, it does notappear to be true for sex differences <strong>in</strong> other traits. Forexample, the strength of societal gender roles is unrelatedto observed sex differences <strong>in</strong> the value assigned to amate’s physical attractiveness (Eagly & Wood, 1999;Lippa, 2007), <strong>and</strong> it is unrelated to sex differences <strong>in</strong> themean levels <strong>and</strong> variability of sex drive (as reported here).Also contrary to social role’s generic hypothesis, stronggender roles are associated with smaller sex differences <strong>in</strong>the value assigned to a mate’s niceness (Lippa, 2007), <strong>and</strong>strong versus weak gender roles are associated with differences<strong>in</strong> the direction of sex differences <strong>in</strong> valu<strong>in</strong>g amate’s <strong>in</strong>telligence (Lippa, 2007). To demonstrate trueexplanatory power, social structural theory must accountfor such variations <strong>in</strong> the relation between social roles <strong>and</strong>sex differences, <strong>and</strong> ideally it should predict, not postdict,such variations. In addition, social role theory must predictthe magnitudes as well as direction of sex differences forvarious traits.The current results present explanatory challenges toevolutionary theories as well. Mean levels of sociosexuality<strong>and</strong> national SDs <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality, particularly mean levels<strong>and</strong> SDs for women, were strongly associated with societalmeasures of gender equality <strong>and</strong> economic development<strong>and</strong>, as a result, sex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality were alsostrongly associated with gender equality <strong>and</strong> economicdevelopment. In comment<strong>in</strong>g upon similar f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>Schmitt’s (2005) study, Buss (2005) wrote that ‘‘…althoughthe modest cultural variation <strong>in</strong> the magnitude of sex differences<strong>in</strong> the SOI is theoretically important, I suggest thatit is not adequately expla<strong>in</strong>ed by nebulous theoretical constructssuch a structural powerlessness, gender empowerment,patriarchy, or social structural roles…’’ (p. 279). Bussproposed <strong>in</strong>stead that ‘‘…both men <strong>and</strong> women possess anevolved menu of mat<strong>in</strong>g strategies, selectively activated byparticular features of the personal, social, <strong>and</strong> ecologicalcontext…’’ (p. 279). In relation to the current results, thetask that confronts theorists like Buss is to expla<strong>in</strong> whymen’s <strong>and</strong> women’s sociosexuality covaried with featuresof the social <strong>and</strong> ecological context, whereas their sex drivedid not. Furthermore, evolutionary theories must expla<strong>in</strong>why gender equality <strong>and</strong> gender empowerment measurescovaried so strongly with sex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality,but not at all with sex differences <strong>in</strong> sex drive. Like socialstructural theories, evolutionary theories should predict, notpostdic, such f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs.Sett<strong>in</strong>g the Stage for More Complex <strong>and</strong> InteractiveTheoriesBecause the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs reported here for sex drive <strong>and</strong>sociosexuality differed so strongly, we now know that noneof the models presented at the start of this article is necessarilyor uniquely true. The future task fac<strong>in</strong>g those whostudy psychological sex differences is to do the hardempirical work of test<strong>in</strong>g various models, for various traits,with hard-to-get cross-cultural data. I predict that empiricalreality will prove to be sufficiently complex so as to confoundmany theorists’ unitary notions about the etiology<strong>and</strong> cross-cultural consistency of sex differences. UnlikeSchmitt (2005), who expressed dissatisfaction with thepossibility that several different theoretical perspectivesmight be necessary to expla<strong>in</strong> sex differences <strong>in</strong> sociosexuality,I am not disappo<strong>in</strong>ted by this prospect. The news thatemerges from both Schmitt’s study <strong>and</strong> the current one—-123


Arch <strong>Sex</strong> Behavnews that should not have been totally unexpected—is thatsex differences, like the rest of human behavior, are complex<strong>and</strong> multifaceted. My hope is that the various modelspresented <strong>in</strong> this article <strong>and</strong> the accompany<strong>in</strong>g methodsproposed to test these models will provide a more systematicmeans to probe the many different ways <strong>in</strong> whichbiological <strong>and</strong> social factors, separately <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ation,come together to <strong>in</strong>fluence sex differences <strong>in</strong> behavior.Acknowledgment I am grateful to BBC TV Science for commission<strong>in</strong>gthis research, <strong>and</strong> to the BBC Science <strong>and</strong> Nature website forprogramm<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> host<strong>in</strong>g the study.ReferencesArcher, J., & Mehdikhani, M. (2003). Variability among males <strong>in</strong>sexually selected attributes. Review of General Psychology, 7,219–236.Baugh, F. (2002). Correct<strong>in</strong>g effect sizes for score reliability: Arem<strong>in</strong>der that measurement <strong>and</strong> substantive issues are l<strong>in</strong>ked<strong>in</strong>extricably. Educational <strong>and</strong> Psychological Measurement, 62,254–263.Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Gender differences <strong>in</strong> erotic plasticity: Thefemale sex drive as socially flexible <strong>and</strong> responsive. PsychologicalBullet<strong>in</strong>, 126, 347–374Baumeister, R. F., Catanese, K. R., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Are theregender differences <strong>in</strong> strength of sex drive? Theoretical views,conceptual dist<strong>in</strong>ctions, <strong>and</strong> a review of relevant evidence. Personality<strong>and</strong> Social Psychology Review, 5, 242–273.Bobko, P., Roth, P. L., & Bobko, C. (2001). Correct<strong>in</strong>g the effect sizeof d for range restriction <strong>and</strong> unreliability. OrganizationalResearch Methods, 4, 46–61.Bond, N. W. (2005). Who’s zoom<strong>in</strong>g who? Behavioral <strong>and</strong> Bra<strong>in</strong>Sciences, 28, 278.Buss, D. M. (1989). <strong>Sex</strong> differences <strong>in</strong> human mate preferences:Evolutionary hypotheses tested <strong>in</strong> 37 cultures. Behavioral <strong>and</strong>Bra<strong>in</strong> Sciences, 12, 1–49.Buss, D. M. (2005). <strong>Sex</strong> differences <strong>in</strong> the design features of sociallycont<strong>in</strong>gent mat<strong>in</strong>g adaptations. Behavioral <strong>and</strong> Bra<strong>in</strong> Sciences,28, 278–279.Deary, I. J., Thorpe, G., Wilson, V., Starr, J. M., & Whalley, L. J.(2003). Population sex differences <strong>in</strong> IQ at age 11: The Scottishmental survey 1932. Intelligence, 31, <strong>53</strong>3–542.Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (1999). The orig<strong>in</strong>s of sex differences <strong>in</strong>human behavior: Evolved dispositions versus social roles.American Psychologist, 54, 408–423.Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (2005). Universal sex differences <strong>across</strong>patriarchal cultures „ evolved psychological dispositions. Behavioral<strong>and</strong> Bra<strong>in</strong> Sciences, 28, 281–283.Gaul<strong>in</strong>, S. J. C., & Boster, J. (1985). Cross-cultural differences <strong>in</strong>sexual dimorphism: Is there any variance to be expla<strong>in</strong>ed?Ethology <strong>and</strong> Sociobiology, 6, 219–225.Gangestad, S. W., & Simpson, J. A. (2000). The evolution of humanmat<strong>in</strong>g: Trade-offs <strong>and</strong> strategic pluralism. Behavioral <strong>and</strong> Bra<strong>in</strong>Sciences, 23, 573–644.Gosl<strong>in</strong>g, S. D., Vazire, S., Srivastava, S., & John, O. P. (2004).Should we trust web-based studies? A comparative analysis ofsix preconceptions about Internet questionnaires. AmericanPsychologist, 59, 93–104.Guttentag, M., & Secord, P. (1983). Too many women? Beverly Hills,CA: Sage.Lazarus, J. (2002). Human sex ratios: Adaptations <strong>and</strong> mechanisms,problems <strong>and</strong> prospects. In I. C. Hardy (Ed.), <strong>Sex</strong> ratios:Concepts <strong>and</strong> research methods (pp. 287–311). Cambridge:Cambridge University Press.Lippa, R. A. (2005). Gender, nature, <strong>and</strong> nurture (2nd ed.). Mahwah,NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Lippa, R. A. (2006). Is high sex drive associated with <strong>in</strong>creasedsexual attraction to both sexes? It depends on whether you aremale or female. Psychological Science, 17, 46–52.Lippa, R. A. (2007). The preferred traits of mates <strong>in</strong> a cross-nationalstudy of heterosexual <strong>and</strong> homosexual men <strong>and</strong> women: Anexam<strong>in</strong>ation of biological <strong>and</strong> cultural <strong>in</strong>fluences. Archives of<strong>Sex</strong>ual Behavior, 36, 193–208.Ostovich, J. M., & Sab<strong>in</strong>i, J. (2004). How are sociosexuality, sexdrive, <strong>and</strong> lifetime number of sexual partners related? Personality<strong>and</strong> Social Psychology Bullet<strong>in</strong>, 30, 255–266.Paoletti, J. B., & Kregloh, C. (1989). The children’s department. In C.B. Kidwell & V. Steele (Eds.), Men <strong>and</strong> women: Dress<strong>in</strong>g thepart (pp. 22–41). Wash<strong>in</strong>gton, DC: Smithsonian Institution Press.Pedersen, F. A. (1991). Secular trends <strong>in</strong> human sex ratios: Their<strong>in</strong>fluence on <strong>in</strong>dividual <strong>and</strong> family behavior. Human Nature, 2,271–291.Reimers, S. (2007). The BBC <strong>in</strong>ternet study: General methodology.Archives of <strong>Sex</strong>ual Behavior, 36, 147–161.Schmitt, D. P. (2005). <strong>Sociosexuality</strong> from Argent<strong>in</strong>a to Zimbabwe: A48-nation study of sex, culture, <strong>and</strong> strategies of human mat<strong>in</strong>g.Behavioral <strong>and</strong> Bra<strong>in</strong> Sciences, 28, 247–311.Simpson, J. A., & Gangestad, S. W. (1991). Individual differences <strong>in</strong>sociosexuality: Evidence for convergent <strong>and</strong> discrim<strong>in</strong>ant validity.Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 60, 870–888.Spector, I. P., Carey, M. P., & Ste<strong>in</strong>berg, L. (1996). The <strong>Sex</strong>ualDesire Inventory: Development, factor structure, <strong>and</strong> evidence ofreliability. Journal of <strong>Sex</strong> <strong>and</strong> Marital Therapy, 22, 175–190.Voracek, M. (2005). Shortcom<strong>in</strong>gs of the Sociosexual OrientationInventory: Can psychometrics <strong>in</strong>form evolutionary psychology?Behavioral <strong>and</strong> Bra<strong>in</strong> Sciences, 28, 296–297.Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (2002). A cross-cultural analysis of thebehavior of men <strong>and</strong> women: Implications for the orig<strong>in</strong>s of sexdifferences. Psychological Bullet<strong>in</strong>, 128, 699–727.Zucker, K. J. (2005). Measurement of psychosexual differentiation.Archives of <strong>Sex</strong>ual Behavior, 34, 375–388.123

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!