11.07.2015 Views

The role of digital video media in second language listening ...

The role of digital video media in second language listening ...

The role of digital video media in second language listening ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

ather than visual conventions” (p. 11) and emphasised that the spoken word, notvisual images, should rema<strong>in</strong> paramount <strong>in</strong> the <strong>language</strong> classroom.First <strong>language</strong> educators have also struggled with def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the act <strong>of</strong> attend<strong>in</strong>g toimages. ‘Visual literacy’, or the ability to decode visual statements with<strong>in</strong> a specificcontext, was a term first co<strong>in</strong>ed by Dondis (1973). Although Dondis claimed thatvisual literacy skills would become “one <strong>of</strong> the fundamental measures <strong>of</strong> education<strong>in</strong> the last third <strong>of</strong> our century” (Dondis, 1973, p. 19), further attempts to def<strong>in</strong>e theterm have been unsatisfy<strong>in</strong>g and rema<strong>in</strong> vague (Seels, 1994). Although researchconcerned with visual literacy is matur<strong>in</strong>g (cf., Moore & Dwyer, 1994), Kress andvan Leeuwen (1996) po<strong>in</strong>t out the central difficulties that have retarded itsdevelopment so far: “<strong>The</strong> problem we face is that literate cultures havesystematically suppressed means <strong>of</strong> analysis <strong>of</strong> the visual forms <strong>of</strong> representation,so that there is not, at the moment, an established theoretical framework with<strong>in</strong> whichvisual forms <strong>of</strong> representation can be discussed” (p. 8).As noted by Kellerman (1992), <strong>of</strong> course, the area <strong>of</strong> <strong>second</strong> <strong>language</strong> listen<strong>in</strong>g alsolacks a basis on which to discuss visual forms <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>put. Although Rost (1990), forexample, writes that determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>tentions <strong>of</strong> a speaker “depends on the<strong>in</strong>terplay <strong>of</strong> verbal, vocal and visual features” (p. 79), he does not develop thisobservation much further. For Rost (1990), visual and gestural clues areconceptualised as ‘edit<strong>in</strong>g strategies’ which help listeners <strong>in</strong>terpret discourse <strong>in</strong> thatthey provide “weak <strong>in</strong>ferences” (p. 79) regard<strong>in</strong>g a speaker’s <strong>in</strong>tentions.Other researchers po<strong>in</strong>t out the provision <strong>of</strong> context as a <strong>role</strong> for visuals. In anattempt to draft listen<strong>in</strong>g assessment bandscales, Br<strong>in</strong>dley and Nunan (1992) placed“the amount <strong>of</strong> nonverbal cues” (p. 6) amongst <strong>in</strong>put factors that relate to thecontextualisation <strong>of</strong> a listen<strong>in</strong>g event. Dunkel and colleagues (1993, p. 180) havecharacterised the <strong>role</strong> <strong>of</strong> dynamic visual <strong>media</strong> <strong>in</strong> listen<strong>in</strong>g assessment as ‘<strong>video</strong>support’ that make up one <strong>of</strong> several ‘levell<strong>in</strong>g variables’ likely to affect thedemonstration <strong>of</strong> listen<strong>in</strong>g abilities. In discussions <strong>of</strong> <strong>video</strong> <strong>in</strong> listen<strong>in</strong>g assessment10

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!