Abstracts - Deutsche Zoologische Gesellschaft

Abstracts - Deutsche Zoologische Gesellschaft Abstracts - Deutsche Zoologische Gesellschaft

www2.uni.jena.de
from www2.uni.jena.de More from this publisher
10.07.2015 Views

24 Behavioral Biology SymposiumO BB.3 (Su) - ENForaging behavior and habitat selection in pit-building antlionsInon ScharfDepartment of Life Sciences, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, IsraelPit-building antlion larvae are holometabolous insects, constructing conical pits in sandy soils,which serve as traps for small arthropods. In a series of experiments, I investigated the effects offeeding regime, predator presence, sand depth, light conditions and temperature on foraging behaviorand habitat selection in antlions. As prey item increased in size antlions showed an increase of pitdimensions followed by a decrease, indicating that they consider both their satiation level and the expectedrate of prey arrivals. Antlions also reduce pit construction rate when exposed to predators. Incomparison to a sit-and-pursue antlion, which does not construct pits, I found that the latter reducedrelocation activity, while the former reduced pit construction rate. Sand depth may affect antlions’ability to evade predators. In deeper sand, antlions built larger pits and showed reduced relocationrate, emphasizing the importance of the habitat’s physical characteristics. Exposing the antlionsto constant light increased their tendency to construct pits, which were also larger than those constructedunder complete darkness. Finally, antlions demonstrated a context-dependent response totemperatures: At low temperatures they preferred illuminated microhabitats, while otherwise shadedmicrohabitats were chosen. In conclusion, antlions use a flexible and context-dependent foragingbehavior with respect to both their biotic and abiotic environments.O BB.4 (Su) - ENBig ears for bats: absolute size matters for foraging efficiencyBjörn M. Siemers 1 , Michael Stauss 2 , Hendrik Turni 31Sensory Ecology Group, Max Planck Institute for Ornithology, Seewiesen; 2 Institut für Verhaltensökologie,Universität Tübingen; 3 Museum für Naturkunde der Humboldt-Universität, BerlinMany predators find their food by listening for prey sounds. Bats do so when hunting in cluttered environmentswhere substrate echoes overlap and mask echoes from the prey. Within the largest genusof bats, Myotis, this strategy of detecting prey in clutter by listening for their sounds evolved severaltimes convergently. All of these “passive listening” specialists have large ears. In a comparativestudy comprising 31 Myotis species, we found that the absolute ear size of all six included “passivelistening” species was quite similar, irrespective of their body size. I.e, smaller “passive listening”species have ears much larger than predicted by the genus regression line for scaling of ear size withbody size whereas larger species do not. This suggests that a certain absolute ear size is requiredand sufficient for efficient detection and localization of rustling arthropods. To test the influence ofear size on the received sound amplitude, we measured idealized bat ears below, at and above thesize found in “passive listening” Myotis. Indeed, the typical ear size of “passive listening” speciesresulted in an increased sensitivity for the sonic frequencies that dominate arthropod rustling sounds.We conclude that absolute ear size matters to efficiently pick up and localize these sounds. Smallerspecies that specialized for “passive listening” foraging behaviour thus had to evolve very large earsrelative to their body size.

Behavioral Biology Symposium 25O BB.5 (Su) - ENWhy do shrews call?Sophie von Merten 1 , Grit Schauermann 2 , Hendrik Turni 3 , Björn M. Siemers 11Sensory Ecology Group, Max Planck Insitute for Ornithology, Seewiesen; 2 Animal Physiology,University of Tübingen; 3 Museum für Naturkunde der Humboldt-Universität, BerlinShrews are very vocal animals. While some vocalisations serve interspecific communication, it isnot yet clear why they produce twittering calls during solitary exploration. One hypothesis is the useof a simple echolocation-like system for orientation. Shrews mainly forage in cluttered substrate.Thus, they could use echoes of their calls to assess the habitat structure at close range, but beyond therange of their vibrissae. A series of classical studies provide equivocal evidence for echolocation inshrews. To complement these data, we used a novel behavioural and acoustic approach on two shrewspecies, representative for the two large shrew subfamilies. In both species, experimental manipulationof substrate density affected the number of twittering calls uttered during exploration. Thisadaptation of call rate to habitat clutterdness parallels bat echolocation and suggests that shrews usereverberations of their calls for probing habitat routing or type. The shrew signals were tonal, larynxproduced calls in the sonic range and not ‘echolocation clicks’, as had been suggested by other studies.To test the utility of those calls for orientation, we insonified different natural shrew habitats withan “artificial shrew” (small speaker mounted close to a sensitive microphone). We found that shrewlikecalls indeed yield echo scenes useful for habitat assessment, routing and spatial orientation.O BB.6 (Su) - ENFunctions of complex signalling in Nightingales (Luscinia megarhynchos)Silke KipperAG Verhaltensbiologie, Freie Universität BerlinMany animal species evolved complex communication systems consisting of different signals thatcan be assembled in various sequences and combinations. Birdsong is one of the most prominent andwell-understood examples to study functions of such signalling systems. Hereby, the complexity ofsong in some bird species has been particularly puzzling. I will present recent findings that elucidatefunctions of song type repertoires in the common nightingale (Luscinia megarhynchos). Males of thespecies possess large song type repertoires (mean 190 songs/male) with remarkable inter-individualdifferences in repertoire size and repertoire composition. To investigate repertoire size, I analysedlong sequences of undisturbed nocturnal song bouts of 12 free ranging territorial nightingales resultingin repertoire curves reaching towards saturation (indicating that not many more new song typeswere going to occur). I will show that, owing to specific song delivery rules, a much shorter songsequence is sufficient to reliably predict a bird’s repertoire size. In addition, to address the role ofrepertoire composition, I conducted playback experiments testing reactions to songs individual birdsdid or did not have in their repertoires. Results show that nightingales differed between these twosong qualities, suggesting that the development of a large repertoire is not only a matter of producinghigh versatility, but also allows fine-tuned interactions among males.

Behavioral Biology Symposium 25O BB.5 (Su) - ENWhy do shrews call?Sophie von Merten 1 , Grit Schauermann 2 , Hendrik Turni 3 , Björn M. Siemers 11Sensory Ecology Group, Max Planck Insitute for Ornithology, Seewiesen; 2 Animal Physiology,University of Tübingen; 3 Museum für Naturkunde der Humboldt-Universität, BerlinShrews are very vocal animals. While some vocalisations serve interspecific communication, it isnot yet clear why they produce twittering calls during solitary exploration. One hypothesis is the useof a simple echolocation-like system for orientation. Shrews mainly forage in cluttered substrate.Thus, they could use echoes of their calls to assess the habitat structure at close range, but beyond therange of their vibrissae. A series of classical studies provide equivocal evidence for echolocation inshrews. To complement these data, we used a novel behavioural and acoustic approach on two shrewspecies, representative for the two large shrew subfamilies. In both species, experimental manipulationof substrate density affected the number of twittering calls uttered during exploration. Thisadaptation of call rate to habitat clutterdness parallels bat echolocation and suggests that shrews usereverberations of their calls for probing habitat routing or type. The shrew signals were tonal, larynxproduced calls in the sonic range and not ‘echolocation clicks’, as had been suggested by other studies.To test the utility of those calls for orientation, we insonified different natural shrew habitats withan “artificial shrew” (small speaker mounted close to a sensitive microphone). We found that shrewlikecalls indeed yield echo scenes useful for habitat assessment, routing and spatial orientation.O BB.6 (Su) - ENFunctions of complex signalling in Nightingales (Luscinia megarhynchos)Silke KipperAG Verhaltensbiologie, Freie Universität BerlinMany animal species evolved complex communication systems consisting of different signals thatcan be assembled in various sequences and combinations. Birdsong is one of the most prominent andwell-understood examples to study functions of such signalling systems. Hereby, the complexity ofsong in some bird species has been particularly puzzling. I will present recent findings that elucidatefunctions of song type repertoires in the common nightingale (Luscinia megarhynchos). Males of thespecies possess large song type repertoires (mean 190 songs/male) with remarkable inter-individualdifferences in repertoire size and repertoire composition. To investigate repertoire size, I analysedlong sequences of undisturbed nocturnal song bouts of 12 free ranging territorial nightingales resultingin repertoire curves reaching towards saturation (indicating that not many more new song typeswere going to occur). I will show that, owing to specific song delivery rules, a much shorter songsequence is sufficient to reliably predict a bird’s repertoire size. In addition, to address the role ofrepertoire composition, I conducted playback experiments testing reactions to songs individual birdsdid or did not have in their repertoires. Results show that nightingales differed between these twosong qualities, suggesting that the development of a large repertoire is not only a matter of producinghigh versatility, but also allows fine-tuned interactions among males.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!