10.07.2015 Views

who asked the first question? - International Research Center For ...

who asked the first question? - International Research Center For ...

who asked the first question? - International Research Center For ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

301with all <strong>the</strong>ir communicative skills that <strong>the</strong>y are extremely happy that <strong>the</strong>ir master hasfinally made <strong>the</strong> best decision of <strong>the</strong> day. This is ano<strong>the</strong>r well-known outcome ofevolutionary "asynchrony" of <strong>the</strong> origins of sound perception and voice production: manyanimals (e.g., cats, dogs, horses, apes) can distinguish spoken signals and react to <strong>the</strong>m,but <strong>the</strong>y cannot produce articulated sounds <strong>the</strong>mselves. Kanzi, a bonobo, a smaller andmuch more peaceful relative of <strong>the</strong> chimpanzee <strong>who</strong> puzzled many defenders of humanuniqueness, successfully proved that he can react not only to simple speech signals (like“go” or “give me…”), but can understand and react adequately to very complex newsentences like “Give <strong>the</strong> doggie some carrots”, or “Put <strong>the</strong> toy gorilla on <strong>the</strong> pot” and“can you throw <strong>the</strong> dog to Kelly?”(Savage-Rumbaugh et al., 1993:116). Panbanisha,Kanzi’s stepsister, proved that Kanzi was not a single genius among <strong>the</strong> bonobos. Alex,<strong>the</strong> grey parrot, proved that you do not have to be a relative of humans to comprehend atleast a few human words and even to use <strong>the</strong>m properly.Not unlike animals, humans can also learn to understand plenty of sounds madeby o<strong>the</strong>r species as well. This knowledge sometimes can be life saving. Everyone <strong>who</strong>has read Jim Corbett’s brilliantly written accounts of hunting man-eating tigers andleopards in <strong>the</strong> Himalayan foothills in India, will remember that his life often dependedon his ability to hear and correctly understand o<strong>the</strong>r animal’s calls (like cheetal andkakar), indicating <strong>the</strong> presence and location of a man-eater. Our distant ancestors, livingcloser to <strong>the</strong> nature, were definitely much better at “reading” animal sounds than we aretoday. Animals can also distinguish calls of o<strong>the</strong>r species and use <strong>the</strong> informationcontained in <strong>the</strong>se calls successfully in <strong>the</strong>ir everyday life without <strong>the</strong> ability to produce<strong>the</strong>se calls <strong>the</strong>mselves. Of course, animals that can copy o<strong>the</strong>r species calls can use thisability for <strong>the</strong>ir advantage, like <strong>the</strong> same Jim Corbett, <strong>who</strong> attracted his last man-eatingtigress by making a call of a tiger looking for a mate.Still, making vocal sounds for communication does not mean singing. “Althoughit is often stated that man is <strong>the</strong> only primate that can talk, it is rarely noted that he is also<strong>the</strong> only one that can sing” wrote Frank Livingstone ra<strong>the</strong>r controversially in his 1973article, arguing that <strong>the</strong> Australopi<strong>the</strong>cines mostly communicated by means of singing.Singing as a means of communication in our non-talking ancestors has been investigatedby o<strong>the</strong>r scholars as well (see, for example, Otto Jespersen, 1895; Brown & Greenhood,1991; Richman, 1993, Mi<strong>the</strong>n, 2005). In comparison with <strong>the</strong> ubiquitous vocalcommunication among animals, singing is a much more complex phenomenon. Anddespite this complexity, singing is still present in many unrelated species (like <strong>the</strong>humpback whale in <strong>the</strong> ocean, <strong>the</strong> flying nightingale and <strong>the</strong> gibbon from <strong>the</strong> jungletreetops (see Marler, 1970, 2000; Nottebohm, 1971, 1972; Bright, 1984; Geissmann,2000; Payne, 2000). Maybe <strong>the</strong> species closest to us that can “sing” is <strong>the</strong> gibbon. Theconspicuous absence of singing among <strong>the</strong> great African apes suggests that humansdeveloped singing abilities later, after separating from our closest living relatives aboutsome 6 million years ago. Thus, <strong>the</strong> singing of <strong>the</strong> geladas (Richman, 1976, 1978, 1987),gibbons and siamangs (Gittens, 1978; Geissmann, 2000), as well as birds (Marler, 1970,2000; Nottebohm, 1971), humpback whales (Payne, 2000) and humans are analogues, nothomologues. It is difficult not to agree with S. Pinker that “in <strong>the</strong> tree of life, traits likeeyes or hands or infinite vocalizations can arise on any branch, or several times ondifferent branches, some leading to humans, some not” (Pinker, 1997:189). It is also

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!