09.07.2015 Views

Practical Information - Generative Linguistics in the Old World

Practical Information - Generative Linguistics in the Old World

Practical Information - Generative Linguistics in the Old World

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Hierarchy and Recursion <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Bra<strong>in</strong>.Cristiano Chesi & Andrea MoroNe.T.S. - IUSS Center for Neurol<strong>in</strong>guistics and Theoretical Syntax, Pavia1. Syntax <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong>.Neuroimag<strong>in</strong>g techniques has offered <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g opportunities to deepen our understand<strong>in</strong>g of<strong>the</strong> relationship between syntax and <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> (Cappa 2012). Two issues appear to be wellestablished:first, syntactic computation activates a dedicated network (Embick et al. 2000,Moro et al. 2001); second, <strong>the</strong> format of rules cannot be traced to arbitrary, cultural orconventional facts but it reflects <strong>the</strong> neuropsychological architecture of <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> circuitry(Tettamanti et al. 2002, Musso et al. 2003, Tettamanti et al. 2008). In this paper we address aspecific issue that raises from <strong>the</strong>se studies on a computational perspective: <strong>the</strong> core result of<strong>the</strong> last three experiments mentioned is that <strong>the</strong> <strong>the</strong>oretical dist<strong>in</strong>ction between grammaticalvs. non-grammatical rules is reflected <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> activity. More specifically, <strong>the</strong> activity of (adeep component of) Broca’s area with<strong>in</strong> a more complex network <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g subcorticalelements such as <strong>the</strong> left nucleus caudatus appears to be sensitive to this dist<strong>in</strong>ction (structuredependentvs. position-dependent) as <strong>the</strong> BOLD signal is <strong>in</strong>creased <strong>in</strong> this area only when <strong>the</strong>subjects <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>the</strong>ir performance <strong>in</strong> manipulat<strong>in</strong>g grammatical (i.e. structure-dependent)rules. Here we want to discuss how this result relate to <strong>the</strong> nature of recursion and hierarchy<strong>in</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guistic process<strong>in</strong>g (Chomsky 1995, Berwick & Chomsky 2001).2. Disentangl<strong>in</strong>g hierarchy from recursion: a computational complexity perspectiveAlthough <strong>the</strong>re are subtle discrepancies look<strong>in</strong>g at reaction times, <strong>the</strong>re is surely completeconvergence with respect to performance: all subjects rapidly acquire <strong>the</strong> same capability tomanipulate both grammatical (e.g. passive construction, Musso et al. 2003) vs. nongrammaticalrules (article, Tettamanti et al. 2002, or negation, Musso et al. 2003, placement<strong>in</strong> a fixed-position; question formation by complete word-sequence <strong>in</strong>version, Musso et al.2003). This fact already constitutes a puzzle, s<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>the</strong> broad dist<strong>in</strong>ction between hierarchical(grammatical) vs. non-hierarchical (non-grammatical, e.g. sequential) rules correspond to adifferent degree of complexity: assum<strong>in</strong>g that each rule can be expressed as a set of(computational) states traversals, be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> number of states to be explored somehowproportional to <strong>the</strong> memory required to perform a certa<strong>in</strong> computation, hierarchical rules areless memory demand<strong>in</strong>g than sequential rules, s<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> vast majority of contexts,hierarchical rules can deal with lexical clusters ra<strong>the</strong>r than s<strong>in</strong>gle items, <strong>the</strong>n operat<strong>in</strong>g onlyon <strong>the</strong> relevant chunk(s) level. If <strong>the</strong> hierarchical rules are also recursive (e.g. X → aXb) <strong>the</strong>very same state can be re-used more times, <strong>in</strong>duc<strong>in</strong>g extra memory sav<strong>in</strong>g. Similarconsiderations on complexity also extend to non-hierarchical, non-recursive rules, that, <strong>in</strong> thissense are more “expensive”. To expla<strong>in</strong> this we must prelim<strong>in</strong>arily def<strong>in</strong>e, from acomputational perspective, <strong>the</strong> typology of (non-)recursive/(non-)hierarchical rules. Here weassume that <strong>the</strong> rules/computations are subsumed by different automata.3. Rank<strong>in</strong>g complexitiesThe (computational) complexity of a task is measured <strong>in</strong> terms of resources (memory andtime) used by a computation while attempt<strong>in</strong>g to complete that task. This def<strong>in</strong>ition ofcomplexity requires a precise formalization of <strong>the</strong> computation <strong>in</strong> order to understand <strong>the</strong>amount of resources used by <strong>the</strong> task we want to analyze. Assum<strong>in</strong>g that <strong>the</strong> rules arecomputed by a simple Push-Down Automata (i. e. a “PDA”, a F<strong>in</strong>ite State Automata endowedwith a Last In First Out memory buffer), we could characterize <strong>the</strong> rule typology as follows:

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!