09.07.2015 Views

Practical Information - Generative Linguistics in the Old World

Practical Information - Generative Linguistics in the Old World

Practical Information - Generative Linguistics in the Old World

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

occur (cf. figure 1). Similar performance was shown by <strong>the</strong> L1-German children, as illustrated<strong>in</strong> figure 2.(1) a. *Versteckt (= sie versteckt sich) '(She) hides herself'b. Kämt ihr Haar (= sie kämt ihr Haar) '(She) combs her hair'c. *Drehen (= sie dreht sich) '(It) turns'd. Die Tür geht zu (<strong>in</strong>stead of schließt sich) 'The door closes'e. *Das Eis schmelzt sich 'The ice-cream melts'Anticausatives I100% 100%German-TurkishGerman-Russian100%97% 100%95% 92%89% 72%Anticausatives IIReflexivesFigure 1: German-Turkish/Russian results Figure 2: L1-German resultsThe monol<strong>in</strong>guals and bil<strong>in</strong>guals showed <strong>the</strong> same performance <strong>in</strong> (a) omitt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> anaphorsich with reflexives and anticausatives II, (b) not overus<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> anaphor sich withanticausatives I and, (c) scor<strong>in</strong>g better with reflexives than anticausatives II. The po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> (a)-(c) suggest that <strong>the</strong> monol<strong>in</strong>guals and bil<strong>in</strong>guals follow <strong>the</strong> same path of development <strong>in</strong>acquir<strong>in</strong>g reflexives and anticausatives <strong>in</strong> German. We expla<strong>in</strong>ed this performance <strong>in</strong> terms ofcomplexity of syntactic computation (Alexiadou 2010; Jakubowicz & Nash 2001), for bothmonol<strong>in</strong>guals and bil<strong>in</strong>guals.However, although it is true that <strong>the</strong> bil<strong>in</strong>guals performed qualitatively similarly to<strong>the</strong>ir monol<strong>in</strong>gual peers, <strong>the</strong> former group performed quantitatively worse than <strong>the</strong> latter (cf.figures 1 and 2). Try<strong>in</strong>g to expla<strong>in</strong> this quantitative difference we first excluded languagetransfer from Turkish/Russian to German. If transfer would occur, different patterns ofproduction should occur due to typological/structural similarities/differences mentionedabove. We expla<strong>in</strong>ed this quantitative difference on <strong>the</strong> basis of <strong>in</strong>put. The tested children aresequential bil<strong>in</strong>guals who are exposed to German later than <strong>the</strong>ir L1 peers. Such age-of-onseteffects are often associated with restricted <strong>in</strong>put and, of course, less amounts of <strong>in</strong>put ascompared to <strong>the</strong>ir L1 peers (Nicoladis et al. 2012, and references <strong>the</strong>re<strong>in</strong>).In conclusion, our results suggest that <strong>the</strong> specific test-sett<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> our experiment, i.e.age of test<strong>in</strong>g (av. 4;0), age-of-onset (av. 2;6-2;8), time of exposure to German until <strong>the</strong>test<strong>in</strong>g time (av. 1;2-1;4 years) and complex structures such as Voice, which are shown to beacquired late <strong>in</strong> life anyway (Borer & Wexler 1987; cf. very high target-deviant scores also byL1s) are responsible for <strong>the</strong> quantitative difference between bil<strong>in</strong>guals and monol<strong>in</strong>guals,ra<strong>the</strong>r than typological/structural similarities/differences.Selected referencesAlexiadou, A. 2010. On <strong>the</strong> morpho-syntax of (anti-)causative verbs. In M. Rappaport Hovav,E. Doron & I. Sichel (eds.) Syntax, Lexical Semantics and Event Structure. OxfordUniversity Press, 177-203.Jakubowicz, C. and L. Nash. 2001. Functional Categories and Syntactic Operations <strong>in</strong>(Ab)normal Language Acquisition, Bra<strong>in</strong> and Language 77, 32l–339.Nicoladis, E., P. Marentette and J. Song. 2012. Do young bil<strong>in</strong>guals acquire past tensemorphology like monol<strong>in</strong>guals, only later? Evidence from French-English and Ch<strong>in</strong>ese-English bil<strong>in</strong>guals. Applied Psychol<strong>in</strong>guistics 33:3, DOI: 10.1017/S0142716411000439.Serratrice, L., A. Sorace, F. Filiaci and M. Baldo. 2012. Pronom<strong>in</strong>al objects <strong>in</strong> English-Italianand Spanish-Italian bil<strong>in</strong>gual children. Applied Psychol<strong>in</strong>guistics 33, 725-751.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!