09.07.2015 Views

Practical Information - Generative Linguistics in the Old World

Practical Information - Generative Linguistics in the Old World

Practical Information - Generative Linguistics in the Old World

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

(4) a. [Raza kogda Mašu i xvalili] ona k,*i nasčitala tri [ … ]time.PAUC when Masha.ACC praised she counted three‘She k,*i found three times when Masha i got praised.’b. [Raz kogda Mašu i xvalili] ona k,i nasčitala tri protime.PL when Masha.ACC praised she counted three‘As for times when Masha i got praised, she k,i counted three.’In contrast, only <strong>the</strong> base-generated non-match<strong>in</strong>g form allows an expletive (<strong>in</strong> red) at <strong>the</strong>post-numeral position because pronouns but not traces alternate with overt elements, (5).(5) a. Želanija u Peti bylo tri (*štuki)wish.PAUC by Petya was three piece.PAUCb. Želanij u Peti bylo tri (štuki)wish.PL by Petya was three piece.PAUC‘Wishes, Petya had three’.We conclude that <strong>the</strong> match<strong>in</strong>g form (2a) <strong>in</strong>volves A'-movement, a syntactic dependency, and<strong>the</strong> non-match<strong>in</strong>g form (2b) <strong>in</strong>volves coreference, a discourse dependency. The contrastreplicates <strong>the</strong> well-known dist<strong>in</strong>ction <strong>in</strong> Romance between topicalization and Hang<strong>in</strong>g TopicLeft Dislocation. The m<strong>in</strong>imal pair is an excellent candidate for a process<strong>in</strong>g study.The hierarchy <strong>in</strong> (1) predicts that (2a) should be processed more easily than (2b). Wetest this prediction <strong>in</strong> a read<strong>in</strong>g time experiment. The results show a strong effect of <strong>the</strong>number difference (p=0.0085), with a statistically significant slowdown <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g time <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong>region after <strong>the</strong> numeral <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> non-match<strong>in</strong>g case (discourse dependency) compared to <strong>the</strong>match<strong>in</strong>g case (syntactic dependency). The result supports Reuland’s hierarchy <strong>in</strong> (1) and isparticularly strik<strong>in</strong>g s<strong>in</strong>ce match<strong>in</strong>g topics are less frequent than non-match<strong>in</strong>g ones (5 tokensof (2a) vs. 12 tokens of (2b) over 1000 randomly selected sentences from <strong>the</strong> RussianNational Corpus, http://www.ruscorpora.ru/en/<strong>in</strong>dex.html).We reject an alternative explanation of <strong>the</strong> data <strong>in</strong> which <strong>the</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g time slow downis due to a simple morphological mismatch, which has been noted by several studies (e.gMol<strong>in</strong>aro et al. 2011). First, agreement mismatches <strong>in</strong> Russian numeral expressions do noto<strong>the</strong>rwise result <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g time slow down (Xiang et al. 2011). Second, native speakers rated<strong>the</strong> two constructions comparably; <strong>in</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r studies, agreement mismatch has yielded loweracceptability rat<strong>in</strong>gs (Fanselow and Frisch 2006).In conclusion, our analysis of a syntactic m<strong>in</strong>imal pair <strong>in</strong> Russian forms <strong>the</strong> basis of aprocess<strong>in</strong>g study compar<strong>in</strong>g a syntactic dependency to a discourse dependency. Ourexperimental <strong>in</strong>vestigation of <strong>the</strong> two constructions shows that syntactic dependencies areprocessed more quickly than discourse dependencies, provid<strong>in</strong>g novel support for <strong>the</strong>hierarchy <strong>in</strong> (1). From a process<strong>in</strong>g perspective, movement is less burdensome thanpronom<strong>in</strong>alization.ReferencesFanselow, G., and S. Frisch. 2006. Effects of process<strong>in</strong>g difficulty on judgments of acceptability. InG. Fanselow, et al. Gradience <strong>in</strong> grammar, 291-316. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Koornneef, A. 2008. Eye-catch<strong>in</strong>g anaphora. Utrecht: LOT International Dissertation Series.Mol<strong>in</strong>aro, N. et al. 2011. Grammatical agreement process<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g: ERP f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs and futuredirections. Cortex 47, 908-930.Re<strong>in</strong>hart, T. 1983. Anaphora and semantic <strong>in</strong>terpretation. London: Croom Helm.Reuland, E. 2011. Anaphora and language design. Cambridge, Ma.: MIT Press.Xiang, M. et al. 2011. Process<strong>in</strong>g morphological ambiguity: An experimental <strong>in</strong>vestigation of Russiannumerical phrases. L<strong>in</strong>gua 121, 548-560.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!