09.07.2015 Views

Practical Information - Generative Linguistics in the Old World

Practical Information - Generative Linguistics in the Old World

Practical Information - Generative Linguistics in the Old World

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Long-Distance Agreement <strong>in</strong> Icelandic revisited: An <strong>in</strong>terplay of locality and semanticsIvona Kučerová (McMaster University)We argue that <strong>in</strong>stances of long-distance agreement (LDA) with Nom<strong>in</strong>ative objects (NOM) <strong>in</strong> Icelandicare fully reducible to a strictly local operation of Agree with v act<strong>in</strong>g as a s<strong>in</strong>gle probe. Thistype of analysis has been refuted <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> past because of non-trivial <strong>in</strong>teractions with Dative <strong>in</strong>terveners(DAT) that seem to <strong>in</strong>volve <strong>in</strong>tricate comb<strong>in</strong>ations of -features (Holmberg and Hróarsdóttir,2003; Sigurðsson and Holmberg, 2008). We argue that <strong>the</strong> -feature-based characterization of <strong>the</strong>pattern is accidental and does not extend beyond a limited set of data. Instead, we propose that vsuccessfully probes NOM only if <strong>the</strong>re is no DAT <strong>in</strong>tervener with<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> prob<strong>in</strong>g doma<strong>in</strong> of v. Sucha configuration arises ei<strong>the</strong>r if <strong>the</strong>re is no DAT to start with, or if DAT underwent an <strong>in</strong>dependentlymotivated movement to Spec,vP. The empirical support for <strong>the</strong> analysis comes from data <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>gObject Shift and Quantifier movement, and from d-l<strong>in</strong>ked vs. non-d-l<strong>in</strong>ked wh-movement.Puzzle: While agreement with NOM is obligatory <strong>in</strong> a mono-clausal environment and no <strong>in</strong>terventioneffects are attested, (1-a), agreement <strong>in</strong> a bi-clausal environment is optional, (1-b), and can beblocked by an <strong>in</strong>terven<strong>in</strong>g DAT, (1-c), (Watanabe, 1993; Schütze, 1997):(1) a. það *var/voru konugi gefnar ambáttir í vettur.EXPL was.sg/were.pl k<strong>in</strong>g.Dat given slaves.Nom <strong>in</strong> w<strong>in</strong>ter‘A k<strong>in</strong>g was given female slaves <strong>in</strong> w<strong>in</strong>ter.’b. E<strong>in</strong>hverjum stúdent f<strong>in</strong>nst/f<strong>in</strong>nast tölvurnar ljótar.some student.Dat f<strong>in</strong>ds.sg/f<strong>in</strong>d.pl <strong>the</strong>-computers.Nom ugly.Nom‘Some student f<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>the</strong> computers ugly.’c. Það virðist/*virðast e<strong>in</strong>hverjum manni hestarnir vera se<strong>in</strong>ir.EXPL seems.sg/seem.pl some man.Dat <strong>the</strong>-horses.Nom be slow.Nom‘A man f<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>the</strong> horses slow.’Holmberg and Hróarsdóttir (2003) observed, however, that some DATs are transparent to agreement,(2). They proposed that DAT <strong>in</strong>tervenes only if <strong>the</strong> -features of <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>tervener and <strong>the</strong> goaldon’t match (3PL+3PL <strong>in</strong> (2) but 3PL+3SG <strong>in</strong> (1-c)).(2) a. Það f<strong>in</strong>nst mörgum stúdentum tölvurnar ljótar.EXPL f<strong>in</strong>ds.sg many students.Dat <strong>the</strong>-computers.Nom ugly.NOmb. Það f<strong>in</strong>nast mörgum stúdentum tölvurnar ljótar.EXPL f<strong>in</strong>d.pl many students.Dat <strong>the</strong>-computers.Nom ugly.Nom‘Many students f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>the</strong> computers ugly.’LDA can <strong>the</strong>n be formalized as parasitic on DAT (cf. Hiraiwa 2005). Such an analysis assumes nontrivialdifferences between local agreement and LDA which may yield parallel prob<strong>in</strong>g of featuresorig<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g on a s<strong>in</strong>gle head (Sigurðsson and Holmberg, 2008). Even though parallel prob<strong>in</strong>g andfeature valuation have been proposed for Reverse Agree (Adger, 2003; Baker, 2008; Haegemanand Lohndal, 2010; Wurmbrand, 2012, among o<strong>the</strong>rs), parallel valuation <strong>in</strong> Reverse Agree differ<strong>in</strong> its directionality. To our knowledge, <strong>the</strong> pattern proposed for Icelandic LDA is unprecedented.Proposal: As observed <strong>in</strong> Kučerová (2007), <strong>the</strong> -feature generalization does not extend to o<strong>the</strong>rDPs with <strong>the</strong> same -feature properties. She proposed that LDA obta<strong>in</strong>s only if DAT can <strong>in</strong>dependentlyundergo Object Shift (Holmberg, 1986) (OS) to Spec,vP. If OS takes place, v is free to probeNOM. S<strong>in</strong>ce adverbs don’t block OS (Holmberg, 1999), <strong>the</strong> correlation can be shown on <strong>the</strong> wordorder with respect to adverbs: if DAT precedes a VP adverb, i.e., it underwent OS, <strong>the</strong> f<strong>in</strong>ite verbmust agree with NOM . In contrast, if DAT follows such an adverb, agreement with NOM blocked.This pattern is entirely unexpected under Holmberg and Hróarsdóttir’s analysis.1

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!