09.07.2015 Views

Theories on Drug Abuse - Scottish Addiction Studies On-line Library

Theories on Drug Abuse - Scottish Addiction Studies On-line Library

Theories on Drug Abuse - Scottish Addiction Studies On-line Library

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

THEORIES ONDRUG ABUSESelected C<strong>on</strong>temporaryPerspectivesEditors:DAN J. LETTIERI, Ph.D.MOLLIE SAYERSHELEN WALLENSTEIN PEARSONNIDA Research M<strong>on</strong>ograph 30March 1980DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH AND HUMAN SERVICESPublic Health ServiceAlcohol, <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>, and Mental Health Administrati<strong>on</strong>Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>Divisi<strong>on</strong> of Research5600 Fishers LaneRockville, Maryland 20857For sale by the Superintendent of Documents, U.S. Government Printing OfficeWashingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C. 20402


The NIDA Research M<strong>on</strong>ograph series is prepared by the Divisi<strong>on</strong> of Research ofthe Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>. Its primary objective is to provide critical reviewsof research problem areas and techniques, the c<strong>on</strong>tent of state-of-the-artc<strong>on</strong>ferences, integrative research reviews and significant original research. Itsdual publicati<strong>on</strong> emphasis is rapid and targeted disseminati<strong>on</strong> to the scientificand professi<strong>on</strong>al community.Editorial Advisory BoardAvram Goldstein, M.D.Addicti<strong>on</strong> Research Foundati<strong>on</strong>Palo Alto, CaliforniaJerome Jaffe, M.D.College of Physicians and Surge<strong>on</strong>sColumbia University, New YorkReese T. J<strong>on</strong>es, M.D.Langley Porter Neuropsychiatric lnstituteUniversity of CaliforniaSan Francisco, CaliforniaWilliam McGlothlin, Ph.D.Department of Psychology, UCLALos Angeles, CaliforniaJack Mendels<strong>on</strong>, M.D.Alcohol and <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong> Research CenterHarvard Medical SchoolMcLean HospitalBelm<strong>on</strong>t, MassachusettsHelen Nowlis, Ph.D.Office of <strong>Drug</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong>, DHEWWashingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.Lee Robins, Ph.D.Washingt<strong>on</strong> University School of MedicineSt. Louis, MissouriNIDA Research M<strong>on</strong>ograph seriesWilliam Pollin, M.D.DIRECTOR, NIDAMarvin Snyder, Ph.D.DIRECTOR, DIVISION OF RESEARCH, NIDARobert C. Petersen, Ph.D.EDITOR-IN-CHIEFEleanor W. WaldropMANAGING EDITORParklawn Building, 5600 Fishers Lane, Rockville, Maryland 20857


THEORIES ONDRUG ABUSESelected C<strong>on</strong>temporaryPerspectives


The Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong> has obtained permissi<strong>on</strong>from the copyright holders to reproduce certain previouslypublished material as noted in the text. Further reproducti<strong>on</strong>of this material is prohibited without specific permissi<strong>on</strong> ofthe copyright holders. All other material, except shortquoted passages from copyrighted sources, is in the publicdomain and may be used and reprinted without permissi<strong>on</strong>.Citati<strong>on</strong> as to source is appreciated.The U.S. Government does not endorse or favor any specificcommercial product or commodity. Trade or proprietarynames appearing in this publicati<strong>on</strong> are used <strong>on</strong>ly becausethey are c<strong>on</strong>sidered essential in the c<strong>on</strong>text of the studiesreported herein.<strong>Library</strong> of C<strong>on</strong>gress catalog card number 80-600058DHHS publicati<strong>on</strong> number (ADM) 80-967Printed 1980NIDA Research M<strong>on</strong>ographs are indexed in the Index Medicus.They are selectively included in the coverage of BiosciencesInformati<strong>on</strong> Service, Chemical Abstracts, Psychological Abstracts,and Psychopharmacology Abstracts.Publicati<strong>on</strong> development services provided by METROTEC,Inc., under c<strong>on</strong>tract No. 271-79-3625 with the Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute<strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>.Cover art by Wendelin Astra Davids<strong>on</strong>, used with permissi<strong>on</strong> of the artist.iv


C<strong>on</strong>tentsFOREWORDA GUIDE TO THE VOLUMExixiiiPart 1THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVESAND OVERVIEWS<str<strong>on</strong>g>Theories</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>On</strong>e’s Relati<strong>on</strong>ship to SelfAN INTERACTIONAL APPROACHTO NARCOTIC ADDICTIONDavid P. AusubelTHE CAP CONTROL THEORY OF DRUG ABUSESteven R. GoldTHE BAD-HABIT THEORY OF DRUG ABUSED<strong>on</strong>ald W. GoodwinINTERACTIVE MODELS OF NONMEDICAL DRUG USERichard L. GorsuchAN EXISTENTIAL THEORY OF DRUG DEPENDENCEGeorge B. GreavesAN EGO/SELF THEORY OF SUBSTANCE DEPENDENCE:A CONTEMPORARY PSYCHOANALYTIC PERSPECTIVEEdward J. KhantzianA GENERAL THEORY OF ADDICTIONTO OPIATE-TYPE DRUGSAlfred R. LindesmithTHEORY OF DRUG USEHarvey Milkman and William Frosch48121824293438v


CONTENTS (C<strong>on</strong>tinued)AN AVAILABILITY-PRONENESSTHEORY OF ILLICIT DRUG ABUSEReginald G. SmartPERCEIVED EFFECTS OF SUBSTANCEUSE: A GENERAL THEORYGene M. SmithA LIFE-THEME THEORY OF CHRONIC DRUG ABUSEJames V. Spotts and Franklin C. Sh<strong>on</strong>tzDRUG USE AS A PROTECTIVE SYSTEMLe<strong>on</strong> Wurmser46505971<str<strong>on</strong>g>Theories</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>On</strong>e’s Relati<strong>on</strong>ship to OthersPSYCHOLOGICAL, SOCIAL, AND EPIDEMIOLOGICALFACTORS IN JUVENILE DRUG USElsidor Chein 76INCOMPLETE MOURNING AND ADDICT/FAMILY TRANSACTIONS: A THEORYFOR UNDERSTANDING HEROIN ABUSESandra B. Coleman 83THE SOCIAL DEVIANT AND INITIALADDICTION TO NARCOTICS AND ALCOHOLHarris E. Hill 90FRAMEWORK FOR AN INTERACTIVE THEORY OF DRUG USEGeorge J. Huba, Joseph A. Wingard, and Peter M. Bentler 95A SOCIAL-PSYCHOLOGICAL FRAMEWORKFOR STUDYING DRUG USERichard Jessor and Shirley JessorTOWARD A THEORY OF DRUG SUBCULTURESBruce D. Johns<strong>on</strong>DEVELOPMENTAL STAGES INADOLESCENT DRUG INVOLVEMENTDenise B. KandelSELF-ESTEEM AND SELF-DEROGATIONTHEORY OF DRUG ABUSEHoward B. Kaplanvi102110120128


CONTENTS (C<strong>on</strong>tinued)A THEORY OF DRUG DEPENDENCE BASED ON ROLE,ACCESS TO, AND ATTITUDES TOWARD DRUGSCharles WinickTHE SOCIAL SETTING AS A CONTROLMECHANISM IN INTOXICANT USENorman E. Zinberg225236<str<strong>on</strong>g>Theories</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>On</strong>e’s Relati<strong>on</strong>ship to NatureADDICTION TO PLEASURE: A BIOLOGICAL ANDSOCIAL-PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORY OF ADDICTIONNils BejerotMETHADONE MAINTENANCE: ATHEORETICAL PERSPECTIVEVincent P. Dole and Marie E. NyswanderA CHRONOBIOLOGICAL CONTROL THEORYMark HochhauserA BIOANTHROPOLOGICAL OVERVIEW OF ADDICTIONDoris F. J<strong>on</strong>as and A. David J<strong>on</strong>asEMERGING CONCEPTS CONCERNING DRUG ABUSEWilliam R. MartinSOMATOSENSORY AFFECTIONAL DEPRIVATION(SAD) THEORY OF DRUG AND ALCOHOL USEJames W. PrescottA THEORY OF ALCOHOL AND DRUGABUSE: A GENETIC APPROACHMarc A. SchuckitOPIATE RECEPTORS AND THEIRIMPLICATIONS FOR DRUG ADDICTIONEric J. Sim<strong>on</strong>246256262269278286297303viii


CONTENTS (C<strong>on</strong>tinued)Part 2THEORY COMPONENTSCONTENTS--PART 2 310INITIATION 313CONTINUATION 336TRANSITION: USE TO ABUSE 357CESSATION 378RELAPSE 402REFERENCES 421ix


In order to facilitate cross-theory comparis<strong>on</strong>s even further, a seriesof guides has been included in the volume. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, we developed,in c<strong>on</strong>juncti<strong>on</strong> with the authors, a set of shorthand or abbreviatedtheory titles. Guide 1 is a listing of all c<strong>on</strong>tributing theorists andtheir affiliati<strong>on</strong>s. The sec<strong>on</strong>d guide is a classificati<strong>on</strong> of the theoriesinto four broad categories, theories <strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>e’s relati<strong>on</strong>ship to self, toothers, to society, and to nature. A more specific classificati<strong>on</strong> of thetheories by academic discip<strong>line</strong> appears in guide 3.The most important of the guides is guide 4, Organizati<strong>on</strong> of theVolume. For each theory, this guide gives the pages <strong>on</strong> which theoverview can be found in part 1, and the page numbers of the corresp<strong>on</strong>dingtheoretical comp<strong>on</strong>ents (if any) in part 2.Guide 5, Theory Boundaries, presents a c<strong>on</strong>cise, comparative summaryof each theory, including its drug focus; the age, sex, and ethnicityof the populati<strong>on</strong> to which the theory applies; and a listing of the keyvariables inherent in the theory.There are several ways to use this reference volume. <strong>On</strong>e could ofcourse read it straight through. <strong>On</strong>e could read a particular theoryoverview in part 1 immediately followed by the corresp<strong>on</strong>ding secti<strong>on</strong>sor comp<strong>on</strong>ents in part 2. Or <strong>on</strong>e may wish to focus <strong>on</strong> a specifictheoretical comp<strong>on</strong>ent of interest in part 2 followed by selective readingof appropriate overview material in part 1. We hope that the volume’sspecialized format will encourage and facilitate its frequent use.Dan J. Lettieri, Ph.D.Chief, Psychosocial BranchDivisi<strong>on</strong> of ResearchNati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>xiv


GUIDE 1.—TheoristsDavid P. Ausubel, M.D., Ph.D.Graduate School and University CenterCity University of New YorkandHochschule der BundeswehrMunich, West GermanyHoward S. Becker, Ph.D.Department of SociologyNorthwestern UniversityEvanst<strong>on</strong>, IllinoisNils Bejerot, M.D.Department of Social MedicineKarolinska InstituteStockholm, SwedenPeter M. Bentler, Ph.D.Department of PsychologyUniversity of California-Los Angeleslsidor Chein, Ph.D.Department of PsychologyResearch Center for Human Relati<strong>on</strong>sNew York UniversitySandra B. Coleman, Ph.D.Department of Mental Health SciencesHahnemann Medical College and HospitalandAchievement Through Counseling andTreatmentPhiladelphia, PennsylvaniaVincent P. Dole, M.D.Rockefeller UniversityNew York CityCalvin J. Frederick, Ph.D.Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute of Mental HealthandDepartment of PsychiatryThe George Washingt<strong>on</strong> UniversityWashingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.William Frosch, M.D.<strong>Drug</strong>/Alcohol InstituteMetropolitan State CollegeDenver, ColoradoSteven R. Cold, Ph.D.Department of PsychologyWestern Carolina UniversityCullowhee, North CarolinaD<strong>on</strong>ald W. Goodwin, M.D.Department of PsychiatryUniversity of Kansas Medical CenterKansas City, KansasRobert A. Gord<strong>on</strong>. Ph.D.Department of Social Relati<strong>on</strong>sJohns Hopkins UniversityBaltimore, MarylandRichard L. Gorsuch, Ph.D.Graduate School of PsychologyFuller Theological SeminaryPasadena, CaliforniaGeorge B. Greaves, Ph.D.Department of PsychologyGeorgia State UniversityAtlantaHerbert Hendin, M.D.Center for Psychosocial <strong>Studies</strong>Veterans Administrati<strong>on</strong>Franklin Delano Roosevelt HospitalM<strong>on</strong>trose, New YorkandProfessor of PsychiatryNew York Medical CollegeValhallaHarris E. HiII, Ph.D. (retired)Nati<strong>on</strong>al lnstitute of Mental HealthAddicti<strong>on</strong> Research CenterLexingt<strong>on</strong>, KentuckyMark Hochhauser, Ph.D.Divisi<strong>on</strong> of School Health Educati<strong>on</strong>University of MinnesotaMinneapolisGeorge J. Huba, Ph.D.Department of PsychologyUniversity of California--Los AngelesRichard Jessor, Ph.D.Department of Psychology and Instituteof Behavioral ScienceUniversity of ColoradoBoulderShirley Jessor, Ph.D.Department of Psychology and Institute ofBehavioral ScienceUniversity of ColoradoBoulderBruce D. Johns<strong>on</strong>, Ph.D.New York State Divisi<strong>on</strong> of Substance<strong>Abuse</strong> ServicesNew York CityA. David J<strong>on</strong>as, M.D.University of WurzburgGerman Federal RepublicDoris F. J<strong>on</strong>as, Ph.D.Fellow, Royal Anthropological Instituteof Great BritainL<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>Denise B. Kandel, Ph.D.School of Public Health andDepartment of PsychiatryColumbia UniversityNew York Cityxv


Howard B. Kaplan, Ph.D.Department of PsychiatryBaylor College of MedicineHoust<strong>on</strong>, TexasEdward J. Khantzian, M.D.Department of PsychiatryHarvard Medical SchoolCambridge, MassachusettsAlfred R. Lindesmith, Ph.D.Department of SociologyIndiana UniversityBloomingt<strong>on</strong>, IndianaJan L<strong>on</strong>ey, Ph.D.Department of PsychiatryThe University of IowaIowa CityIrving F. Lukoff, Ph.D.Columbia UniversitySchool of Social WorkNew York CityWilliam R. Martin, M.D.Department of PharmacologyAlbert B. Chandler Medical CenterUniversity of KentuckyLexingt<strong>on</strong>William E. McAuliffe, Ph.D.Department of Behavioral SciencesHarvard School of Public HealthBost<strong>on</strong>, MassachusettsHarvey Milkman, Ph.D.<strong>Drug</strong>/Alcohol InsitituteMetropolitan State CollegeandDepartment of PsychiatryUniversity of Colorado Medical CenterDenverRajendra K. Misra, D. Phil.Northeast Community Mental Health CenterEast Cleveland, OhioMarie Nyswander, M.D.Rockefeller UniversityNew York CityStant<strong>on</strong> Peele, Ph.D.Department of Health Educati<strong>on</strong>Teachers College at Columbia UniversityNew York CityJames W. Prescott, Ph.D.Institute of Humanistic ScienceWest Bethesda, MarylandLee N. Robins, Ph.D.Department of PsychiatryWashingt<strong>on</strong> University School of MedicineSt. Louis, MissouriGUIDE 1.—Theorists—C<strong>on</strong>tinuedMarc A. Schuckit, M.D.Department of PsychiatrySchool of MedicineUniversity of California, San DiegoLa JollaFranklin C. Sh<strong>on</strong>tz, Ph.D.Department of PsychologyUniversity of KansasLawrenceEric J. Sim<strong>on</strong>, Ph.D.New York University Medical CenterNew York CityReginald G. Smart. Ph.D.Addicti<strong>on</strong> Research Foundati<strong>on</strong>Tor<strong>on</strong>to, <strong>On</strong>tario, CanadaGene M. Smith, Ph.D.Erich Llndemann Mental Health CenterHarvard Medical SchoolBost<strong>on</strong>, MassachusettsJames V. Spotts, Ph.D.Greater Kansas City Mental Health Foundati<strong>on</strong>Kansas City, MissouriM. Duncan Stant<strong>on</strong>. Ph.D.Philadelphia Child Guidance ClinicVeterans Administrati<strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> DependenceTreatment CenterandUniversity of Pennsylvania School of MedicinePhiladelphiaR.A. Steffenhagen, Ph.D.Department of SociologyUniversity of Verm<strong>on</strong>tBurlingt<strong>on</strong>W.K. van Dijk, M.D.Department of PsychiatryPsychiatric University ClinicAcademisch Ziekenhuis Gr<strong>on</strong>ingenGr<strong>on</strong>ingen, The NetherlandsAbraham Wikler, M.D.Albert B. Chandler Medical CenterUniversity of KentuckyLexingt<strong>on</strong>Joseph A. Wingard, Ph.D.Department of PsychologyUniversity of California--Los AngelesCharles Winick, Ph.D.Department of SociologyThe City College of the City Universityof New YorkNew York CityLe<strong>on</strong> Wurmser, M.D.Alcohol and <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong> ProgramSchool of MedicineUniversity of MarylandBaltimoreNorman Zinberg, M.D.Harvard Medical SchoolThe Cambridge HospitalCambridge, Massachusettsxvi


GUIDE 2.—Theoryclassificati<strong>on</strong>Theory classificati<strong>on</strong> was in the main chosen by the authors and reflects the authors’ first choice of category.It is recognized that many of these theories could be classified in more than <strong>on</strong>e category.--ED.1 The choosing of this classificati<strong>on</strong> was somewhat arbitrary; other classificati<strong>on</strong>s would also have been appropriate.


GUIDE 2. —Theoryclassificati<strong>on</strong>–C<strong>on</strong>tinued1 The choosing of this classificati<strong>on</strong> was somewhat arbitrary; other classificati<strong>on</strong>s would also have been appropriate.


GUIDE 3.—Disciplinary foci of the theories


GUIDE 3.—Disciplinary foci of the theories–C<strong>on</strong>tinued


GUIDE 3.—Disciplinary foci of the theories–C<strong>on</strong>tinued


GUIDE 3.—Disciplinary foci of the theories–C<strong>on</strong>tinued


GUIDE 4.—Organizati<strong>on</strong> of the volume


GUIDE 4.—Organizati<strong>on</strong> of the volume–C<strong>on</strong>tinued


GUIDE 4.—Organizati<strong>on</strong> of the volume–C<strong>on</strong>tinued


GUIDE 4.—Organizati<strong>on</strong> of the volume–C<strong>on</strong>tinued


GUIDE 4.—Organizati<strong>on</strong> of the volume–C<strong>on</strong>tinued


GUIDE 5.—Theory boundaries


GUIDE 5.—Theory boundaries–C<strong>on</strong>tinued


GUIDE 5.—Theory boundaries–C<strong>on</strong>tinued


GUIDE 5.—Theoryboundaries–C<strong>on</strong>tinued


GUIDE 5.—Theoryboundaries–C<strong>on</strong>tinued


GUIDE 5.—Theoryboundaries–C<strong>on</strong>tinued


GUIDE 5.—Theory boundaries–C<strong>on</strong>tinued


GUIDE 5.—Theoryboundaries–C<strong>on</strong>tinued


GUIDE 5.—Theory boundaries–C<strong>on</strong>tinued


GUIDE 5.—Theory boundaries–C<strong>on</strong>tinued


GUIDE 5.—Theory boundaries–C<strong>on</strong>tinued


GUIDE 5.—Theorybounderies–C<strong>on</strong>tinued


GUIDE 5.—Theory boundaries–C<strong>on</strong>tinued


PART 1THEORETICALPERSPECTIVESAND OVERVIEWS


THEORIES ON<strong>On</strong>e’sRelati<strong>on</strong>shipto Self


An Interacti<strong>on</strong>al Approachto Narcotic Addicti<strong>on</strong>David P. Ausubel, M.D., Ph.D.As in other fields of medicine and the behavioral sciences, an interacti<strong>on</strong>alapproach to the etiology, epidemiology, psychopathology, andtreatment of narcotic addicti<strong>on</strong> implies the operati<strong>on</strong> of multiple causalitywithin the pers<strong>on</strong>, in the envir<strong>on</strong>ment, and in the interacti<strong>on</strong> betweenthem. <strong>On</strong>e must c<strong>on</strong>sider both l<strong>on</strong>g-term predisposing factors andmore immediate precipitating factors.The most important precipitating factor in narcotic addicti<strong>on</strong> is degreeof access to narcotic drugs. This factor, for example, explains inpart why narcotic addicti<strong>on</strong> rates are higher in the urban slums thanin middle-class suburbs and why the incidence of narcotic addicti<strong>on</strong>approached the zero level during World War II when normal commercialchannels in the illicit narcotics trade were disrupted. Thus, no matterhow great the cultural attitudinal tolerance for addictive practices is,or how str<strong>on</strong>g individual pers<strong>on</strong>ality predispositi<strong>on</strong>s are, nobody canbecome addicted to narcotic drugs without access to them. Hence thelogic of a law enforcement comp<strong>on</strong>ent in preventi<strong>on</strong>.The sec<strong>on</strong>d most important predisposing factor in the etiology of narcoticaddicti<strong>on</strong> is the prevailing degree of attitudinal tolerance towardthe practice in the individual’s cultural, subcultural, racial, ethnic,and social class milieu. This factor explains differences in incidencerates between lower class and middle-class groups, between Europeans,Americans, and Orientals (except the Japanese), and between membersof the medical and allied health professi<strong>on</strong>s and other occupati<strong>on</strong>algroups (Ausubel 1961, 1962, 1966).The crucial and determinative predisposing factor, which, therefore,c<strong>on</strong>stitutes the most acceptable basis for the nosological categorizing ofnarcotic addicts, is the possessi<strong>on</strong> of those idiosyncratic or developmentalpers<strong>on</strong>ality traits for which narcotic drugs have adjustiveproperties. Thus it is obvious that narcotic drugs are more addictivethan, say, milk of magnesia, because their greater psychotropic effectshave adjustive value for these pers<strong>on</strong>ality traits. Chief am<strong>on</strong>g theseeffects is euphoria, which is highly adjustive for inadequate pers<strong>on</strong>alities,i.e., motivati<strong>on</strong>ally immature individuals lacking in such criteriaof ego maturity as l<strong>on</strong>g-range goals, a sense of resp<strong>on</strong>sibility,4


self-reliance and initiative, voliti<strong>on</strong>al and executive independence,frustrati<strong>on</strong> tolerance, and the ability to defer the gratificati<strong>on</strong> ofimmediate hed<strong>on</strong>istic needs for the sake of achieving l<strong>on</strong>g-term goals(Ausubel 1947, 1948, 1952a,b, 1958,a,b, 1961, 1962, 1966, 1980a,b;Ausubel and Ausubel 1963; Ausubel and Spalding 1956). Severalclinical studies of hard-core addict populati<strong>on</strong>s (e.g., Ausubel 1947;Dai 1937; Pescor 1939; Research Center for Human Relati<strong>on</strong>s 1957a;Zimmering et al. 1951, 1952) have shown that most chr<strong>on</strong>ic narcoticaddicts fall in this diagnostic category. Other studies (Ausubel 1947;Chein et al. 1964; Dai 1937; Research Center for Human Relati<strong>on</strong>s1957a) have uncovered in the life histories of such addicts those typesof parent-child relati<strong>on</strong>ships, i.e., overpermissive (underdominating),overprotecting, and overdominating parents, that tend to foster thedevelopment of the inadequate pers<strong>on</strong>ality syndrome.C<strong>on</strong>tributory factors in the development of this syndrome are probablygenic (polygenic) in origin and are undoubtedly fostered by lowersocial-class membership, particularly in families that have been <strong>on</strong>welfare for <strong>on</strong>e or more generati<strong>on</strong>s. Most of such latter youth, ofcourse, are not motivati<strong>on</strong>ally inadequate and tend to be sporadicnarcotic users who do not become either physiologically or psychologicallydependent up<strong>on</strong> the drugs in questi<strong>on</strong>. Epidemiological studiesby the New York University Research Center for Human Relati<strong>on</strong>s(1957a) have developed various behavioral, familial, and socioec<strong>on</strong>omiccriteria for differentiating between these two groups.Because of these euphoric properties of narcotic drugs effected throughdepressi<strong>on</strong> of the self-critical faculty and the positive pleasure of the“rush,” addicts receive an immediate, unearned form of gratificati<strong>on</strong>and ego enhancement. These same euphoric properties are also obviouslyadjustive for pers<strong>on</strong>s with histories of recurrent reactive depressi<strong>on</strong>.Recent studies with endogenously produced opiates, i.e.,endorphins and enkephalins (Costa and Trabucchi 1978; Goldstein1976c; Snyder 1977), suggest that in some instances deficiencies inthe producti<strong>on</strong> of the substances that c<strong>on</strong>tribute to normal optimism inthe face of life’s vicissitudes (and hence have evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary survivalvalue for the species) c<strong>on</strong>tribute toward the incidence of narcoticaddicti<strong>on</strong>. A recent study of psychiatrically disabled, treated narcoticaddicts (Ausubel 1980a) shows that lower middle- and working-classaddicts tend almost exclusively to develop severe anxiety states andreactive depressi<strong>on</strong>s when under psychological or envir<strong>on</strong>mental stress,whereas addicts from urban slum welfare backgrounds almost invariablydevelop schizophrenic symptoms under similar circumstances. Thisdifference in pathological outcome probably reflects some insidiousinternalizati<strong>on</strong> of mature motivati<strong>on</strong>al traits by the lower middle- andworking-class addicts despite the overt dominati<strong>on</strong> of the pers<strong>on</strong>alitytraits of the inadequate pers<strong>on</strong>ality.Another psychopharmacological effect of opiates, namely, sedati<strong>on</strong> orrelief of anxiety, probably accounts for the small minority of narcoticaddicts who suffer from disabling neurotic anxiety. Such individuals,particularly members of the medical and allied health professi<strong>on</strong>s,typically take small, well-c<strong>on</strong>trolled doses of morphine subcutaneously(rather than large doses of heroin intravenously) for their sedativerather than their euphoric properties. Typically their addicti<strong>on</strong> is welldisguised and seldom recognizable (Jaffe 1970a,b).Widespread sporadic use of heroin in adolescents with relatively normalpers<strong>on</strong>ality structures is generally reflective of the aggressive, antiadult5


orientati<strong>on</strong> characterizing adolescents in our culture. Here the pers<strong>on</strong>alitypredispositi<strong>on</strong> is developmental rather than idiosyncratic.Apart from the aforementi<strong>on</strong>ed affirmative clinical evidence supportingthe existence of pers<strong>on</strong>ality predispositi<strong>on</strong>s for which narcotic drugshave adjustive value, the very logic of this propositi<strong>on</strong> itself is compelling.How else could <strong>on</strong>e explain why, in a given urban slumneighborhood with uniform access to narcotic drugs and uniform subculturalor ethnic attitudinal tolerance for narcotic addicti<strong>on</strong>, the vastmajority of adolescents become <strong>on</strong>ly sporadic, n<strong>on</strong>addicted drug users,whereas a relatively small minority become chr<strong>on</strong>ically addicted?A separate nosological category of addicti<strong>on</strong> can probably be made toinclude minority-group youths with normal or even better-than-averagemotivati<strong>on</strong>al maturity who use narcotic drugs chr<strong>on</strong>ically for limitedperiods of time because they perceive the odds of achieving any ordinarydegree of academic or vocati<strong>on</strong>al success as so overwhelmingly stackedagainst them.Finally, a very small minority of narcotic addicts may be classified aspsychopathic or sociopathic pers<strong>on</strong>alities (Kolb 1925a,b). <strong>Drug</strong> addicti<strong>on</strong>,insofar as it is regarded as a disreputable or socially disapprovedhabit, obviously has n<strong>on</strong>specific adjustive value for such pers<strong>on</strong>s;however, it provides <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e of many available n<strong>on</strong>specific outlets foraggressi<strong>on</strong> or “acting out” behavior against society. Such addictstend to commit the violent, remorseless crimes that are popularly anderr<strong>on</strong>eously associated in the public mind with drug addicts generally.Actually, of course, the sedative acti<strong>on</strong> of narcotics tends to in!hibitviolence of any kind unless addicts are particularly desperate for theirnext “fix.”For the most part, except for the relatively rare psychopathic addict,most chr<strong>on</strong>ic addicts engage in n<strong>on</strong>violent, remunerative crimes primarilyto support their habits, e.g., “pushing,” “c<strong>on</strong>” games, shoplifting,check forgery, “paperhanging,” fraudulent magazine subscripti<strong>on</strong>s,etc. (Chein et al. 1964; Kolb 1925a). The percentage of addictsinvolved in preaddicti<strong>on</strong> delinquency is generally lower than that ofn<strong>on</strong>addict narcotic users who are members of delinquent gangs inurban slum areas (Ausubel 1958a,b; Research Center for Human Relati<strong>on</strong>s1957a). In any case, delinquent addicts tend to be involved inmore remunerative delinquencies directed toward satisfying their drughabits than in the more violent, predatory gang activities and “rumbles”(or gang warfare) (Research Center for Human Relati<strong>on</strong>s 1957a).SPECIAL POPULATIONSALCOHOLISM AND OTHER DRUGABUSE IN NARCOTIC ADDICTSAddicts in methad<strong>on</strong>e maintenance programs, when deprived of theirheroin-induced euphoria, turn to the euphoria-inducing properties ofalcohol, large doses of barbiturates, amphetamines, benzodiazepines,and amitripty<strong>line</strong>. Sometimes overdosage of these drugs leads to accidentalor, in reactive depressives, to deliberate suicide. Reference hasalready been made to the relati<strong>on</strong>ship between addicti<strong>on</strong>, <strong>on</strong> the <strong>on</strong>ehand, and psychopathology and criminality, <strong>on</strong> the other.6


It is generally agreed that most addicts have a preferred drug that ismost adjustive for their particular idiosyncratic or developmental pers<strong>on</strong>alitydefects and that they use other drugs <strong>on</strong>ly when deprived ofaccess to their drug of choice. Heroin and marijuana, for example,each have their own separate c<strong>on</strong>stituencies based <strong>on</strong> their distinctivepsychopharmacological effects. The use of marijuana does not predisposean individual to heroin use except insofar as it may “break theice” for more dangerous drug use. Narcotic addicts tend in general tohave a history of prior marijuana use because the latter drug is moreaccessible, cheaper, and c<strong>on</strong>sidered less dangerous and less sociallydisapproved. The c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong> between the two types of drugs is notcausal: The c<strong>on</strong>verse of this propositi<strong>on</strong> is not true, i.e., the vastmajority of marijuana users exhibit no later history of heroin abuse(Robins et al. 1970).PHYSICIAN ADDICTSClinical experience with large numbers of physician addicts at theLexingt<strong>on</strong> Hospital indicates that there are essentially two differentkinds of underlying predispositi<strong>on</strong>s: (1) the intelligent, overdominatedinadequate pers<strong>on</strong>ality who was forced into the professi<strong>on</strong> by parentsseeking vicarious ego enhancement, and who later rejects the goals ofadult maturity as a measure of revenge against parental overdominati<strong>on</strong>as so<strong>on</strong> as the parent dies or ceases to be autocratic, and (2) theanxiety neurotic who uses small, c<strong>on</strong>trolled doses of morphine subcutaneouslyto relieve anxiety rather than to obtain euphoria. These aretypically highly achievement-oriented pers<strong>on</strong>s who seek in unusualaccomplishment the ego enhancement and sense of intrinsic self-esteemnever possessed because their parents either rejected them or failed toaccept them for themselves (perceiving them solely as sources forvicarious ego enhancement).7


The CAP C<strong>on</strong>trol Theoryof <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>Steven R. Gold, Ph.D.With our current incomplete understanding of drug use and abuse, theappropriate functi<strong>on</strong> of any theoretical model may be to stimulate newwork in the area. The aim of this paper is to describe a theory ofdrug abuse that can be empirically evaluated and to encourage additi<strong>on</strong>alresearch and theory development.The CAP c<strong>on</strong>trol theory emphasizes the interacti<strong>on</strong> of the individual’sstyle and the affective experience of drug use with the drug’s pharmacogeniceffect. These are the basic ingredients of the cognitiveaffective-pharmacogenic(CAP) c<strong>on</strong>trol theory of addicti<strong>on</strong> (Coghlan etal. 1973; Gold and Coghlan 1976). The cognitive style of the drugabuser is viewed as the pivotal factor in an individual’s moving fromdrug experimentati<strong>on</strong> to drug abuse. The cognitive dimensi<strong>on</strong> willtherefore be discussed first.There is a current trend in behavior therapy emphasizing cognitiveapproaches (Lazarus 1976; Mah<strong>on</strong>ey 1977; Meichenbaum 1977). Themajor tenets of cognitive behavior therapy are that human behavior ismediated by unobservables that intervene between a stimulus and theresp<strong>on</strong>se to that stimulus. Beliefs, sets, strategies, attributi<strong>on</strong>s, andexpectancies are examples of the types of mediating c<strong>on</strong>structs currentlyc<strong>on</strong>sidered crucial to an understanding of emoti<strong>on</strong> and behavior.Sec<strong>on</strong>d, the way an individual labels or evaluates a situati<strong>on</strong> determineshis or her emoti<strong>on</strong>al and behavioral resp<strong>on</strong>se to it. A third basicassumpti<strong>on</strong> is that thoughts, feelings, and behaviors are causallyinteractive (Mah<strong>on</strong>ey 1977).To tie the cognitive approach to drug abusers, the CAP c<strong>on</strong>trol theoryposits that the abuse process begins with c<strong>on</strong>flict as a predisposingfactor. People who are having difficulty in meeting demands or expectati<strong>on</strong>splaced up<strong>on</strong> them by society or by themselves are in c<strong>on</strong>flict,and a c<strong>on</strong>sequence of the stress of c<strong>on</strong>flict is anxiety. Anxiety is auniversal feeling, something most of us experience to some degree eachday. It is not the experience of anxiety but the individual’s interpretati<strong>on</strong>of the anxiety that is crucial to the theory. Underlying theanxiety of drug abusers is a belief that they cannot alter or c<strong>on</strong>trolthe situati<strong>on</strong>; that they are powerless to affect their envir<strong>on</strong>ment and8


decrease or eliminate the sources of stress. The belief that they arepowerless to cope with stress is the major cognitive distorti<strong>on</strong> of drugabusers. <strong>On</strong>e c<strong>on</strong>sequence of this is the intense feeling of low selfesteemthat is a well-known clinical entity am<strong>on</strong>g drug abusers (Krystaland Raskin 1970). Feelings of self-depreciati<strong>on</strong>, which form the beliefthat <strong>on</strong>e is powerless, represent the affective comp<strong>on</strong>ent of the CAPtheory.The experience of anxiety is, of course, uncomfortable, and a meansof anxiety reducti<strong>on</strong> is necessary. A primary pharmacogenic effect ofheroin is anxiety reducti<strong>on</strong>. Not <strong>on</strong>ly does the drug provide relieffrom anxiety, but the individual obtains a temporary ecstatic feeling--a“high.” Under the influence of the drug the individual temporarilyexperiences an increased sense of power, c<strong>on</strong>trol, and well being.The sense of powerlessness is replaced by an exaggerated sense ofbeing all powerful--no task is too great and no feat impossible while“high.” Thus, drugs can do for abusers what they believe theycannot do for themselves: get rid of anxiety, lead to good feelingabout themselves, and make them believe they are competent, in c<strong>on</strong>trol,and able to master their envir<strong>on</strong>ment.Unfortunately for the drug abuser, the drug effects are short livedand any temporary gains turn into l<strong>on</strong>g-term losses. Inevitably, afterthe high wears off some internal or external source of stress willrekindle the c<strong>on</strong>flict and anxiety. Not <strong>on</strong>ly do the old feelings of lackof c<strong>on</strong>trol return but they are likely to be even str<strong>on</strong>ger than before.It is this increasing sense of powerlessness with increased drug usethat leads the individual from drug use to abuse. Each time drugusers rely <strong>on</strong> a drug to relieve tensi<strong>on</strong> and feel good about themselves,they become a little less capable of coping <strong>on</strong> their own. By usingdrugs to cope, the individual is cut off from learning other moreadaptive coping mechanisms and becomes less tolerant of the pain ofanxiety. The drug user now knows that anxiety does not have to betolerated, as drug taking has been successful in the past in removingtensi<strong>on</strong> and producing good feelings. It is therefore expected thatdrug use will increase both in frequency and in the number of differentsituati<strong>on</strong>s in which it is employed. For example, arguments with parentsmay be a primary source of c<strong>on</strong>flict and anxiety for the adolescent drugabuser. <strong>Drug</strong> taking will frequently follow such an argument. Anadolescent experiencing school-related stress, having learned that drugtaking is an effective means of anxiety reducti<strong>on</strong>, may turn to additi<strong>on</strong>aldrug taking to compensate for academic failures. The reliance <strong>on</strong> drugsto cope with stress therefore creates a vicious cycle; the more drugsare used, the more the individual believes they are necessary. Eachdrug experience serves to c<strong>on</strong>firm for users the belief that they arepowerless to functi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> their own.The CAP model of drug abuse also makes several assumpti<strong>on</strong>s aboutthe treatment of drug abuse. First, effective and lasting change isbased <strong>on</strong> learning that behavior has c<strong>on</strong>sequences and that <strong>on</strong>e canhave an effect <strong>on</strong> his or her own life. To replace a sense of powerlessnesswith a sense of mastery, the abuser has to be taught alternativeways of resp<strong>on</strong>ding to external or internal stress. These alternativeways cannot, however, be developed, practiced, and adopted as l<strong>on</strong>g asthe individual c<strong>on</strong>tinues to use drugs.A sec<strong>on</strong>d assumpti<strong>on</strong> is that an effective treatment plan must be multimodal(Lazarus 1976). A complete treatment plan must assess not <strong>on</strong>lythe overt behavior of drug taking but the negative emoti<strong>on</strong>s (e.g.,9


anxiety), unpleasant physical sensati<strong>on</strong>s (e.g., aches and pains thataccompany withdrawal), intrusive images (e.g., recollecti<strong>on</strong>s of pastfailures), faulty cogniti<strong>on</strong>s (e.g., “nothing I do will ever be successful”),and interpers<strong>on</strong>al inadequacies (e.g., difficulty in makingfriends with n<strong>on</strong>-drug-taking peers). Each of the individual’s problemareas may require a specific treatment strategy. For example, systematicdesensitizati<strong>on</strong> may be used to help the abuser cope with anxiety,while cognitive restructuring may be needed to correct the faultycognitive processes.The multimodal therapy approach is c<strong>on</strong>sistent with the CAP theory inthat both stress the interacti<strong>on</strong> between pers<strong>on</strong>ality modalities, andboth suggest that in complex human problems a lasting result dependsup<strong>on</strong> addressing all relevant aspects of the individual’s functi<strong>on</strong>ing.The high recidivism rate, characteristic of drug abuser treatment, maybe due to treatment focusing <strong>on</strong> a limited aspect of the abuser’s overallpers<strong>on</strong>ality functi<strong>on</strong>ing and lifestyle (Platt and Labate 1976).RESEARCH SUPPORT FOR THE CAP THEORYThe CAP theory of drug abuse was developed primarily <strong>on</strong> experiencesgained working with adolescent drug abusers at Holy Cross Campus, acoed residential treatment center in Rhinecliff, New York (Coghlan etal. 1973). To evaluate the effectiveness of the treatment program andthe CAP model, adolescents completed two pers<strong>on</strong>ality tests, <strong>on</strong>ceapproximately 30 days after admissi<strong>on</strong> and again six m<strong>on</strong>ths later (Goldand Coghlan 1976). The Rotter Locus of C<strong>on</strong>trol (I-E) Scale (Rotter1966) was used to assess whether an individual believed reinforcementto be c<strong>on</strong>tingent <strong>on</strong> pers<strong>on</strong>al efforts and behavior (internal c<strong>on</strong>trol) ora result of luck, fate, chance, or more powerful others (externalc<strong>on</strong>trol). A sec<strong>on</strong>d scale, the Self-Esteem Survey (SES) was also usedas a measure of self-evaluati<strong>on</strong> (Coopersmith 1967). It was predictedthat after six m<strong>on</strong>ths in residential treatment the adolescents wouldmove toward more internal c<strong>on</strong>trol and greater self-esteem. Data based<strong>on</strong> 32 males and 21 females provided some support for the hypotheses.Females became significantly more internally oriented. Both males’ andfemales’ scores <strong>on</strong> the SES reflected higher self-esteem, though thechange was not statistically significant. A sec<strong>on</strong>d important findingwas a significant correlati<strong>on</strong> for the females between low self-esteemand both running away and self-destructive acts (Gold and Coghlan1976).The role of perceived c<strong>on</strong>trol has been examined in a series of studiesby Seligman and his associates (Seligman 1975; Maier and Seligman1976). A belief in external causati<strong>on</strong> or c<strong>on</strong>trol may dramaticallyimpair learning and functi<strong>on</strong>ing. The research paradigm is as follows:<strong>On</strong>e group of subjects is exposed to a situati<strong>on</strong> in which their behaviorcan c<strong>on</strong>trol the occurrence of an aversive event, while another groupexperiences the same situati<strong>on</strong> except that the aversive event is bey<strong>on</strong>dtheir c<strong>on</strong>trol. When both groups are next presented with a new situati<strong>on</strong>in which learning is required, the typical finding is that peoplewho previously experienced c<strong>on</strong>trol learn faster in the new situati<strong>on</strong>.Moreover, some subjects, after experiencing the lack of c<strong>on</strong>trol, maynot learn at all even though the task is often quite simple. Seligman(1975) interprets such findings as indicating that, when an organism’sbehavior has no effect <strong>on</strong> its envir<strong>on</strong>ment, “learned helplessness” isthe result. The learned-helplessness theory has been suggested as a10


model for the development of reactive depressi<strong>on</strong>. It also points out away in which the sense of helplessness or powerlessness may be acharacteristic of drug abusers. Individuals pr<strong>on</strong>e to drug abuse maybe those who have a history of lack of relati<strong>on</strong>ship between theirresp<strong>on</strong>ses and c<strong>on</strong>sequences--a series of learning experiences whichteach them they are not effective in altering or influencing theirenvir<strong>on</strong>ment. For example, studying may have no effect <strong>on</strong> gradesreceived; behaving as demanded by parents may not lead to beingloved; hard work may not lead to a promoti<strong>on</strong> or better job; etc.The similarities between a model of reactive depressi<strong>on</strong> and drug abuseare not surprising, as there are aspects of drug abuse that paralleldepressi<strong>on</strong>. <strong>Drug</strong> abuse can be described as a self-destructive activityand often is clinically viewed as a form of “slow suicide.” Gold andCoghlan (1976) found a relati<strong>on</strong>ship between adolescent female abusers’belief in external c<strong>on</strong>trol and low self-esteem with overt self-destructivebehavior. Wetzel (1976) studied 154 suicide attempters, threateners,and psychiatric c<strong>on</strong>trols and found that a sense of hopelessness washighly correlated with suicidal behavior, even more so than depth ofdepressi<strong>on</strong>.The effects of perceived c<strong>on</strong>trol have also been studied with referenceto coping with aversive stimulati<strong>on</strong>. For example, Geer et al. (1970)found that college students who falsely believed they had c<strong>on</strong>trol overthe durati<strong>on</strong> of shocks received displayed less physiological resp<strong>on</strong>seto the shock. The finding of less arousal suggests that the shockswere becoming less stressful for them. Turk (1975) trained volunteersto develop different coping strategies to deal with pain to encouragethem to believe they could successfully manage it. Cognitively trainedsubjects were able to tolerate the pain for almost twice as l<strong>on</strong>g asuntrained subjects.In summary, the CAP theory of drug abuse emphasizes the interacti<strong>on</strong>of cognitive-affective-pharmacogenic effects of drug taking. The beliefthat <strong>on</strong>e is powerless to affect the envir<strong>on</strong>ment and cope with stressplays a central role in the theory. The CAP theory is seen as beingc<strong>on</strong>sistent with newer cognitive models which emphasize the role ofinternal thoughts and beliefs in the development of maladaptive behavior.Research findings support the hypothesis that an individual’s belief inthe ability to c<strong>on</strong>trol a situati<strong>on</strong> str<strong>on</strong>gly influences behavior. Successfultreatment of the drug abuser requires a multimodal approach whichalters faulty thinking, teaches new interpers<strong>on</strong>al skills, helps theabuser cope with pain and anxiety, and encourages the development ofa positive self-image.11


The Bad-Habit Theoryof <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>D<strong>on</strong>ald W. Goodwin, M.D.INTRODUCTIONBy “bad habit” I refer to repetitious, harmful, semireflexive behaviorresulting from classical c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing in “susceptible” individuals. Withregard to drugs, “susceptibility” may be specific for certain drugs orn<strong>on</strong>specific, i.e., the individual may be susceptible to abusing anumber of drugs, perhaps <strong>on</strong>ly in certain classes (e.g., the sedativehypnotics)or perhaps across classes (e.g., opiates, sedative-hypnotics,nicotine, etc.). Susceptibility may be partly inherited (under somedegree of genetic c<strong>on</strong>trol), or it may reflect purely psychosocial influences,or both. These issues are complicated, and a global theory ofaddicti<strong>on</strong> may be premature. My theory is limited to alcoholism, but Ihave included a brief discussi<strong>on</strong> of the possibility that theories ofalcoholism may help to explain other forms of substance abuse.WHAT IS INHERITED?Perhaps the str<strong>on</strong>gest evidence for a genetic factor in alcoholism is theevidence that alcoholism str<strong>on</strong>gly runs in families (Cott<strong>on</strong> 1979). This,combined with findings from twin and adopti<strong>on</strong> studies, at least suggeststhe possibility of a hereditary factor (Goodwin 1979). If so, what isinherited?Certain behaviors associated with drinking must be explained before itis known why serious drinking problems develop in perhaps <strong>on</strong>e of 12or 15 drinkers in Western countries. These core features must beexplained: (1) loss of c<strong>on</strong>trol, (2) tendency to relapse, and (3)tolerance. The following explanati<strong>on</strong>s blend possible genetic andn<strong>on</strong>genetic factors.Indisputably, there is a wide range of innate variati<strong>on</strong>s in resp<strong>on</strong>se toalcohol. This is true in humans and every species studied. Thereare not <strong>on</strong>ly strain and species differences but also differences between12


individuals. It is difficult to account for this variati<strong>on</strong> other than toascribe it to innate, probably genetically c<strong>on</strong>trolled influences.In humans, the most c<strong>on</strong>spicuous example of innate variati<strong>on</strong> in alcoholresp<strong>on</strong>se has been shown in Orientals, whose low alcoholism rates haveusually been attributed to social factors. However, three studies havenow shown that small amounts of alcohol cause a cutaneous flush andunpleasant reacti<strong>on</strong>s in about three-quarters of Orientals (Wolff 1973;Ewing et al. 1974; Seto et al. 1978), indicating that a large number ofOrientals are physiologically intolerant of alcohol. The biochemicalbasis for these adverse reacti<strong>on</strong>s has not been determined, but recentdata indicate a high frequency of atypical liver alcohol dehydrogenaseam<strong>on</strong>g Japanese (Stamatoyannopoulas et al. 1975). This coenzyme mayalter the metabolism of alcohol, leading to increased formati<strong>on</strong> of acetaldehyde,and this may explain the flush and other ill effects (such asnausea).Other groups with relatively low alcoholism rates may be similarlyprotected by an innate sensitivity to alcohol. For example, fewerwomen than men are alcoholic, and <strong>on</strong>e study reports that women havehigher blood alcohol levels after ingesting a given amount of alcoholthan do men (J<strong>on</strong>es and J<strong>on</strong>es 1976). Informal surveys suggest that asubstantial proporti<strong>on</strong> of women experience unpleasant physical effectsafter modest amounts of alcohol (e.g., nausea and headache). Anecdotalevidence also suggests that more Jews than n<strong>on</strong>-Jews have adversephysical reacti<strong>on</strong>s to modest amounts of alcohol, which may c<strong>on</strong>tributeto the low prevalence of alcoholism am<strong>on</strong>g Jews.It is obviously essential to be able to drink large quantities of alcoholto be alcoholic. Many people are prevented from this because ofinnate cutoff points almost certainly under genetic c<strong>on</strong>trol. Thatgenetic c<strong>on</strong>trol is an important factor in drug metabolism in generalhas been dem<strong>on</strong>strated by numerous studies showing that identicaltwins metabolize a wide variety of drugs (including alcohol) at almostidentical rates, while fraternal twins have widely disparate rates ofmetabolism (Vesell et al. 1971). Whether the development of alcoholismis also subject to some genetic c<strong>on</strong>trol remains c<strong>on</strong>jectural.It is widely believed that tolerance to alcohol is acquired mainly from“practice”; the more a pers<strong>on</strong> drinks, the more he or she needs todrink to get the same effect. With opiates, this clearly is true; withalcohol, it is not so clearly true. Animals fail to show much toleranceto alcohol, even after repeated exposure. Also, young men withalmost no prior drinking experience vary widely in their resp<strong>on</strong>se toalcohol in experimental studies (Goodwin et al. 1969). Some showalmost no effect, while others are quite easily intoxicated. Since thisvariability does not correlate with prior drinking history, the <strong>on</strong>lyother explanati<strong>on</strong> is that innate biological factors are resp<strong>on</strong>sible.To summarize, large numbers of people are more or less “protected”from becoming alcoholic because of genetically determined adversephysical reacti<strong>on</strong>s to alcohol. If anything is inherited in alcoholism, itis probably the lack of intolerance for alcohol. (Parenthetically, it isinteresting that Alcoholics An<strong>on</strong>ymous often refers to allergy as afactor in alcoholism, usually properly bracketing “allergy” in quotati<strong>on</strong>marks. It now seems that this is indeed true, but it is the n<strong>on</strong>alcoholics, not the alcoholics, who are allergic!)13


WHAT IS LEARNED?Here, in brief, is a descripti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>on</strong>e way genetic and experientialfactors may interact to produce alcoholism.1.2.3.4.The potential alcoholic must be able to drink a lot (i.e., lack anintolerance for alcohol).Some people experience more euphoria from alcohol than others do(Goodwin et al. 1979). This factor is also quite possibly undergenetic c<strong>on</strong>trol. Because euphoria is a positive reinforcer, presumablypeople who experience the most euphoria are the <strong>on</strong>esmost likely to drink.Like most drugs of abuse, alcohol is quickly absorbed and eliminated;the effects occur rapidly and disappear rapidly. Experimentalstudies indicate that alcoholics experience dysphoric as wellas euphoric effects from alcohol (Mello 1975). Those individualswho experience the most euphoria (because of genetic factors)quite possibly also experience the most dysphoria, the cure forwhich is more alcohol. After a few drinks, these people may drinkmore to relieve the dysphoria than to restore the euphoria. Inany case, during a single drinking period there may be two reinforcersinvolved: producti<strong>on</strong> of euphoria and reducti<strong>on</strong> of dysphoria.This peak-valley effect may explain loss of c<strong>on</strong>trol. The height ofthe peak and the depth of the valley may be genetically c<strong>on</strong>trolled.For reas<strong>on</strong>s described above, alcohol in genetically susceptibleindividuals may be massively reinforcing. The reinforcementsoccur during individual drinking periods and most strikingly “themorning after,” when the “hair of the dog” swiftly relieves thatformidable dysphoria known as a hangover. When loss of c<strong>on</strong>trolleads to binge drinking, withdrawal symptoms occur (a superhangover).5.After periods of abstinence, binge drinkers often relapse. This is<strong>on</strong>e of the mysteries of alcoholism. If it is true that alcoholicsc<strong>on</strong>tinue heavy drinking mainly to curb dysphoriant effects, and ifit is true that alcohol is a relatively weak euphoriant compared,say, to cocaine or amphetamines (Mayfield and Allen 1967), thenwhy should a binge drinker start drinking again after experiencinghorrendous effects from previous binges? Some alcoholics aresociopaths, and in their case relapse may be explainable as anotherinstance of “not learning from experience.” Most alcoholics, however,seem to learn from most experiences as well as the nextpers<strong>on</strong>. Why relapse?Stimulus generalizati<strong>on</strong> may be the answer. As noted, alcohol ingenetically susceptible individuals is a massively reinforcing agent.Both the positive (euphoriant) and negative (dysphoriant) effectsresemble mood states and physical feelings experienced in sobriety.The terms “euphoriant” and “dysphoriant” are used here as shorthandfor “positive reinforcer” and “negative reinforcer,” respectively. Theformer may resemble any type of rewarding experience, e.g., sex orthe pleasure of receiving a gift. The latter may resemble hunger,fatigue, or feelings of l<strong>on</strong>e<strong>line</strong>ss, anxiety, and depressi<strong>on</strong>.14


Through the process of stimulus generalizati<strong>on</strong>, the ups and downsintroduced by alcohol become cued to a wide variety of internal statesand external circumstances. Even heavy drinkers drink more <strong>on</strong> someoccasi<strong>on</strong>s and in certain settings. These occasi<strong>on</strong>s and settings becomeassociated with both the highs and the lows of drinking. They becomec<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed stimuli, just as do the internal feelings that resemble thehighs and lows of drinking.Relapse represents a c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed resp<strong>on</strong>se to these c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed stimuli.Since relapse is usually erratic and unpredictable, it is quite likelythat a combinati<strong>on</strong> of “interoceptive” and “exteroceptive” c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>edstimuli are required to produce relapse. The necessary combinati<strong>on</strong>very likely differs between individuals and even in each individualfrom time to time. As Keller (1972) wrote,For any alcoholic there may be several or a whole battery ofcritical cues or signals. By the rule of generalizati<strong>on</strong>, anycritical cue can spread like the tentacles of a vine over awhole range of analogs, and this may account for the growingfrequency of bouts, or for the development of a pattern ofc<strong>on</strong>tinuous inebriati<strong>on</strong>. An exaggerated example is the manwho goes out and gets drunk every time his mother-in-lawgives him a certain wall-eyed look. After a while he has toget drunk whenever any woman gives him that look.The c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing theory is not new. Wikler, Ludwig, and their associates(Ludwig and Wikler 1974; Ludwig et al. 1974) have described it inmuch detail. It remains a theory, and not an easy theory to test, atthat. Combined with the genetic data, it has the advantage of showinghow genetic factors may interact with learning (c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing) to produceproblem drinking. As Ludwig and Wikler (1974) have pointed out,social and psychological “modifiers” obviously influence the “addictivecycle.” For example, studies indicate that alcoholics differ from n<strong>on</strong>alcoholicsin having a dominant mother and a weak, passive father(Barry 1974). There is also evidence that ordinal birth positi<strong>on</strong>influences who becomes alcoholic (Barry et al. 1969). A host of otherpsychological and social modifiers have been described in the alcoholismliterature; few would dispute the importance of some or all of thesemodifiers in promoting or discouraging the hypothetical geneticc<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ingsequence proposed above.HOW APPLICABLE?Assuming the above hypothesis has some validity for alcoholism, towhat extent can it explain other forms of substance abuse? Attemptingto shown comm<strong>on</strong> features in alcoholism and drug abuse in general, Iwill break down the problem into the traditi<strong>on</strong>al triad of agent, host,and envir<strong>on</strong>ment.AGENTComm<strong>on</strong>ly abused psychotropic substances have, I propose, somefeatures in comm<strong>on</strong>. First, they are short acting, that is, rapidlyassimilated and rapidly eliminated. Nicotine perhaps better meets thisdefiniti<strong>on</strong> than any other compound widely used and abused today (andsome believe nicotine is the most abused of readily available substances).15


Historically, phenobarbital, a l<strong>on</strong>g-acting drug, was not c<strong>on</strong>sideredaddictive, but with the introducti<strong>on</strong> of short- and intermediate-actingbarbiturates in the 1930s. the addicti<strong>on</strong> problem with this class ofdrugs became quickly apparent. Alcohol (which is rapidly absorbedand elimlnated at about the rate of 15 ml per hour), opiates, newerbarbiturates and their analogs, amphetamines and other stimulants, andnicotine rank am<strong>on</strong>g the most abused substances in the world. Thereis still some doubt about marijuana, which, if smoked, is rapidlyassimilated but has metabolites with very l<strong>on</strong>g half-lives. Its abusepotential in Western countries still remains c<strong>on</strong>troversial, but all theother drugs listed above have the comm<strong>on</strong> feature of being shortacting.HOSTGenetic factors could operate in two ways to increase or decrease thepossibility of an individual becoming dependent <strong>on</strong> a substance orsubstances.First, many individuals are “protected” from developing specific substanceabuses because they develop aversive physiological and subjectiveeffects from the drug or drugs in small quantities. There aremany anecdotal reports of individuals who can never smoke cigarettes,drink alcohol, use sleeping pills, or tolerate amphetamines or opiates,and the reas<strong>on</strong> appears to be genetic. In the case of alcoholism, manymilli<strong>on</strong>s of people are thus protected; how many are protected from useor abuse of other substances is not known.A sec<strong>on</strong>d means by which peak-and-valley drugs, such as thosedescribed earlier, may produce dependence in “unprotected” individualsis probably also under genetic c<strong>on</strong>trol and involves varying degrees ofpositive reinforcement from the substance followed quickly by aversiveeffects which can <strong>on</strong>ly be relieved by reuse of the substance thatproduced the reinforcement-aversive sequence in the first place. If,for example, after many years of not smoking, a former chain smokersmokes a cigarette, he or she receives some reinforcing effects. Fromthat point <strong>on</strong>, however, the need to smoke is based more <strong>on</strong> a “drughunger” or craving produced by that first cigarette than it is <strong>on</strong> adesire to obtain whatever gratificati<strong>on</strong> the first cigarette produced.The initial reinforcing effect, by the way, obviously is not the samefor all comm<strong>on</strong>ly abused substances. The euphoria from amphetaminesand cocaine is apparently much str<strong>on</strong>ger than that produced by alcohol,and the reinforcer that drives the cigarette habit clearly is not euphoria.To recapitulate, a drug of abuse is <strong>on</strong>e that quickly enters and leavesthe body, producing aversive effects during the sec<strong>on</strong>d stage whichcan <strong>on</strong>ly be relieved by reintroducti<strong>on</strong> of the substance (a chocolatebar, a tranquilizer, or even a pipe cannot truly substitute for a cigarettein the chain smoker who has started the addictive cycle).<strong>On</strong>e last word about the host: However available the agent, andhowever susceptible the host, it must be remembered that the host isalso born with other traits and susceptibilities, and in the intricatebyplay of genetic and envir<strong>on</strong>mental factors, forces may emerge whichoppose or nullify tendencies to use or abuse a particular substance.These countervailing forces must always be taken into account inevaluating individuals at risk.16


ENVIRONMENTThere is no questi<strong>on</strong> that availability influences use. During Prohibiti<strong>on</strong>,hospitalizati<strong>on</strong>s for drinking problems and cirrhosis rates droppedprecipitously. This was also true during the Sec<strong>on</strong>d World War incountries like France and England where wine and beer were scarce,expensive, and often rati<strong>on</strong>ed. But it is important to note that moreis involved than legality and commercial availability. Prices, ages ofbuyers, prevailing attitudes toward the substance, and a multitude ofother factors will influence use.17


Interactive Models ofN<strong>on</strong>medical <strong>Drug</strong> UseRichard L. Gorsuch, Ph.D.Gorsuch and Butler (1976a,b) developed a multiple-model theory ofn<strong>on</strong>medical drug use in an attempt to provide relatively c<strong>on</strong>crete anddetailed descripti<strong>on</strong>s of factors leading to specific types of n<strong>on</strong>medicaldrug use. The primary focus of the models is <strong>on</strong> illicit “hard” drugabuse, such as abuse of heroin and cocaine. The models, however,are not restricted solely to “hard” drug abuse but probably apply tothe n<strong>on</strong>medical use and abuse of several types of substances. Thefirst secti<strong>on</strong> below provides the theoretical background for the models’development. The sec<strong>on</strong>d secti<strong>on</strong> out<strong>line</strong>s the models themselves.The research up<strong>on</strong> which the models were based was detailed previously(Gorsuch and Butler 1976a,b) and is not repeated here. While occasi<strong>on</strong>alstudies will be referenced to illustrate major c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>s, the point ofthe present paper is to explain the models and their perspectivesrather than to review the literature. Other recent research reviews(e.g., Sadava 1975; Jessor 1979) have identified the same empiricallyestablished characteristics as we did. Recent research programs havec<strong>on</strong>tinued to document these c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>s (e.g., Jessor 1976; Nail et al.1974; Sadava and Forsyth 1977; Kandel 1978b).ORIGIN AND NATURE OF THE MODELSPSYCHOLOGICALThe theory presented here is psychological, focusing up<strong>on</strong> the individual,with drug behavior as the dependent variable. Groups are important<strong>on</strong>ly insofar as they influence the behavior of the members of thatgroup.The author gratefully acknowledges the research assistance of PatRose in the preparati<strong>on</strong> of this paper.18


The psychological focus identifies as the major causative factors thosewhich operate directly within the pers<strong>on</strong>’s life space. Individuals aredirectly influenced <strong>on</strong>ly by internal processes or by that which happensin their immediate envir<strong>on</strong>ment. Internal processes include physiologicalprocesses; the residuals of past experiences, including beliefs, opini<strong>on</strong>s,expectati<strong>on</strong>s, attitudes, and values; and psychological processes.Direct envir<strong>on</strong>mental influences c<strong>on</strong>sist of the objects and events in theimmediate envir<strong>on</strong>ment which actually affect the individual. For example,friends taking drugs when the individual is not present is not a directinfluence, but learning about friends taking hard drugs is.This psychological perspective defines other envir<strong>on</strong>mental influencesas indirect factors which produce or influence the objects and eventsin an individual’s life space. For example, a law which increases theavailability of a particular drug would be an indirect influence, producingthe direct influence: the presence of the drug in the pers<strong>on</strong>’senvir<strong>on</strong>ment.MULTIVARIATEWhile a simple, <strong>on</strong>e-element theory is widely desired, our own experiencessuggest that such univariate theories are seldom appropriate.Many decisi<strong>on</strong>s, including those about drugs, are the result of multiplefactors. Because of this, we held open the opti<strong>on</strong> in developing ourmodels for multiple causative elements, although, following Occam’srazor, we did not wish it to be so unless it were necessary.Multivariate models are basically of two types. The first and mostcomm<strong>on</strong> is the <strong>line</strong>ar model, in which each element is applied equally toevery<strong>on</strong>e. Ordinary statistical analyses operate from this model; forexample, <strong>on</strong>e mean is the estimate of the performance of every<strong>on</strong>e in aparticular group. The multivariate <strong>line</strong>ar models of causati<strong>on</strong> give aunique weight to each causative factor, and the predicti<strong>on</strong> for anindividual is a functi<strong>on</strong> of the weight for that causative factor and thedegree to which it is present for that pers<strong>on</strong>.In a sec<strong>on</strong>d type of multivariate model, it is recognized that differentindividuals may be influenced by radically different situati<strong>on</strong>s, producingdifferent effects <strong>on</strong> their behavior. Moreover, the same behaviormay have totally different causes in different people; what is sufficientcause for <strong>on</strong>e individual to engage in illicit drug use may not be foranother. In these situati<strong>on</strong>s, simple multiple regressi<strong>on</strong> weights, forexample, do not apply equally to every<strong>on</strong>e, and the ordinary statisticalprocedures of chi-square and ANOVA can be misleading. Instead,several different causative models need to be developed so that themodel applied to an individual is the most appropriate for his or hersituati<strong>on</strong>. In this theory, each of the different causative models whichcan lead to the same illicit drug use provides a descripti<strong>on</strong> of a differentpath by which a pers<strong>on</strong> might proceed to a particular behavior.The allowance for multiple paths as separate models makes the theorymore comprehensive. For example, a path in existence prior to the1914 Harris<strong>on</strong> Act may no l<strong>on</strong>ger exist because of the impact of thatact. But the ability to describe that path with the general theory isimportant for two reas<strong>on</strong>s. First, <strong>on</strong>ly as we are aware of a formerpath to illicit drug use will we be able to avoid accidentally recreatingit. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, it is possible that there are special groups which, from a19


psychological perspective, exist today in an atmosphere comparable tothat of the general public prior to 1914.If multiple models are possible, the questi<strong>on</strong> of whether <strong>on</strong>e model isthe model does not occur. Instead the questi<strong>on</strong> is whether a modelactually describes a group of people who currently or potentially exist.If so, then that model is important for our total understanding of thephenomena. It is hoped that demographic studies will provide us withdescripti<strong>on</strong>s of which models apply to the greatest number of people,but the therapist interested in the drug abuse of a particular clientwill be c<strong>on</strong>cerned with the most appropriate model for that individualrather than the “popularity” of the model in society.MULTIPLE STAGESAnother characteristic of our theory is the explicit c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> ofmultiple stages of drug involvement. It does not assume that initialdrug use and drug addicti<strong>on</strong> have the same causes. Admittedly, sometheories do take a single-stage, “take it <strong>on</strong>ce and hooked for life”approach. However, we found the evidence str<strong>on</strong>g that many who dohave an initial experience with a particular drug do not become c<strong>on</strong>tinualusers, and that many who become c<strong>on</strong>tinual users do not becomeaddicts. Hence, the causes for each stage may be different, and a setof stages is necessary. Our stages are initial drug use, c<strong>on</strong>tinualuse, and addicti<strong>on</strong>.While the paths and the stages are summarized here as discrete andunique, they can be expected to blend more in life than they do <strong>on</strong>paper. A pers<strong>on</strong> may follow <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e or may follow many paths todrug use and may even functi<strong>on</strong> at intermediate points between thestages. The paths and stages are merely theoretical devices to aidour c<strong>on</strong>ceptualizati<strong>on</strong> for research and interventi<strong>on</strong> purposes and, so,oversimplify the phenomen<strong>on</strong> somewhat.THE MODELSEach of the three secti<strong>on</strong>s below provides a model for how individualsmay try a drug for the first time. Each model represents a major anddistinct pathway, but it is important to bear in mind that there may bemany individuals who wander back and forth between two or morepaths.NONSOCIALIZED DRUG USERS MODEL<strong>On</strong>e of the more c<strong>on</strong>sistently found precursors of illicit drug use is alack of socializati<strong>on</strong>. Numerous studies have compared the pers<strong>on</strong>alitycharacteristics of those who use illicit drugs with those of n<strong>on</strong>users.(See Gorsuch and Butler 1976a.) Regardless of the pers<strong>on</strong>ality scaleused, drug abusers are lower <strong>on</strong> social c<strong>on</strong>formity and social resp<strong>on</strong>sibilityscales than are n<strong>on</strong>drug abusers. This is to be expected, forthe pers<strong>on</strong> without internalized norms against drug abuse is a pers<strong>on</strong>who is open to being swayed into drug use by situati<strong>on</strong>al factors. AsBowers (1968) showed, those with str<strong>on</strong>g pers<strong>on</strong>al norms against it willnot use a substance even if the envir<strong>on</strong>ment allows it, but those20


without str<strong>on</strong>g norms will fluctuate widely in their usage dependingup<strong>on</strong> the envir<strong>on</strong>mental characteristics.According to our theory, not being socialized to the traditi<strong>on</strong>al cultureis a necessary but not a sufficient c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> for drug abuse. Hence,socializati<strong>on</strong> is expected to be a unidirecti<strong>on</strong>al predictor, with thehighly socialized not being involved in drug abuse regardless of peerpressure or the availability of the drug, for example, but with then<strong>on</strong>socialized pers<strong>on</strong> engaging in use as a functi<strong>on</strong> of situati<strong>on</strong>al aspectsof availability, peer pressure, and so forth.For the n<strong>on</strong>socialized pers<strong>on</strong>, peers play a major role in our c<strong>on</strong>temporaryculture. The role they play is twofold. First, it is most oftenthrough peers that illicit drugs are made available, since these drugscan seldom be purchased through ordinary means. The peer groupmay either supply the drug directly or provide informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> obtainingit. Having a large number of drug-using friends means that then<strong>on</strong>socialized individual has ready access to drugs. Since there is nointernal mechanism to prevent drug usage for this pers<strong>on</strong>, such readyaccess leads to the high rate of initial use. This is what gives thepeer group the predictive strength often found in research studies(e.g., Johns<strong>on</strong> 1973).Sec<strong>on</strong>d, the peer group may provide models for drug usage, teachingits members when, where, and how to use the drugs. This theorydoes not, however, require socializati<strong>on</strong> by the peer group into a drugculture for the n<strong>on</strong>socialized individual to have the initial drug experience.The effect is more casual than that--the peer group needs <strong>on</strong>lyto provide models for attainment and use of the illicit drugs.The impact of the peer group will differ for different age groups as afuncti<strong>on</strong> of the amount of time spent within that group and the extentto which it is free of external c<strong>on</strong>trols. With children, peer-groupactivity is almost never free of adult supervisi<strong>on</strong>, so there is littleavailability of drugs for a n<strong>on</strong>socialized child. But adolescents oftenfuncti<strong>on</strong> without supervisi<strong>on</strong>, and hence the channels are more openfor illicit drug passage.Parents influence their children, when not actually supervising them,<strong>on</strong>ly through the internal standards which they have imparted to them,and with the n<strong>on</strong>socialized youth such internal standards are absent.Parents who have not socialized their children regarding drugs havelittle or no impact <strong>on</strong> whether the children will have an initial drugexperience.There is some literature to suggest that the absence of the mother orfather relates to illicit drug use (Gorsuch and Butler 1976a), and thisis probably true because such absences sometimes disrupt the socializati<strong>on</strong>patterns. However, the fact that this effect is not always foundis not surprising, because the major variable should be the parenting,not the presence of a particular biological parent. The literature doesindeed suggest that parental relati<strong>on</strong>ships are poorer am<strong>on</strong>g thoseabusing drugs than am<strong>on</strong>g those not abusing drugs. Unfortunatelythe literature is incomplete, and it is difficult to decipher whether thisphenomen<strong>on</strong> is a result of a lack of proper parenting or a reacti<strong>on</strong> ofthe parents to a child who is n<strong>on</strong>socializable, if such a child exists.Religious membership has been included in more research studies thanalmost any other variable and has a highly c<strong>on</strong>sistent ability to predict21


the n<strong>on</strong>drug user (Corsuch and Butler 1976a). Unfortunately therehas been <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e article specifically c<strong>on</strong>cerned with the impact ofreligi<strong>on</strong> (Linden and Currie 1977), so the “why” behind this relati<strong>on</strong>shipis just beginning to be explored. In the n<strong>on</strong>socialized model, religiousmembership theoretically could be expected to operate in three ways.First, membership in a religious body indicates that the parentingfigures have themselves been a part of and support traditi<strong>on</strong>al socializati<strong>on</strong>and can be expected to pass such norms <strong>on</strong> to their children.Sec<strong>on</strong>d, participati<strong>on</strong> in a traditi<strong>on</strong>al group would provide for substituteparenting figures if the biological parents were incapable of or unwillingto provide appropriate models and traditi<strong>on</strong>al socializati<strong>on</strong>. Third, thereligious membership provides a peer group whose members are morelikely to be traditi<strong>on</strong>ally socialized and supportive of traditi<strong>on</strong>al socializati<strong>on</strong>.Such a peer group would be unlikely to make illicit drugs availableto the n<strong>on</strong>socialized individual. And since n<strong>on</strong>socialized individualshave no particular drive for drugs per se, they will fit in with andc<strong>on</strong>form to a n<strong>on</strong>drug-using subculture just as well as a drug-usingsubculture.PRODRUGSOCIALIZATION MODELIt is often the case that a pers<strong>on</strong> is socialized into a prodrug lifestyle.Some of the clearest examples of this can be found in certain NativeAmerican tribes or religious or quasi-religious groups that use drugsfor cerem<strong>on</strong>ial or other such purposes. The socializati<strong>on</strong> need not beto illicit drugs. A widely replicated finding in the research literatureis that children who use a drug illicitly often come from families where<strong>on</strong>e or more of the parenting figures used drugs. Even though parentingfigures generally used licit drugs--over-the-counter drugs andtranquilizers prescribed by doctors--the effect was to teach theirchildren that drugs are good and provide a soluti<strong>on</strong> for <strong>on</strong>e’s problems.It is a small step from buying drugs at the corner drug store tobuying drugs <strong>on</strong> the corner.The parents described by this model are prodrug socializing forces.Because they are highly respected by and spend more time with theirchildren, the youths are likely also to be prodrug and hence to usedrugs, whether licit or illicit. Note that this model does not describeparents who teach moderate or prescribed usage of drugs.Peers are another source of prodrug socializati<strong>on</strong>. The extent towhich encouragement and active solicitati<strong>on</strong> by peers actually occurs iscurrently debated, for there are counterarguments that the illicit drugsubculture, which developed because of comm<strong>on</strong> needs for drugs, doesnot engage actively in socializing others into the culture. Despite thefact that the degree to which this occurs is unknown, it is apparentthat it can occur, at least in some cases, and so must be included inthe general model.In additi<strong>on</strong> to socializati<strong>on</strong> regarding drugs per se, socializati<strong>on</strong> into aset of “sympathetic” pers<strong>on</strong>ality characteristics may be also importantin this model. It is comm<strong>on</strong>ly found that the n<strong>on</strong>traditi<strong>on</strong>al values ofindividualism and experimentati<strong>on</strong>, as well as the American “left wing”value systems, are predisposing to the use of illicit drugs in that theyprovide a set of attitudes and values that encourage the type of experimentsthat can include illicit drug use.22


The model assumes that there are prodrug socializing agents in theindividual’s immediate envir<strong>on</strong>ment that provide relatively easy accessto illicit drugs, numerous opportunities for drugs to be used, andmodels for their use. With such a background, the motivati<strong>on</strong> neednot be str<strong>on</strong>g for an initial drug experience to occur. The normaldrive in children and youths simply to try whatever they see othersdoing is sufficient to account for the actual initial drug experience.To the extent that motivati<strong>on</strong> plays any part in this scheme, the majormotivating factors would be the need for status (e.g., to be “adult”),novelty seeking, curiosity, relief from boredom, and a motivati<strong>on</strong>unique to this particular model: c<strong>on</strong>formity.IATROGENIC MODELThe origin of the iatrogenic model is found in the initial use of opiumand its derivatives for medical purposes before 1900. For many yearsthe addictive properties of such drugs were not understood, andpeople unknowingly became addicted to these drugs which were usedfor medical purposes.In this model the primary motivati<strong>on</strong> for the initial illicit drug use isthe relief of physical pain or mental anguish. A pers<strong>on</strong> will seek outa drug not when life is going well--as could occur for the n<strong>on</strong>socializedor prodrug socialized individual--but when life is going poorly. Thefact that many individuals who try drugs illicitly have already underg<strong>on</strong>euse of similar drugs in hospital settings suggests that they maybe influenced by the success of the medical use of these drugs, andperceive illicit drug use as a simple extensi<strong>on</strong> of comm<strong>on</strong> medical procedures“without bothering the doctor.”Physicians and other medical workers have a c<strong>on</strong>siderably higher illicitdrug use rate than the normal populati<strong>on</strong>. The iatrogenic model stressesthe fact that these are the people who see <strong>on</strong> a day-by-day basis thepositive uses of drugs for medical reas<strong>on</strong>s and hence may succumb tothe temptati<strong>on</strong> to self-prescribe.23


An Existential Theory of<strong>Drug</strong> DependenceGeorge B. Greaves, Ph.D.Existential psychology deals primarily with the phenomenal and emoti<strong>on</strong>alstate of individuals, with a pers<strong>on</strong>’s experience of the quality andmeaning of his or her life, and of means and methods of therapeuticinterventi<strong>on</strong>, both verbal and n<strong>on</strong>verbal, which can lead to an enhancementof an individual’s life state. Within the framework of existentialtheory, human beings are seen to be motivated primarily to satisfy andsustain basic needs and to fulfill certain aspirati<strong>on</strong>s (Maslow 1954).The payoff for such satisfacti<strong>on</strong> and fulfillment is a sense of pers<strong>on</strong>alwholeness and well being (Maslow 1962; Rogers 1962). The failure tosecure basic needs and self-enhancing aspirati<strong>on</strong>s leads to a sense ofdisease and despair, which, in turn, gives rise to activities, bothdestructive and productive, aimed at reducing such disease anddespair. My existential theory represents an attempt to understandand account for destructive patterns of drug use within the frameworkof existential psychology (Greaves 1974).Ever since the 1920s. clinicians and researchers studying drugdependentand drug-dysfuncti<strong>on</strong>al pers<strong>on</strong>s have commented <strong>on</strong> thepathological pers<strong>on</strong>ality patterns of such individuals and have offeredvarious tax<strong>on</strong>omies to describe the range of pers<strong>on</strong>ality disorders seen.This <strong>line</strong> of speculati<strong>on</strong> received a major boost with the publicati<strong>on</strong> ofPescor’s work in 1943, based <strong>on</strong> a very large sample of drug-addictedpers<strong>on</strong>s at the then new Federal narcotics rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> center inLexingt<strong>on</strong> (Pescor 1943a).The prevailing impressi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>e gathers from a reading of this literatureis that certain individuals, as a result of aberrant or unhealthy pers<strong>on</strong>alities,represent high risks for drug dependency if they areexposed to certain psychoactive drugs. In other words, in any Nsample of individuals under identical stimulus c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s, there is notan equal chance that any given individual will become or remain drugdependent. Rather, there are systematic and identifiable pers<strong>on</strong>alityfactors which interact with the drug-taking behavior that leads todependency. This apparent phenomen<strong>on</strong> has traditi<strong>on</strong>ally been called“addicti<strong>on</strong> pr<strong>on</strong>eness” (Gendreau and Gendreau 1970).24


Critics of the noti<strong>on</strong> of addicti<strong>on</strong> pr<strong>on</strong>eness have argued that the verymethods which drug researchers have used have guaranteed the results.Thus, the kinds of people who wind up in pris<strong>on</strong>s, hospitals, anddrug programs to be available for study are exactly those who have ahigher incidence of aberrant pers<strong>on</strong>ality traits: the young, the minorities,the poor. But later studies which have tapped other samples,and studies using matched-sample c<strong>on</strong>trol groups, have tended to quietthe critics. Am<strong>on</strong>g physician addicts, for instance, the familiar elevati<strong>on</strong>in the psychopathic deviancy scale of the Minnesota MultiphasicPers<strong>on</strong>ality Inventory (MMPI) was found, as in other addicts, althoughsuch an elevati<strong>on</strong> in the Pd scale is not typical of physicians in general.Similarly, I found that middle-class adolescents who were drug dependentresembled other adolescents who were hospitalized in a psychiatrichospital but were very unlike their adolescent peers residing in thesame city (Greaves 1971).Those researchers currently working within the area of addicti<strong>on</strong>pr<strong>on</strong>eness are no l<strong>on</strong>ger c<strong>on</strong>tent to document addicti<strong>on</strong> pr<strong>on</strong>eness butare now working <strong>on</strong> specifying the pers<strong>on</strong>ality variables at work inspecific kinds of addicti<strong>on</strong>s, usually defined in terms of the abuser’sdrug of choice. Major distincti<strong>on</strong>s have been drawn, for instance,between the pers<strong>on</strong>alities of those who prefer heroin and those whoprefer amphetamines or barbiturates as drugs of dependency (Greaves,in press; Milkman and Frosch 1973).Although I have been <strong>on</strong>e of the c<strong>on</strong>tributors to the literature <strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>e’sdrug of choice as a functi<strong>on</strong> of pers<strong>on</strong>ality variables, my main interesthas remained with the general phenomen<strong>on</strong> of addicti<strong>on</strong> pr<strong>on</strong>eness.For a clue as to why pers<strong>on</strong>s come to abuse drugs, I first turned tothe phenomen<strong>on</strong> of mind-altering or mood-altering drug-use behavior,of which abuse is an extensi<strong>on</strong>.William James was the first to state explicitly and explore the existenceof altered states of c<strong>on</strong>sciousness within the Western phenomenalisttraditi<strong>on</strong>. Writing in the Principles of Psychology, James observes:Our normal c<strong>on</strong>sciousness, rati<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>sciousness as we callit, is but <strong>on</strong>e special type of c<strong>on</strong>sciousness, whilst all aboutit, parted from it by the flimsiest of screens, there liepotential forms of c<strong>on</strong>sciousness entirely different.(James 1890)While James fell short of stating that individuals have an innate driveto experience these altered states, he did state that the popularity ofalcohol derived from its ability to stimulate such states:It is the power of alcohol to stimulate the mystical c<strong>on</strong>sciousnessthat has made it such an important substance in man’shistory.(James 1907)It remained for Andrew Weil,James’ hypothesis explicitly:another Harvard physician, to stateIt is my belief that the desire to alter c<strong>on</strong>sciousness periodicallyis an innate, normal drive analogous to hunger or thesexual drive.(Weil 1972)25


If James’ hypothesis is true--that there are naturally existing alternativestates of c<strong>on</strong>sciousness, and it seems almost certain that thereare--then several hypotheses seem readily to follow:1. Such alternative states serve an adaptive purpose to the organism.2. It is natural to pursue such states (Weil 1972).3. Children, due to their relative lack of rati<strong>on</strong>al enculturati<strong>on</strong>, aremore readily in touch with some of these states (Fraiberg 1959;Weil 1972).4. The use of drugs is <strong>on</strong>e way to facilitate access to these states(Weil 1972).I would further hypothesize that--1. Some adolescents and adults are less able to access altered states ofc<strong>on</strong>sciousness due to intervening anxiety states and other pathologicalstates;2. Such pers<strong>on</strong>s make use of drugs bey<strong>on</strong>d the motive of accessingsuch states, using them rather to restore themselves to a state ofbeing by which they are able to access both usual and alternatestates;3. The taking of drugs in an attempt to rectify an abnormal state ofpers<strong>on</strong>ality is a form of automedicati<strong>on</strong>, and forms the cornerst<strong>on</strong>eof all drug dependency; and4. If pers<strong>on</strong>s could access altered states to a more normal degree,i.e., in the ways pers<strong>on</strong>s with normal pers<strong>on</strong>alities do, they mightuse drugs, but would not abuse (be dependent <strong>on</strong>) them.The automedicati<strong>on</strong> hypothesis is, of course, not new (Wahl 1967).Alcoholics have been thought by many to be “treating” themselveschemically for depressi<strong>on</strong>, heroin addicts have been described as“numbing” emoti<strong>on</strong>al pain, and so forth.What characterizes the theory proposed here is the specific range ofvariables believed to lie at the pers<strong>on</strong>ality and emoti<strong>on</strong>al core of allsubstance abusers. These variables were derived from three sets ofempirical observati<strong>on</strong>s. As originally set forth, these were as follows:The first observati<strong>on</strong> is that drug-dependent pers<strong>on</strong>s seemto have fundamentally disturbed sex lives. They are frigid,impotent, indifferent, prudish, angry, or resentful c<strong>on</strong>cerningsex. Whatever their particular disturbance, sex is not agreat or reliable source of pleasure. For many it is franklydysphoric. Furthermore, this lack of sexual enjoymentseems to predate the period of drug dependence and iscertainly aggravated by drug use. Am<strong>on</strong>g humans, I havecome to suspect that drug dependence does not supersedesexual pleasure--it replaces it (Bell and Trethowan 1961).(Greaves 1972)The sec<strong>on</strong>d of my observati<strong>on</strong>s has been that drug-dependentpers<strong>on</strong>s as a group do not know how to play--at least notwithout their drug. Very few things hold interest in the26


straight world; almost nothing is seen as exciting. Theyoften appear jaded and disinterested in anything aroundthem that does not directly relate to the drug life style.They have lost c<strong>on</strong>tact with their natural child within them,and with it their sp<strong>on</strong>taneity, creativity, and joy.The third observati<strong>on</strong>, and this may be the primary factor<strong>on</strong> which the other two are based, is that drug-dependentpers<strong>on</strong>s seem to be remarkably out of touch with pleasurablesomatic feedback. Alcohol-dependent pers<strong>on</strong>s are observedto drink massively more alcohol than n<strong>on</strong>dependent pers<strong>on</strong>sas a functi<strong>on</strong> of their blocking the pleasurable effects ofalcohol in low doses. Because of this, they are less able topace themselves as drinkers. Whether this lack of somaticfeedback is due to some physiological deficiency whichrequires higher dosages of the drug to obtain arousal, orwhether there are specific psychodynamics at work is anothermoot point, but an empirical <strong>on</strong>e. My own work str<strong>on</strong>glysuggests that there are chiefly psychological and attitudinalfactors at work. Whatever the case, if pers<strong>on</strong>s who aredrug dependent, or who become drug dependent, are,indeed, out of touch with primary somatic feedback whichother people would experience as pleasure, this may be thereas<strong>on</strong> that they do not enjoy sex or play--there is simplynothing in it for them.(Greaves 1974)In summary, “pers<strong>on</strong>s who become drug dependent are those who aremarkedly lacking in pleasurable sensory awareness, who have lost thechild-like ability to create natural euphoria through active play, includingrecreati<strong>on</strong>al sex, and who, up<strong>on</strong> experimentati<strong>on</strong> with drugs, tendto employ these agents in large quantities as a passive means ofeuphoria, or at least as a means of removing some of the pain andanxiety attending a humorless, dysphoric life style” (Greaves 1974).Based <strong>on</strong> this work and subsequent clinical experience which tends toc<strong>on</strong>firm it, I have been an outspoken critic of drug-treatment programsbased <strong>on</strong> asceticism, privati<strong>on</strong>, and harsh behavioral treatment. Suchprograms, by their nature, tend to promote dependence <strong>on</strong> passiveforms of euphoria, undermining the very purpose for which they wereallegedly designed. As originally put:The therapeutic implicati<strong>on</strong>s of this present set of c<strong>on</strong>tenti<strong>on</strong>sare clear. If we are to minimize drug dependence, we needto teach drug-dependent pers<strong>on</strong>s to turn themselves <strong>on</strong> as asubstitute for the euphoria-producing properties of drugs,and to relax in order to replace the anxiety-reducing effectsof drugs. The reas<strong>on</strong> our present methods of treating drugdependence are failing so miserably is that we are bothmaking unreas<strong>on</strong>able demands <strong>on</strong> our clients and focusing <strong>on</strong>the wr<strong>on</strong>g things. Our major unreas<strong>on</strong>able demand is thatwe want a pers<strong>on</strong> to give up something that gives him pleasureand/or relieves distress, while offering little in returnexcept vague, distant promises of a better life and improvedself-esteem. As to focusing <strong>on</strong> the wr<strong>on</strong>g things, we areheaded in precisely the wr<strong>on</strong>g directi<strong>on</strong> in drug programming:toward asceticism, which emphasizes good behavior andde-emphasizes the importance of pleasurable feelings, thusunwittingly encouraging passive-dependence <strong>on</strong> chemical27


sources of pleasure; and away from humanism, whichemphasizes the importance of pleasurable experience and issuspicious of passive-dependence <strong>on</strong> drugs. We seem tohave drawn the absolutely backward c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> about thedrug addicted pers<strong>on</strong> that he is an actively hed<strong>on</strong>istic,pleasure-seeking, turn-<strong>on</strong> freak when he never was that.What he was and is is a chr<strong>on</strong>ically uptight individual whoexperiences great difficulty securing his need for pleasure inways that others do.We emphasize the importance of the drug dependent pers<strong>on</strong>’sacquiring a job as a c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> of his rehabilitati<strong>on</strong>, whenvery little evidence supports the c<strong>on</strong>tenti<strong>on</strong> that having ajob is a decisive element in successful withdrawal from drugs.Instead of c<strong>on</strong>ceiving of drugs as the enemy and seeing drugabstinence as a great struggle against the enemy, to behopefully brought about through great striving and strictlyregimented behavior, we need to adopt a human growth andneed-fulfillment model. We need to help pers<strong>on</strong>s to becomethe agents of their pleasure, not the passive recipients. Weneed to provide body-sensory awareness programs, meditati<strong>on</strong>,expressive art therapy, psychotherapy. We need toturn our clients <strong>on</strong> to music, dancing, fishing, camping,boating, photography, and sex. . . . We need to helpclients to realize that not <strong>on</strong>ly is it all right to pursueactively a wide range of pleasurable experiences, but howto. Yet n<strong>on</strong>e of the five major treatment modalities overviewedby Ball (1972)--a) detoxificati<strong>on</strong>, b) maintenance,c) individual and group psychotherapy, d) therapeuticcommunities, and e) religious communities--effectively, inand of themselves, come to grips with the dysphoric underlayof drug dependence. 1 (Greaves 1974)During the past several years, drug abuse treatment programers,using these and other ideas, have placed increasing emphasis <strong>on</strong>“alternatives” to drug-abusing behavior. The jury is still out asregards the outcome benefits of this approach, though preliminaryresults are encouraging.SPECIAL POPULATIONSAs a general theory of drug dependence, the existential theory doesnot deal with special risk populati<strong>on</strong>s except to comment that inherentin special subpopulati<strong>on</strong>s are the factors that give rise to pers<strong>on</strong>alitymaldevelopment, situati<strong>on</strong>al stress pathology, or unusual opportunity(such as availability or peer support), which give rise to abuse.1 Reprinted with permissi<strong>on</strong> from G. Greaves. “Toward an ExistentialTheory of <strong>Drug</strong>-Dependence,” Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease,159(1974):263-274. Copyright © 1974 by The Williams & Wilkins Co.,Baltimore, Md.28


An Ego/Self Theory ofSubstance DependenceA C<strong>on</strong>temporary PsychoanalyticPerspectiveEdward J. Khantzian, M.D.INTRODUCTION<strong>Drug</strong> dependence is tied intimately to an individual’s attempt to copewith his or her internal emoti<strong>on</strong>al and external social and physicalenvir<strong>on</strong>ment. Viewed from a c<strong>on</strong>temporary psychoanalytic perspective,drug dependency can best be understood by examining how such apers<strong>on</strong>’s ego organizati<strong>on</strong> and sense of self serve or fail the individual’sattempts to cope, and how the specific effects of various substancesfacilitate or impede such attempts.Although early psychoanalytic investigators appreciated the presence ofunderlying depressi<strong>on</strong>, tensi<strong>on</strong>, and distress in addicts, most of theearly psychoanalytic formulati<strong>on</strong>s of substance dependence emphasizedthe instinctive, pleasurable aspects of drug use to explain the compellingnature of addicti<strong>on</strong> (Yorke 1970; Khantzian 1974; Khantzian andTreece 1977). More recent psychoanalytic formulati<strong>on</strong>s have placedgreater emphasis <strong>on</strong> problems in adaptati<strong>on</strong>, ego and self disturbances,and related psychopathology as etiological factors in drug dependence(Krystal and Raskin 1970; Wurmser 1974; and Khantzian 1978).A variety of drug-use patterns and degrees of dependence in whicheveryday problems of living are involved may be identified (Khantzianet al. 1974). Nevertheless, I have become c<strong>on</strong>vinced, as has Wurmser(1974), that becoming and remaining addicted to drugs is in mostinstances associated with severe and significant psychopathology.Necessarily, some of the observed pathology evident in addicts is theresult of drug use and its attendant interpers<strong>on</strong>al involvements (Zinberg1975; Mirin et al. 1976; Khantzian and Treece 1979). However, it ismy opini<strong>on</strong> that drug-dependent individuals are predisposed to use andto become dependent up<strong>on</strong> their substances mainly as a result ofsevere ego impairments and disturbances in the sense of self, involvingdifficulties with drive and affect defense, self-care, dependency, and29


need satisfacti<strong>on</strong>. Hence, my theoretical work has focused <strong>on</strong> theseimpairments and disturbances in the ego and the sense of self.ADAPTATION AND DRUG USEIn <strong>on</strong>e of our first papers <strong>on</strong> substance dependence (Khantzian et al.1974), we explored the relati<strong>on</strong>ship of heroin use to a range of humanproblems, including pain, stress, and dysphoria. In attempting toadapt to <strong>on</strong>e’s emoti<strong>on</strong>s and envir<strong>on</strong>ment, the powerful acti<strong>on</strong> of heroinand immersi<strong>on</strong> in the attendant rituals and subculture could be used tomute, extinguish, and avoid a range of feelings and emoti<strong>on</strong>s. Thatis, rather than settling for more ordinary defensive, neurotic, characterological,or other adaptive mechanisms as a way of dealing withdistress, heroin addicts had adopted a more extraordinary soluti<strong>on</strong> byusing a powerful drug and immersing themselves in the associatedrituals, practices, and pseudoculture. In this early report, we stressedthe costly c<strong>on</strong>sequences of the heroin involvement and why the addictwas so desperately dependent <strong>on</strong> the drug, that is, “the central problemfor most people who have become addicted to opiates is that theyhave failed to develop effective symptomatic, characterologic, or otheradaptive soluti<strong>on</strong>s in resp<strong>on</strong>se to developmental crises, stress, deprivati<strong>on</strong>,and other forms of emoti<strong>on</strong>al pain which may not in themselves beextraordinary. Their resp<strong>on</strong>se has been to revert repeatedly to theuse of opiates as an all powerful device, thereby precluding othersoluti<strong>on</strong>s that would normally develop and that might better sustainthem” (p. 164).AGGRESSION AND HEROIN DEPENDENCEIn c<strong>on</strong>trast to a general sense that heroin could be used to deal with arange of human emoti<strong>on</strong>s and troubles, I also quickly became impressedwith a rather specific reas<strong>on</strong> why opiates could be so appealing tomany heroin addicts. From the outset of my clinical-investigative workwith drug dependency, I was immediately impressed with the enormous,lifel<strong>on</strong>g difficulties heroin addicts had with feelings and impulsesassociated with aggressi<strong>on</strong>. In repeated life histories obtained fromaddicts, I was impressed with how dysphoric feelings associated withanger, rage, and restlessness were relieved in the short term byheroin and other opiates. This was even more apparent when observingaddicts in treatment as they became stabilized <strong>on</strong> methad<strong>on</strong>e and theiraggressi<strong>on</strong> and restlessness subsided. I began to suspect that heroinaddicts might be using opiates specifically as an antiaggressi<strong>on</strong> drug.As a result of these initial impressi<strong>on</strong>s, I published a preliminaryreport (1972) and subsequently expanded and formulated a hypothesis(Khantzian 1974) which proposed that problems with aggressi<strong>on</strong> predisposedcertain individuals to opiate dependence and was central in thedevelopment and maintenance of an addicti<strong>on</strong>. I emphasized how addictstook advantage of the antiaggressi<strong>on</strong> acti<strong>on</strong> of opiates in the service ofdrive defense. I stressed the disorganizing influence of aggressi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>ego functi<strong>on</strong>s in individuals whose ego stability was already subject todysfuncti<strong>on</strong> and impairment as a result of developmental arrest orregressi<strong>on</strong>. I also proposed that the same but sustained, l<strong>on</strong>gerantiaggressi<strong>on</strong> acti<strong>on</strong> of methad<strong>on</strong>e was the basis for “success” ofmethad<strong>on</strong>e maintenance.30


SELF (NARCISSISTIC) PATHOLOGYOver the past decade, c<strong>on</strong>siderable attenti<strong>on</strong> has been focused <strong>on</strong> selfpathology. In c<strong>on</strong>trast to ego pathology, in which the emphasis is <strong>on</strong>disturbance in structure and functi<strong>on</strong> in coping with drives and emoti<strong>on</strong>s,self pathology relates more to troubled attitudes and experiencesabout the self and others. Kohut (1971) and Kernberq (1975) haveexplored how disrupti<strong>on</strong>s and disturbances in a pers<strong>on</strong>’s early development,particularly around nurturance and dependency needs, lead toself pathology in adult life. Both investigators c<strong>on</strong>sider substancedependencies as manifestati<strong>on</strong>s of such disorders, although neitherKohut nor Kernberg has systematically explored this relati<strong>on</strong>ship. Anumber of investigators have attempted to relate this recent betterunderstanding of narcissistic processes and disturbances to substancedependence. Reports by Wieder and Kaplan (1969), Wurmser (1974),and Krystal and Raskin (1970) have stressed narcissistic vulnerabilitiesand decompensati<strong>on</strong> as predisposing factors. Wurmser, in particular,has emphasized how drugs are used to counteract the distress anddysphoria associated with decompensated narcissistic states.In my own psychotherapeutic work with addicts, I became interested insome of the unique and characteristic traits of compensated addicts(i.e., addicts who were either drug free or <strong>on</strong> drug maintenance) thatare related to underlying narcissistic processes and disturbances, andhow such traits might predispose an individual to drug dependence. Irepeatedly observed the addict’s special problems in accepting dependencyand actively acknowledging and pursuing goals and satisfacti<strong>on</strong>srelated to needs and wants. Extreme and alternating patterns inpursuing need satisfacti<strong>on</strong> were evident: Cooperati<strong>on</strong> and compliancemight suddenly alternate with outbursts of rage, refusal, or resistance;passivity and indifference could shift rapidly or coexist with active,intense, and restless involvements that often led to danger, violence,and death; disavowal of needs and solicitousness of others mightsuddenly c<strong>on</strong>vert to angry demands and an entitlement that was totallyoblivious of other people.To explain such patterns, I proposed that the rigid character traitsand alternating defenses employed by addicts were adopted againstunderlying needs and dependency in order to maintain a costly psychologicalequilibrium. Prominent defenses and traits included extremerepressi<strong>on</strong>, disavowal, self-sufficiency, activity, and assumpti<strong>on</strong> ofaggressive attitudes. I c<strong>on</strong>cluded that “defenses (and the associatedcharacter traits) are employed in the service of c<strong>on</strong>taining a wholerange of l<strong>on</strong>gings and aspirati<strong>on</strong>s, but particularly those related todependency and nurturance needs. It is because of massive repressi<strong>on</strong>of these needs that such individuals feel cut off, hollow and empty . . .[and that the] . . . addicts’ inability to acknowledge and pursueactively their needs to be admired, and to love and be loved, leavethem vulnerable to reversi<strong>on</strong> to narcotics” (Khantzian 1978, p. 196).SELF-SELECTION AND THE SPECIFICAPPEAL OF HEROINMost substance-dependent individuals prefer and self-select a particulardrug. This preference and selecti<strong>on</strong> is the result of the drug of31


choice and its distinctive psychopharmacologic effects interacting withthe unique pers<strong>on</strong>ality organizati<strong>on</strong> and reactive patterns of an individual.It is this interacti<strong>on</strong> between drug effect and pers<strong>on</strong>ality organizati<strong>on</strong>that predisposes a pers<strong>on</strong> to dependency <strong>on</strong> a particular drug.The specific appeal of opiates, stimulants, sedative-hypnotic drugs(including alcohol), and other drugs has been explored from a psychodynamicperspective (Wieder and Kaplan 1969; Wurmser 1974; Milkmanand Frosch 1973; Khantzian 1975). Wieder and Kaplan, and others,c<strong>on</strong>tinue to stress the regressive and pleasurable ego states producedby these drugs (including opiates) to explain their appeal, whileWurmser and I have placed greater emphasis <strong>on</strong> the progressive andadaptive use of drugs. In this respect, I have been particularlyinterested in the narcotic addict’s preference for opiates. As alreadyindicated, my early work with heroin addicts led me to c<strong>on</strong>clude thatthe compelling nature of opiates for many narcotic addicts resides in aspecific antiaggressi<strong>on</strong> acti<strong>on</strong> of narcotics, namely, to relieve andcounteract regressed, disorganized, and dysphoric ego states relatedto overwhelming feelings of rage, anger, and related depressi<strong>on</strong>.Whereas the use of drugs such as the amphetamines and hypnotics(including alcohol) results in the mobilizati<strong>on</strong> and expressi<strong>on</strong> of aggressiveand sexual impulses, opiates have the opposite effect. This effectis particularly needed and welcomed in certain individuals whose egomechanisms of defense, particularly against aggressive drives, areshaky or absent. <strong>On</strong> close examinati<strong>on</strong>, we have been impressedrepeatedly that the so-called “high” or euphoria produced by opiates ismore correctly a relief of dysphoria associated with unmitigated aggressi<strong>on</strong>.The short-term effect of the drug is to reverse regresseddysphoric ego states by muting and c<strong>on</strong>taining otherwise unc<strong>on</strong>trollablerage and aggressi<strong>on</strong> (Khantzian 1972, 1974, 1978).SELF-CARE DISTURBANCESThe previous secti<strong>on</strong>s have focused <strong>on</strong> how drug addicts attempt touse drugs adaptively to overcome and cope with ego and self problems.In this final secti<strong>on</strong> I would like to focus <strong>on</strong> a more obvious maladaptiveaspect of drug use.The influences of early psychoanalysis are evident in “id” formulati<strong>on</strong>sof addicti<strong>on</strong>s that invoke and presuppose the existence of unc<strong>on</strong>sciousdeath wishes and self-destructive trends (death instincts) to accountfor the destructiveness and dangers associated with drug dependence.Clearly, certain individuals are driven or are compelled to be selfdestructive,with suicide the most extreme manifestati<strong>on</strong> of such acompulsi<strong>on</strong>. Indeed, it has been suggested rather cynically by somethat drug dependence and abuse is a form of suicide <strong>on</strong> the installmentplan. Menninger (1938) is representative in presenting such a pointof view, referring to such behavior as “chr<strong>on</strong>ic suicide.” The psychologyof c<strong>on</strong>scious and unc<strong>on</strong>scious human destructiveness is complexand may well be a comp<strong>on</strong>ent in the destructive aspects of substancedependence. However, in my experience, many of the self-destructiveaspects of drug dependence represent failures in ego functi<strong>on</strong>s involvingself-care and self-protecti<strong>on</strong>.Self-care functi<strong>on</strong>s originate and are established in early phases ofhuman development. They become internalized as a result of andthrough the ministrati<strong>on</strong>s of the caring and protective role of theparents, particularly the mother. If optimal, children gradually32


incorporate a capacity to care for themselves and to protect againstand anticipate harm and danger. Extremes of indulgence and deprivati<strong>on</strong>may do injury to the individual’s developing ego and sense of selfaround vital functi<strong>on</strong>s of self-preservati<strong>on</strong> and care, and may leaveindividuals vulnerable to a whole range of hazards and dangers, notthe least of which is the use of dangerous drugs.Self-care as an ego functi<strong>on</strong> is complex. It is probably the result of anumber of comp<strong>on</strong>ent functi<strong>on</strong>s and defenses such as signal anxiety,reality testing, judgment, c<strong>on</strong>trol, and synthesis, and when impaired,such defenses as denial, justificati<strong>on</strong>, projecti<strong>on</strong>, etc. We are allsubject to our instincts, drives, and impulses, and if they are expressedindiscriminately, we are subject to hazard and danger. Most of uscheck ourselves more or less and automatically exercise cauti<strong>on</strong>, or weare appropriately worried and fearful of the prospects of danger orhazardous involvements. Such checking or cauti<strong>on</strong>ary resp<strong>on</strong>ses arean integral part of our ego mechanisms of defense. However, it isexactly in this regard that addicts are deficient in their ego.These are problems that I c<strong>on</strong>sider to be related to self-care(ego) functi<strong>on</strong>s that are impaired, deficient or absent in somany of the addicts we see. The problems with self-careand regulati<strong>on</strong> are apparent in their past histories (predatingtheir addicti<strong>on</strong>) by a high incidence of preventable medicaland dental problems, accidents, fights, violent behavior anddelinquent behavioral problems. Their impaired self-carefuncti<strong>on</strong>s are also evident in relati<strong>on</strong> to their drug/alcoholproblems, where despite obvious deteriorati<strong>on</strong> and imminentdanger as a result of their substance use, there is littleevidence of fear, anxiety or realistic assessment about theirsubstance involvement. <strong>On</strong>e might correctly argue that inthis latter instance, the lack of self-care is sec<strong>on</strong>dary toregressi<strong>on</strong> as a result of prol<strong>on</strong>ged substance use. Althoughthis is probably true, we have been impressed with thepresence and persistence of these described tendencies insuch individuals both prior to becoming addicted and subsequentto becoming detoxified and stabilized.”(Khantzian 1978, p. 193)33


A General Theory of Addicti<strong>on</strong>to Opiate-Type <strong>Drug</strong>sAlfred R. Lindesmith, Ph.D.I formulated my theory of addicti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the basis of an investigati<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>e in Chicago during the years 1934-35 by, at first, observing andinterviewing Chicago street addicts. Approximately 50 addicts wereinterviewed repeatedly over a period of a number of m<strong>on</strong>ths, and someothers were c<strong>on</strong>tacted <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>ce or a few times. I did not c<strong>on</strong>sult theliterature <strong>on</strong> the subject until I had developed a preliminary hypothesis.<str<strong>on</strong>g>Theories</str<strong>on</strong>g> prevalent at the time were generally unsatisfactory, seemingto reflect the ideological commitments and training of their authorsrather than the evidence. Most claimed to apply <strong>on</strong>ly to limited populati<strong>on</strong>s,making it impossible to prove them false by citing negativeevidence since such instances were written off in advance. I beganmy study with the assumpti<strong>on</strong> that a scientific theory of addicti<strong>on</strong>ought to be generally applicable regardless of whether the addict wasa physician, a medical patient, or a street derelict from the urbanslums. It also was assumed that the theory should be applicable nomatter how the drug was taken and that it should apply to addicti<strong>on</strong> inearlier centuries and in countries other than the United States.After I entertained a few preliminary hypotheses and rejected themwhen negative evidence was found, I reached a c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cerningthe dominant and basic characteristics of addicti<strong>on</strong>--the causal processthat produces the powerful craving for opiates. When I sought negativeevidence or excepti<strong>on</strong>s to this c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> and its implicati<strong>on</strong>s, I failedto find them. Instead, it seemed to me that the theory made sense ofwhat had at first seemed like a chaotic jigsaw puzzle filled with paradoxesand inc<strong>on</strong>sistencies.In brief, the theory I formulated is that opiate drug users develop thecraving, or become addicted or “hooked,” after physical dependencehas been established, in the process of using the drug to alleviate thewithdrawal distress that begins to appear several hours after the lastdose, provided that the user correctly identifies and understandsthese symptoms (Lindesmith 1947).After I had formulated this hypothesis and was checking and workingout its implicati<strong>on</strong>s in interviews with users and by c<strong>on</strong>sulting the34


scientific literature, I stumbled <strong>on</strong> the same c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> stated by aprominent German investigator, A. Erlenmeyer, in 1926. (See references.)Being interested mainly in physiological aspects and themedical treatment of addicts, Erlenmeyer did not develop this statementin detail as a theory but simply stated it as a fact and passed <strong>on</strong> toother matters.Noting and documenting the organic effects that occur as morphine isused <strong>on</strong> a regular daily basis, Erlenmeyer describes the process as a“reversal.” He adds:The morphine originally foreign to the body, becomes anintrinsic part of the body, as the uni<strong>on</strong> between it and thebrain cells keeps growing str<strong>on</strong>ger; it then acquires thesignificance and efffectiveness of a heart t<strong>on</strong>ic, of an indispensableelement of nutriti<strong>on</strong> and subsistence, of a means forcarrying <strong>on</strong> the business of the entire organism. . . .(Cited in Terry and Pellens 1928, pp. 601-602)He describes the withdrawal syndrome that occurs after the reversal ofeffects has taken place as a “host of painful sensati<strong>on</strong>s, intolerablefeelings, oppressive organic disturbances of every sort, combined withan extreme psychic excitement, intense restlessness, and persistentinsomnia.” He then remarks:In such moments the craving for morphine is born andrapidly becomes insatiable, because the patient has learnedthat these terrible symptoms are banished as if by magic bya sufficiently large dose of morphine.(Cited in Terry and Pellens 1928, pp. 601-602)The cognitive feature of my theory, which is also implicit in Erlenmeyer’sstatement, is designed to explain how it happens that medical patientsrelatively rarely become addicted even when opiates are administered<strong>on</strong> a regular daily basis for prol<strong>on</strong>ged periods sufficient to establishphysical dependence. It is widely recognized in medical practice thatin the administrati<strong>on</strong> of such addicting drugs, keeping patients inignorance or deceiving them about the identity of the drug are effectivetactics in preventing subsequent use. If withdrawal symptoms occur,they may be explained to the patient as symptoms of a disease, asside-effects of other medicati<strong>on</strong>, and so <strong>on</strong>. If a patient who has beenattracted to the effects of morphine that has been regularly administeredis deceived into the belief that the drug was strychnine or arsenic, heor she will lose interest in it.Similar c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s also apply to the fact that physical dependencein very young children, such as occurs in infants born of addictedmothers, apparently never produces addicti<strong>on</strong>. In India, a lower castecustom that involved keeping very young children quiet by providingthem with opium often produced physical dependence. The drug wasusually withdrawn by the age of five. No addicti<strong>on</strong> appears to haveresulted from this practice, and there was no c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong> observedbetween it and adult use.An important and often overlooked aspect of opiate effects that is basicto the theory and that is str<strong>on</strong>gly emphasized by Erlenmeyer is thechanges in these effects that take place gradually during the progressi<strong>on</strong>from initial use <strong>on</strong> a regular basis to the point of physical dependence.Disregarding a few unpleasant effects following from the first35


few doses, initial effects may be described as depressant and areperceived by the recipient as generally pleasant in that they relievepain and discomfort and produce a feeling of relaxati<strong>on</strong> and well being.It is these first effects and the impact of a dose that are spoken of asthe “high” or “rush” by addicts. As usage c<strong>on</strong>tinues, these euphoriceffects become progressively briefer in durati<strong>on</strong> and harder to obtain.The original sedative effect gives way to and is replaced by an oppositeor stimulating effect as the drug begins gradually to be used mainly toalleviate withdrawal distress. Organic changes are of a parallel nature.The first injecti<strong>on</strong> creates abnormal bodily changes which tend toreturn to normal as bodily adaptati<strong>on</strong> occurs. When the latter processis complete, bodily abnormalities occur when the drug is withdrawnand return roughly to “normal” when another dose is taken. In thissituati<strong>on</strong> the user feels approximately normal between shots but stillhas the solace of brief euphoric episodes at the time of injecti<strong>on</strong>, thesebecoming progressively more difficult to achieve as use c<strong>on</strong>tinues.This reversal of effects creates some important logical problems andparadoxes for the theorist. If initial euphoric effects are said to bethe key factor, <strong>on</strong>e may ask why addicts seem so miserable and sopr<strong>on</strong>e to suicide. If euphoria is the addict’s goal, an obvious way tomaximize it would be to stop regular use and, instead, use the drugepisodically--say , every other day. This would unquesti<strong>on</strong>ably reducecosts, risks, and misery generally and would also permit the user toenjoy the “high” for c<strong>on</strong>siderably l<strong>on</strong>ger time periods. <strong>On</strong>e might alsow<strong>on</strong>der why, after the user has experienced the miseries and frustrati<strong>on</strong>sof addicti<strong>on</strong>, she or he does not kick the habit and take up aeuphoria-producing drug that does not produce physical dependence,like cocaine or marijuana.Since the proposed theory does not view the euphoric effects of opiatesas the key factor in addicti<strong>on</strong>, these c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s are not an embarrassmentto it. From this standpoint <strong>on</strong>e may describe the initialperiod of use as the stage at which the user learns to like the drug,and subsequent use, to c<strong>on</strong>trol withdrawal after the reversal of initialeffects, as the stage in which she or he learns to love it.The proposed theory has been corroborated in a variety of ways whichcannot all be dealt with here. Two of these will be briefly indicated.Since there are addicts who have become physically dependent <strong>on</strong> anopiate before the sequence of regular use that made them addicts, it isrelevant to the theory to ask how they escaped addicti<strong>on</strong> in theirearlier experience. The theory implies that they must have beenignorant of what was happening to them, and this was borne out inevery instance of this sort that came to my attenti<strong>on</strong> from interviewsor from the literature. <strong>On</strong>e such addict simply said, “I was hookedand didn’t know it.”The sec<strong>on</strong>d corroborati<strong>on</strong>, of a partial nature, has to do with the factthat, if <strong>on</strong>e deletes the cognitive feature of the theory, it may becalled <strong>on</strong>e of negative reinforcement and fitted into the pattern ofc<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing and reinforcement theory of psychology. It was adaptedin this way by an experimental psychologist and tested with rats(Nichols 1963, 1965). It was c<strong>on</strong>firmed in the sense that rats thatwere made physically dependent <strong>on</strong> morphine by being compelled todrink a morphine soluti<strong>on</strong> with a bitter taste became attached to thisdrink <strong>on</strong>ly when they were permitted to experience relief from36


withdrawal distress after drinking it. These rats also chose the bittermorphine drink in preference to pure water often enough after theyhad become abstinent to reestablish physical dependence. All of theother rats that had been physically dependent <strong>on</strong> morphine but hadhad no experience with the relief from withdrawal retained a verystr<strong>on</strong>g dislike for the water laced with morphine.These findings raise a host of complex issues c<strong>on</strong>cerning the differencesbetween human beings and lower animals that cannot be coveredhere. They illustrate that the theory is experimental and could probablybe tested and improved through experimentati<strong>on</strong> with human subjectsif this were permissible.37


Theory of <strong>Drug</strong> UseHarvey Milkman, Ph.D.William Frosch, M.D.This theoretical approach is based <strong>on</strong> the formulati<strong>on</strong> that disturbancesin the normally expected mastery of phase-specific c<strong>on</strong>flicts duringearly childhood may induce severe “primitive” psychopathologies, theaddicti<strong>on</strong>s being prominent am<strong>on</strong>g these. Failure to cope adequatelywith the rage, overstimulati<strong>on</strong>, and disorganized sensory input of suchexperiences leaves residual sensory overload and disorganizati<strong>on</strong>. Thedrug user is hypothesized to achieve relief via the specific altered egostates induced by psychotropic drugs. The drug of choice will be thepharmacologic agent that proves harm<strong>on</strong>ious with the user’s characteristicmode of reducing stress.Having <strong>on</strong>ce experienced the gratificati<strong>on</strong> of a supportive, drug-inducedpattern of ego functi<strong>on</strong>ing, the user may attempt to repeat this uniquelysatisfying experience for defensive purposes, as a soluti<strong>on</strong> to c<strong>on</strong>flict,or for primary delight. The compulsi<strong>on</strong> to seek out repeatedly aspecial ego state will be related to the individual’s previous needs forthe resoluti<strong>on</strong> of c<strong>on</strong>flict or anxiety. If a particular drug-induced egostate provides a mechanism for easing the discomfort of c<strong>on</strong>flict, anindividual may seek out that particular drug when that c<strong>on</strong>flict isreexperienced. Wikler’s formulati<strong>on</strong>s regarding the selecti<strong>on</strong> of stimulants,depressants, and hallucinogens closely parallel our own, i.e.,chosen substance is related to style of coping with anxiety or stress. 1The user’s drug of choice appears to produce an altered ego statewhich is reminiscent of and may recapture specific phases of earlychild development (e.g., heroin, first year; amphetamine, sec<strong>on</strong>d tothird year).EMPIRICAL FINDINGSWe have provided empirical support for this theory through the c<strong>on</strong>trolledinvestigati<strong>on</strong> of ego functi<strong>on</strong>s in users of heroin or amphetamine.1 A. Wikler. Pers<strong>on</strong>al communicati<strong>on</strong> (cited in Blachly 1970).38


Using Bellak et al.'s (1973) interview and rating scale for ego functi<strong>on</strong>ing,“preferential” users of heroin (N=10) or amphetamine (N=10) wereinterviewed under c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s of abstinence and intoxicati<strong>on</strong> with theirrespectively chosen drugs. Normals (N=10) were interviewed twice whileabstinent. Data were analyzed qualitatively and quantitatively toanswer--(a) How do preferential users differ from normals and eachother under abstinent c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s? (b) How do they differ under c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>sof intoxicati<strong>on</strong>? (c) How does the drug user differ within himselfunder c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s of abstinence and intoxicati<strong>on</strong>? Subjects were white,male, middle class, 20 to 30 years of age, and n<strong>on</strong>psychotic. Doselevels were 15 mg morphine, intramuscular, and 30 mg amphetamine,oral. The purposeful decisi<strong>on</strong> to study preferential users of widelydisparate pharmacologic agents highlighted differential pers<strong>on</strong>alitystructures as well as basic similarities. Although our observati<strong>on</strong>s andfindings derive from our low-dose study of preferential users of heroinand amphetamine, similar investigati<strong>on</strong>s could examine the preferentialuse of other psychoactive agents, e.g., barbiturates and hallucinogens.For the purposes of this presentati<strong>on</strong>, we will discuss <strong>on</strong>ly a porti<strong>on</strong>of our empirical findings. The full data are available elsewhere (Milkmanand Frosch 1973; Frosch and Milkman 1977).Under the abstinent c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, both drug-using populati<strong>on</strong>s showedsubnormal ego-functi<strong>on</strong> ratings in most categories (figure 1). Amphetamineusers showed significantly higher total ego strength than heroinusers, whether or not they were intoxicated. Within groups, egofuncti<strong>on</strong>ing was usually lower in the intoxicated c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> with significantdifferences observed for judgment (amphetamine), regulati<strong>on</strong> andc<strong>on</strong>trol of drives (both groups), and sense of competence (heroin).Although ego functi<strong>on</strong>ing is more adaptive in amphetamine users whenboth groups are in the intoxicated c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, <strong>on</strong>e cannot, unequivocally,extend this finding bey<strong>on</strong>d the laboratory situati<strong>on</strong>. Experimentaldoses of 30 mg and 15 mg for amphetamine and heroin users, respectively,may not be comparable in effect to average “field” doses of 310mg and 100 mg. Even at our reduced doses, however, the resultssuggest a trend, in both groups, for ego functi<strong>on</strong>ing to be negativelyaffected by the utilizati<strong>on</strong> of their respective drugs. It is expectedthat under c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s of higher doses, greater impairment of egofuncti<strong>on</strong>ing may be observed and more significance obtained. Differentialdescripti<strong>on</strong> of selected ego functi<strong>on</strong>s are provided below.Regulati<strong>on</strong> and c<strong>on</strong>trol of drives, affects, and impulses refers to thedirectness of impulse expressi<strong>on</strong> and the effectiveness of delay andc<strong>on</strong>trol mechanisms; the degree of frustrati<strong>on</strong> tolerance; and the extentto which drive derivatives are channeled through ideati<strong>on</strong>, affectiveexpressi<strong>on</strong>, and manifest behavior. Both groups display significantlyless regulati<strong>on</strong> and c<strong>on</strong>trol of drives, affects, and impulses in theintoxicated c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. The significant drug effect for this functi<strong>on</strong> isparticularly interesting because it suggests that under intoxicati<strong>on</strong>both groups might be expected to have less impulse c<strong>on</strong>trol and presenta greater danger to themselves and/or the community. The heroinuser appears as an individual given to sporadic rages, tantrums, orbinges. Periods of overc<strong>on</strong>trol may alternate with flurries of impulsivebreakthroughs. This may be observed dramatically when the uservoluntarily submits himself to extended periods of increased envir<strong>on</strong>mentalstructure, in drug programs, where impulse expressi<strong>on</strong> isminimized. Temporarily the user appears to have adequate impulsec<strong>on</strong>trol. Suddenly and without warning, however, impulses gain theupper hand and the user is seen <strong>on</strong> a self-destructive binge. Disciplinaryacti<strong>on</strong> is taken and <strong>on</strong>ce again impulses are quieted through39


FIGURE 1.–Mean ego functi<strong>on</strong> rating for amphetamlne S’s, heroin S’s,and normals In the abstinent c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> with ratings for sexual and aggressive drive strengthsReprinted with permissi<strong>on</strong> of STASH, Inc.. from H. Milkman and W. Frosch. “The <strong>Drug</strong> of Choice,” Journal of Psychedelic <strong>Drug</strong>s9(Winter 1977):15. Copyright © 1972.


self-regulati<strong>on</strong>, authority, and peer pressures. The cycle tends torepeat.For the amphetamine user, impulse expressi<strong>on</strong> is less direct, pervasive,and frequent. Aggressive behavior is more often verbal thanphysical, and fantasies predominate over unusual behavior. Manifestati<strong>on</strong>sof drive-related fantasies are seen in quasi-artistic producti<strong>on</strong>s,such as “speed freak” drawings, where primitive and threateningfantasies are portrayed. The amphetamine user may sit for hoursdrawing frightened faces, decapitated bodies, and the like.Object relati<strong>on</strong>s takes into account the degree and kind of relatednessto others, the extent to which present relati<strong>on</strong>ships are adaptivelypatterned up<strong>on</strong> older <strong>on</strong>es, and the extent of object c<strong>on</strong>stancy. It isinteresting to note that for heroin users, the obtained mean for thisfuncti<strong>on</strong> was higher in the intoxicated c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. Perhaps in this doserange, heroin tends to reduce anxiety and to allow for a smoother andmore relaxed communicati<strong>on</strong> between people. This noti<strong>on</strong> supportsHartmann’s (1969) observati<strong>on</strong> that “there is an attempt to overcomethe lack of affecti<strong>on</strong>ate and meaningful object relati<strong>on</strong>s through thepseudo-fusi<strong>on</strong> with other drug takers during their comm<strong>on</strong> experience.”The heroin user is generally detached from others while under stressand strives for nurturant relati<strong>on</strong>ships of a dependent nature, leadingto stormy or strained attachments. The amphetamine user, althoughmore successful in object relati<strong>on</strong>s, tends to become involved in relati<strong>on</strong>shipswith str<strong>on</strong>g, unresolved oedipal elements. Castrati<strong>on</strong> fears tendto manifest themselves in unusual and extreme sexual behaviors, suchas D<strong>on</strong> Juanism and homosexuality. Underlying c<strong>on</strong>cerns about masculinityand adequacy are expressed through repetitive sexual activityand a boasting attitude of sexual prowess and potency. Relati<strong>on</strong>shipsmay, however, endure for l<strong>on</strong>g periods of time, although they rarelyhave the stability and sustaining power of the idealized marital situati<strong>on</strong>.Stimulus barrier indicates the subject’s threshold for, sensitivity to,or awareness of stimuli impinging up<strong>on</strong> various sensory modalities; thenature of resp<strong>on</strong>ses to various levels of sensory stimulati<strong>on</strong> in terms ofthe extent of disorganizati<strong>on</strong>, withdrawal, or active coping mechanismsemployed to deal with medium or low stimulus barriers. Amphetamineusers showed significantly higher stimulus barriers than did heroinusers in the abstinent c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. Examinati<strong>on</strong> of the raw data revealedthat 9 of 10 heroin users were rated low. Although it may be arguedthat l<strong>on</strong>g-term involvement with particular drugs may have specificeffects <strong>on</strong> stimulus thresholds, stimulus barrier is c<strong>on</strong>sidered to be themost c<strong>on</strong>stituti<strong>on</strong>ally based ego functi<strong>on</strong> (Bellak et al. 1973). The datasuggest that amphetamine users, with biologically high thresholds forexcitatory stimulati<strong>on</strong>, are seeking homeostasis (equilibrium) throughself-medicati<strong>on</strong>. Amphetamine seems to put the user into closer touchwith envir<strong>on</strong>mental stimuli which might otherwise be unavailable becauseof c<strong>on</strong>stituti<strong>on</strong>ally based, high stimulus barriers. C<strong>on</strong>versely, theheroin user may have a predispositi<strong>on</strong> toward excessive vulnerability toenvir<strong>on</strong>mental stimuli. The user seeks to raise stimulus thresholds,allowing more adaptive functi<strong>on</strong> in a world of relatively painful andextreme stimulati<strong>on</strong>.Aggressive drive strength assesses overt aggressive behavior (frequencyand intensity); associated and substitute aggressive behavior(verbal expressi<strong>on</strong>s, etc.); fantasies and other ideati<strong>on</strong>: dreams,symptoms, defenses, and c<strong>on</strong>trols. The heroin user is seen as an41


individual whose overt acts of aggressi<strong>on</strong> are c<strong>on</strong>siderably more intenseand frequent than average. The occurrence of physical assaultivenessand multiple suicide gestures is comm<strong>on</strong>. Hostile punning and wittyrepartee are often observed. It is speculated that the relative successof residential treatment programs is related to this phenomen<strong>on</strong>.Intensive c<strong>on</strong>fr<strong>on</strong>tati<strong>on</strong> in group therapy (a major treatment modality indrug programs) provides an outlet for excessive aggressive energy.For the amphetamine user, aggressive energy appears to be less excessiveand is channeled more adaptively. Periodic breakthroughs ofviolence occur, but, with the excepti<strong>on</strong> of amphetamine psychosis,these expressi<strong>on</strong>s are usually not as frequent or intense as the heroinuser’s. Fantasies of violence are usually expressed verbally andsometimes find their expressi<strong>on</strong> through identificati<strong>on</strong> with radicalpolitical groups. This finding of greater hostility in heroin addictsthan amphetamine abusers is echoed in a study (Gossop and Roy 1976)using different scales and a different populati<strong>on</strong>.DISCUSSIONAlthough the observati<strong>on</strong>s for this study were made while male userswere under abstinent and somewhat intoxicated c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s, it must berecalled that our subjects had all been heavy drug users for severalyears. It is, therefore, difficult to know if our findings represent afactor in the etiology of the pattern of drug use or the result of suchdrug use and its imposed life patterns. However, quantitative analysesand clinical impressi<strong>on</strong>s provide a framework for c<strong>on</strong>ceptualizing possiblepsychological differences between preferential users of heroin andamphetamine. Some speculate that these differences are related toearly predrug patterns of childhood experiences.The heroin user, who characteristically maintains a tenuous equilibriumvia withdrawal and repressi<strong>on</strong>, bolsters these defenses by pharmacologicallyinducing a state of decreased motor activity, underresp<strong>on</strong>sivenessto external situati<strong>on</strong>s, and reducti<strong>on</strong> of perceptual intake: “. . . [a]state of quiet lethargy . . . [is] . . . c<strong>on</strong>ducive to hypercathectingfantasies of omnipotence, magical wish-fulfillment and self-sufficiency.A most dramatic effect of drive dampening experienced subjectively assatiati<strong>on</strong> may be observed in the loss of libido and aggressi<strong>on</strong> and theappetites they serve.” (Wieder and Kaplan 1969).Our empirical observati<strong>on</strong>s support these formulati<strong>on</strong>s. Under c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>sof low-dose morphine intoxicati<strong>on</strong>, heroin users showed improvedscores for object relati<strong>on</strong>s and sense of reality, suggesting greaterrelaxati<strong>on</strong> and less pressure from the drives. The finding of decreasedlibidinal drive strength points to a dampening of sexual appetite. Thisstyle of coping is reminiscent of the narcissistic regressive phenomen<strong>on</strong>described by Mahler (1967) as an adaptive pattern of the sec<strong>on</strong>d halfof the first year of life. It occurs after the specific tie to the motherhas been established and is an attempt to cope with the disorganizingquality of even her brief absences. It is as if the child must shutout affective and perceptual claims from other sources during themother’s absence. This c<strong>on</strong>cept is c<strong>on</strong>sistent with earlier remarks byFenichel (1945). Addicts are “fixated to a passive-narcissistic aim”where objects are need-fulfilling sources of supply. The oral z<strong>on</strong>e andskin are primary, and self-esteem is dependent <strong>on</strong> supplies of food andwarmth. The drug represents these supplies. Furthermore, heroinusers show intolerance for tensi<strong>on</strong>, pain, and frustrati<strong>on</strong>. <strong>Drug</strong>42


effects partially alleviate these difficulties by reducing the impact ofexternal stimulati<strong>on</strong> through sensory numbing. The specific needgratificati<strong>on</strong> of the passive-narcissistic experience reinforces drugtakingbehavior.Relative to abstinence, however, the intoxicated heroin user shows anoverall decrement in ego functi<strong>on</strong>ing. Regulati<strong>on</strong> and c<strong>on</strong>trol of drives,affects, and impulses and sense of competence were significantly loweredin our experimental situati<strong>on</strong>. Deficiencies in general adaptive strengthand the pressures of physiologic dependency set the groundwork for avicious cycle. The heroin user must rely increasingly <strong>on</strong> a relativelyintact ego to procure drugs and attain satiati<strong>on</strong>. Ultimately, she or heis driven to withdrawal from heroin by the discrepancy between intrapsychicforces and external demands. Hospitalizati<strong>on</strong>, incarcerati<strong>on</strong>,and self-imposed abstinence subserve the user’s need to resolve growingc<strong>on</strong>flicts with reality.In c<strong>on</strong>trast to heroin and other sedative drugs, amphetamines have thegeneral effect of increasing functi<strong>on</strong>al activity. Extended wakefulness,alleviati<strong>on</strong> of fatigue, insomnia, loquacity, and hypomania are am<strong>on</strong>gthe symptoms observed. Subjectively, there is an increase in awarenessof drive feelings and impulse strength as well as heightenedfeelings of self-assertiveness, self-esteem, and frustrati<strong>on</strong> tolerance.Our observati<strong>on</strong>s support most of these generalizati<strong>on</strong>s. Amphetamineintoxicati<strong>on</strong> produced in our subjects elevated scores <strong>on</strong> aut<strong>on</strong>omousfuncti<strong>on</strong>ing and sense of competence. Analysis of interview materialshows subjective experience of heightened perceptual and motor abilityaccompanied by feelings of increased potency and self-regard.As in the case of heroin, the alterati<strong>on</strong>s induced by amphetamineintoxicati<strong>on</strong> are synt<strong>on</strong>ic with the user’s characteristic modes of adaptati<strong>on</strong>.This formulati<strong>on</strong> is in agreement with the observati<strong>on</strong>s of Angristand Cersh<strong>on</strong> (1969) in their study of the effects of large doses (up to50 mg/hour) of amphetamine: “. . . it appears that in any <strong>on</strong>e individual,the behavioral effects tend to be rather c<strong>on</strong>sistent and predictable. . . moreover these symptoms tended to be c<strong>on</strong>sistent with eachpers<strong>on</strong>’s pers<strong>on</strong>ality and style.”Energizing effects of amphetamine serve the user’s needs to feel activeand potent in the face of an envir<strong>on</strong>ment perceived as hostile andthreatening. Massive expenditures of psychic energy are geared todefend against underlying fears of passivity. Wieder and Kaplan(1969) suggest that the earliest precursor to the amphetamine user’smode of adaptati<strong>on</strong> is the “practicing period” described by Mahler(1967). This period “culminates around the middle of the sec<strong>on</strong>d yearin the freely walking toddler seeming to feel at the height of his moodof elati<strong>on</strong>. He appears to be at the peak of his belief in his ownmagical omnipotence which is still to a c<strong>on</strong>siderable extent derived fromhis sense of sharing in his mother’s magic powers.” There is aninvestment of cathexis in “the aut<strong>on</strong>omous apparatuses of the self andthe functi<strong>on</strong>s of the ego; locomoti<strong>on</strong>, percepti<strong>on</strong>, learning.” Oursubjects’ inflated self-value and emphasis <strong>on</strong> perceptual acuity andphysical activity support the noti<strong>on</strong> that amphetamine use is related tospecific premorbid patterns of adaptati<strong>on</strong>. The c<strong>on</strong>sistent finding thatego structures are more adaptive in the amphetamine user than theyare in the heroin user suggests that regressi<strong>on</strong> is to a developmentallymore mature phase of psychosexual development.43


Reich’s (1960) comments <strong>on</strong> the “etiology of compensatory narcissisticinflati<strong>on</strong>” may provide further insight into the pers<strong>on</strong>ality structure ofamphetamine users. “The need for narcissistic inflati<strong>on</strong> arises from astriving to overcome threats to <strong>on</strong>e’s bodily intactness.” Under c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>sof too-frequently repeated early traumatizati<strong>on</strong>s, the primitiveego defends itself via magical denial. “It is not so, I am not helpless,bleeding, destroyed. <strong>On</strong> the c<strong>on</strong>trary, I am bigger and better thanany<strong>on</strong>e else. ” Psychic interest is focused “<strong>on</strong> a compensatory narcissisticfantasy whose grandiose character affirms the denial.” Thehigh-level artistic and political aspirati<strong>on</strong>s witnessed in our subjectsappear to be later developmental derivatives of such infantile fantasiesof omnipotence. Although the amphetamine user subjectively experiencesincrements in functi<strong>on</strong>al capacity and self-esteem, biological andpsychological systems are ultimately drained of their resources. As inthe case of heroin, our study points to an overall decrement in egofuncti<strong>on</strong>ing under the influence of amphetamine. The recurrent disintegrati<strong>on</strong>of mental and physical functi<strong>on</strong>ing is a dramatic manifestati<strong>on</strong>of the amphetamine syndrome.Differences in pers<strong>on</strong>ality structure and functi<strong>on</strong>, such as those wedescribe in preferential users of heroin and amphetamine, provideclues which may permit careful de<strong>line</strong>ati<strong>on</strong> of a variety of treatmentprograms designed to meet the needs of particular groups of drugusers. In accord with the theoretical and empirical formulati<strong>on</strong>s above,an experimental treatment milieu is projected in which drug users arepresented with tangible, n<strong>on</strong>chemical alternatives, allowing for thecrucial reversal from a chemically oriented regimen to a n<strong>on</strong>drug orientati<strong>on</strong>.In the case of heroin, for example, treatment may be gearedtoward replacing previously drug-induced ego states characterized by(1) fantasies of omnipotence and wish fulfillment, (2) dampening ofdrive energies, (3) reducti<strong>on</strong> of external stimulus input, (4) externalregulati<strong>on</strong> of self-esteem (Milkman and Metcalf, in press). Anotherneed-specific treatment approach may be first to diagnose and then totreat differentially users who vary al<strong>on</strong>g the dimensi<strong>on</strong>s of trust anddenial (Burke and Milkman 1978). Referral of preferential drug usersto specialized treatment programs might increase the likelihood that theuser will remain in treatment and that the outcome will be successful.By viewing the problem from the perspective of the drug preferred,we have defined differences between users, but we also note basicsimilarities. An underlying sense of low self-esteem is defended againstby the introducti<strong>on</strong> of a chemically induced altered state of c<strong>on</strong>sciousness.The drug state helps to ward off feelings of helplessness in theface of a threatening envir<strong>on</strong>ment. The pharmacologic effect bolstersthe characteristic defenses deployed to reduce anxiety. <strong>Drug</strong>gedc<strong>on</strong>sciousness appears to be a regressive state which is reminiscent ofand may recapture specific phases of early child development. Thechild-like pattern of behavior is characterized by immediacy of rewardwithout regard for the l<strong>on</strong>g-term, detrimental c<strong>on</strong>sequences of <strong>on</strong>e’sacti<strong>on</strong>s.The parallels and overlap between the drug addicti<strong>on</strong>s and other“addictive processes,” e.g., suicide, promiscuity, cults, crime, etc.,are striking. It is believed that the predominant medical, social, andlegal emphasis <strong>on</strong> substances may obscure fundamental psychosocial andcultural determinants of drug abuse and related problem behavior.The relative failure of c<strong>on</strong>temporary “treatment” in the area of substanceabuse highlights the need for increased understanding through innovativeintegrative channels. Blachly (1970) provides an early model for44


such a broadened scope. He sees drug use as <strong>on</strong>e of a class of “seductivebehaviors” characterized by (1) active participati<strong>on</strong> by the victim,(2) negative attitude toward c<strong>on</strong>structive c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong>, (3) immediacyof reward, (4) potential for l<strong>on</strong>g-term impairment of functi<strong>on</strong>ing.While there is c<strong>on</strong>tinued need for research and theory specific to druginvolvement, e.g., cognitive style and physiologic resp<strong>on</strong>siveness, wesuggest an expanded focus <strong>on</strong> the “addictive processes.” These maybe collectively defined as the progressive or repetitious patterns ofsocioculturally and psychophysically determined seductive behaviors,detrimental to the individual, the society, or both (Milkman 1979).45


An Availability-Pr<strong>on</strong>enessTheory of Illicit <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>Reginald G. Smart, Ph.D.Most simply stated, the availability-pr<strong>on</strong>eness theory of drug abuseinvolves the propositi<strong>on</strong> that drug abuse occurs when a pr<strong>on</strong>e individualis exposed to a high level of availability. It is argued that theavailability of or ease of access to all drugs varies enormously, asdoes pr<strong>on</strong>eness to use of these drugs for social or psychological reas<strong>on</strong>s.Tendencies to use drugs should vary directly with both availabilityand pr<strong>on</strong>eness, and the two should sum to create an “addicti<strong>on</strong> tendency.” This suggests that both availability and pr<strong>on</strong>eness need not behigh for all drug abusers. Where availability is excessively high, thelevel of pr<strong>on</strong>eness required am<strong>on</strong>g users could be lower than in situati<strong>on</strong>sof low availability. Where an individual’s psychological or socialpr<strong>on</strong>eness is very high, he or she may become a drug abuser insituati<strong>on</strong>s in which availability is low. Treatment of drug abusersshould be successful <strong>on</strong>ly where large reducti<strong>on</strong>s are made in availabilityor pr<strong>on</strong>eness. Where relapses occur after treatment they should be insituati<strong>on</strong>s in which a return to earlier levels of availability or pr<strong>on</strong>enessis made. C<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> of drug use should occur whenever availabilityand pr<strong>on</strong>eness remain c<strong>on</strong>stant and acceptable to the drug user orabuser.In general, this two-factor availiability-pr<strong>on</strong>eness theory makes use ofmuch published research, integrating it into propositi<strong>on</strong>s which takeaccount of many of the findings. The theory has some similarities tothe vulnerability-acceptance theory of alcoholism adopted by Jel<strong>line</strong>k(1960) years ago but many differences as well. Unfortunately thetheory has not had a large-scale independent test and has some weaknessesas well as some strengths. The theory attempts to account forinitiati<strong>on</strong>, c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong>, and relapse from drug abuse with <strong>on</strong>ly twofactors. Examinati<strong>on</strong> of the meaning and measurement of these factorsis crucial to the understanding and further development of the theory.AVAILABILITYAt the lowest level of drug availability are the proverbial Robins<strong>on</strong>Crusoe families set disc<strong>on</strong>solately <strong>on</strong> a desert island with no46


pharmaceuticals or plant-origin drugs available. No matter what theirdesires or previous habits there can be no drug abuse. <strong>On</strong>ly availabledrugs can be used. There are many situati<strong>on</strong>s where availability ofdrugs is very great, e.g., in ghettos where heroin and other illicitdrugs are routinely for sale. Opiates are also available to many ruralfarmers living in areas where opium-bearing plants grow, the bestexamples being farmers in Southeast Asia, Turkey, and parts of Mexico.The c<strong>on</strong>cept of availability has several different meanings or facets.Availability refers to the set of physical, social, and ec<strong>on</strong>omic circumstancessurrounding the ease or difficulty of obtaining drugs, especiallywith respect to their costs and the amount of physical effort requiredto obtain them. When costs are high or the effort required is great,the tendency to use drugs will be low but can be overcome by a highlevel of pr<strong>on</strong>eness in the user. Availability may also refer to socialaspects because drugs are more available in some social groups than inothers. In some school, neighborhood, or other social situati<strong>on</strong>s,drugs are used by many if not all of the members. The availability ofany drug, then, for a pers<strong>on</strong> new to this kind of envir<strong>on</strong>ment is fargreater than it would be in a n<strong>on</strong>-drug-using group or in a schoolwhich does not countenance drug users.Availability is also greater in some family situati<strong>on</strong>s than in others. Ithas been frequently noted that heroin addicts usually associate withother addicts, partly in order to keep their supply of drugs. Observati<strong>on</strong>smade in ghetto situati<strong>on</strong>s show that heroin is highly available andthat many young men sample heroin, although few actually becomeaddicts. Those who do tend to drop their n<strong>on</strong>using friends. It isknown (Smart and Fejer 1972; Kandel 1974) that drugs are frequentlyused by more than <strong>on</strong>e member of a family. <strong>Studies</strong> of male drugaddicts show that their spouses tend to use heroin even when they didnot at the time of marriage.Availability may be “perceived” as well as “actual.” Actual availabilitytakes into account the cost of drugs, number of sellers nearby, andthe number of places to buy drugs. Perceived availability involvessubjective estimates of that availability by users or n<strong>on</strong>users. Inpractice, actual availability is unknown, and we must depend up<strong>on</strong>subjective estimates. Research supporting the idea that perceivedavailability was important in predicting drug use came from a study ofhigh school students by Smart (1977). A multivariate analysis foundthat perceived availability was a significant predictor for four of sixdrugs--cannabis, heroin, alcohol, and tobacco, but not LSD or n<strong>on</strong>prescribedtranquilizers.Further support for the crucial importance of availability in drug usecomes from studies of professi<strong>on</strong>al and medical addicts. It is knownthat doctors, nurses, and pharmacists, who come into regular c<strong>on</strong>tactwith drugs in work situati<strong>on</strong>s, have rates of opiate and other addicti<strong>on</strong>smany times greater than other professi<strong>on</strong>als. They tend also to havebetter recovery rates than street addicts.PRONENESSPr<strong>on</strong>eness to drug use or abuse may be of many types. <strong>Studies</strong> haveshown that opiate addicts have numerous psychological problems beforetheir addicti<strong>on</strong> is developed, am<strong>on</strong>g them, impulsivity, psychopathic or47


sociopathic traits, low tolerance for frustrati<strong>on</strong>, weak ego functi<strong>on</strong>s,border<strong>line</strong> schizophrenia (in some cases), depressi<strong>on</strong>, and alienati<strong>on</strong>.Opiate addicti<strong>on</strong> and other types of drug abuse are a coping mechanismfor dealing with these psychological problems. However, another typeof pr<strong>on</strong>eness can also exist, particularly in ghetto situati<strong>on</strong>s. Muchresearch indicates that drug abuse is not merely an escapist activitybut that it offers a chance at a life which is well paid, prestigious,and exciting in comparis<strong>on</strong> to legitimate opportunities. (See Catt<strong>on</strong>and Shain [1976] for a review of this area.) There are some indicati<strong>on</strong>s(Glaser et al. 1971) that typical heroin addicts are especially pr<strong>on</strong>e tothe frustrati<strong>on</strong>s of the ghetto world. Because they have more goalsand aspirati<strong>on</strong>s their failure is more frustrating to them, creating atype of “social psychological” pr<strong>on</strong>eness to heroin addicti<strong>on</strong> which isnot merely of the escapist sort. Many heroin addicts, perhaps inadditi<strong>on</strong> to an escapist motivati<strong>on</strong>, seek a lifestyle with a sense ofpurpose, group bel<strong>on</strong>ging, and excitement. Ghetto dwellers with pooreducati<strong>on</strong>al attainment and poor job prospects have difficulty achievingsuch lifestyles legitimately. Because heroin and other drugs are soavailable, they are pr<strong>on</strong>e to develop an interest in them, use them,and perhaps become addict-dealers. They may, if opportunities existand heroin is not available, become criminals to achieve the same sortof lifestyle.The formulati<strong>on</strong> of pr<strong>on</strong>eness as a seeking of a new lifestyle mayexplain ghetto heroin addicti<strong>on</strong> but is less adequate for explainingprofessi<strong>on</strong>al or medical addicti<strong>on</strong>. In professi<strong>on</strong>al addicti<strong>on</strong> the addictdoes not usually change lifestyle; there is no group bel<strong>on</strong>ging andlittle excitement in obtaining the drug. In such cases, pr<strong>on</strong>eness willbe of the “psychological deficit” sort and based <strong>on</strong> depressi<strong>on</strong>, anxiety,or a sense of frustrati<strong>on</strong> which is “treated” by the drug. As statedabove, the level of pr<strong>on</strong>eness required for professi<strong>on</strong>als to becomeaddicts should be low given the high level of availability to which theyare exposed.STRENGTHS OF THE THEORYThe two-factor availability-pr<strong>on</strong>eness theory has a number of positivefeatures. <strong>On</strong>e is parsim<strong>on</strong>y--with <strong>on</strong>ly two factors, the theory generatesa few propositi<strong>on</strong>s which can be easily understood. It can account(post hoc) for many research findings c<strong>on</strong>cerning the habits and livesof addicts and can make specific predicti<strong>on</strong>s about a variety of phenomena.The theory makes predicti<strong>on</strong>s about beginning, c<strong>on</strong>tinuing, ceasing,and relapsing into drug usage. Although intended primarily as atheory of opiate addicti<strong>on</strong>, the major propositi<strong>on</strong>s seem suited to anytype of drug use where addicti<strong>on</strong> or abuse occur. The theory has acertain surface validity about it and is specific enough in many aspectsto be tested empirically.The theory has some linkage with a theory of alcoholism and could beapplied to other social problems, such as criminality, with some changes.It helps to explain multiple-drug use in an individual, drug use infamily and peer groups, and the reas<strong>on</strong>s for poor recovery ratesam<strong>on</strong>g addicts. The theory attempts to account for both “street”addicts and professi<strong>on</strong>al and medical addicti<strong>on</strong>. It recognizes both the“escapist” and the more positive or “seeking” aspects of drug use, andallows both some importance in the same pers<strong>on</strong>.48


Lastly, the theory suggests methods for preventi<strong>on</strong>--reducti<strong>on</strong>s in bothavailability and pr<strong>on</strong>eness. It is likely that governments can reduce<strong>on</strong>ly availability over the short run (through laws, enforcement, etc.)and that reducti<strong>on</strong>s in pr<strong>on</strong>eness (reorganizing society?) will be muchmore difficult.WEAKNESSES OF THE THEORYThe major weaknesses of the theory appear to be the following:1. The theory is essentially a post hoc analysis and integrati<strong>on</strong> ofideas and research findings. It has not received an independentempirical validati<strong>on</strong> for most of its propositi<strong>on</strong>s.2. The major c<strong>on</strong>cepts of “availability” and “pr<strong>on</strong>eness” are not veryspecific, but they are global c<strong>on</strong>cepts with a variety of possiblemeanings. In any <strong>on</strong>e empirical test they would require clear,unambiguous definiti<strong>on</strong>.3. There is a physical analogy that can be made about the theory--that of a hydraulic pump: Where availability is high, pr<strong>on</strong>enessneed not be and vice-versa. It remains to be seen whether this isan adequate representati<strong>on</strong> of reality.4. There are several situati<strong>on</strong>s in which availability is high but druguse is low--e.g., Turkish and Mexican farmers who grow opium d<strong>on</strong>ot appear to use it. It is difficult to believe that pr<strong>on</strong>eness iszero in those areas, and other explanatory variables are perhapsrequired.5.6.The relative weight to be given to availability and pr<strong>on</strong>enessfactors in a given situati<strong>on</strong> can be expressed <strong>on</strong>ly in generalterms. Further detailed or mathematical expressi<strong>on</strong>s of the c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong>of each are required.Special problems exist with the c<strong>on</strong>cept of availability in that theactual availability is almost never known for individual drugs. Itmay be surmised, but research will often be d<strong>on</strong>e with perceivedavailability or with <strong>on</strong>e single aspect of actual availability.49


Perceived Effects ofSubstance UseA General TheoryGene M. Smith, Ph.D.The theory is referred to as being “general” because it attempts toidentify comm<strong>on</strong> processes and mechanisms that might be involved inthe use of a wide variety of substances: caffeine; cigarettes; alcohol;marijuana and hashish; LSD and similar hallucinogens; sedatives, suchas barbiturates and tranquilizers; stimulants, such as amphetaminesand cocaine; heroin and other opiates. The term “substance” isemployed rather than “drug” to avoid an unprofitable debate over theappropriateness of using the term “drug” to refer to certain substancesjust listed. Our focus is <strong>on</strong> the effects of substance use as perceivedby the user, whether or not those percepti<strong>on</strong>s accord with otherevidence.This chapter specifies assumpti<strong>on</strong>s and speculates about mechanismsthat might advance the understanding of the complex and often perplexingprocesses that range from initiati<strong>on</strong> to compulsive substance use.The perspective presented here has been helpful to the author.Obviously, however, it is <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e of many ways to c<strong>on</strong>ceptualize theprocesses under discussi<strong>on</strong>.SATISFACTION, SECURITY,AND SELF-ENHANCEMENTWe assume that most acts are intended to benefit the actor; to promotehis or her self-protecti<strong>on</strong> and self-enhancement; to produce gratificati<strong>on</strong>;and to reduce frustrati<strong>on</strong>, boredom, depressi<strong>on</strong>, anxiety, guilt,and other forms of psychic distress. The fact that substance use isoften in direct c<strong>on</strong>flict with those objectives raises important theoreticalquesti<strong>on</strong>s regarding the dynamics underlying such use. It is notenough simply to observe that c<strong>on</strong>scious and/or unc<strong>on</strong>scious motivesoften lead to behavior that is irrati<strong>on</strong>al and self-defeating, and thatcompulsive substance use is merely <strong>on</strong>e instance of such irrati<strong>on</strong>ality.Although true, that statement does not clarify the genesis of compulsive50


substance use 1 or help identify the mechanisms that permit such use toprogress to levels of severe self-destructiveness.First, we must acknowledge that substance use often is not selfdestructive.Indeed, recognizing the satisfying and self-enhancingnature of substance use is essential to understanding the processes ofinitiati<strong>on</strong>, c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> of use, escalati<strong>on</strong>, cessati<strong>on</strong>, and relapse.When and how is substance use satisfying and/or self-enhancing?What mechanisms enable use to c<strong>on</strong>tinue and escalate even after itsdisadvantages have become substantially greater than its advantages?The topics discussed below present observati<strong>on</strong>s and assumpti<strong>on</strong>sbearing <strong>on</strong> those and related questi<strong>on</strong>s.PERCEIVED CONSEQUENCESOF SUBSTANCE USEC<strong>on</strong>sequences of substance use reported by the subject can of coursebe highly biased. Some c<strong>on</strong>sequences may be grossly misperceived.Some may not be recognized at all. However, if perceived c<strong>on</strong>sequencesreflect the subject’s estimate of the costs and benefits of his or heruse, they can provide valuable informati<strong>on</strong> regarding the reinforcementc<strong>on</strong>tingencies that facilitate or inhibit the c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> of substanceuse. Paradoxically, informati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cerning perceived c<strong>on</strong>sequences ofsubstance use might be more useful in clarifying the causes of usethan in identifying its true c<strong>on</strong>sequences. We assume that the user’spercepti<strong>on</strong>s of the costs and benefits of his or her substance use arecritically important in determining c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> or cessati<strong>on</strong> of use--however err<strong>on</strong>eous those percepti<strong>on</strong>s might be.Although the process of evaluati<strong>on</strong> need not be deliberative (or evenc<strong>on</strong>scious), we assume that substance use will c<strong>on</strong>tinue as l<strong>on</strong>g as theperceived aggregate benefits are valued more highly by the user thanthe perceived aggregate costs. This cost-benefit relati<strong>on</strong>ship depends<strong>on</strong> many variables, such as which substance is used, its strength, thefrequency of its use, the immediacy and intensity of its perceivedeffects, the needs the substance is perceived to satisfy and frustrate,the intensity of those needs, their importance and centrality in theuser’s life, and the effects use has <strong>on</strong> the user’s c<strong>on</strong>cepts of Self andIdeal Self.1 The categorical terms “use” and “abuse” are c<strong>on</strong>venient for distinguishingwell-regulated (and often beneficial) substance ingesti<strong>on</strong> fromunregulated, compulsive, and clearly detrimental ingesti<strong>on</strong>. Unfortunately,when the term “abuse” is used, the nature and degree of abuseis rarely specified. The boundaries that seperate use from abuse areambiguous and debatable; and those boundaries vary from substanceto substance and from user to user. In additi<strong>on</strong>, the categoricalnature of the terms “use” and “abuse” tend to obscure the c<strong>on</strong>tinuousprocess by which substance use shades into substance abuse, and itdiverts attenti<strong>on</strong> from the fact that the transiti<strong>on</strong> is a multivariateprocess that occurs c<strong>on</strong>currently al<strong>on</strong>g numerous dimensi<strong>on</strong>s whichthemselves are apt to be c<strong>on</strong>tinuously distributed processes. For thesereas<strong>on</strong>s, we will not use the term “abuse” but rather will speak of usethat is, or is not, compulsive.51


We assume that any single act of substance use produces numerous andvaried positive and negative effects. Some effects are perceived withgreater accuracy than others; some with greater clarity and certitudethan others. Some effects are not perceived at all, and some that areperceived are accorded little or no significance. We assume that dimlyperceived substance effects, and even some effects that are bey<strong>on</strong>dc<strong>on</strong>scious awareness altogether, can influence future use; but that, ingeneral, influence varies directly with the clarity and certitude of thepercepti<strong>on</strong> of each effect and with the significance attributed to it bythe user.SEDUCTIVENESS OF PERCEIVED BENEFITSOF EARLY SUBSTANCE USEAlthough most initiates believe that the benefits of occasi<strong>on</strong>al useoutweigh its risks, any particular initiate will have varied and mixedattitudes, beliefs, and expectati<strong>on</strong>s regarding the potential advantagesand disadvantages of substance use. This complex mix of attitudes,beliefs, and expectati<strong>on</strong>s generates a net effect representing an overallpredispositi<strong>on</strong> that can range from extremely positive to extremelynegative. The more positive the net effect, the higher the probabilityof use, and the earlier it is likely to begin.In the manner that caffeine is usually c<strong>on</strong>sumed, most users perceivethe beneficial effects (mood elevati<strong>on</strong>, increased alertness, and improvedmental and physical performance) as easily outweighing the costs.Alcohol, in small amounts, is widely perceived as promoting c<strong>on</strong>viviality,enhancing the pleasure of social interacti<strong>on</strong>, and reducing unwantedinhibiti<strong>on</strong>s. Marijuana is perceived to produce euphoria and enhanceenjoyment of food, sex, art, music, and hobbies for many users.Amphetamines and cocaine can produce mood elevati<strong>on</strong> and perceivedenhancement of performance. Barbiturates and tranquilizers candiminish psychic and physical discomfort; so can opiates.Prior to compulsive use, the percepti<strong>on</strong> that the benefits outweigh thecosts may indeed be valid, but as escalati<strong>on</strong> proceeds, the actualaggregate net effects can become damagingly negative. <strong>On</strong>e rarely (ifever) becomes a compulsive user without a c<strong>on</strong>siderable amount ofprevious n<strong>on</strong>compulsive use. The prep<strong>on</strong>derance of perceived positiveeffects over perceived negative effects during the early stages ofsubstance use can be the seductive bait that ultimately leads the userinto the trap of addicti<strong>on</strong>.INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES INFLUENCINGSUBSTANCE USEIn the preceding secti<strong>on</strong> we emphasized that well-regulated, n<strong>on</strong>compulsivesubstance use can be satisfying and rewarding. Yet, type andamount of substance use vary dramatically from pers<strong>on</strong> to pers<strong>on</strong>. Forany particular substance, some individuals begin using as children,some begin later, and some avoid use altogether. At the adolescentand preadolescent age levels, what accounts for these differences?Relevant factors include (a) substance availability, (b) type and52


amount of substance use by members of friendship groups, role models,and other significant pers<strong>on</strong>s, (c) demographic variables, (d) geneticvariables, (e) beliefs regarding the risks and benefits of substanceuse, and (f) attitudes, values, and behavioral propensities that comprisewhat is referred to here as “pers<strong>on</strong>ality.” Space limitati<strong>on</strong>s precludediscussi<strong>on</strong> of all such potential determinants of use. We will comment<strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong> the possible separate and interactive effects of substanceavailability, friendship groups, and pers<strong>on</strong>ality.Although illicit substances can be purchased at most schools, they arenot equally available to all students. Availability depends <strong>on</strong> who theadolescent or preadolescent knows and how he or she is perceived bypotential suppliers. If friendship groups include users, availability isgreater, and the likelihood of use is increased; so is the likelihood ofvery early initiati<strong>on</strong> of use.Attitudes and behavior regarding substance use <strong>on</strong> the part of friendsand role models (e.g., older siblings, parents, salient members ofreference groups) influence the probability of initiati<strong>on</strong>. If use ispracticed by (or is acceptable to) such “significant others,” initiati<strong>on</strong>is more likely; it is also more likely to occur at an early age.The l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal evidence now available indicates that n<strong>on</strong>using adolescentswho are most likely to use marijuana and/or hard drugs duringlater adolescence tend to be more rebellious and deviance pr<strong>on</strong>e; morealienated from parents; more critical of society; more impulsive; moreemoti<strong>on</strong>al; more pessimistic and sad; more adventuresome and thrillseeking;more sociable and extroverted; less traditi<strong>on</strong>al and c<strong>on</strong>servativeregarding values; less oriented toward religi<strong>on</strong>; less orderly,diligent, and effective in work and study habits; less intellectuallycurious and interested; less determined, persistent, and motivatedtoward achievement; less likely to feel valued and accepted by others;less trustworthy and resp<strong>on</strong>sible; less tender and c<strong>on</strong>siderate of others;and less self-c<strong>on</strong>trolled. Moreover, many of those same pers<strong>on</strong>alitycharacteristics differentiate early initiates from later initiates and, inadditi<strong>on</strong>, predict subsequent degrees of drug involvement (Jessor1976; Mellinger et al. 1975; Segal 1975; Smith and Fogg 1977, 1978).The results just menti<strong>on</strong>ed reflect statistical regularities that apply tolarge groups of individuals. There are, of course, many excepti<strong>on</strong>s atthe individual level of analysis. For example, Smith and Fogg (1978)studied attitude and pers<strong>on</strong>ality variables in a group of 651 students,all of whom reported being n<strong>on</strong>users of marijuana when tested asseventh or eighth graders. When studied subsequently, 206 studentsreported that they had remained n<strong>on</strong>users for the full five-year periodof the l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal study; 128 reported <strong>on</strong>e or more instances of marijuanause before completing the ninth grade; and 317 reported usingmarijuana during their high school years. A multiple discriminantfuncti<strong>on</strong> analysis involving five predictor variables 2 enabled the c<strong>on</strong>tinuingn<strong>on</strong>users to be discriminated from the early initiates with 80percent accuracy. That classificati<strong>on</strong> analysis focused <strong>on</strong> the two mostdistinctly different groups am<strong>on</strong>g the three groups studied; but, evenso, 80 percent is a very high degree of classificati<strong>on</strong> accuracy--especially2 The five predictor variables were obedience as measured by a selfreportscale, obedience as measured by peer ratings, sociability asmeasured by peer ratings, and two self-report measures of attitudestoward cigarette smoking.53


when it is remembered that all students in the analysis were n<strong>on</strong>usersat the time the predictor variables were measured. Nevertheless, 20percent of the students in the analysis were misclassified, and those20 percent reflect various aspects of individual uniqueness not capturedin the analysis.SUBSTANCE USE AND THE INTERACTIONBETWEEN EARLY PERSONALITY DEVELOPMENTAND PEER-GROUP INFLUENCESFriendship groups begin to form in the primary school grades, and itis likely that the behavioral predispositi<strong>on</strong>s of children comprising anygiven group tend to c<strong>on</strong>verge as members of the group share witheach other the percepti<strong>on</strong>s, experiences, values, beliefs, and lifeorientingc<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>s that influence pers<strong>on</strong>ality development. Childrenwith similar values, attitudes, and other pers<strong>on</strong>al characteristics gravitatetoward each other; and that associati<strong>on</strong> strengthens the verycharacteristics that brought them together in the first place.Children with pers<strong>on</strong>ality characteristics that promote rejecti<strong>on</strong> of adultdemands and expectati<strong>on</strong>s exhibit that rejecti<strong>on</strong> in many ways: e.g.,disparaging academic achievement, smoking cigarettes, breaking schoolrules, and engaging in other types of early childhood deviance. Suchchildren tend to aggregate and form friendship groups, some membersof which are precocious regarding both their motivati<strong>on</strong> to use substancesand their ability to find sources of supply.Similarly, children with pers<strong>on</strong>ality characteristics that facilitate acceptanceof, and/or compliance with, the rules and expectati<strong>on</strong>s of adultauthorities tend to become members of friendship groups that supportfurther development of those characteristics; and such groups arelikely to c<strong>on</strong>tain fewer members who are precocious regarding accessto, and motivati<strong>on</strong> for, substance use.Thus, early in preadolescence, an interactive process begins that isinfluenced by (a) pers<strong>on</strong>ality formati<strong>on</strong> of individual children; (b) reinforcementof that formati<strong>on</strong> through interacti<strong>on</strong> with like-minded children;(c) differences am<strong>on</strong>g groups regarding attitudes toward, andthe use of, substances; and (d) differential availability of substancesto such groups. We believe this interactive process c<strong>on</strong>tributes substantiallyto the c<strong>on</strong>siderable success with which substance use can bepredicted from pers<strong>on</strong>ality characteristics and attitudes measured priorto initiati<strong>on</strong> of use.FACTORS CONTRIBUTING TOCONTINUATION OF USEThe match between the needs of the user and the changes he or sheattributes to the substance is important in determining whether or notuse will c<strong>on</strong>tinue. The individual who places high value <strong>on</strong> feelingstr<strong>on</strong>g, alert, decisive, and masterful is apt to find amphetamine orcocaine much more satisfying than a pers<strong>on</strong> who emphasizes peace,physical relaxati<strong>on</strong>, and the c<strong>on</strong>templati<strong>on</strong> of philosophical and54


metaphysical issues. A pers<strong>on</strong> of the latter type would probably finddrugs like marijuana and LSD far more enjoyable. The better thematch between the perceived substance effects and the user’s needs,the more likely use is to c<strong>on</strong>tinue.Future use is also influenced by the intensity of the needs that areperceived as being satisfied by use. The greater the importanceascribed by the user to these needs, the more likely it is that use willc<strong>on</strong>tinue.The mood and cognitive changes caused by use of certain substancescan temporarily alter the user’s c<strong>on</strong>cepts of Self and Ideal Self. Ifuse reduces the discrepancy between the user’s percepti<strong>on</strong>s of Selfand Ideal Self, c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> of use is likely--even if those changes last<strong>on</strong>ly as l<strong>on</strong>g as the drug effect itself.It is also possible for substance use to produce changes in pers<strong>on</strong>alitythat are more or less enduring; e.g., increased sociability and improvedsocial skills in an adolescent who previously was painfully shy. Ifsuch changes are highly valued by the user, the probability of c<strong>on</strong>tinueduse will be increased substantially.During the relatively early phases of escalati<strong>on</strong> toward compulsive use,it is possible for c<strong>on</strong>sciously recognized dangers that are associatedwith substance use to facilitate rather than inhibit use if those dangersare experienced as more exhilarating than anxiety-provoking; if theself-initiated risks bring status and social approval to the user; or ifthe user pits any perceived dangers against his or her competence andself-c<strong>on</strong>trol, and then treats the matter as a c<strong>on</strong>test which he or sheis sure to win. As l<strong>on</strong>g as the user c<strong>on</strong>tinues to perceive the overallgain as greater than the overall cost, use will c<strong>on</strong>tinue; and the riskof escalati<strong>on</strong> to more dangerous levels of use becomes more likely.It should also be noted that some behavior that appears to be completelyself-defeating might in fact be aimed at achieving objectives thatsimply are not easily recognized by an outside observer. For thatmatter, they might not be recognized by the actor. The adolescentwho (for whatever reas<strong>on</strong>) has a str<strong>on</strong>g need to punish the Self, aparent, or some other significant pers<strong>on</strong> might find the ag<strong>on</strong>izing costsof compulsive substance use more than offset by the benefits producedby the punishment inflicted.FACTORS CONTRIBUTING TOCESSATION OF USEAlthough cessati<strong>on</strong> itself is a single event, we assume that it reflectsthe outcome of a protracted process of assessment that has been <strong>on</strong>going(c<strong>on</strong>sciously and unc<strong>on</strong>sciously) throughout most of the period ofuse. Factors that determine when (if ever) the advantages of cessati<strong>on</strong>will be seen as outweighing the disadvantages include the following:changes in the user’s life circumstances; increasing anxiety and c<strong>on</strong>cernregarding various potential losses associated with use; substituti<strong>on</strong>of more cost-effective satisfacti<strong>on</strong>s for those previously obtainedthrough substance use; increased attributi<strong>on</strong> of importance to l<strong>on</strong>gerterm costs and benefits associated with use; and a clearer recogniti<strong>on</strong>of the obstacles to achievement of important life goals posed by c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong>of use.55


Am<strong>on</strong>g children and young adults, examples of altered life circumstancesthat might facilitate cessati<strong>on</strong> are moving from <strong>on</strong>e neighborhoodto another; changing friendship groups; graduating from high school;going to college; getting a full-time job; getting married; havingchildren; and accepting new resp<strong>on</strong>sibilities associated with adulthood.Anxieties and c<strong>on</strong>cerns that might lead to cessati<strong>on</strong> include c<strong>on</strong>flictswith parents, school authorities, and police regarding substance use;having a severely frightening drug experience or series of such experiences;fear of losing a valued job or jeopardizing <strong>on</strong>e’s career advancement;c<strong>on</strong>cern over the possibility of having a serious accident orsuffering impaired physical or psychological health; fear of losing therespect and esteem of loved <strong>on</strong>es and friends; reduced self-respect;and fear that an immediate choice must be made between cessati<strong>on</strong> nowor a lifel<strong>on</strong>g dependency <strong>on</strong> substance use.Certain patterns of heavy substance use can cause hobbies, sportsactivities, and other previously enjoyable ways of spending time tobecome less rewarding. Success in rekindling those earlier interests,or in developing new <strong>on</strong>es, is apt to increase the likelihood that usewill cease.The probability of cessati<strong>on</strong> is increased by any shift in orientati<strong>on</strong>away from the present toward the future, or by any increased capacityto forego immediate gratificati<strong>on</strong>s to achieve more important subsequent<strong>on</strong>es. That probability is also increased if the user views c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong>as being incompatible with achievement of l<strong>on</strong>g-term, significant lifegoals, especially if those goals are part of a clearly defined, carefullyc<strong>on</strong>sidered career plan that seems both achievable and likely to bringimportant future occupati<strong>on</strong>al, financial, social, and pers<strong>on</strong>al satisfacti<strong>on</strong>s.IMPAIRED REALITY TESTING,COMPULSIVE SUBSTANCE USE,ADDICTION, AND READDICTIONWhatever its amount, frequency, and pattern, substance use willc<strong>on</strong>tinue until the user perceives the disadvantages of use as outweighingits benefits. The subjective character of this cost-benefit relati<strong>on</strong>shipis emphasized <strong>on</strong>ce again because in many (perhaps most) instancesof compulsive use, the user perceives use as having a net positiveeffect l<strong>on</strong>g after most outside observers would have c<strong>on</strong>cluded that thecost-benefit relati<strong>on</strong>ship had shifted from positive to negative.As escalati<strong>on</strong> progresses, cognitive functi<strong>on</strong>s (percepti<strong>on</strong>, memory, andjudgment) tend to be altered in a manner that restricts and vitiatesthe feedback available to the user regarding the benefits and costs ofuse. This undermines the reality testing processes that might otherwisealert the user to his or her increasing vulnerability to addicti<strong>on</strong>.C<strong>on</strong>victi<strong>on</strong>s based <strong>on</strong> early evidence that the aggregate net effect ofsubstance use is positive may cause new and c<strong>on</strong>tradictory evidence tobe discounted, misinterpreted, or denied altogether.It is well known that memory is highly selective. This may be importantin explaining why addicts fight and win the ag<strong>on</strong>izing battle tobecome free of addicti<strong>on</strong>, <strong>on</strong>ly to become readdicted after a period of56


abstinence. Perhaps the suffering is remembered as being less intensethan it actually was. The likelihood of readdicti<strong>on</strong> is increased bysuch retrospective cognitive distorti<strong>on</strong>s or by any other failures inreality testing that cause the recollecti<strong>on</strong> of past negative c<strong>on</strong>sequencesto appear diminished in importance or that cause the recollecti<strong>on</strong> ofpast positive c<strong>on</strong>sequences to appear enhanced in importance.Impaired reality testing might also promote readdicti<strong>on</strong> by enabling theuser to believe, err<strong>on</strong>eously, that the factors accounting for his orher previous addicti<strong>on</strong> no l<strong>on</strong>ger apply. For example, the user mightbelieve that he or she is now clearly aware of the warning signs thatappear prior to the stage of compulsive use, will vigilantly heed anysuch warnings, and, in that manner, can achieve the pleasure ofoccasi<strong>on</strong>al, well-regulated, n<strong>on</strong>compulsive use without running the riskof readdicti<strong>on</strong>. Or, if the individual’s abstinent periods are themselvespsychologically distressing (due to depressi<strong>on</strong>, anxiety, guilt, anger,etc.) and substance use reduces those discomforts, it might be quiteeasy for the user to misjudge the risks of readdicti<strong>on</strong> and c<strong>on</strong>cludethat just enough substance can be taken to c<strong>on</strong>trol those distressingmood states without returning to the level of compulsive use.Impaired reality testing may also play a role in allowing the user toaccord undue importance to immediate gratificati<strong>on</strong>s at the expense ofmore distant <strong>on</strong>es. C<strong>on</strong>tinued use is facilitated by ambiguity of l<strong>on</strong>gtermgoals; by undervaluing either their importance or their likelihoodof attainment; and by failing to recognize the relati<strong>on</strong>ship betweenc<strong>on</strong>tinued use and the likelihood of achieving those goals. If supportfor the belief that substance use has a net positive effect becomessufficiently weak, then the defenses that previously permitted the userto discount, misinterpret, or deny the true costs of substance usebecome harder and harder to sustain. The self-decepti<strong>on</strong> may then berecognized, and use may cease.SUBSTANCE-INDUCED CHANGES IN MOODAND SOMATIC FEELING STATESAlthough most aspects of this theory c<strong>on</strong>cern mood states rather thansomatic feelings, the latter are very important in determining usagepatterns. Present informati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cerning the separate and interactiveroles of mood and somatic feeling states in sustaining substance use ismeager--even with a substance as widely used and as frequentlystudied as cigarettes. This is <strong>on</strong>e of many issues regarding substanceuse that will require further investigati<strong>on</strong>.WITHDRAWAL DISTRESS AND THESELF-PERPETUATION OF USESubstances differ regarding the producti<strong>on</strong> of negative mood andsomatic feeling states after their use. They also differ regarding thesuccess with which such effects can be reduced by readministrati<strong>on</strong> ofthe original substance. The nervousness and jittery feelings thatresult from excessive c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong> of caffeine are increased, not reduced,by ingesting additi<strong>on</strong>al caffeine; but those and other symptoms of57


excessive alcohol c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong> can be reduced by taking additi<strong>on</strong>alalcohol. Escalati<strong>on</strong> to compulsive use is a danger with any substancethat can be ingested to alleviate withdrawal distress resulting fromprevious ingesti<strong>on</strong>--particularly if the substance is <strong>on</strong>e for whichtolerance develops rapidly, with a resultant need for higher and higherdose levels to produce a given effect. It is well known, for example,that the aversiveness of withdrawal distress is powerfully important indriving the heroin addict to readminister.Of course, the amount of substance used (and other factors, such asthe route of administrati<strong>on</strong>) influences the likelihood that a user willbe drawn into a cycle of self-perpetuating compulsive use. Cocaine,as presently used in the United States, rarely generates compulsiveuse, but it has been reported that in Peru and other South Americancountries, where coca paste is inexpensive and is smoked in largequantities, some users are catapulted to levels of intensely compulsiveuse with frightening rapidity (Jeri et al. 1978).58


A Life-Theme Theory ofChr<strong>on</strong>ic <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>James V. Spotts, Ph.D.Franklin C. Sh<strong>on</strong>ts, Ph.D.A complete account of the causes of drug use and abuse must c<strong>on</strong>siderat least three groups of factors: physiological, social, and psychological.Furthermore, it must explain both grouped data (such as meansand correlati<strong>on</strong>s between variables that are measured by normativetests) and individuals. No <strong>on</strong>e theory is capable of including allrelevant factors at both group and individual levels. C<strong>on</strong>sequently,the research scientist or clinical diagnostician must be in a positi<strong>on</strong> toevaluate all possibilities, weighing each according to its probablesignificance for the problem at hand.The theory of drug abuse presented here c<strong>on</strong>centrates <strong>on</strong> psychologicalfactors in chr<strong>on</strong>ic drug abusers. It is pers<strong>on</strong>alistic in that it dealswith individuals in all their complexity and uniquenesses. The ideas itc<strong>on</strong>tains are not “laws of behavior” but guides for understandingindividual human beings.This theory is also distinctive in that it calls special attenti<strong>on</strong> to theimportance of the numinous aspects of human experience. “Numinous”means, roughly, spiritual and refers to the universal human tendencyto c<strong>on</strong>strue the world and <strong>on</strong>eself animistically. In cases of drug useand abuse, numinous factors become most obvious when substances areassigned magical or mystical properties by their users, when drugs areincorporated into religious rituals, or when such substances are themeans for producing transcendental experiences (which the comm<strong>on</strong>lyused term “euphoria” is hopelessly inadequate to describe). Numinousfactors operate in every<strong>on</strong>e’s life, and it is important that they berecognized and understood.Since 1974, the Greater Kansas City Mental Health Foundati<strong>on</strong> has beenengaged in a program of research <strong>on</strong> the relati<strong>on</strong>ships between druguse/abuse and lifestyle. The program uses the representative casemethod (Sh<strong>on</strong>tz 1965, 1976; Spotts and Sh<strong>on</strong>tz 1980) an approach toresearch that must not be c<strong>on</strong>fused with ordinary case-study techniques.59


A representative case is not a sample of a populati<strong>on</strong> or a pers<strong>on</strong> of“unusual clinical interest,” but an exemplar of a variable or type ofbehavior that is of specific theoretical or practical c<strong>on</strong>cern. Forexample, in a study of the effects and use of cocaine the ideal representativecase is not a pers<strong>on</strong> who takes the drug occasi<strong>on</strong>ally forrecreati<strong>on</strong>al use, but <strong>on</strong>e who is a genuine expert <strong>on</strong> the substance,who is committed to its use and who has tried it at all dosage levelsand by all forms of ingesti<strong>on</strong>. This pers<strong>on</strong> must epitomize cocaine useas clearly as possible and must be studied extensively and intensively,using both quantitative and qualitative means. He must be treated notas a “subject” but as a “c<strong>on</strong>sultant” or, at the very least, as an equalpartner in the scientific enterprise. Research of this type can serveexploratory purposes, but it also provides a powerful tool for testinghypotheses that have been developed in large-scale studies but havenot yet been validated in individuals (Spotts and Sh<strong>on</strong>tz 1980).It is, perhaps, tempting to c<strong>on</strong>clude that a method which advocates thestudy of “single cases” promises an easy or quick way to c<strong>on</strong>ductresearch. Nothing could be further from the truth. First of all, itshould be obvious that truly exemplary cases can be extremely difficultto locate. Hundreds of candidates may have to be screened before theappropriate individual is found. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, data collecti<strong>on</strong> is l<strong>on</strong>g,arduous, and demanding because it involves not <strong>on</strong>ly days of intensivetesting and interviewing but usually requires repeated evaluati<strong>on</strong>s overmany m<strong>on</strong>ths. Third, data analysis is complex and time c<strong>on</strong>suming.Each subproject of the Greater Kansas City studies required factoranalyzing nine correlati<strong>on</strong> matrices of 15 variables each, nine correlati<strong>on</strong>matrices of 27 variables each, 18 analyses of variance, each involving acomplex, mixed model design, c<strong>on</strong>sisting of five factors, crossed andnested in most unorthodox fashi<strong>on</strong>--all this to analyze a single type ofdata (Q-sorts). And these analyses c<strong>on</strong>stituted just a small part ofwhat had to be accomplished to prepare the descripti<strong>on</strong>s of each of ninerepresentative cases. Finally, integrati<strong>on</strong> of cross-secti<strong>on</strong>al and l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal,dimensi<strong>on</strong>al and morphogenic, qualitative and quantitative data,both within and between cases, poses a huge problem in data c<strong>on</strong>densati<strong>on</strong>,interpretati<strong>on</strong>, and communicati<strong>on</strong>.The research from which this theory was derived is based <strong>on</strong> thepropositi<strong>on</strong> that the intensive study of carefully selected individualsprovides a unique perspective <strong>on</strong> the problems of drug abuse and, ifproperly c<strong>on</strong>ducted, yields as much informati<strong>on</strong> about specific drugs,their effects, dynamics and determinants of use, antecedents, c<strong>on</strong>sequences,and social correlates as more traditi<strong>on</strong>al methods.This program of research has focused up<strong>on</strong> the intensive study ofclosely matched pers<strong>on</strong>s, each of whom had engaged in l<strong>on</strong>g-term useof cocaine, amphetamine or its c<strong>on</strong>geners, narcotics, or barbiturates.The men were chosen from am<strong>on</strong>g hundreds of candidates because eachwas an expert who could speak with authority about himself as well asabout his drug of choice, its effects, and the factors associated withits use. All were studied intensively and extensively with structuredinterviews and with dimensi<strong>on</strong>al and morphogenic tests.Our theory draws heavily up<strong>on</strong> the germinal ideas of Carl G. Jung. Itis appropriate that this be the case, for Jung derived his theory fromthe intensive study of individuals. Like any theory, this <strong>on</strong>e isanchored in the methodology from which it was derived. Therefore, itis almost certain to differ in significant ways from theories based up<strong>on</strong>other approaches, research methods, and data-collecti<strong>on</strong> procedures.60


The theory has three parts: a c<strong>on</strong>cepti<strong>on</strong> of pers<strong>on</strong>al structure, ac<strong>on</strong>cepti<strong>on</strong> of how pers<strong>on</strong>al structure develops, and a framework fordescribing the drug experiences of chr<strong>on</strong>ic, heavy users of severalsubstances.PERSONAL STRUCTUREEvery pers<strong>on</strong> is a complex mediator between two realities: the externalphysical/social envir<strong>on</strong>ment, <strong>on</strong> the <strong>on</strong>e hand, and the internal psyche,<strong>on</strong> the other. An investigator who observes or studies another pers<strong>on</strong>starts in the envir<strong>on</strong>ment and first encounters that other pers<strong>on</strong>’sovert acti<strong>on</strong>s. By noting regularities of behavior, the observer drawsinferences about the outermost layer of the observed pers<strong>on</strong>’s totalstructure, the ego.The relati<strong>on</strong>ship between a pers<strong>on</strong>’s ego and the envir<strong>on</strong>ment is that offigure and ground. In the optimal state, the ego is clearly differentiatedand maintains its integrity in relati<strong>on</strong> to the envir<strong>on</strong>ment. Toomuch expansi<strong>on</strong> or c<strong>on</strong>tracti<strong>on</strong> of the ego or too much effort either totranscend or to obliterate it is biologically maladaptive.In additi<strong>on</strong> to describing a pers<strong>on</strong>’s ego, the observer may drawinferences about deeper levels of the pers<strong>on</strong>al structure. The firstlevel below the ego is the lifestyle: the c<strong>on</strong>sistent and pervasivepattern, system, or organizati<strong>on</strong> of preferences, regularities, andorientati<strong>on</strong>s that underlies overt behavioral adaptati<strong>on</strong>. Lifestyle variablesinclude those described in other theories by such terms ashabits, traits, or defense mechanisms. However, the c<strong>on</strong>cepts ofhabit, trait, and defense mechanism do not take into account thepatterning, organizati<strong>on</strong>, and hierarchic structuring that make thelifestyle a system rather than a simple c<strong>on</strong>glomerate or profile.At first glance, the lifestyle of a pimp obviously differs from that of areal estate agent. Yet at a deeper level, both pimp and real estateagent may share the same determinati<strong>on</strong> to be indomitable, to be themost successful at what they do. We call the next level of basicorganizing principles from which the lifestyle and ego derive theircharacter life themes. In other theories, life themes might be calledcore c<strong>on</strong>flicts, character structure, or dominant tendencies. However,most theories that rely <strong>on</strong> such c<strong>on</strong>structs are c<strong>on</strong>tent to c<strong>on</strong>siderthem to be wholly learned and to be the most fundamental level ofpers<strong>on</strong>al structure. According to this theory, neither is the case.For <strong>on</strong>e thing, preprogrammed (archetypal) processes set the stage forlearning of the life themes. For another, relati<strong>on</strong>s with the psyche byway of the pers<strong>on</strong>al myth are more basic than the life themes.We found that most of our c<strong>on</strong>sultant-participants could be describedin terms of no more than four to six themes. For example, <strong>on</strong>e druguser’s life is dominated by the determinati<strong>on</strong> to make a great scientificdiscovery that will justify his mother’s faith that he is a genius.Another’s is pervaded by efforts to gain love and attenti<strong>on</strong> from apowerful but affecti<strong>on</strong>less father, while yet another’s life is pervadedby the need to c<strong>on</strong>quer women sexually in order to neutralize thepower he feels they would otherwise have to emasculate him.Finally, it became evident from our research that yet <strong>on</strong>e more inferentialstep was necessary, for we discovered that, as Freud recognized61


in his c<strong>on</strong>cept of repetiti<strong>on</strong> compulsi<strong>on</strong> (1929), each man seemed to beliving out a destiny over which he had little c<strong>on</strong>trol. Thus, at thedeepest level of inference lies the myth or numen that gives eachpers<strong>on</strong>’s existence a fate-like, an entelechial, quality as if possessedby life-shaping forces over which pers<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>trol is not possible. InJung’s terms, the myth is the kernel or core of an “aut<strong>on</strong>omous complex,”a numinous, m<strong>on</strong>adic formati<strong>on</strong> that remains subliminal andoperates according to its own inherent tendencies, independent ofthe c<strong>on</strong>scious will. A well-integrated myth may be expressed in creativework. Poorly integrated into the rest of the pers<strong>on</strong>al structure,it may cause maladjustment (Jung 1971). The myth serves a purposein human life that is equally important to that served by the ego.The functi<strong>on</strong> of the ego is to insure biological survival, and in modernsociety, that typically takes place by means of technological or ec<strong>on</strong>omicachievement. The functi<strong>on</strong> of the myth is to insure wholeness orunity of the pers<strong>on</strong>. Like Janus, the two-faced god, each individualfaces both envir<strong>on</strong>mental and psychic realities. A balanced resp<strong>on</strong>sivenessto both is necessary if equilibrium is to be maintained (Larsen1976). In a pers<strong>on</strong> who is functi<strong>on</strong>ing well, the ego insures biologicalsurvival by adapting to envir<strong>on</strong>mental realities, while the myth insureswholeness through insuring the ego’s relatedness to psychic realities.OPERATIONALCONSIDERATIONSDirect observati<strong>on</strong>s of behavior provide the basis for inferring egostructure. Psychological tests penetrate to at least the level of lifestyle.Intensive interviews and projective examinati<strong>on</strong>s usually permitreas<strong>on</strong>able rec<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong> of life themes. However, discovery of anotherpers<strong>on</strong>’s numen or myth requires not <strong>on</strong>ly a knowledge of the pers<strong>on</strong>but some familiarity with mythology as well as pers<strong>on</strong>al empathic andintuiti<strong>on</strong>al freedom <strong>on</strong> the part of the investigator. To insure reliability,therefore, it is desirable for the process of myth identificati<strong>on</strong> toinvolve more than <strong>on</strong>e pers<strong>on</strong>.NORMAL DEVELOPMENTDuring the earliest years of life, the human infant is dominated byinfluences from the psyche, the most important of which is the imageof the mother (a precursor of the anima archetype). Although thenewborn infant is not totally helpless, human beings are born unfinished,unready to meet the world, and the child must spend some timein the psychic atmosphere of the parents, in a sec<strong>on</strong>d womb, as itwere, where it must rely heavily up<strong>on</strong> others for safety, security, andsurvival (Campbell 1949). This is the stage in which, at the level ofmyth, the elementary or nuturant mother predominates (Neumann1972). After a year or so, ego tools (speech, ambulati<strong>on</strong>, motorcoordinati<strong>on</strong>) start to develop, and a stage of emerging individualitybegins. At this point the normal mother takes <strong>on</strong> the functi<strong>on</strong> oftransformati<strong>on</strong> by helping the child break away from her and becomean independent pers<strong>on</strong>. The child’s first experience of the process oftransformati<strong>on</strong> is reminiscent of being born and is incorporated intothe child’s myth as a prototype of the theme of rebirth that may beactivated later in life during religious or quasi-religious experiences.Normally, at least for boys, the father enters the picture at this stageand eventually becomes a model according to which the child’s pers<strong>on</strong>al62


myth is elaborated in relati<strong>on</strong> to the animus, or archetype of masculinity.If the father is absent or provides an unsuitable model, the transformingmother may assume this functi<strong>on</strong> and become, in effect, the animus ofher own anima. The result is c<strong>on</strong>fusi<strong>on</strong> over sexual identity in thechild. More typically, the transforming anima requires that the boyincrease in competence to win her approval, while the father teachesthe boy how to accomplish this and to displace the anima away fromthe biological mother to a more suitable woman. During these formativeyears, the child’s life themes begin to take form.At adolescence, the boy becomes initiated into adulthood and beginsdeveloping his own lifestyle, the ways in which he chooses to expresshis life themes. Adolescence c<strong>on</strong>tains an important danger point. Atthis time, tolerance for numinous experiences is diminishing, but thepressure from such experiences may not shrink sufficiently rapidly andthe ego may not yet be str<strong>on</strong>g enough to solve the problem realistically.This is the so-called adolescent crisis, and it is the culminati<strong>on</strong> of ac<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> that develops from early childhood (Edinger 1973, pp. 3-36).After this crisis is passed, growth is for several decades a process ofego development and gradual alienati<strong>on</strong> from numinous psychic influences.At mid-life another phase begins. The now overdeveloped ego maybecome so estranged from its mythical roots in psychic experience thatthe pers<strong>on</strong> begins to feel a need for spiritual wholeness, for a meaningin life. During this period the pers<strong>on</strong> counteracts the growing senseof alienati<strong>on</strong> by returning to inner experiences or spiritual and religioussources for support and reintegrati<strong>on</strong> (Edinger 1973, pp. 37-71). Ifhe is successful, the result is the emergence of a new, more completeidentity called the self. This is the culminati<strong>on</strong> of pers<strong>on</strong>al actualizati<strong>on</strong>;the process of self-development (called individuati<strong>on</strong>) may c<strong>on</strong>tinuefor the rest of the pers<strong>on</strong>’s life.PATTERNS OF DEVELOPMENT INCHRONIC DRUG ABUSERSIn this secti<strong>on</strong>, we briefly describe the typical or modal developmentalpatterns that have emerged from our research with men committed toheavy or chr<strong>on</strong>ic use of amphetamine, cocaine, narcotics, or barbiturates.Although the developmental patterns of these relatively “pure”drug-user types show striking differences am<strong>on</strong>g groups, explainablevariati<strong>on</strong>s and even occasi<strong>on</strong>al reversals of these modal patterns alsoappeared.This discussi<strong>on</strong> does not c<strong>on</strong>cern individuals who use drugs <strong>on</strong>ly forsocial-recreati<strong>on</strong>al purposes. Unlike recreati<strong>on</strong>al drug users, chr<strong>on</strong>icdrug users do not take drugs merely for pleasure. Individuals whoare committed to the heavy, l<strong>on</strong>g-term use of drugs do so to--1. Fill gaps in their pers<strong>on</strong>al structure and mediate serious breaksbetween their rati<strong>on</strong>al (ego) and psychic (mythical) lives;2. Attain by chemical means, even if <strong>on</strong>ly temporarily, ego statesthey cannot attain by their own efforts; and3. Cope with ego deficiencies that have a developmental origin andhandicap them in their efforts to achieve individuati<strong>on</strong>.63


This theory does not assume that problems of adjustment can always beblamed <strong>on</strong> the parents. People do not react to their parents as theyreally are but to parental imagoes--unc<strong>on</strong>scious images that are heavilyinfluenced by fantasies and archetypal c<strong>on</strong>tents. Furthermore, inspecific cases, a host of c<strong>on</strong>stituti<strong>on</strong>al, social, and envir<strong>on</strong>mentalfactors also enter into a pers<strong>on</strong>’s decisi<strong>on</strong> to try or use drugs. Nevertheless,the theory does recognize the existence of certain modalpatterns in drug users’ reports of their early developmental years.Most typically, the chr<strong>on</strong>ic amphetamine users we have studied reportthat they grew up in families with relatively str<strong>on</strong>g but highly manipulativemothers and passive or ineffectual fathers. The mothers ofthese men emerge as devious, ensnaring women (“spider women”) whoskillfully, though not always c<strong>on</strong>sciously, practice complex acts ofdeceit and decepti<strong>on</strong> to keep their men firmly within their web ofc<strong>on</strong>trol. The c<strong>on</strong>trolling and potentially castrating mother handicappedthe boy in developing a str<strong>on</strong>g ego, and the absence of a str<strong>on</strong>gfather left the boy without a firm sense of mascu<strong>line</strong> identity. Thesoluti<strong>on</strong> comm<strong>on</strong>ly adopted by the boy was not <strong>on</strong>ly to deny feelings ofhelplessness and fears of impotence but to c<strong>on</strong>vert them into theiropposites by assuming a phallic and hypermascu<strong>line</strong> posture towardlife. As adults, these men fear the feminine and view women as creaturesto be c<strong>on</strong>quered, overcome, used, or exploited. They takegreat pride in their sexual prowess, for it provides them proof oftheir manhood and emancipati<strong>on</strong> from the “spider” mother of childhood.Chr<strong>on</strong>ic amphetamine users tend to be driven, sometimes violent, butachievement-oriented men who are str<strong>on</strong>gly reactive against threats ofweakness or impotence. Typically, they are unreflective acti<strong>on</strong>-orientedmen who lack insight or rigidly deny the reality of their psychic lives.Nevertheless, they are subject to numinous influences that seem to bedragging them inexorably downward into the maw of the everthreateningmaternal figure.In c<strong>on</strong>trast, narcotics abusers typically said they came from psychologicallydisabled families, in which <strong>on</strong>e parent (often the father) wasabsent or was an overpowering tyrant, while the other parent (oftenthe mother) was too weak or ineffectual to protect the s<strong>on</strong> from theattacks or intimidati<strong>on</strong>s of the other. As adults, the opiate users westudied were seriously disabled individuals who maintained tenuous andunstable adjustments. Their egos were poorly or weakly differentiated.Although they showed greater overall pers<strong>on</strong>al disturbance than cocaineor amphetamine users, they did not display a distinctive set of symptoms.With few excepti<strong>on</strong>s, the narcotics abusers were vulnerablepeople who relied <strong>on</strong> ego c<strong>on</strong>stricti<strong>on</strong> as a primary defense. Typically,they are isolated individuals who live quiet, l<strong>on</strong>ely, and unambitiousIives. Unlike cocaine and amphetamine abusers, narcotics abusers d<strong>on</strong>ot seek stimulati<strong>on</strong> but steadfastly avoid it. They seek a tranquil,serene existence through ego c<strong>on</strong>stricti<strong>on</strong>; they would rather withdrawfrom the problems of life than c<strong>on</strong>quer them.The cocaine users we studied seemed to have progressed further al<strong>on</strong>gthe developmental path than men in the other groups. Most describedearly lives characterized by a rather high level of positive familyfeeling. Most described their mothers as warm and their fathers asstr<strong>on</strong>g and encouraging. As adults, the cocaine users are ambitious,intensely competitive men who work hard to become successful. Theylike to take risks and live by their wits. They have str<strong>on</strong>ger andmore resilient egos than men in the other drug-user groups. Theydisplay a more intense commitment and willingness to struggle to64


overcome their envir<strong>on</strong>ment but are highly pr<strong>on</strong>e to symptoms of alienati<strong>on</strong>from the psyche. They think of themselves as self-directed,self-sufficient, competent people--proud, energetic men who live life tothe full and are capable of carrying pleasure to its extreme. The keyto understanding the cocaine users we studied is their intense counterdependency,their need to be completely self-sufficient. They cannotlean <strong>on</strong> others, turn to other people for help, or admit weakness ofany kind. They take cocaine to expand their egos and their selfc<strong>on</strong>fidence.In additi<strong>on</strong>, they report that the drug produces temporarypsychological states that are so ecstatic that life and fulfillment seemcomplete, if <strong>on</strong>ly for a moment.By c<strong>on</strong>trast, barbiturate users seemed to grow up lacking meaningfulrelati<strong>on</strong>ships with either parent. Most described families with uninterested,neglecting fathers and timorous, dependent, and ineffectualmothers. Most were reared in emoti<strong>on</strong>al wastelands and might havebeen better off psychologically if their parents had been openly rejecting.The typical s<strong>on</strong> seems to have c<strong>on</strong>cluded that, if he could notgain recogniti<strong>on</strong> by pleasing his parents, perhaps he could make themacknowledge his existence by granting their apparent wishes andfailing at everything. Barbiturate abusers repeatedly perform actswhich seemingly tempt fate to destroy them. They report an alarminglyhigh incidence of fights, car wrecks, accidents, and drug overdoses.From an observer’s point of view, these men as adults seem actively toseek defeat. However, from their own point of view they seek escapefrom their pers<strong>on</strong>al distress, frustrati<strong>on</strong>s, and failures, and barbituratesprovide them a vehicle which allows them to do it. It is not that thesemen enjoy defeat. Each succeeding setback and reversal adds to thegradual disintegrati<strong>on</strong> of the self and increases the internal pressuresand frustrati<strong>on</strong>s these men feel. They are like boilers about to explode,for their frequent failures and frustrati<strong>on</strong>s cause a rapid buildup oftensi<strong>on</strong> that they are unable to express in a c<strong>on</strong>trolled way. Forthem, barbiturates precipitate the inevitable; by artificially reducingego inhibiti<strong>on</strong>s, these drugs provide the counterfeit courage the menneed to release pent-up destructive forces. The drugs give the usera ticket to oblivi<strong>on</strong>, thereby permitting him to get away from his senseof failure for a period of time, or they set the c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s which allowthe user to release his tensi<strong>on</strong>s in arguments, brawls, and accidents,with no subsequent sense of guilt, resp<strong>on</strong>sibility, or even awarenessof what happened.DRUG-INDUCED EGO STATESAs indicated above, relati<strong>on</strong>s between ego and envir<strong>on</strong>ment are likethose between figure and ground in gestalt psychology. These mayvary al<strong>on</strong>g two major dimensi<strong>on</strong>s. The first is ego expansi<strong>on</strong> versusego c<strong>on</strong>tracti<strong>on</strong>. Ego expansi<strong>on</strong> implies growth in the pers<strong>on</strong>’s figuralego, his sense of dominance or c<strong>on</strong>trol over both self and envir<strong>on</strong>ment.Ego c<strong>on</strong>tracti<strong>on</strong> implies reducti<strong>on</strong> of ability to manage the envir<strong>on</strong>ment;in c<strong>on</strong>tracti<strong>on</strong>, the ego protects its integrity by limiting its figuralrelati<strong>on</strong> with the ground of the surrounding world.The sec<strong>on</strong>d dimensi<strong>on</strong> is ego/self synthesis versus ego/self dissoluti<strong>on</strong>.Synthesis of ego and self occurs when transcendent experiences leadthe pers<strong>on</strong> to believe that the bounds of ordinary reality have beensurpassed and a mystical truth discovered. In ego/self dissoluti<strong>on</strong>, allsense of pers<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>tinuity and resp<strong>on</strong>sibility is lost, so that thestate is <strong>on</strong>e of psychological oblivi<strong>on</strong>.65


Chr<strong>on</strong>ic drug users attempt to avoid the suffering that would benecessary to reestablish normal individuati<strong>on</strong>. They use pharmacologicalmeans either to escape their pers<strong>on</strong>al dilemmas or to achieve ego statesthat would in other pers<strong>on</strong>s be associated with increasing selfhood.The states they achieve are actually counterfeit, because, while theirpractical effects are real enough, they produce no permanent changein pers<strong>on</strong>al structure and typically do not outlast the period of drugusage.STIMULANTSOur studies indicate that stimulants, such as amphetamines and cocaine,produce ego inflati<strong>on</strong> or expansi<strong>on</strong>, which is experienced by the useras an increase in bodily warmth, exhilarati<strong>on</strong> and euphoria, enhancedself-awareness, feelings of supreme self-c<strong>on</strong>fidence, and a sense ofmastery over fate and the envir<strong>on</strong>ment. With large doses and chr<strong>on</strong>icuse of these drugs, the figural ego becomes so grossly inflated thatthe normal ego-envir<strong>on</strong>ment relati<strong>on</strong>ship is overbalanced. The bloatedego becomes threatened by impulses it can no l<strong>on</strong>ger c<strong>on</strong>trol, andreality testing becomes impaired. At this stage, the ego may implode,producing the well-known amphetamine or cocaine psychosis.Several differences exist between the expansive effects produced bythese two drugs. First, chr<strong>on</strong>ic cocaine users do not display thestereotypy or patterns of compulsive behavior that are found in amphetamineabuse. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, cocaine abuse seems less c<strong>on</strong>ducive to directviolence than does amphetamine abuse. Finally, cocaine produces lesshyperactivity than amphetamine. The amphetamine-induced ego statemobilizes the user for acti<strong>on</strong>. The cocaine-induced ego state is not ameans but an end. The user has no further goal. His <strong>on</strong>ly problemis that he must c<strong>on</strong>tinue using the drug to stay where he is.NARCOTICSThe ego states induced by narcotics are the opposites of those arousedby stimulants. Narcotics (i.e., opium, its derivatives, and semisyntheticsubstitutes) produce ego c<strong>on</strong>tracti<strong>on</strong>: a disengagement from theenvir<strong>on</strong>ment and withdrawal into a quiescent state and detachment ofc<strong>on</strong>cern. Massive doses may induce a stuporous or comatose c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>that could culminate in death due to respiratory arrest. The euphoriathat accompanies narcotics use is not the sort that is associated withc<strong>on</strong>quest or achievement but with relief from tensi<strong>on</strong> or from engagementwith worldly affairs. Although care is suspended, the typical userdoes not seek complete loss of ego relati<strong>on</strong>ships with the envir<strong>on</strong>ment;even an addict who is <strong>on</strong> a deep and pleasurable nod may be provokedinto activity by stimulating or irritating events. Narcotics addictsseem to feel they have achieved an ethereal experience of peace,c<strong>on</strong>tentment, and serenity which makes normal activity, striving, orachievement unnecessary or trivial.BARBITURATESDespite the fact that barbiturates are classified as depressants and arethought of as having effects similar to narcotics, the two types ofsubstances produce strikingly different ego states. In low doses,barbiturates produce mild sedati<strong>on</strong>. However, in chr<strong>on</strong>ic heavy use,66


the pers<strong>on</strong> becomes increasingly disabled, and a state of ego/self dissoluti<strong>on</strong>ultimately ensues. All ego functi<strong>on</strong>s are crippled; thinking andreality-testing dec<strong>line</strong>, and visual-motor coordinati<strong>on</strong>, speech, memory,c<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong>, and judgment are impaired. In the ego state inducedby barbiturates the user aband<strong>on</strong>s resp<strong>on</strong>sibility for and awareness ofhis acti<strong>on</strong>s; some fall into a comatose c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> which can bring aboutdeath. Others enter a disoriented state (similar, apparently, to thatproduced by heavy use of alcohol) in which they commit destructiveacts with no c<strong>on</strong>cern for or awareness of c<strong>on</strong>sequences. They becomebelligerent, quarrelsome, and abusive, and engage in fights, arguments,and other violent c<strong>on</strong>fr<strong>on</strong>tati<strong>on</strong>s. They are frequently the victims ofaccidents. The psychological state induced by barbiturate abuse mightwell be called oblivi<strong>on</strong>, because, while in it, the users are all butegoless.PSYCHEDELICSWe have not studied pers<strong>on</strong>s with a commitment to psychedelic drugs(such as LSD-25). However, the literature suggests that such drugsinduce a state of apparent transcendence, or synthesis of the ego andthe self. These drugs produce profound alterati<strong>on</strong>s in sensory experiencesas well as mood. It is believed by some that psychedelics producea religious or spiritual state in which the user feels outsideordinary reality, at <strong>on</strong>e with the cosmos. Some users report gaininginsight into the nature of the universe and purpose of life, the <strong>on</strong>eness,brotherhood, and togetherness of all living things. However, since“bad trips” may also occur, it is likely that set and setting str<strong>on</strong>glyinfluence resp<strong>on</strong>ses to psychedelic substances.AN INTEGRATING MODELClearly, chr<strong>on</strong>ic use or abuse of differing classes of drugs producesradically different ego states. These transformati<strong>on</strong>s are diagrammedin figure 1. This figure shows a matrix of dimensi<strong>on</strong>s that may beused to define the ego states induced by five major substances ofabuse. The horiz<strong>on</strong>tal axis represents ego c<strong>on</strong>tracti<strong>on</strong> (minus) andego expansi<strong>on</strong> (plus). The vertical axis represents ego/self dissoluti<strong>on</strong>(minus) at the bottom and self/ego integrati<strong>on</strong> at the top.As products of like signs, the upper right and lower left quadrantsare labeled plus. They represent generally pleasant experiences,which differ mainly in that those in the upper right are active, whilethose in the lower left are passive. The upper left and lower rightquadrants are labeled minus and represent generally unpleasant,ego-alien, or disintegrative experiences. Those in the lower right aredirected outward toward the envir<strong>on</strong>ment either through project<strong>on</strong> ordirect physical attack, while those in the upper left are directedinward, arise from, and are c<strong>on</strong>tained within the psychological structureof the pers<strong>on</strong>. The representati<strong>on</strong> of two dark and two light quadrantsindicates that the counterfeit states induced by abuse of various drugscan have both positive (or light) and negative (or dark) side effects.The top of the diagram is also labeled transcendence. It representsexperiences that lead the user to believe that the bounds of ordinaryreality have been surpassed and a mystical truth discovered. Thebottom of the diagram is labeled oblivi<strong>on</strong> and represents the fact that,in this state, all sense of pers<strong>on</strong>-c<strong>on</strong>tinuity is lost. The left side of67


FIGURE 1.—Counterfeit ego states induced by chr<strong>on</strong>ic abuse of several types of drugs


the diagram is labeled disengagement to reflect the fact that ego c<strong>on</strong>tracti<strong>on</strong>implies a reducti<strong>on</strong> of pers<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>tact with the envir<strong>on</strong>ment;the weak or threatened ego retains its integrity by limiting its figuralrelati<strong>on</strong> with the ground of the surrounding world. The right side ofthe diagram is labeled engagement to reflect the fact that ego expansi<strong>on</strong>implies an increase in the pers<strong>on</strong>’s sense of dominance or c<strong>on</strong>trol overboth the self and the envir<strong>on</strong>ment; the figural ego swells, providingincreased belief in <strong>on</strong>e’s pers<strong>on</strong>al power and importance.To the right of the vertical coordinate is a dimensi<strong>on</strong> that runs fromecstasy (which arises from achieving a near-transcendent state through<strong>on</strong>e,s own efforts), to peace or serenity (which stems from the passivedisengagement of ego c<strong>on</strong>cerns). Next IS a dimensi<strong>on</strong> that runs fromstriving (which is oriented more toward c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al than spiritualachievement), to passivity and relaxati<strong>on</strong>, or loss of interest in thenormal activities of lifeJust below the horiz<strong>on</strong>tal coordinate <strong>on</strong> the right a dimensi<strong>on</strong> runsfrom paranoia, or projecti<strong>on</strong> of unc<strong>on</strong>scious impulses outward, toemergence of the unc<strong>on</strong>scious or the flooding of awareness with impulsesof a psychic nature; some psychoanalysts might call this the “return ofthe repressed. ” Finally, a dimensi<strong>on</strong> runs from violence, which usuallyc<strong>on</strong>sists of attacks <strong>on</strong> others but mav have self-destructive c<strong>on</strong>sequencesas well, to sacrifice, which means opening up (disintegrating) the egoto the acceptance of psychic reality. As used here, the term violencedoes not mean c<strong>on</strong>trolled violence, of the sort used by professi<strong>on</strong>alathletes or policemen, but an explosive, unthinking rage in which allsense of identity is lost.As indicated, the ego states induced by chr<strong>on</strong>ic, heavy abuse ofstimulants, narcotics, depressants, and psychedelic drugs fall intodistinctive sectors of figure 1. The placements of different drugs areindicated by broken <strong>line</strong>s <strong>on</strong> the diagram. These reflect our currentjudgments regarding the states these substances produce in individualscommitted to chr<strong>on</strong>ic heavy use of each substance. For example, theplacement of cocaine shows that, while it may produce paranoia, itdoes not typically precipitate the violence that is often associated withamphetamine or barbiturates. Also, although it shares features incomm<strong>on</strong> with amphetamine, users report that cocaine produces a stateof ecstasy that is apparently unique.Narcotics induce ego c<strong>on</strong>tracti<strong>on</strong>, disengagement, or serenity. However,especially in very large chr<strong>on</strong>ic doses, they may awaken terrifyingphantasmagoric images (such as those that affected Thomas DeQuinceyand Samuel Taylor Coleridge), which indicates that unmanageable unc<strong>on</strong>sciousmaterial has broken loose and is emerging into awareness.Barbiturates may produce some positive effects, like relaxati<strong>on</strong>, butwith heavy use, the dark side effects of violence, self-destructiveness,and loss of self-identity outweigh the pleasant <strong>on</strong>es. Indeed, heavybarbiturate use seems to yield more socially and pers<strong>on</strong>ally destructiveacts than any other form of drug abuse.We have not studied alcohol abusers but feel that the ego state producedby alcohol is similar to that produced by barbiturates, thoughat a less intense level. We also believe that people who take psychedelicdrugs regularly may do so in an attempt to achieve something akin tospiritual enlightenment. However, psychedelic drugs are unpredictable,69


and there is always the danger of a bad trip, the emergence of chaoticmaterial from the psyche into c<strong>on</strong>sciousness.We have not studied l<strong>on</strong>g-term polydrug abusers, but it appears thatattainment of a specific ego state may be irrelevant for these individuals.They seem to be willing to enter any drug-induced ego state,so l<strong>on</strong>g as it differs in str<strong>on</strong>g or noticeable ways from the ego statewhich exists during abstinence. These individuals may well be moredisturbed and have fewer pers<strong>on</strong>al resources than any other groupdepicted in figure 1.REASONS FOR DRUG CHOICESThe various groups of drug users we studied have each gravitated tosubstances that induce temporary states that help them cope withspecific disturbances in ego/psyche relati<strong>on</strong>ships. Because of adversedevelopmental c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s, these individuals are unwilling or unable topursue a normal course of individuati<strong>on</strong>. They seek shortcuts, apseudointegrati<strong>on</strong> by means of counterfeit ego states produced bypharmacological agents.Amphetamine abusers have failed to develop the tender, compassi<strong>on</strong>atesides of themselves and view women as terrifying creatures who threatento render them impotent. With the aid of amphetamine, these menattempt to live a precarious but hypermascu<strong>line</strong> existence. Cocaineabusers have similar traits but are somewhat more resourceful andbetter integrated; they fight for what they want and regard normaldependency as defeat. They are subject to crises of alienati<strong>on</strong> andfeelings of emptiness and despair. Cocaine provides them a powerful,though counterfeit, substitute for the warmth and nurturance theyneed.Narcotics abusers are poorly defended against the envir<strong>on</strong>ment andtheir own psychic lives; they use drugs to help shrink their egos sothat they can maintain limited pers<strong>on</strong>al integrity in the face of internaland external forces they cannot c<strong>on</strong>trol. They seek serenity and anally which will provide temporary relief from anxieties and help themfuncti<strong>on</strong> in a limited but more integrated way.Barbiturate abusers are neglected individuals who believe they havereceived little love, attenti<strong>on</strong>, or respect in life. <strong>Drug</strong>s provide thesemen temporary release and escape from identities which mark them asperpetual failures in their own eyes.70


<strong>Drug</strong> Use as aProtective SystemLe<strong>on</strong> Wurmser, M.D.The following discussi<strong>on</strong> is restricted to the psychodynamic study of“compulsive drug use,” the latter being defined as any form of substanceabuse where there is substantial subjective psychological need toresort to or to c<strong>on</strong>tinue using mind-altering substances in disregard ofpossible noxious c<strong>on</strong>sequences that such use socially , legally, somatically,or psychologically entails. It is a relative, not an absolute c<strong>on</strong>cept, a“more or less,” not an “either/or.” What is experienced as “freedomof choice” versus “irresistible intense necessity” is arrayed in a “complementalseries” (Freud 1926). As a rule, such use is based <strong>on</strong>severe inner c<strong>on</strong>flicts, developmental disturbances, and serious familypathology, unless it is used to cope with the effects of “minimal braindamage.”AFFECT DEFENSE<strong>Drug</strong> use is preeminently a pharmacologically reinforced denial--anattempt to get rid of the feeling import of more or less extensive porti<strong>on</strong>sof undesirable inner and outer reality. It is a defense making the emoti<strong>on</strong>alsignificance of a percepti<strong>on</strong> of the outer or inner reality unc<strong>on</strong>scious,inoperative, irrelevant. The broader such blocked-out emoti<strong>on</strong>alsignificance becomes, the more the pers<strong>on</strong>ality is drained of vitality, ofidentity, of inner richness.What is centrally denied in compulsive drug users are affects of apotentially overwhelming nature. In short, drugs are used to forestallor soothe affective storms or nagging dysphoric moods.This presupposes not solely a pr<strong>on</strong>eness for this particular archaicdefense, but also an inclinati<strong>on</strong> for what has been described as affectregressi<strong>on</strong> (Krystal 1974)--the global, undifferentiated nature of emoti<strong>on</strong>sthat can often <strong>on</strong>ly scantily be put into words and other symbolicforms (hyposymbolizati<strong>on</strong>), but is instead partly c<strong>on</strong>verted into somaticsensati<strong>on</strong>s. (Many drug addicts are today’s versi<strong>on</strong> of c<strong>on</strong>versi<strong>on</strong> hysterics!)Anxiety of an overwhelming nature and the emoti<strong>on</strong>al feelingsof pain, injury, woundedness, and vulnerability appear to be a feature71


comm<strong>on</strong> to all types of compulsive drug use. The choice of drugs showssome fairly typical correlati<strong>on</strong>s with otherwise unmanageable affects--narcotics and hypnotics are deployed against rage, shame, jealousy, andparticularly the anxiety related to these feelings; stimulants againstdepressi<strong>on</strong> and weakness; psychedelics against boredom and disillusi<strong>on</strong>ment;alcohol against guilt, l<strong>on</strong>e<strong>line</strong>ss, and related anxiety. This meanswe immediately recognize the following layering: (1) drug use,(2) affective storms or chr<strong>on</strong>ic dysphoria representing such unpleasantaffects, (3) underlying pathology of a hysterical or obsessive-compulsive,of a phobic or depressive, or occasi<strong>on</strong>ally of a psychotic or organicnature. Symptom and character neuroses usually coexist.Where such broad chunks of reality are sapped of their lifeblood, so tospeak--due to widespread denial--we find something very characteristicof many drug users--depers<strong>on</strong>alizati<strong>on</strong> and the impressi<strong>on</strong> of a “falseself”--of a double pers<strong>on</strong>ality, split into a docile, submissive, c<strong>on</strong>formingself, and <strong>on</strong>e of violent rebelli<strong>on</strong> and deep hurt.PHOBIC COREAddicti<strong>on</strong>s and phobias parallel each other in structure, though withinverted valence. While the addict compulsively seeks an externalobject to serve as protector mainly against vague anxiety of unknownmeaning, the phobic compulsively avoids an external object to serve asrepresentative for vague anxiety of unknown meaning. Even morespecifically, we find in the history of most addicts phobic systems asantecedents of their current problem. More and more I see, at leastin most addicts, a phobic core as the infantile neurosis underlying thelater pathology, typically the fears (and wishes) around being closedin, captured, entrapped by structures, limitati<strong>on</strong>s, commitments,physical and emoti<strong>on</strong>al closeness and b<strong>on</strong>ds, This c<strong>on</strong>crete or metaphoricalclaustrophobia is seen, as primary phobia, very close to theoriginal experience of traumatic anxiety, the strangling feeling ofbeing closed in and c<strong>on</strong>fined. The limits given by <strong>on</strong>e’s c<strong>on</strong>scienceand outer societal limitati<strong>on</strong>s and watched over by the so importantguardian feelings of guilt and shame are foremost examples of claustrathat must be broken or eluded.Where there are phobias, there are protective fantasies--fantasies ofpers<strong>on</strong>al protective figures or of impers<strong>on</strong>al protective systems, specificallycounterpoised to these threats. This search for a protectoragainst the phobic object and the anxiety situati<strong>on</strong> almost inevitablyleads to a compelling dependency <strong>on</strong> such a counterphobic factor--alove partner, a fetish, a drug, a system of acti<strong>on</strong>s, the analyst. Mosttypically, drug addicti<strong>on</strong> is fulfilling a protective fantasy defendingagainst the phobic core. Protective objects and protective systemsshow “return of the repressed.” Many of the frightening features arecovertly present in the protector. Paradoxically, the claustrophobicseeks the shelter that turns into a new claustrum; he or she will findthis in the transference to the therapist as well.Similarly these protectors are highly (narcissistically) overvalued.They are expected to be all-powerful, all-absolving, all-giving, yetalso feared to be all-destructive, all-c<strong>on</strong>demning, all-depriving.72


HELPLESSNESS AND REVERSALMany compulsive drug users were severely traumatized as children.Child abuse is, in the simplest and str<strong>on</strong>gest terms, <strong>on</strong>e of the mostimportant etiologic factors for later drug abuse. A child cruellybeaten or exposed to severe, often homicidal violence in the immediatesurrounding, a child involved in sexual acti<strong>on</strong>s of adults, a childsubjected to relentless intrusi<strong>on</strong>s or endlessly deceived and mystifiedhas a number of other defenses at its disposal to deal with the abysmalsense of helplessness (besides denial).The helplessness reflected by the state of primary phobia (claustrophobia)especially and the pain of repeated feelings of having beenunc<strong>on</strong>trollably overwhelmed, traumatized, are defended against by athick crust of narcissism. Grandiosity and haughty arrogance, moreor less extensive and deep withdrawal of feelings from the painfulenvir<strong>on</strong>ment and, hence, coldness and ruthlessness are typical featuresof such a narcissistic defense. It is often papered over by a superficialamiability, friendly compliance, and flirtatious charm--the hallmarksof the “sociopath.”Even more broadly, <strong>on</strong>e can recognize the c<strong>on</strong>sistent use of the defenseof turning passive into active. Just as the patient suffers and fearsdisappointment as a main theme of life, he or she does everythingpossible first to enlist help, but then to turn the tables and to provethe therapist helpless and defeated. Very closely related to this isthe pervasive use of defense by externalizati<strong>on</strong>. It is a counterpart todenial, just as projecti<strong>on</strong> is to repressi<strong>on</strong>. In it “the whole internalbattle ground is changed into an external <strong>on</strong>e” (A. Freud 1965). It isthe defensive effort to resort to external acti<strong>on</strong> in order to supportthe denial of inner c<strong>on</strong>flict; the latter is changed back into an externalc<strong>on</strong>flict; for example, ridicule, rejecti<strong>on</strong>, and punishment are provokedby, not just suspected from, the outside world. Limit-setting is invitedand demanded but then endlessly fought against. Its aim is to takemagical, omnipotent c<strong>on</strong>trol over the unc<strong>on</strong>trollable, frightening. Suchacti<strong>on</strong> for acti<strong>on</strong>’s (and, implicitly, punishment’s) sake is reflected notmerely in excessive drug taking, but in gambling, racing, motorcyclejumping, lying, cheating, and violence.EGO AND SUPEREGO SPLITSIt is part of the archaic defenses, the affect regressi<strong>on</strong>, and the traumatizedego core that there is a remarkable disc<strong>on</strong>tinuity of the sense ofself. Patients often are or resemble “split” or “multiple” pers<strong>on</strong>alities.What is characteristic is the sudden total flipflop, a global lability withno mediati<strong>on</strong> and no perspective. It is the unreliability that is soinfuriating for others, so humiliating for themselves.This is not a defense, but an “ego defect,” a functi<strong>on</strong>al disparity thataffects not solely the ego, but no less the superego. Ideals andloyalties are suddenly replaced by more primitive commitments andpursuits of grand designs.73


SHAMEAs a c<strong>on</strong>sequence of the predominance of narcissistic c<strong>on</strong>cerns andvulnerability, shame and the compulsive provocati<strong>on</strong> of humiliati<strong>on</strong>s andputdowns assume particular prominence. Shame is the experience ofbeing exposed as weak, a failure, as not living up to an image that<strong>on</strong>e wishes to have of <strong>on</strong>eself. With str<strong>on</strong>gly grandiose self-images,coupled with exaggerated expectati<strong>on</strong>s of what others could and shoulddo, there is a c<strong>on</strong>tinued pr<strong>on</strong>eness to massive disappointments, to“narcissistic crises.”74


THEORIES ON<strong>On</strong>e’sRelati<strong>on</strong>shipto Others


Psychological, Social, andEpidemiological Factors inJuvenile <strong>Drug</strong> Uselsidor Chein, Ph.D.OVERVIEWOver the last several decades, the use of narcotics by juveniles hasreached “epidemic” proporti<strong>on</strong>s. When this phenomen<strong>on</strong> became widelypublicized, not a great deal was known about its meaning, origin, orthe dangers implicit in it. Although this may not have been the firstsuch flareup of drug use in this country, and although it was “viewedwith alarm” when it came to public attenti<strong>on</strong>, no valid and systematicstudies of the problem had been made (Lindesmith 1947; Da’i 1937;Terry and Pellens 1928). Thanks to the interventi<strong>on</strong> of the Nati<strong>on</strong>alInstitute of Mental Health (United States Public Health Service), however,the wave of juvenile drug use which occurred in the early 1950sbecame the subject of relatively intensive study. We are now in possessi<strong>on</strong>of a wealth of informati<strong>on</strong>, collected systematically and with a viewto testing specific hunches and hypotheses.This paper, prepared by Jean B. Wils<strong>on</strong> and reviewed by Harold B.Gerard, Ph.D., is based largely <strong>on</strong> findings originally presented in“Juvenile Narcotics Use,” by I. Chein and E. Rosenfeld, reprinted froma symposium entitled NARCOTICS, appearing in Law and C<strong>on</strong>temporaryProblems, volume 22 (no. 1, Winter 1957):52-68, published by DukeUniversity School of Law, Durham, North Carolina, copyright 1957 byDuke University. It also includes findings highlighted in “Psychological,Social and Epidemiological Factors in <strong>Drug</strong> Addicti<strong>on</strong>,” published in 1966.The reader who is interested in pursuing Dr. Chein’s work further isreferred to his 1965 article, “The Use of Narcotics as a Pers<strong>on</strong>al andSocial Problem,” and to The Road to H (1964), coauthored with D.L.Gerard, R.S. Lee, and E. Rosenfeld. Dr. Chein particularly urgesthe reader to refer to his article titled “Psychological Functi<strong>on</strong>s of<strong>Drug</strong> Use,” in Scientific Basis of <strong>Drug</strong> Dependence: A Symposium,edited by H. Steinberg (L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>: Churchill), 1969.76


What, then, do we know about the juvenile drug user and the path hefollowed to addicti<strong>on</strong>?’SOME BASIC FACTSEvery year, several hundred new cases of young men (aged 16 to 20,inclusive) who are involved with narcotics become known in New Yorkto the city courts, the Probati<strong>on</strong> Department, city hospitals, and theYouth Council Bureau. The majority of these cases are users ofheroin; <strong>on</strong>ly a few are n<strong>on</strong>using sellers of heroin or are involvedexclusively with marijuana. These figures, however, give <strong>on</strong>ly aminimal estimate of the true incidence of drug involvement.<strong>Drug</strong> use am<strong>on</strong>g juveniles flourishes in the most deprived areas of thecity. The incidence of illicit narcotics use <strong>on</strong> the c<strong>on</strong>temporary urbanscene is associated with the distributi<strong>on</strong> of c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s of human misery,in almost any way that you might define the latter. It is overwhelmingly,though not exclusively, c<strong>on</strong>centrated in areas of the city thatare underprivileged in virtually every aspect of life that could possiblybe relevant and <strong>on</strong> which there are data from which to derive indexes.These areas are also obviously underprivileged in ways that we do notindex, e.g., with respect to quality of housing and of educati<strong>on</strong>alfacilities.The chr<strong>on</strong>ic users come not <strong>on</strong>ly from the worst neighborhoods, butfrom homes where family life is most disrupted, where the populati<strong>on</strong> isof the lowest socioec<strong>on</strong>omic status, and where there are highly c<strong>on</strong>centratedethnic groups who are often discriminated against. Despiteefforts to discover c<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong>s of young users from less deprivedareas, all available evidence pinpoints drug use am<strong>on</strong>g juveniles as atype of behavior characteristically associated with neighborhoods ofgross socioec<strong>on</strong>omic deprivati<strong>on</strong>.<strong>Drug</strong> use leads to a criminal way of life. The illegality of purchaseand possessi<strong>on</strong> of opiates and similar drugs makes the drug user adelinquent ipso facto. The high cost of heroin, the drug generallyused by juvenile users, also forces specific delinquency against property,for cash returns. The average addicted youngster is too youngand too unskilled to be able to support his habit by his earnings. Not<strong>on</strong>ly have many users freely admitted having committed crimes likeburglary, but there is also independent evidence that in those areas ofthe city where drug rates have g<strong>on</strong>e up, the proporti<strong>on</strong> of juveniledelinquencies likely to result in cash income has also g<strong>on</strong>e up, whilethe proporti<strong>on</strong> of delinquencies which are primarily behavior disturbances(rape, assault, auto theft, disorderly c<strong>on</strong>duct) has g<strong>on</strong>e down(Research Center for Human Relati<strong>on</strong>s 1954b). Available knowledgeabout the behavior of drug users in juvenile gangs also indicates thatthey show a preference for income-producing crimes, as against participati<strong>on</strong>in gang warfare, vandalism, and general hell-raising (ResearchCenter for Human Relati<strong>on</strong>s 1954c). It takes most youngsters who1 All but <strong>on</strong>e of our studies were focused <strong>on</strong> males. However, what wehave had to say about the pers<strong>on</strong>ality problems of drug users and whatis needed for their cure and rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> may well be equally applicableto users of both sexes and varying ages.77


eventually become addicted several m<strong>on</strong>ths, sometimes a year or more,to change from the status of an occasi<strong>on</strong>al weekend user to that of ahabitual user who needs two, three, or even more doses a day (ResearchCenter for Human Relati<strong>on</strong>s 1957a). Many occasi<strong>on</strong>al users never takethe crucial step to addicti<strong>on</strong>, with its physiological manifestati<strong>on</strong>s ofdependence, increasing tolerance, and withdrawal symptoms. Thus,we must distinguish between experimentati<strong>on</strong> and habitual use, and,corresp<strong>on</strong>dingly, between factors c<strong>on</strong>ducive to experimentati<strong>on</strong> andfactors c<strong>on</strong>ducive to habituati<strong>on</strong> and addicti<strong>on</strong>.Youngsters who experiment with drugs know that what they are doingis both illicit and dangerous. While they may not be fully aware of allfacts about addicti<strong>on</strong>, they are likely to have seen addicts and certainlyhave heard about addicts being jailed, about the pains of withdrawal,and about the high cost of drugs. <strong>On</strong>e would expect, therefore, thatwillingness to experiment with an illegal and dangerous activity presupposesa certain attitude toward <strong>on</strong>e’s self, <strong>on</strong>e’s future, and the society.And indeed we find that chr<strong>on</strong>ic addicts, as people, tend to be characterizedby certain pers<strong>on</strong>ality deficiences and by hostility to society.They suffer from excepti<strong>on</strong>ally low panic and frustrati<strong>on</strong> thresholdswhen c<strong>on</strong>fr<strong>on</strong>ted by the demands implicit in enduring intimate relati<strong>on</strong>shipsor, for that matter, in any time-c<strong>on</strong>suming resp<strong>on</strong>sible activity,or even when c<strong>on</strong>fr<strong>on</strong>ted by the likelihood of such demands. They areafflicted by a profound distrust of their fellow human beings, comprehendinginterpers<strong>on</strong>al relati<strong>on</strong>ships exclusively in terms of c<strong>on</strong>ning,manipulating, and pushing other people around. Am<strong>on</strong>g the thingsthese young addicts want “much more than almost anything else in theworld” is “to be able to get other people to do what you want,” and“to enjoy life by having lots of thrills and taking chances.” Theircharacteristic mood is suffused by a sense of futility, expectati<strong>on</strong> offailure, and general depressi<strong>on</strong>.What are the factors involved in the generati<strong>on</strong> and perpetuati<strong>on</strong> of thekind of pers<strong>on</strong> represented by the typical young addict? If you thinkof the including society, the ethnic group, the neighborhood, thefamily, the school, the pers<strong>on</strong> as he goes through his various developmentalphases, and if you c<strong>on</strong>sider almost any pair of these, you finda vicious cycle generating the pers<strong>on</strong>ality type or the c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s thatbreed the type. There are, of course, many instances in which particularcircumstances break the cycle or even generate a c<strong>on</strong>trary, beneficentcycle. We will discuss these circumstances later in this paper.But first let us examine the situati<strong>on</strong>s which are likely to lead toadolescent drug addicti<strong>on</strong>.C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s within the family, the lifestyles of his peers, and the schoolhe attends all influence the young urban male and make it probable orimprobable that he will become a drug addict.It is not surprising that the urban slum is a particularly good breedingplace for families in which the parents, assuming that this basic familyunit has managed to remain intact, are so preoccupied and fatigued bytheir struggles to keep their own heads above water that they havelittle time, patience, or perspective to deal with their children ashuman beings rather than as instrumentalities and sources of frustrati<strong>on</strong>;in which fathers have been so emasculated by their own incompetenciesand dearth of opportunity as to be unable to set an appropriatemodel of the male role; in which momentary moods rather than stablepatterns of pers<strong>on</strong>al relati<strong>on</strong>ships govern the applicati<strong>on</strong> of reward and78


punishment and the demands made of the child; in which society, itsinstituti<strong>on</strong>s, and its instituti<strong>on</strong>al representatives are regarded withsuspici<strong>on</strong> and distrust; in which hopes for the future take the form ofunrealistic dreams and in which there are no realistic aspirati<strong>on</strong>s foror expectati<strong>on</strong>s of the child.It is precisely from such families that chr<strong>on</strong>ic addicts tend to come.From this background they emerge into the larger world of the streetand the school. In this larger world, they find the basic less<strong>on</strong>s oftheir earlier childhood reinforced in various ways. In school, they aremisfits, taught frequently by inexperienced teachers who tend to lookat them as incorrigible and unteachable. In the streets, they hangaround aimlessly, dreaming of an affluent life achieved effortlessly,gravitating toward the delinquent subculture, but, as a rule, lackingthe inner resources to become effective delinquents. Such boys arelikely to have a favorable attitude toward the use of drugs. Aband<strong>on</strong>edat the threshold of a frightening adulthood by their more successfulpeers, narcotics offer them relief, an alibi, and a way out. In thedeprived areas of a city there are sizable minorities of such youngsters.In some subgroups, this delinquent orientati<strong>on</strong> is even more widespread,although not all delinquents become drug addicts.Not all delinquents become drug addicts, and not all young boys whogrow up in slums, even in the most ec<strong>on</strong>omically deprived families,become delinquent. It is not easy to escape the pull of the delinquentsubculture. The child who succeeds is the <strong>on</strong>e whose initial aut<strong>on</strong>omyis great enough, or whose early family envir<strong>on</strong>ment is wholesomeenough, or who, in his early school years, encounters teachers whoare sensitive and encouraging. Such a child is more likely than thosein otherwise similar circumstances to pass successfully what Eriks<strong>on</strong>(1950) has described as the developmental hurdle of establishing basictrust. Having d<strong>on</strong>e so, he is more likely to benefit from such favorableopportunities as may present themselves, to develop competencies andc<strong>on</strong>fidence, to become independent of the slum envir<strong>on</strong>ment, and toestablish relati<strong>on</strong>ships with wise and sympathetic adults who can helphim through crises.In deprived areas, many youngsters with a delinquent attitude towardlife become members of street gangs. However, n<strong>on</strong>e of the juvenilegangs we studied was organized to sell drugs. Since most of thesegangs were the most troublesome <strong>on</strong>es to be found in the high drug useareas of the city and they engaged in many gang-sp<strong>on</strong>sored illegalactivities, this finding makes it most unlikely that juvenile streetgangs operate <strong>on</strong> an organized basis to recruit users.Most gangs set limits <strong>on</strong> drug use by their members. The majority ofthe members of most of the gangs we studied were either opposed orambivalent to the use of heroin. However, use of heroin am<strong>on</strong>g gangsis by no means rare, and the smoking of marijuana is extremely comm<strong>on</strong>.The general attitude seems to be that it’s okay to use heroin “as l<strong>on</strong>gas you make sure you d<strong>on</strong>’t get hooked” (Research Center for HumanRelati<strong>on</strong>s 1954c). The reas<strong>on</strong>s why gangs seem to resist the spread ofimmoderate drug use in their midst are practical, not moral. Anaddict is thought to be “unreliable <strong>on</strong> the job,” and, also, able to getthe whole gang into trouble if they are all arrested together. Moreover,users tend to form little cliques that threaten the cohesiveness of thegang. For these reas<strong>on</strong>s, a gang leader who starts to use drugs islikely to be demoted. To the gang members, the habitual use of drugsand their kind of “acting out” delinquency are incompatible. In <strong>line</strong>79


with this attitude, a pusher who is a member of a gang will not tempta vulnerable fellow member, but will have no hesitati<strong>on</strong> about temptinga n<strong>on</strong>member or a member of another gang.Most boys who grow up in deprived areas are exposed to drugs. Agreat many experiment with their use. From whom do they get theirfirst dose? C<strong>on</strong>trary to widespread belief, most addicts were notinitiated into the habit by an adult narcotics peddler. <strong>On</strong>ly ten percentof the addicts whom we interviewed received their first dose from anadult. The overwhelming majority of the boys took their first dose ofheroin in the company of a single youngster in their own age group orwhile with a group of teenagers. This first trial of narcotics was freeto most of the boys (Research Center for Human Relati<strong>on</strong>s 1957a).Getting the first shot of narcotics <strong>on</strong> school property was the excepti<strong>on</strong>rather than the rule. In fact, most of the boys did not try heroinuntil their last year of school or later (Research Center for HumanRelati<strong>on</strong>s 1957a). That first dose was most often taken in the home of<strong>on</strong>e of the boys, although a large number first try heroin <strong>on</strong> thestreet, <strong>on</strong> a rooftop, or in a cellar. Frequently the first dose istaken shortly before going to a dance or party, presumably becausethe youngster thought it would be a bracer, giving him poise andcourage.But not all juveniles who try heroin become habitual users, and not allhabitual users become true addicts; that is, they are not hooked, notdependent <strong>on</strong> the drug.A juvenile drug user is by definiti<strong>on</strong> delinquent, since drugs areillegal. But am<strong>on</strong>g drug users, some were delinquent before theybegan using drugs, and others became delinquent in order to supporttheir habit. We know that the typical user lives in a poor, disorganizedneighborhood. But our research shows that the drug user whowas not delinquent prior to becoming a user is likely to come from afamily of slightly higher socioec<strong>on</strong>omic status than the users who werealso otherwise delinquent. For the sake of c<strong>on</strong>venience, we can speakof them as delinquent and n<strong>on</strong>delinquent users. It is probable thatthese two groups differ in certain aspects of their pers<strong>on</strong>alities, butall we can say at present is that the n<strong>on</strong>delinquent users appear to besomewhat more intelligent and more likely to remain in school bey<strong>on</strong>dthe tenth grade. They are also somewhat more oriented toward thefuture.But all juvenile addicts are severely disturbed individuals. Psychiatricresearch into the pers<strong>on</strong>ality of juvenile opiate addicts indicates thatadolescents who become addicts have major pers<strong>on</strong>ality disorders (Gerardand Kornetsky 1955). These disorders were evident either in overtadjustment problems or in serious intrapsychic c<strong>on</strong>flicts, usually both,prior to their involvement with drugs. Although there are markedindividual differences, a certain set of symptoms appears to be comm<strong>on</strong>to most juvenile addicts: They are not able to establish prol<strong>on</strong>ged,close, friendly relati<strong>on</strong>s with either peers or adults; they have difficultyin assuming a mascu<strong>line</strong> role; they are frequently overcome by a senseof futility, expectati<strong>on</strong> of failure, and general depressi<strong>on</strong>; and theyare easily frustrated and made anxious, finding both frustrati<strong>on</strong> andanxiety intolerable. <strong>On</strong>e may say that the potential addict suffersfrom a weak ego, an inadequately functi<strong>on</strong>ing superego, and inadequatemascu<strong>line</strong> identificati<strong>on</strong>.80


<strong>On</strong>e would expect that such serious pers<strong>on</strong>ality problems would beacquired in the family setting. And as we stated earlier, this isindeed the case. Addicts are most likely to come from families whichare not <strong>on</strong>ly ec<strong>on</strong>omically deprived, but families in which relati<strong>on</strong>sbetween parents are seriously disturbed, as evidenced by separati<strong>on</strong>,divorce, overt hostility, or lack of warmth and mutual interest. Aschildren, the addicts were either overindulged or harshly frustrated.Moreover, the parents are either pessimistic about their own future orhave the fatalistic attitude that life is a gamble (Research Center forHuman Relati<strong>on</strong>s 1956). They are also distrustful of representatives ofthe society, such as teachers and social workers. This combinati<strong>on</strong> ofattitudes toward themselves, toward society, and toward the boy arealmost certain to undermine his c<strong>on</strong>fidence in himself and dampenwhatever ambiti<strong>on</strong> and initiative he might otherwise have. With such abackground, and without familial support at adolescence, it is notprobable that the boy will have the strength necessary to stay awayfrom the delinquent subculture by which he is surrounded.The potential addict is much like the delinquent gang member in hisactivities, interests, and attitudes. But many gang members, as theyapproach adulthood, make their peace with society, find jobs, steadygirlfriends, and so <strong>on</strong>. But for the potential addict, with his weakself-c<strong>on</strong>fidence, the need to face adulthood creates the additi<strong>on</strong>alstress which often precipitates the <strong>on</strong>set of drug use. We know, forinstance, that the age of 16 is of special importance in the process ofaddicti<strong>on</strong>.Heroin reduces the pressure of the addict’s pers<strong>on</strong>al difficulties. Thepositive reacti<strong>on</strong> to a drug is not always immediate, but the addicti<strong>on</strong>pr<strong>on</strong>eyoungster will try again, hoping to capture the experience offeeling “high,” of increased c<strong>on</strong>fidence, of the serenity and relaxati<strong>on</strong>he can observe in the behavior of regular users. And the weaker theyoungster’s ego, the more likely he is to become an addict. While theless severely disturbed youngsters are satisfied with an occasi<strong>on</strong>alshot, the unhappy, anxious <strong>on</strong>es learn to use the drug as a means ofrelief from their everyday difficulties. In a less direct but morepervasive way, the use of the drug plays a malignantly adaptivefuncti<strong>on</strong> in their lives by making it easy for them to deny and to avoidfacing their deep-seated pers<strong>on</strong>al problems. The drug habit is a wayof life which takes the user outside real life. The habitual user ofheroin spends a good deal of time procuring and taking his dailydoses; he becomes less interested in sports, girls, parties.This picture of the addict, or the addicti<strong>on</strong>-pr<strong>on</strong>e youngster, is rathera general <strong>on</strong>e. There are, however, different kinds of narcoticsusers. These groups are not sharply differentiated, and little researchhas been d<strong>on</strong>e <strong>on</strong> them. <strong>On</strong>e of my students, however, has foundevidence of differences between two of the types. I believe that thec<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> of what is involved in these differences is quite centralto much of the discussi<strong>on</strong> of treatment approaches.There is an amazing paradox in the English treatment system. Theaddict within the system is limited to maintenance doses. As a c<strong>on</strong>sequenceof tolerance, he should be having no effects other than thepreventi<strong>on</strong> of withdrawal symptoms. Why not, then, get himselfhumanely detoxified and c<strong>on</strong>tinue without the threat of sudden withdrawal?Obviously, the addict who stays in the system is gettingsomething out of it that has nothing to do with the psychopharmacologicaleffects of the drug.81


In America, too, severe withdrawal reacti<strong>on</strong>s am<strong>on</strong>g heroin addictshave become quite rare. Many boys, for instance, when deprived ofdrugs because of some sort of detenti<strong>on</strong>, go through so mild a reacti<strong>on</strong>that the authorities do not recognize them as drug users. The clicheis that the real dosage levels in available heroin are so low that nosevere physiological dependency develops. Most users, however,c<strong>on</strong>tinue to take drugs, even if they seem to get little out of it.From the viewpoint of the abuser of drugs, there are three majorkinds of motivati<strong>on</strong>s: the psychopharmacological effects of the drug,motivati<strong>on</strong> that has to do with the taking of the drug rather than itseffects per se, and motivati<strong>on</strong> that has to do with the counternormativebehavior involved. An individual addict may be resp<strong>on</strong>ding to <strong>on</strong>e,two, or all three of these motivati<strong>on</strong>s.The important psychopharmacological effect sought--especially with theopiate drugs--is, I believe, detachment, not oblivi<strong>on</strong> nor the cloudingof c<strong>on</strong>sciousness nor euphoria, and certainly not vivid hallucinatoryexperiences, but rather the relief from overwhelming distress thatcomes with detachment. To be able to get this kind of relief, thedosage levels must exceed the levels of physiological tolerance. Evidenceindicates that for most c<strong>on</strong>temporary, urban addicts, this effectof the drug is, at most, a relatively minor asset.Of the three possible motivati<strong>on</strong>s for drug use, the <strong>on</strong>e I believe to bethe major factor in chr<strong>on</strong>ic urban opiate users is that taking it providessocial benefits that are an answer to emptiness. There are threeinterrelated benefits the addict acquires from his involvement withnarcotics: He gains an identity, <strong>on</strong>e posing little to live up to. Hegains a place in a subsociety where he is unequivocally accepted as apeer, a not-too-demanding place am<strong>on</strong>g his fellow men. He acquires acareer, at which he is reas<strong>on</strong>ably competent, devoted to maintaininghis supply, avoiding the police, and the rituals of taking the drug.If he is arrested, this provides an alternate phase of the identicalcareer. In the instituti<strong>on</strong>, whether jail or hospital, he still has hisidentity and after a time may become a model and guide to newcomers.If, in the instituti<strong>on</strong>, he has no great need for the drug, it is becausehis other needs are being met. It is not he who has changed, but hissituati<strong>on</strong>, and <strong>on</strong>ly temporarily.The third motivati<strong>on</strong> menti<strong>on</strong>ed, that having to do with counternormativebehavior, is seen in individuals who are deeply alienated from society,but who have sufficient inner resources left to want to hit back. Forsuch pers<strong>on</strong>s, drugs, any drugs, are attractive precisely to the extentthat their use is frowned up<strong>on</strong>, c<strong>on</strong>demned, and persecuted by therepresentatives of the respectable society.82


Incomplete Mourning andAddict/Family Transacti<strong>on</strong>sA Theory for UnderstandingHeroin <strong>Abuse</strong>Sandra B. Coleman, Ph.D.INTRODUCTIONRecent developments in the drug abuse field suggest that drug-takingbehavior is a functi<strong>on</strong> of certain variables that emerge from the psychosocialenvir<strong>on</strong>ment of the family. Rather than focusing <strong>on</strong> individualdynamics as the source of <strong>on</strong>e’s need for drugs, the family’s interlocking,transacti<strong>on</strong>al patterns are c<strong>on</strong>sidered essential elements of compulsivedrug abuse. Theoretical explanati<strong>on</strong>s indicate that drugs play animportant role in maintaining family homeostasis or equilibrium. As asubset of psychosocial theory, family systems theory explains how thefamily encourages, reinforces, and sustains drug-seeking behavior(Harbin and Maziar 1975; Klagsbrun and Davis 1977; Seldin 1972;Stant<strong>on</strong> 1979d).The theoretical perspective presented in this chapter is derived fromfamily systems theory; it includes major c<strong>on</strong>structs, such as homeostasis,role selecti<strong>on</strong>, intergenerati<strong>on</strong>al boundaries, etc., and their specificadaptati<strong>on</strong>s to the drug abuse field (Steinglass 1976; Stant<strong>on</strong> 1977a;Stant<strong>on</strong> and Coleman 1979; Coleman 1979a). This model focuses <strong>on</strong>death, separati<strong>on</strong>, and loss as significant precursors of drug abuse,given the necessary addicti<strong>on</strong>-producing elements of family behavior(Stant<strong>on</strong> 1977a; Stant<strong>on</strong> and Coleman 1979; Coleman 1979a).Because the family, rather than the individual, is the designatedpatient, the term “drug addict family is used to refer to those familiesin which at least <strong>on</strong>e member is engaged in compulsive drug use in amanner that suggests physical and psychological dependency. Thegeneral focus is <strong>on</strong> narcotics addicti<strong>on</strong>--mainly heroin--and the distinguishingdeath-related family processes and properties that appear tobe associated with it.Specifically, this theory of drug addicti<strong>on</strong> suggests that the addictivebehavior is a functi<strong>on</strong> of an unusual number of traumatic or premature83


deaths, separati<strong>on</strong>s, and losses which are not effectively resolved ormourned. The homeostatic family processes and feedback mechanismsmake heroin abuse a likely resp<strong>on</strong>se for coping with the overwhelmingstress associated with the loss experience. <strong>Drug</strong> use further servesto keep the abusing member helpless and dependent <strong>on</strong> the family, aprocess which unifies and sustains family intactness. Within thecomplex set of interlocking behaviors, there is an overall sense ofhopelessness and a lack of purpose or meaning in life which accompaniesthe repetitious cycle of family transacti<strong>on</strong>s.THEORY OVERVIEWDEATH AS A PSYCHOSOCIAL ISSUEDeath has c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>ally been regarded as the logical cessati<strong>on</strong> of life,the other end of the birth phenomen<strong>on</strong>. Except for its relati<strong>on</strong>ship totheological issues and philosophical thought, death was associated mostoften with the terminally ill or the elderly. In recent years, however,views of death have changed. In the late 1960s. Kubler-Ross undertookher classic study of dying patients and de<strong>line</strong>ated four stages whichterminal patients seem to experience prior to their death. Subsequently,she expanded her original work and presented her view of death asthe final stage of human growth (1975). This idea of death as anintegral part of life was shared by Becker (1973). the major theme ofhis psychological mystico-religious writing being that death served acentral functi<strong>on</strong> to all mankind.Bey<strong>on</strong>d the view of death as an individual experience is the c<strong>on</strong>cept ofdeath in its social c<strong>on</strong>text. Some years ago, Hamovitch (1964) andWahl (1960) suggested that the family system of the dying pers<strong>on</strong> hadnot been given enough attenti<strong>on</strong>. The dying pers<strong>on</strong> does not die al<strong>on</strong>ebut in relati<strong>on</strong>ship to others--family, friends, etc. (Pattis<strong>on</strong> 1977).Kastenbaum and Aisenberg (1972) propose that the dying memberassumes special status in the family and may even serve as a symbolicrepresentati<strong>on</strong> of all of the family’s deceased ancestors. They giveparticular attenti<strong>on</strong> to the social participati<strong>on</strong> imposed by death, a viewfirst expressed by Slater (1964), who was intrigued by the way peoplesurround the corpse at a funeral, giving it love and attenti<strong>on</strong>. Kastenbaumand Aisenberg add to the noti<strong>on</strong> that death accelerates groupinteracti<strong>on</strong>al processes by noting that the dying pers<strong>on</strong> also participatesin idiosyncratic rituals related to the terminal c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. This causesthe dying pers<strong>on</strong> to become a participant in his or her own death.The authors feel that the closer <strong>on</strong>e is to death, the greater theprobability of becoming an active part of the process.The functi<strong>on</strong>al or purposive nature of death suggests that it mayprecipitate additi<strong>on</strong>al types of behavior. Recently, Eisenstadt (1978)proposed a theory of the eminence of genius as a c<strong>on</strong>sequent of parentalbereavement. Eisenstadt states that there is a creative mourningprocess that “is related to a sequence of events whereby the losstriggers off a crisis requiring mastery <strong>on</strong> the part of the bereavedindividual. . . . If the crisis is worked through, that is, if the destructiveelements and the depressive features of the experience of bereavementare neutralized, then a creative product or creatively integratedpers<strong>on</strong>ality can result.” Eisenstadt suggests that a major interveningvariable between the death of a parent and the desire for fame, eminence,and occupati<strong>on</strong>al excellence is the nature of the family unit84


prior to the disruptive period preceding the death. The author offerssupport for his theory by rec<strong>on</strong>structing parental loss profiles of 699eminent pers<strong>on</strong>s who experienced early loss of <strong>on</strong>e or both parents.Comparative orphanhood data from the general populati<strong>on</strong>, i.e., actuarialinformati<strong>on</strong>, indicated that the eminent group had a c<strong>on</strong>siderablygreater degree of parental loss. Comparis<strong>on</strong>s with delinquent groups,however, showed that they were orphaned at rates comparable to thosefound am<strong>on</strong>g the eminents. Thus, Eisenstadt suggests that the criticalissue is not necessarily the loss itself but the way it is mastered; theeminent group seemed to invest c<strong>on</strong>siderable energy in intellectualpursuits, which may represent <strong>on</strong>e creative approach to coping withbereavement.The important questi<strong>on</strong> arising from Eisenstadt’s theory is, what happenswhen bereavement is not mastered? The delinquency data suggest thatthe inability to mourn creatively may well be a functi<strong>on</strong> of familycharacteristics which emerge at the time a member dies. If this is thecase, the important variable is not death but the family transacti<strong>on</strong>sand interrelati<strong>on</strong>ships that lead to the successful or unsuccessfulresoluti<strong>on</strong> of death.Death, Loss, and Separati<strong>on</strong>Background. The basic tenets of the present theory were developedseveral years before Eisenstadt’s work was published (Coleman 1975),but the central c<strong>on</strong>cepts are remarkably similar. The foundati<strong>on</strong> ofthis theoretical model lies in a study that I began in the early 1970s.From doing therapy with recovering heroin addicts and their families, Iobserved somewhat serendipitously a recurring pattern of unusualdeaths which had occurred many years earlier, yet which still seemedto have profound effects <strong>on</strong> the surviving family members. This led toa pilot investigati<strong>on</strong> of the histories of 25 drug addict families, theprimary purpose of which was to determine the prevalence of death intwo generati<strong>on</strong>s, i.e., the family of procreati<strong>on</strong> and the family oforigin. Severe or life-threatening illnesses were also studied becausecritical illness is so often followed by death. <strong>On</strong>ly the untimely,premature, or unexpected deaths were quantified; deaths resultingfrom normal aging processes were not included in analyzing the data.Thus, the majority of deaths included in this study took place duringthe addicts’ or parents’ developmental years. Results indicated thatsome families felt the impact of more than <strong>on</strong>e death; 18 (72 percent)experienced at least <strong>on</strong>e traumatic or unexpected loss of a loved <strong>on</strong>e.Seventeen (68 percent) were witness to a severe or unusual illness,and a similar number of families had an alcoholic parent or sibling ineither of the two generati<strong>on</strong>s studied. When the variables were combined,13 families (52 percent) experienced death and severe illness,and 12 families (48 percent) were found to have death and alcoholism intheir backgrounds. Eleven (44 percent) of the families had a combinati<strong>on</strong>of illness and alcoholism, but when alcoholism was subsumed under thecategory of illness, there were 24 families affected. The latter figuresuggests that 96 percent of all the families studied were in some wayaffected by either alcoholism or some other chr<strong>on</strong>ic debilitating illness.Nine families (36 percent) experienced a combinati<strong>on</strong> of death, illness,and alcoholism. Although this was not a c<strong>on</strong>trolled study, these datasuggest that this is an area that needs further systematic investigati<strong>on</strong>.Further clinical evidence of the significance of death to addict familiesemerged from findings that death and death-related issues were major85


themes in my group therapy sessi<strong>on</strong>s with siblings of recovering addicts(Coleman 1978a,b) . In additi<strong>on</strong> to talking about death, this smallgroup of 20 preadolescents experienced several traumatic family deathsduring the course of the two-year project.Supporting Research. A comprehensive review of the literature <strong>on</strong>death, separati<strong>on</strong>, and loss appears in previously published literatureand will not be elaborated up<strong>on</strong> here (Coleman and Stant<strong>on</strong> 1978;Stant<strong>on</strong> and Coleman 1979; Coleman 1979a). It is important to emphasize,however, that there are three central sources of support forviewing death and death-related phenomena as major theoretical comp<strong>on</strong>ents.The first is that addict deaths at an early age occur relatively often,suggesting that there may be an intrinsic suicidal element (Colemanand Stant<strong>on</strong> 1978; Stant<strong>on</strong> and Coleman 1979). The discriminati<strong>on</strong>between intenti<strong>on</strong>al and accidental drug overdose is difficult and goesbey<strong>on</strong>d the purpose of this chapter; however, it is important to notethat death is a frequent corollary to drug-abusing behavior.In additi<strong>on</strong> to the statistics <strong>on</strong> addict deaths, and more central to thistheoretical positi<strong>on</strong>, is the degree to which an unusual number ofuntimely deaths occur am<strong>on</strong>g addict family members. Supportive evidencefor the data presented in the pilot study discussed earlier(Coleman 1975) indicates that there is a high incidence of early loss ofat least <strong>on</strong>e of the addict’s parents due to death (Ellinwood et al.1966; Blum and Associates 1972b; Miller 1974; Harbin and Maziar 1975;Klagsbrun and Davis 1977). It is interesting to note that a morerecent study <strong>on</strong> treatment outcome (Harris and Linn 1978) found that<strong>on</strong>e of the few background characteristics that significantly differentiatedheroin addicts from n<strong>on</strong>heroin drug users was that the heroinaddicts were more likely to have experienced the death of their fathersbefore the age of 16.Sec<strong>on</strong>d, the prevalence of death symbols further reflects the uniquerole which death plays in addict families. From my study of the roles,communicati<strong>on</strong>s, and interacti<strong>on</strong>s within the 25 families (Coleman 1975)three symbolic, death-related phases could be distinguished <strong>on</strong> theaddicti<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tinuum, i.e., the imminence of death (early drug use);the funeral (removal from the home to a residential therapeutic community);and the resurrecti<strong>on</strong> (family treatment). In this sense addicti<strong>on</strong>is analogous to a slow dying process. Coleman and Stant<strong>on</strong> (1978)suggest that addicti<strong>on</strong> facilitates the family’s death-related participatorybehavior. By treating the drug abuser as if he or she is going througha slow, tedious death, the family members are able to perpetuate(vis-a-vis the addict) the premature and unresolved death of a formermember. The addict thus becomes a substitute or revenant of thedeceased. This is c<strong>on</strong>sistent with Stant<strong>on</strong>’s (1977b) view of the addictas the sacrificial member who martyrs himself/herself in order to fulfillthe family’s need for a death. Stant<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>siders that the addict’s roleas “savior” allows the family to become mutually involved in a suicidalc<strong>on</strong>spiracy.Finally, in additi<strong>on</strong> to separati<strong>on</strong> caused by real death, any type offamilial disengagement is particularly difficult for addict families.Stant<strong>on</strong> (1977a, 1979d) and Stant<strong>on</strong> and Coleman (1979) have writtenextensively about the c<strong>on</strong>flicting elements of separati<strong>on</strong> and doubt thatit is mere coincidence that drug use becomes intensified during adolescencewhen separati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>flicts are at a peak. As Stant<strong>on</strong> et al.86


(1978) point out, heroin abuse is a “paradoxical resoluti<strong>on</strong>” to growingup and leaving the family. The drug permits the user to leave as ameans of establishing some independence, but it also facilitates thereturn to the hearth when it is time to “crash.” This perpetuates thecyclical pattern of leaving and not leaving, keeping the heroin addictstraddled between home and the outside world of drugs. The profoundc<strong>on</strong>flict which separati<strong>on</strong> presents for these families has been discussedextensively in other publicati<strong>on</strong>s.Religiosity and Philosophical Meaning of LifeAkin to exploring the role of death in addict families is the investigati<strong>on</strong>of the functi<strong>on</strong> of religi<strong>on</strong> 1 in family life. The family’s religiousbeliefs or philosophical systems of thought are apt to be the majorinterface between death and the future pattern of adaptati<strong>on</strong>. A senseof faith may either alleviate or exacerbate the c<strong>on</strong>comitant sorrow,rage. and guilt that accompany or follow the loss of a loved <strong>on</strong>e.Feifel (1959) feels that, in additi<strong>on</strong> to other factors, <strong>on</strong>e’s religiousorientati<strong>on</strong> and coping mechanisms are str<strong>on</strong>gly related to that individual’spers<strong>on</strong>al reacti<strong>on</strong> to death. The major thesis underlying Frankl’s(1963) logotherapeutic system is that the primary life force is thesearch for meaning. He suggests that the loss of feeling creates an“existential vacuum” in which <strong>on</strong>e lacks a rati<strong>on</strong>ale for living, thuscreating hopelessness and despair. He even explains alcoholism as afuncti<strong>on</strong> of the “existential vacuum” and further suggests that thefrustrated will to meaning may be compensated for by the substituti<strong>on</strong>of a will to pleasure. Could <strong>on</strong>e then suppose, in view of such atheoretical premise, that drug addicti<strong>on</strong> is also a means of coping withthe spiritual void?In Blum and Associates 1 (1972b) study of high- and low-drug-riskfamilies, the role of religi<strong>on</strong> was found to be significant with respectto the developmental trend of its incepti<strong>on</strong>. For example, high-drugriskfamilies were uncertain about their belief in God and tended toallow their children to determine their own beliefs. As the childrenbegan to reach preadolescence, however, the parents became worriedand began to c<strong>on</strong>sider forcing their children to become exposed toformal religi<strong>on</strong>. In c<strong>on</strong>trast, the low-drug-risk families affirmed astr<strong>on</strong>g belief in God’s existence during the early childhood years butafter adolescence did not insist <strong>on</strong> church attendance. They felt thatthey had instilled the foundati<strong>on</strong>s for belief and were not preoccupiedwith religious participati<strong>on</strong>, per se. It is important to note, however,that Blum and Associates 1 definiti<strong>on</strong> of religi<strong>on</strong> is a traditi<strong>on</strong>al <strong>on</strong>e andrefers to church attendance and formal doctrine as opposed to thebroader c<strong>on</strong>cept used in the present theory.Although these findings are interesting, it is felt that the nature ofthe interacti<strong>on</strong>s between child and parents is perhaps more importantthan the specific religious practices in which they are engaged.1 Religi<strong>on</strong>, or religiosity, as used here extends bey<strong>on</strong>d formal doctrineand includes any system of philosophical belief which represents a specificview about the meaning of life. Thus, the term “religi<strong>on</strong>”embraces a sociological view or weltanschauung that includes the c<strong>on</strong>ceptualizati<strong>on</strong>of the purpose of <strong>on</strong>e’s existence. This is c<strong>on</strong>sideredas <strong>on</strong>e of the motivating forces which guide purposive behavior--aninternal determinant, to some extent, of <strong>on</strong>e’s life process.87


Kastenbaum and Aisenberg (1972) relate object loss, in the form ofdeath, to alienati<strong>on</strong> from God in that significant loss tends to increasethe fear of further loss. In additi<strong>on</strong>, they suggest that the fear ofwhat happens after death can be involved with the threat of punishment.Such fears can result in severe alienati<strong>on</strong> from God and religi<strong>on</strong>, whichagain impinges <strong>on</strong> the role of rejecti<strong>on</strong>. The loss or lack of a beliefsystem may, especially in c<strong>on</strong>juncti<strong>on</strong> with the loss of the loved <strong>on</strong>e,produce even more feelings of despair, helplessness, and loss of power,thus reinforcing the depressive state. In a study focusing <strong>on</strong> religi<strong>on</strong>as a critical influence <strong>on</strong> attitudes toward death am<strong>on</strong>g religious andn<strong>on</strong>religious students, Alexander and Alderstein (1959) found some significantdifferences, yet failed to find the anticipated degree of variance.The authors explained the results as indicating that both groups had areligious belief system and were not in doubt or c<strong>on</strong>flict about theirreligiosity.Although some of the evidence is still inc<strong>on</strong>clusive, there is overallc<strong>on</strong>sensus that, with regard to alcohol abuse, religi<strong>on</strong>s that supportabstinence are apt to have fewer problem drinkers am<strong>on</strong>g their populati<strong>on</strong>s(Maddox 1970; Snyder 1958; Gusfield 1970). Even more significantthan doctrinal orientati<strong>on</strong> toward alcohol is the evidence that religi<strong>on</strong> isa c<strong>on</strong>sistent predictor of those who can be expected not to use drugs.Although the data have not sufficiently explained the reas<strong>on</strong>s for therelati<strong>on</strong>ship between religi<strong>on</strong> and drug use, the associati<strong>on</strong> between thetwo is supported by results from a relatively large number of investigati<strong>on</strong>s(Gorsuch and Butler 1976a). These authors suggest that thecorrelati<strong>on</strong> between religi<strong>on</strong> and abstinence may be due to the fact thatan individual’s basic needs are most likely to be met by traditi<strong>on</strong>alparental socializati<strong>on</strong> factors, which generally include the instituti<strong>on</strong> ofreligi<strong>on</strong>. This c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> appears even more likely when it is c<strong>on</strong>sideredin c<strong>on</strong>juncti<strong>on</strong> with Blum and Associates 1 (1972b) findings that youngstersfrom traditi<strong>on</strong>al families, regardless of social class, race, orethnicity, are least apt to engage in drug usage.The drug experience itself is steeped in what might be c<strong>on</strong>sidered ahybrid versi<strong>on</strong> of c<strong>on</strong>temporary religi<strong>on</strong>. The administrati<strong>on</strong> of drugsis surrounded by ritual, including verbal and n<strong>on</strong>verbal gestures,music produced by instruments that emit unique, captivating, andhaunting sounds and an aura of mystical sacrifice. The spiritual statethat evolves during the “high” imposes a sense of love, awareness,and communi<strong>on</strong>. Percepti<strong>on</strong>s are heightened during intense drugexperiences which alter the state of c<strong>on</strong>sciousness and create a senseof being at <strong>on</strong>e with others and with nature. These rites can readilybe compared with traditi<strong>on</strong>al religious cerem<strong>on</strong>ies or services and thepowerful gestures of the priest, minister, or rabbi in regal garb. Theexperience derived from the inducti<strong>on</strong> of drugs simulates the effect ofthe choir and the res<strong>on</strong>ating organ.It is also important to c<strong>on</strong>sider some of the recent treatment alternativesfor drug addicti<strong>on</strong>. Religious groups such as Hare Krishna and theborn-again Christians are often successful in c<strong>on</strong>verting the drug addictinto a religious advocate. The strength of newfound “religi<strong>on</strong>s” in diminishingdrug use indicates that, at least for some addicts, adopting abelief can make an important difference. The fact that the Eastern religi<strong>on</strong>stend to dominate is particularly pertinent. This may be due tothe fact that Eastern philosophy is more readily integrated into <strong>on</strong>e’sdaily life.88


Whether or not <strong>on</strong>e believes in God, Christ, Buddha, or any otherformal deity or doctrine is not felt to be as significant as the fact thata spiritual philosophy has been pers<strong>on</strong>ally derived. As Alexander andAdlerstein (1959) noted in their study, death anxiety was not thatdifferent between religious and n<strong>on</strong>religious groups. This leads to thepostulate that belief in any system--deism, atheism, etc.--is in itself aresoluti<strong>on</strong> and represents a philosophical-religious c<strong>on</strong>struct regardinglife and the meaning derived from <strong>on</strong>e’s life experiences. The lack ofsuch a system is then similar to being n<strong>on</strong>committed, which can lead tofeeling helpless, powerless, and frustrated. If the loss of a significantloved <strong>on</strong>e results in a sense of loss of a viable self as well as the lossof belief in a viable other, including God and/or spiritual faith, thenit is logical to assume that there may be a loss of total meaning to<strong>on</strong>e’s existence, so that drugs may represent a search for and adefense against <strong>on</strong>e’s own mortality.Certainly <strong>on</strong>e’s value system and <strong>on</strong>e’s religious orientati<strong>on</strong> evolve fromwithin the family system. In order to understand how families resp<strong>on</strong>dto death and loss, the family value system regarding the philosophy oflife needs explorati<strong>on</strong>.SPECIAL POPULATIONSThe extensi<strong>on</strong> of the incomplete loss theory to other populati<strong>on</strong>s gainssupport from a nati<strong>on</strong>al drug abuse survey (Coleman 1976; Colemanand Davis 1978). where separati<strong>on</strong> and loss were reported as relevantissues in many families. Further comparis<strong>on</strong> of characteristics of drugabusers from multiethnic families suggests that a comm<strong>on</strong> element isthat of loss and separati<strong>on</strong> due to divorce, marital breakup, or death.<strong>On</strong>e of the most striking types of loss exists am<strong>on</strong>g the Navajo, whoare in danger of losing their religious rituals to the new revivalistsects. <strong>On</strong>e sensitive worker has said, “Unless the Indian can keep hisrituals, he will most assuredly die” (Coleman 1979b). A dispute withthe Hopi also threatens them with a severe land loss and c<strong>on</strong>comitantdeprivati<strong>on</strong> of large numbers of livestock. Navajo counselors feel thatthe stripping of cultural needs exacerbates and c<strong>on</strong>tributes to addicti<strong>on</strong>.SUMMARYIn summary, this theory is based <strong>on</strong> the premise that death, separati<strong>on</strong>,and loss are significant etiological factors in heroin-addict families.The death and death-related variables are integral parts of a homeostaticpattern that keeps the drug-abusing member helpless and dependent<strong>on</strong> staying at home with the family. Within the complex set of feedbackmechanisms involved in the drug-taking process lies an overall senseof family hopelessness and lack of purpose or meaning in life whichaccompanies the repetitive drug-sustaining cycle of family interacti<strong>on</strong>s.89


The Social Deviant andInitial Addicti<strong>on</strong> toNarcotics and AlcoholHarris E. Hill, Ph.D.Generally research <strong>on</strong> the addicti<strong>on</strong>s has been c<strong>on</strong>cerned with variousphases of chr<strong>on</strong>ic intoxicati<strong>on</strong> and relapse, or with behavioral changesthat accompany these phases of addicti<strong>on</strong>. The present suggesti<strong>on</strong>s,<strong>on</strong> the c<strong>on</strong>trary, are mainly directed toward study of the developmentof initial addicti<strong>on</strong> and the possible significance of social deviance andthe psychopathic pers<strong>on</strong>ality in this process. Definite evidence ofsocial pathology in all preaddicti<strong>on</strong> pers<strong>on</strong>alities is lacking. There isnow good reas<strong>on</strong> to believe, however, that in the United States allalcoholic and narcotic addicts studied as groups show social devianceas the <strong>on</strong>ly comm<strong>on</strong> characteristic, and that this characteristic existedprior to addicti<strong>on</strong>.For the present discussi<strong>on</strong> it will be assumed, in c<strong>on</strong>trast to viewssuch as those stated by Lindesmith (1947), that alcoholics and narcoticaddicts in general are social deviants prior to the initial addicti<strong>on</strong>.This does not imply that all such individuals are aggressive and antisocial.In this respect it is perhaps unfortunate that “psychopathicdeviate” was used as a label for this scale of the MMPI which differentiatesat a high level between individuals who are fairly well adjustedin our society and those who exhibit a diverse array of social pathology.It may be that a generic term, such as “c<strong>on</strong>duct disorder,” would bemore appropriate (Hill et al. 1960; Meehle 1956). Camer<strong>on</strong> and Magaret(1951) cogently state that although some social deviants are aggressivelyantisocial, many are simply “inept” or “inadequate” pers<strong>on</strong>alities.This paper, prepared by Jack E. Nels<strong>on</strong> and reviewed by Harris E.Hill, is based largely <strong>on</strong> an earlier publicati<strong>on</strong> written by Dr. Hill,titled “The Social Deviant and Initial Addicti<strong>on</strong> to Narcotics andAlcohol.” It is reprinted by permissi<strong>on</strong> from Quarterly Journal of<strong>Studies</strong> <strong>on</strong> Alcohol, vol. 23, pp. 562-582, 1962. Copyright by Journalof <strong>Studies</strong> <strong>on</strong> Alcohol, Inc., New Brunswick, New Jersey 08903.90


The present discussi<strong>on</strong> of the social deviant is an attempt to discovermore fully the behavioral characteristics which make him uniquelysusceptible to the effects of narcotics and alcohol. Identificati<strong>on</strong> andclassificati<strong>on</strong> of deviant attitudes and overt resp<strong>on</strong>ses appears to bethe most critical and the most difficult task to accomplish in research<strong>on</strong> the psychopath. If this could be d<strong>on</strong>e with even a fair degree ofsuccess, criteria might be available for the study of antecedents, forthe predicti<strong>on</strong> of behavioral trends which result from particular antecedents,and for the predicti<strong>on</strong> of specific drug effects which are acceptableand desirable to particular pers<strong>on</strong>alities.There appear to be several powerful interacting factors which determinethe vulnerability of the social deviant to initial addicti<strong>on</strong>. The first,which has been discussed at some length by others, is that suchbehavioral equipment is found most frequently in the underprivilegedand slum areas in which opiates and other drug supplies have “high”availability (Chein and Rosenfeld 1957; Cohen 1955; Clausen 1957) andin which both narcotic addicti<strong>on</strong> and alcoholism are comm<strong>on</strong>. Theenvir<strong>on</strong>mental c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s which produce the deviant in these areas alsoprovide more ready access to opiates than in the larger society, andwith regard to both opiates and alcohol, provide a greater degree ofexposure to models of excessive use. But, to a more limited degree,this would appear to hold also for the social deviant in all societalstrata. Sec<strong>on</strong>dly, lack of social c<strong>on</strong>trols (shared resp<strong>on</strong>ses) appearsto determine the degree of acceptability, to the deviant, of experimentati<strong>on</strong>with drugs as well as with other forms of unusual behavior(Chein and Rosenfeld 1957). Although a certain degree of privati<strong>on</strong>and social isolati<strong>on</strong> in the “fringe” areas are c<strong>on</strong>tributing factors tosocial deviance as well as to addicti<strong>on</strong>, they appear to be neithernecessary nor sufficient causal antecedents of such behavior. Thedescripti<strong>on</strong>s given by Chein and Rosenfeld (1957) and by Clausen(1957) of n<strong>on</strong>delinquent n<strong>on</strong>addict adolescents and their families residentin deprived areas suggest that familial discip<strong>line</strong>, and inculcati<strong>on</strong> ofother shared resp<strong>on</strong>ses, such as a variety of interests and activities,provide deterrents to the use of drugs and other deviant behavior.In c<strong>on</strong>trast, but in keeping with the psychopath of the deprivedareas, the social deviant of the middle class, while not deprived ecologically,usually has a family background which provides inc<strong>on</strong>sistent orunrealistic discip<strong>line</strong> and little c<strong>on</strong>sistent warm guidance in developinginterests. Thus when adolescence and, finally, adulthood arrive,individuals have not developed behavior which is appropriate for eithertheir status or their age, and could not be expected to exhibit socialc<strong>on</strong>trols which they have not acquired.It seems reas<strong>on</strong>able to assume that the degree of social deviance exhibitedby an individual is a measure of the effectiveness of his socialc<strong>on</strong>trols, and that the degree of such effectiveness is determined bythe development of preferences and inhibiti<strong>on</strong>s which are held incomm<strong>on</strong> by the larger society. The social deviant is deficient inreacti<strong>on</strong>s of self-criticism, counteranxiety, or “guilt” which might deterunusual behavior. Since the social deviant is deficient in these socialvalues or shared resp<strong>on</strong>ses of the larger society, counteranxiety is lowand retrial or c<strong>on</strong>tinuance of the use of drugs is acceptable.In additi<strong>on</strong> to being deficient in social c<strong>on</strong>trols, the deviant appears tobe more accepting of short-term satisfacti<strong>on</strong>s, or at least less able todefer short-term gains for l<strong>on</strong>g-range satisfacti<strong>on</strong>. Few experimentalbut many clinical data indicate in this regard that the social deviantdoes not gain the degree of satisfacti<strong>on</strong> (reinforcement) from daily91


pursuits that the “normal” individual does (Chein and Rosenfeld 1957;Cohen 1955; Clausen 1957). Stable interests which provide c<strong>on</strong>tinuedreinforcement were found to be present in general in teenage n<strong>on</strong>delinquentswho were not drug “users” but who lived in “high use” areas,whereas there was a paucity of such interests in comparable teenageaddicts. The deviant thus appears to be more vulnerable to repetiti<strong>on</strong>and c<strong>on</strong>tinuance of unusual activities that provide even temporarysatisfacti<strong>on</strong>s. With fewer social deterrents to drug use, and c<strong>on</strong>comitantlyfewer satisfying daily pursuits, it would be predicted thatdrug-produced euphoria is more acceptable to and more easily inducedin the deviant.Euphoria as an acceptable drug effect is, clearly, not exclusivelyassociated with social deviance--the functi<strong>on</strong>s of the cocktail party arenot directed entirely toward business or political ends. At present,although few have difficulty in accepting clinical definiti<strong>on</strong>s and selfreport,“euphoria” has no precise, scientific referent.A c<strong>on</strong>siderable number of narcotic addicts state that their initial trialof opiates was extremely pleasant. Although an estimate of the proporti<strong>on</strong>cannot be made at present, some of these individuals used opiatesfirst to alleviate alcohol withdrawal symptoms. Other addicts maintainthat their initial use of opiates was very unpleasant, but that throughrepeated trials the effects became very desirable. C<strong>on</strong>tinued use evenmakes vomiting a “good sick.” It thus seems reas<strong>on</strong>able to assumethat social deviants attain euphoria more easily than normal pers<strong>on</strong>s,since they find experimentati<strong>on</strong> with drugs acceptable, and sinceacceptability, desirability, and euphoria are closely allied.The above appear to be the chief factors which produce the specialvulnerability of social deviants to addicti<strong>on</strong>. They are deficient indaily pursuits which are reinforced by and bring satisfacti<strong>on</strong> to thelarger society; they are not deterred from unusual behavior by counteranxiety,which in the “mature” adult can be partially identified asinhibiti<strong>on</strong>s; because of these deficiencies they are especially susceptibleto short-term satisfacti<strong>on</strong>s, and if drugs are available they can themselvesrapidly manipulate their pers<strong>on</strong>al state.If these views have <strong>on</strong>ly partial validity the devising of such an ideallearning situati<strong>on</strong> in the laboratory would be difficult. It must bec<strong>on</strong>sidered, also, that both opioids and alcohol produce many effects,in additi<strong>on</strong> to those menti<strong>on</strong>ed, which may be desirable to social deviantsbut which do not seem to be peculiar to them.The lowering of social c<strong>on</strong>trols and the producti<strong>on</strong> of euphoria bydrugs has received little attenti<strong>on</strong> in the literature, compared to drugproducedalterati<strong>on</strong> in pain and discomfort, anxiety and depressi<strong>on</strong>,and c<strong>on</strong>flict and aggressi<strong>on</strong>. Since these latter effects presumably canbe attained in the n<strong>on</strong>deviant individual (and the n<strong>on</strong>addict), sucheffects per se do not appear to be the critical elements in the processof initial addicti<strong>on</strong>. But an individual so unfortunate as to be sociallydeviant and at the same time either neurotic or schizoid is doublyvulnerable to addicti<strong>on</strong>, since some indicati<strong>on</strong>s of these tendencies can92


e altered by drugs (Haertzen and Hill 1959). 1 Unfavorable c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>sare still further compounded when withdrawal symptoms appear whichcan be alleviated by c<strong>on</strong>tinued drug use. With these additi<strong>on</strong>al factors,it would appear that no investigator, even in the most euphoric moments,has even approximated the devising of such optimal c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s forlearning. With such an array of behavioral determinants, any learningtheoretician could find support for whatever systematic positi<strong>on</strong> he orshe wished to assume. It may well be that this c<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong> of reinforcements<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>e form of behavior--drug use--is partially explanatoryof the strength of both alcoholism and opioid addicti<strong>on</strong>, “loss of c<strong>on</strong>trol”with respect to these substances, and the resistance of the addicti<strong>on</strong>sto therapy.<strong>On</strong>e of the most difficult problems in the etiology of the addicti<strong>on</strong>s,and <strong>on</strong>e which apparently has a direct c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong> with specific effectsof drugs, is c<strong>on</strong>cerned with the use of a particular agent when othersare equally available. Alcohol and opiates, although having someeffects in comm<strong>on</strong>, perhaps even some comm<strong>on</strong> effects <strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>flict andanxiety, frequently produce diametrically oppposite acti<strong>on</strong>s. Althoughno study is available which compares the initial use of alcohol andopiates in naive subjects, a not insignificant number of narcotic addictsreport previous alcoholism. It is known also that initially the verygreat majority of narcotic addicts have experimented with alcohol andthat it is as available to them, or more so, than are narcotics. Frequentlythey maintain that they become aggressive and assaultive, orcomatose, under alcohol. To them, these effects are opposite to thepreferred acti<strong>on</strong>s of the opiates. Especially in the social deviantalcohol may produce euphoria, reduce c<strong>on</strong>flict, and make possible theoccurrence of behavior which was inhibited by either c<strong>on</strong>flict or counteranxiety.It thus seems apparent that alcohol and opiates differentiallybut specifically alter the probability of occurrence of particular classesof resp<strong>on</strong>ses.Briefly, in this c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>, it is assumed for the general case that thebehavioral equipment of the individual is composed of specific resp<strong>on</strong>sesor resp<strong>on</strong>se patterns which have certain probabilities of occurrence(strength) in any given situati<strong>on</strong>. Since different resp<strong>on</strong>ses of theindividual differ in strength, they form a resp<strong>on</strong>se hierarchy for agiven situati<strong>on</strong> ranging from the resp<strong>on</strong>se which is most likely to thatwhich is least likely to occur (Hull 1934; Miller and Dollard 1941). Asan organizing principle in research <strong>on</strong> psychopharmacology, and for itsapplicability to the addicti<strong>on</strong>s, it is here hypothesized that drugsrearrange the individual’s resp<strong>on</strong>se hierarchy in ways which are specificfor a particular drug and for a given situati<strong>on</strong>. (C<strong>on</strong>ger [1956]presented a somewhat similar formulati<strong>on</strong> for some of the acti<strong>on</strong>s ofalcohol.)Psychodynamic mechanisms by which desirability (to the user) of drugeffects are determined have been proposed by many, but few havefocused <strong>on</strong> social deviance in this process. However, since deviantsmust live in a society to which they are not well adapted, they not<strong>on</strong>ly face the difficulties encountered by the average individual but1 Probably both neurotic and schizoid tendencies involve anxiety andcounteranxiety, but it appears evident that when these reacti<strong>on</strong>s arecombined with social deviance, the inhibiting effects of counteranxietyare not as effective as are the reinforcing effects of the drug.93


also those imposed by their own differences and deficiencies in behavior.If it is postulated, as in the present paper, that certain drug-producedchanges are acceptable to the social deviant, the previously givenfactor classificati<strong>on</strong> is suggestive of some of the alterati<strong>on</strong>s whichmight be desirable. Since some of the behavioral acti<strong>on</strong>s of narcoticsand alcohol are known, it suggested that the immature, inadequatedeviant who has not found independence in solving problems of adultlife, may find in alcohol temporary independence from frustrati<strong>on</strong>,c<strong>on</strong>flict, anxiety, and m<strong>on</strong>ot<strong>on</strong>y, or in opiates nearly complete dissoluti<strong>on</strong>of such difficulties. While some of the same acti<strong>on</strong>s of thesedrugs would presumably occur in all deviants, the primary psychopathmight be especially susceptible, depending <strong>on</strong> the degree of socializati<strong>on</strong>,either to enhanced expressi<strong>on</strong> of hostility and aggressi<strong>on</strong> by alcohol,or to their eliminati<strong>on</strong> by opiates.94


Framework for an InteractiveTheory of <strong>Drug</strong> UseGeorge J. Huba, Ph.D.Joseph A, Wingard, Ph.D.Peter M. Bentler, Ph.D.INTRODUCTIONOur theory of drug use takes the positi<strong>on</strong> that drug-taking behavior iscaused by several large c<strong>on</strong>stellati<strong>on</strong>s of intraindividual and extraindividualforces. These domains of influences interact to modify eachother while determining the presence or absence of a large variety oflifestyle behaviors, including drug and alcohol use. Many previoustheories of drug taking have provided valuable c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong>s to thefield and are correct as far as they go. Flaws in these theories stemless from incorrectness than from incompleteness; they focus <strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>eset of forces to the exclusi<strong>on</strong> of others. In order to provide a morecomprehensive view of drug use than is typical, we will discuss themodels or domains of influences which form the major subsystems inour larger theory, and then present more specific ideas <strong>on</strong> how differentinfluences work to modify each other as well as to determine theperformance of behaviors.The detailed theory we will c<strong>on</strong>sider is presented graphically in figure1. This diagram represents sets of influences as large boxes. Weshould point out quite forcefully that we believe each box representsmany different variables, factors, or latent influences, some of whichmay be largely uncorrelated with <strong>on</strong>e another; that is, what we havepresented are relatively abstract domains of influences. In figure 1we have also drawn a large number of single-headed arrows to signifypresumed causal influences. Where no arrow appears, we believe thatthere is not a str<strong>on</strong>g direct effect. While the mandated length of thischapter precludes a literature review, we should emphasize that mostof the links have been substantiated empirically and are recognized asmajor c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>s by many researchers. The diagram is an abstractsummary statement of our theory, which permits detailed empiricaltests using a variety of research and analytic techniques, includingthe new methods for causal modeling with latent variables, a variety ofc<strong>on</strong>tinuous and discrete multivariate methods, and experimentati<strong>on</strong>.95


FIGURE 1.–Framework for a theory of drug use


Within the framework, we will try to claim a rather modest role forourselves, if such a stance is possible given the grandiose nature ofthe figure.FRAMEWORKHaving presented the model, we would like to digress somewhat andclarify several points about the framework we are suggesting for thedevelopment of a comprehensive theory of drug use. In general, wehave attempted to integrate various major themes of research developedby previous workers. While the labels chosen for various domains ofinfluence may not be entirely syn<strong>on</strong>ymous with the terminology used byspecialists in different fields, we feel that the general set of domainscan be differentiated into those variables addressed in studies thatspan discip<strong>line</strong>s from psychopharmacology to psychology, sociology,and ec<strong>on</strong>omics. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, the act of differentiati<strong>on</strong> and greater specificati<strong>on</strong>in various systems is a desirable goal for both current researchand future theory. We feel that systems of interest, such as pers<strong>on</strong>ality.must be successfully charted by determining the major structuraland dynamic comp<strong>on</strong>ents. Our current framework is a largely undifferentiatedand unelaborated <strong>on</strong>e which should develop naturally as moreinformati<strong>on</strong> about the various domains becomes available through basicand applied research in the major scientific fields of relevance to druguse. As a c<strong>on</strong>sequence, we expect that the future elaborati<strong>on</strong> of ourframework will possess some degree of ecological validity throughempirical derivati<strong>on</strong> rather than theoretical superimpositi<strong>on</strong>. Finally,we feel that the current framework allows the kind of differentiati<strong>on</strong>which may permit c<strong>on</strong>firmatory tests with such theory evaluati<strong>on</strong> proceduresas causal modeling with latent variables. That is, the currentframework is explicitly designed to permit the comparis<strong>on</strong> of varioustheories within a sophisticated, hypothesis-testing correlati<strong>on</strong>al methodology.While experimentati<strong>on</strong> may well provide the best method forclarifying certain specific comp<strong>on</strong>ents of our model (e.g., the effect ofcertain products <strong>on</strong> various organismic variables), naturalistic researchwill be required to interrelate those many comp<strong>on</strong>ents that are noteasily or ethically subject to manipulati<strong>on</strong> (e.g., the effects of lifestress <strong>on</strong> drug use) (Bentler 1978).As this theory goes through successive generati<strong>on</strong>s of developmentthere are several paths it must take. As a first task, we feel thatwithin each domain there should be a clarificati<strong>on</strong> of major variablesthat are relevant to understanding drug use. There are certaindomains which traditi<strong>on</strong>ally have been the province of a given academicspecialty , and we feel it is important to combine informati<strong>on</strong> fromvarious discip<strong>line</strong>s so that the sphere may be charted with a c<strong>on</strong>sentaneousset of structural referents. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, we believe that there shouldbe a focus <strong>on</strong> the development of various submodels within the moregeneral framework. Indeed, there is probably a lifetime of researchproductivity involved in determining the major structural pers<strong>on</strong>alitycharacteristics related to drug use. As informati<strong>on</strong> accumulates withineach specialty area, we would wish to see further elaborati<strong>on</strong> of thecomp<strong>on</strong>ent systems. Third, there should be an attempt to integratealternative empirical and theoretical systems into our overall c<strong>on</strong>cept.While we make no pretense of being able to explain all the phenomenaof drug use, we propose the broad framework primarily because wehope that it has some potential for unifying more narrowly basedc<strong>on</strong>cepts of drug use.97


THEORETICAL ELABORATIONTurning away now from the abstract framework to the more detailedformulati<strong>on</strong>s we have chosen in our first attempt at theory, the readerwill first note in figure 1 that we have included c<strong>on</strong>struct domains thatdo not directly influence either drug taking or its alternatives. Wefeel that it is necessary to include these more c<strong>on</strong>textual domains in atheoretical and empirical specificati<strong>on</strong> so that we can assess indirecteffects as well as derive unbiased estimates of the amount of theirinfluence. Furthermore, we must remember that many different domainsare changed directly and indirectly as c<strong>on</strong>sequences of drug takingand its alternatives. It is thus critical to c<strong>on</strong>sider the dynamic interacti<strong>on</strong>sof many different domains when c<strong>on</strong>sidering drug-taking behavior.A sec<strong>on</strong>d major characteristic of our structural model is that the behaviorsof drug and alcohol ingesti<strong>on</strong> are embedded in a larger set ofpreferred behavioral styles which may complement or preclude <strong>on</strong>eanother. Indeed, it is necessary to speak of the psychosocial causesof drug taking and its alternatives because many of the alternativesshare the same psychosocial causes and may bring the same c<strong>on</strong>sequencesfor the individual. The structural properties of individualbehaviors must be c<strong>on</strong>sidered within the interactive, ecological c<strong>on</strong>textof other characteristic behaviors performed and precluded so as toelaborate a theoretical network that has both c<strong>on</strong>vergent and discriminantvalidity.At this time, we do not pretend to know whether it is more fruitful toapproach a domain of behavioral styles from either dimensi<strong>on</strong>al ortypological viewpoints. That is, we are not sure if there are de<strong>line</strong>ablebehavioral types or whether there are some major dimensi<strong>on</strong>s of behavioralperference and acti<strong>on</strong>. We do believe that it is important to knowwhat other behaviors drug users also perform characteristically and touse co-occurrence with other behaviors as a way of differentiatingam<strong>on</strong>g drug users. The present approach seems to open an avenuefor c<strong>on</strong>ceptualizing other habitual behaviors, such as overeating,gambling, or obsessive shopping, in relati<strong>on</strong> to the dynamic causes ofdrug taking. Our use of the phrase “behavioral styles” is meant toimply that the focus of our theory is <strong>on</strong> behavior that spans temporaland c<strong>on</strong>textual effects. We are not particularly c<strong>on</strong>cerned with ad hocand fleeting behaviors.We are c<strong>on</strong>tinuing to revise and expand the theoretical model. C<strong>on</strong>sequently,the dynamic and structural properties implied by figure 1should be perceived as a model in the process of evoluti<strong>on</strong>. Our goalis to develop and test many of the different submodels implied by theframework.Proceeding to a detailed c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> of the figure, we have differentiatedfour major areas of interest at the highest level of abstracti<strong>on</strong>.These are biological, intrapers<strong>on</strong>al, interpers<strong>on</strong>al, and socioculturalinfluences. At the very foundati<strong>on</strong> of the biological area, we wouldplace genetic influences. We also wish to differentiate a domain whichwe call organismic status and which includes such variables as healthor efficient functi<strong>on</strong>ing as well as major anatomical and physiologicalsystems. Those aspects of physiology directly c<strong>on</strong>founded with thepsychological status of the individual should be specified into a separatedomain labeled psychophysiology.98


Dynamically, we have posited that the organismic status is a functi<strong>on</strong>of genetic influences as well as psychophysiology and various behavioraland social forces. The dynamic lattice is presented in the figure ascausal arrows. While there is residual, or unspecifiable, causati<strong>on</strong> foreach of the domains de<strong>line</strong>ated, we have not indicated these in thefigure. Specifying the nature of these residual influences is <strong>on</strong>e ofthe major tasks to be completed in future generati<strong>on</strong>s of the presentmodel.Turning now to the intrapers<strong>on</strong>al sphere, we have been most c<strong>on</strong>cernedwith differentiating those systems which comprise psychological status.We distinguish between subsystems of cogniti<strong>on</strong>, affect, pers<strong>on</strong>ality,percepti<strong>on</strong>, and c<strong>on</strong>sciousness, each of which is a specialty area withinthe social sciences. Am<strong>on</strong>g the dimensi<strong>on</strong>s in the pers<strong>on</strong>ality systemthat appear relevant to drug taking and its alternatives are extroversi<strong>on</strong>,law abidance, social adjustment, rebelliousness, anxiety,sensati<strong>on</strong>-seeking tendencies, and aut<strong>on</strong>omy and achievement strivings.We should note, parenthetically, that any of several sets of “sec<strong>on</strong>dorder” pers<strong>on</strong>ality factors are reas<strong>on</strong>able c<strong>on</strong>structs, in toto, foraffecting drug use behavioral styles.Within the affect subsystem of psychological status, it appears thatTomkins 1 (1962, 1963) derivati<strong>on</strong> of positive and negative affects andtheir relati<strong>on</strong>ship to cogniti<strong>on</strong>, percepti<strong>on</strong>, c<strong>on</strong>sciousness, and pers<strong>on</strong>alitymay be the most elegant. This theory has already proven usefulin differentiating types of cigarette smokers. C<strong>on</strong>structs which mustbe c<strong>on</strong>sidered as the cogniti<strong>on</strong> system (or cognitive style) of theindividual include the deployment of attenti<strong>on</strong>, memory capacity andorganizati<strong>on</strong>, various intellectual ability skills such as reas<strong>on</strong>ing,hemispheric dominance, and level of cognitive development. Perceptualc<strong>on</strong>structs of interest include attenti<strong>on</strong> utilizati<strong>on</strong>, figure/groundrelati<strong>on</strong>ships, distinctiveness, and ambiguity. Within the area ofc<strong>on</strong>sciousness, it may be fruitful to c<strong>on</strong>sider the dimensi<strong>on</strong>s of c<strong>on</strong>tentand structure out<strong>line</strong>d by Huba (1980) as derived from the theoreticalwritings of Singer (1975). We realize, of course, that the study ofpsychological status is a complicated <strong>on</strong>e, encompassing virtually thewhole field of psychology, and we do not mean to oversimplify itsimportance within our diagram. <strong>On</strong> the other hand, when we try toc<strong>on</strong>ceptualize a very specific behavior such as drug use, or even abehavioral style which includes drug use, it may be necessary to usemore abstract summaries of other domains so that they might all beincluded.It also seems important to c<strong>on</strong>sider the socioec<strong>on</strong>omic resources of theindividual when c<strong>on</strong>sidering a dependent variable of behavior. Financialresources are a functi<strong>on</strong> of the individual’s psychological status as wellas various social-system variables. Socioec<strong>on</strong>omic resources, or status,will also have an influence <strong>on</strong> the individual’s psychological status.Am<strong>on</strong>g the interpers<strong>on</strong>al domains, we differentiate intimate supportsystems and sociocultural influence systems. We c<strong>on</strong>sider the intimatesupport systems to be family, friends, and significant others for theindividual. Am<strong>on</strong>g the important aspects of the intimate culture areproviding relevant, valued models and reinforcers for various behaviorsand a sense of identity and bel<strong>on</strong>ging. We believe that the socioculturalinfluence system is a set of the more distal influences from theculture, including subcultural norms, models, and impers<strong>on</strong>al socializati<strong>on</strong>influences such as advertising. These influence systems arecentral to the criminal justice system’s belief in the efficacy of demand99


educti<strong>on</strong> methods through modifying the social envir<strong>on</strong>ment of thedrug user.In the sociocultural domain, we distinguish social sancti<strong>on</strong>s, socialexpectati<strong>on</strong>s, product availability and envir<strong>on</strong>mental stress. Thedomain of social sancti<strong>on</strong>s includes such forces as laws, reinforcementsor punishments, rituals, trends, fads, prevailing mores, and modalbehavior patterns within the society. Within the domain of productavailability we would include dimensi<strong>on</strong>s of cost and accessibility.While this domain does not appear as a central focus of psychologicaltheories of drug use, the supply reducti<strong>on</strong> strategy of dealing withdrug use clearly implicates this domain in a central way as affectingbehavioral styles. The domain of envir<strong>on</strong>mental stress has recentlybecome <strong>on</strong>e of wide interest. Am<strong>on</strong>g the dimensi<strong>on</strong>s which might bec<strong>on</strong>sidered are the c<strong>on</strong>trollability, predictability, nature, magnitude,and durati<strong>on</strong> of the stressors.That most of the domains c<strong>on</strong>sidered influence <strong>on</strong>e another is somethingwe take as given. N<strong>on</strong>etheless, it is important to try to determinewhen <strong>on</strong>e domain does not influence another str<strong>on</strong>gly, or when somesources of influence are more important than others. While the generalmodel is intended to explain the various stages of drug taking andcessati<strong>on</strong>, we believe that certain domains exert more influence atdifferent stages. For instance, it appears that the influences of theintimate support system may be particularly important in the initiati<strong>on</strong>of drug taking, while organismic status changes due to the drug mayaccount more fully for c<strong>on</strong>tinued drug ingesti<strong>on</strong>. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, wemust ask when trait factors are more important than intimate supportsystem factors in determining drug use, or when affective c<strong>on</strong>sequencesof drug taking outweigh legal punishments. Therefore, we wouldwelcome individual research groups to include measures of our variousdomains in order to determine the most important influences and c<strong>on</strong>sequencesof drug use in a particular populati<strong>on</strong>.In our current research program <strong>on</strong> young adolescents and theirparents, we are seeking to interrelate the various domains by usingstructural equati<strong>on</strong> models with latent variables (Bentler 1980) as wellas various other hypothesis-testing procedures. These revoluti<strong>on</strong>arynew procedures allow the theorist to posit various linkages between theimportant variables of a model and then to determine, through the useof goodness-of-fit statistics <strong>on</strong> the data, whether the model is sufficientto test the formulati<strong>on</strong>. In our early empirical work preliminary todetailed causal modeling, a variety of findings <strong>on</strong> adolescent drug useemerged. Perceived supply and support for drug use, importantcharacteristics of the intimate culture, seem to be much more importantdeterminants of drug taking than the more general characteristics ofthe peer culture, which are indicators of the domains of socioculturalinfluences (Huba et al. 1979a,b). Sources of support and supply seemto be differentiated for various drug-taking styles. In additi<strong>on</strong> torebelliousness, pers<strong>on</strong>ality measures such as liberalism, leadership,extroversi<strong>on</strong>, and the lack of deliberateness and diligence are importantpredictors of drug use (Wingard et al. 1979a,b). Logical introspecti<strong>on</strong>of the costs and benefits of drug use, as reflected in c<strong>on</strong>scious decisi<strong>on</strong>sregarding drug use, is not str<strong>on</strong>gly predictive of changes in subsequentuse (Huba et al. 1979c, in press; Bentler and Speckart 1979),indicating that behavioral pressures may not be purely logical functi<strong>on</strong>sof “objective” pressures. <strong>Drug</strong> use seems to cluster al<strong>on</strong>g <strong>line</strong>s pharmacologicallyrelated to mood alterati<strong>on</strong> as well as legal penalties andavailability (Huba et al. 1979d). Not <strong>on</strong>ly are drug-related behavioral100


styles quite stable in young adults, but previous drug-taking behaviorserves as a major predictor of future drug-taking behavior (Huba etal. 1979b; Wingard et al. 1979b), and a behavioral style involving adangerous drug like PCP is an organized outgrowth from a history ofprior substance use (Huba and Bentler 1979).In a sample of the mothers of our adolescent sample, Wingard et al.(1979c) have shown that drug use is related to self-perceived organismicstatus as well as to various pers<strong>on</strong>ality dimensi<strong>on</strong>s. The Wingard etal. (1979c) and Huba et al. (1979c) studies represent early applicati<strong>on</strong>sof causal models with latent variables to drug use data.In the future, our work will c<strong>on</strong>sist of integrating various results intothe framework of the model shown in figure 1 as we seek to expand,elaborate, and revise the specific causal ideas pictured. It is ourbelief that utilizing such a sequential process allows a dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> ofecological validity for the model by submitting it to periodic tests toestablish or refute our specific claims. For example, our model proposesthat the intimate support system affects drug use through perceivedbehavioral pressure, but not directly. Although we havedem<strong>on</strong>strated that perceived support for use is a major predictor ofdrug use, we have, as yet, no specific evidence <strong>on</strong> the mechanism orpathway by which the influence occurs.101


A Social-PsychologicalFramework forStudying <strong>Drug</strong> UseRichard Jessor, Ph.D.Shirley Jessor, Ph.D.The c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> of drug use in the c<strong>on</strong>text of a more general socialpsychologicalframework grew out of a larger interest in exploring theutility of a social-psychological theory of problem behavior and developmentin youths. Formulated initially to account for deviant behavior,especially heavy alcohol use, in a triethnic community (Jessor et al.1968), the framework was modified and extended to bear <strong>on</strong> problembehavior am<strong>on</strong>g youths in c<strong>on</strong>temporary American society--drug use;drinking and problem drinking; sexual experience; activist protest;and general deviance, including lying, stealing, and aggressi<strong>on</strong>.The most recent formulati<strong>on</strong> is referred to as “problem behavior theory”(Jessor and Jessor 1977). The theory is made up of specific c<strong>on</strong>ceptsthat are organized into three explanatory systems--pers<strong>on</strong>ality, envir<strong>on</strong>ment,and behavior--interrelated and organized so as to generate aresultant: a dynamic state designated “problem behavior pr<strong>on</strong>eness”that has implicati<strong>on</strong>s for a greater or lesser likelihood of occurrence ofproblem behavior. When a behavior such as drug use is embedded insuch a network of c<strong>on</strong>cepts, the theoretical framework makes it possibleto see the logical relati<strong>on</strong> of drug use to other behaviors and to variati<strong>on</strong>sin pers<strong>on</strong>ality and envir<strong>on</strong>mental characteristics as well.This paper, prepared by Deborah Willoughby and reviewed by RichardJessor, is based largely <strong>on</strong> three previously published sources. (1) R.Jessor and S.L. Jessor, Problem Behavior and Psychosocial Development(New York: Academic Press, 1977). pp. 17-42. (2) R. Jessor and S.L.Jessor, “Theory Testing in L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal Research <strong>on</strong> Marijuana Use,”in L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal Research <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> Use, ed. D.B. Kandel (Washingt<strong>on</strong>,D.C.: Hemisphere, 1978). (3) R. Jessor, “Marihuana: A Review ofRecent Psychosocial Research,” in Handbook <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>, eds.R.L. DuP<strong>on</strong>t, A. Goldstein, and J. O’D<strong>on</strong>nell (Rockville, Md.: Nati<strong>on</strong>alInstitute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>, 1979).102


The c<strong>on</strong>ceptual structure of problem-behavior theory c<strong>on</strong>sists, therefore,of the pers<strong>on</strong>ality system, the perceived-envir<strong>on</strong>ment system,and the behavior system. The variables in all three of the systems lieat what is essentially a social-psychological level of analysis. Thec<strong>on</strong>cepts that c<strong>on</strong>stitute pers<strong>on</strong>ality (values, expectati<strong>on</strong>s, beliefs,attitudes, orientati<strong>on</strong>s toward self and others) are cognitive and reflectsocial meaning and social experience. The c<strong>on</strong>cepts that c<strong>on</strong>stitute theenvir<strong>on</strong>ment (supports, influence, c<strong>on</strong>trols, models, expectati<strong>on</strong>s ofothers) are those that are amenable to logical coordinati<strong>on</strong> with thepers<strong>on</strong>ality c<strong>on</strong>cepts and that represent envir<strong>on</strong>mental characteristicscapable of being cognized or perceived; that is, they are sociallyorganized dimensi<strong>on</strong>s of potential meaning for actors. Behavior, too,is treated from a social-psychological perspective, emphasizing itssocially learned purposes, functi<strong>on</strong>s, or significance rather than itsphysical parameters. The actual occurrence of behavior is c<strong>on</strong>sideredto be the logical outcome of the interacti<strong>on</strong> of pers<strong>on</strong>ality and envir<strong>on</strong>mentalinfluence; in this respect, the formulati<strong>on</strong> represents a socialpsychologicalfield theory, assigning causal priority neither to pers<strong>on</strong>nor to situati<strong>on</strong>. A schematic representati<strong>on</strong> of the overall socialpsychologicalframework appears in figure 1.STRUCTURE OF THE PERSONALITY SYSTEMIn problem-behavior theory, the pers<strong>on</strong>ality system is represented bya number of specific variables bel<strong>on</strong>ging to three comp<strong>on</strong>ent structures--a motivati<strong>on</strong>al-instigati<strong>on</strong> structure, a pers<strong>on</strong>al belief structure, and apers<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>trol structure.The theoretical c<strong>on</strong>cern of the variables in the motivati<strong>on</strong>al-instigati<strong>on</strong>structure is with the directi<strong>on</strong>al orientati<strong>on</strong> of acti<strong>on</strong>, that is, with thegoals toward which a pers<strong>on</strong> strives and with the motivati<strong>on</strong>al sourcesor pressures that instigate particular behaviors. Both the valueplaced <strong>on</strong> goals and the expectati<strong>on</strong> of attaining goals have motivati<strong>on</strong>alproperties that influence whether behavior in the directi<strong>on</strong> of thosegoals is likely to occur. High value <strong>on</strong> a goal, for example, the goalof achievement, implies a higher likelihood of acti<strong>on</strong> in that directi<strong>on</strong>than does low value.Am<strong>on</strong>g the variety of sociopsychological goals that animate acti<strong>on</strong>,three are c<strong>on</strong>sidered central and salient for school-aged youths andrelevant to problem behavior--the goals of academic achievement,independence, and peer affecti<strong>on</strong>. The value placed <strong>on</strong> each of thesegoals, and the expectati<strong>on</strong> of being able to attain each of them, c<strong>on</strong>stitutevariables in the motivati<strong>on</strong>al-instigati<strong>on</strong> structure. An additi<strong>on</strong>alvariable represents the relative value placed <strong>on</strong> the goals of academicachievement and independence, since the relati<strong>on</strong> between these twogoals appears to have especially clear and direct c<strong>on</strong>sequences foryouthful problem behavior.The theoretical c<strong>on</strong>cern of the variables in the pers<strong>on</strong>al belief structureis with cognitive c<strong>on</strong>trols of a more general nature that are exertedagainst the occurrence of problem behavior. The variables in thisstructure refer to those restraints <strong>on</strong> engaging in n<strong>on</strong>c<strong>on</strong>formity thatoriginate in a variety of beliefs about self, society, and self in relati<strong>on</strong>to society. The c<strong>on</strong>ceptual role of such variables is to c<strong>on</strong>strainagainst the instigati<strong>on</strong>s to engage in problem behavior that derive fromthe variables in the preceding motivati<strong>on</strong>al-instigati<strong>on</strong> structure.103


FIGURE 1.—The c<strong>on</strong>ceptual structure of problem-behavior theory


Four variables are included in the pers<strong>on</strong>al belief structure--socialcriticism, alienati<strong>on</strong>, self-esteem, and internal-external locus of c<strong>on</strong>trol--and, depending <strong>on</strong> whether they are high or low, each is interpretedas c<strong>on</strong>trolling against engaging in problem behavior.The theoretical c<strong>on</strong>cern of the variables in the pers<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>trol structureis with more specific c<strong>on</strong>trols against engaging in n<strong>on</strong>normativebehavior. There are three variables in the pers<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>trol structure--attitudinal tolerance of deviance, religiosity, and the discrepancybetween positive and negative functi<strong>on</strong>s of (reas<strong>on</strong>s for and againstengaging in) behaviors such as drug use, premarital sexual intercourse,or drinking. These pers<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>trol variables are more directlyand obviously linked to the behavior involved.Of primary importance for the pers<strong>on</strong>ality system as a whole is thedynamic relati<strong>on</strong> between instigati<strong>on</strong>s and c<strong>on</strong>trols; their interacti<strong>on</strong>yields a theoretical resultant reflecting the balance between pers<strong>on</strong>alitysystempressure toward engaging in problem behavior and pers<strong>on</strong>alitysystemc<strong>on</strong>straints against it. The main characteristics of pr<strong>on</strong>enessto problem behavior in the pers<strong>on</strong>ality system include lower value <strong>on</strong>academic achievement; higher value <strong>on</strong> independence; greater value <strong>on</strong>independence relative to value <strong>on</strong> achievement; lower expectati<strong>on</strong>s foracademic achievement; greater social criticism and alienati<strong>on</strong>; lowerself-esteem and an orientati<strong>on</strong> toward an external locus of c<strong>on</strong>trol;greater attitudinal tolerance of deviance; lesser religiosity; and moreimportance attached to the positive, relative to the negative, functi<strong>on</strong>sof problem behavior. The more these pers<strong>on</strong>ality characteristics obtainfor a pers<strong>on</strong> at a given point in time--the more that they c<strong>on</strong>stitute acoherent pattern, c<strong>on</strong>stellati<strong>on</strong>, or syndrome--the more pers<strong>on</strong>alitypr<strong>on</strong>eness to problem behavior they theoretically c<strong>on</strong>vey.STRUCTURE OF THEPERCEIVED-ENVIRONMENTSYSTEMThe c<strong>on</strong>ceptual focus in the envir<strong>on</strong>mental system is <strong>on</strong> the envir<strong>on</strong>mentas perceived, as it has meaning for the actor, the social-psychologicalrather than the physicogeographic or social structural or demographicenvir<strong>on</strong>ment. Logically, the perceived envir<strong>on</strong>ment is the <strong>on</strong>e that hasthe most invariant relati<strong>on</strong> with behavior because it is the envir<strong>on</strong>mentof immediate meaning and the <strong>on</strong>e to which the actor is resp<strong>on</strong>ding.Within the perceived envir<strong>on</strong>ment, an important distincti<strong>on</strong> between“regi<strong>on</strong>s” is made in terms of their proximal versus distal relati<strong>on</strong> tobehavior. Proximal variables (for example, peer models for marijuanause) directly implicate a particular behavior, whereas distal variables(for example, the degree of normative c<strong>on</strong>sensus between parents andpeers) are more remote in the causal chain and therefore requiretheoretical linkage to behavior. This distincti<strong>on</strong> helps make clear whysome envir<strong>on</strong>mental variables are likely to be more powerfully relatedto problem behavior than others. (The same distincti<strong>on</strong> can be appliedalso in the pers<strong>on</strong>ality system; the motivati<strong>on</strong>al-instigati<strong>on</strong> variablesand the pers<strong>on</strong>al belief variables are more distal from problem behavior,while the pers<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>trol variables are more proximal to problembehavior.)105


In the distal structure of the perceived envir<strong>on</strong>ment, the variablesserve mainly to characterize whether the social c<strong>on</strong>text in which ayouth is located is <strong>on</strong>e that is more parent and family oriented thanfriends or peer oriented, or vice versa. Locati<strong>on</strong> in or orientati<strong>on</strong>toward an adult or parental c<strong>on</strong>text is interpretable as being lessproblem-behavior pr<strong>on</strong>e than locati<strong>on</strong> in a peer c<strong>on</strong>text. In the former,there would be more involvement with c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al norms, less exposureto models for problem behavior, and greater social c<strong>on</strong>trol over transgressi<strong>on</strong>.Six variables are included in the distal structure of the perceivedenvir<strong>on</strong>ment: perceived support from parents and from friends, perceivedc<strong>on</strong>trols from parents and from friends, compatibility or c<strong>on</strong>sensusbetween parents and friends in the expectati<strong>on</strong>s they hold for agiven adolescent, and the perceived influence <strong>on</strong> the adolescent ofparents relative to that of friends.Together, these six variables represent a patterned social envir<strong>on</strong>mentthat is more or less c<strong>on</strong>ducive to problem behavior, depending <strong>on</strong>whether supports and c<strong>on</strong>trols are perceived to be present, whethermore influence comes from parents or peers, and whether there isc<strong>on</strong>cordance or c<strong>on</strong>flict between these two reference groups, the twothat have the most regulatory significance for youths. When thepattern of variables in the distal structure is such that it defines anattenuated reference orientati<strong>on</strong> to parents, that is, when it suggeststhat a youth is located in a peer rather than a parental c<strong>on</strong>text, itdefines greater pr<strong>on</strong>eness to problem behavior.The variables included in the proximal structure of the perceivedenvir<strong>on</strong>ment c<strong>on</strong>cern the degree to which an adolescent is located in asocial c<strong>on</strong>text where problem behavior is prevalent and where there issocial support for its occurrence. Three major variables are includedin the proximal structure of the perceived envir<strong>on</strong>ment: friendsapproval-disapproval for problem behavior, parental approval-disapprovalof problem behavior, and friends models for problem behavior.Of all the variables in the overall social-psychological framework, it isreas<strong>on</strong>able to expect that those in the proximal structure of the perceivedenvir<strong>on</strong>ment should be am<strong>on</strong>g the most powerful. A c<strong>on</strong>text inwhich <strong>on</strong>e’s friends are perceived as engaging in problem behavior andas providing potential approval (if not pressure) for it is likely to beof direct and substantial influence. High prevalence of friends modelsand support c<strong>on</strong>stitutes not <strong>on</strong>ly a direct influence <strong>on</strong> problem behaviorbut is probably also an indirect reflecti<strong>on</strong> of other problem-pr<strong>on</strong>efactors--those that would also account for an adolescent’s membershipin a friendship network that has these particular characteristics ratherthan in <strong>on</strong>e that is more c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>ally oriented. It would require thepercepti<strong>on</strong> of str<strong>on</strong>g parental disapproval, or the presence of str<strong>on</strong>gpers<strong>on</strong>ality-system c<strong>on</strong>trols, to offset such problem-behavior pr<strong>on</strong>enessin the proximal structure of the perceived envir<strong>on</strong>ment.The primary dynamic relati<strong>on</strong> within the perceived-envir<strong>on</strong>ment systemis between the percepti<strong>on</strong> of social c<strong>on</strong>trols against problem behavior,<strong>on</strong> the <strong>on</strong>e hand, and the percepti<strong>on</strong> of models and supports for problembehavior <strong>on</strong> the other. The balance of these percepti<strong>on</strong>s determinesthe resultant c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> of the perceived-envir<strong>on</strong>ment system to thelikelihood of problem behavior.106


Problem-behavior pr<strong>on</strong>eness in the distal structure of the perceivedenvir<strong>on</strong>ment system c<strong>on</strong>sists of low parental support and c<strong>on</strong>trols; lowpeer c<strong>on</strong>trols; low compatibility between parent and peer expectati<strong>on</strong>s;and low parent, relative to peer, influence. In the proximal structure,problem-behavior pr<strong>on</strong>eness includes low parental disapproval of problembehavior and both high friends models for and high friends approvalof engaging in problem behavior.STRUCTURE OF THE BEHAVIOR SYSTEMThe specificati<strong>on</strong> of behavior relies up<strong>on</strong> a variety of c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>sbey<strong>on</strong>d the physical parameters of the act itself--its pers<strong>on</strong>al meaning,its social definiti<strong>on</strong>, its relati<strong>on</strong> to age and status, the c<strong>on</strong>text of itsoccurrence, and its time in history.The behavior system is differentiated into a problem-behavior structureand a c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al behavior structure. Problem behavior refers tobehavior socially defined either as a problem, as a source of c<strong>on</strong>cern,or as undesirable by the norms of c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al society or the instituti<strong>on</strong>sof adult authority; it is behavior that usually elicits some kind ofsocial-c<strong>on</strong>trol resp<strong>on</strong>se. The latter, of course, may be as minimal asan expressi<strong>on</strong> of disapproval or as extreme as incarcerati<strong>on</strong>. Thepossibility that phenotypically very different behaviors (for example,smoking marijuana, engaging in sexual intercourse, or taking part in apeaceful dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong>) may all serve the same social-psychologicalfuncti<strong>on</strong> (for example, overt repudiati<strong>on</strong> of c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al norms orexpressing independence from parental c<strong>on</strong>trol) is what underlies thenoti<strong>on</strong> of a structure of problem behavior. C<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al behavior,e.g., church attendance or working hard at school, is behavior that issocially approved, normatively expected, and codified and instituti<strong>on</strong>alizedas appropriate for adolescents and youths.Problem behavior can functi<strong>on</strong> in a variety of ways. It may representan instrumental or goal-directed effort to attain goals that seem otherwiseunattainable. (The youth who is unable to secure aut<strong>on</strong>omy fromparental supervisi<strong>on</strong> may gain a sense of independence through the useof drugs.) Its purpose may be to express oppositi<strong>on</strong> to c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>alsociety, whose norms and values have been rejected. It may representan affirmati<strong>on</strong> of maturity or a negotiati<strong>on</strong> for transformati<strong>on</strong> of statusfrom adolescent to adult. Its meaning may lie in defining, for self andothers, important attributes of pers<strong>on</strong>al identity (being able to hold<strong>on</strong>e’s liquor, being a n<strong>on</strong>virgin). It can functi<strong>on</strong> also to establishsolidary relati<strong>on</strong>s with peers, or to enable access to youth subgroups,or to permit identificati<strong>on</strong> with the youth subculture. Or, finally, itcan serve as a way of coping with frustrati<strong>on</strong> and anticipated failure(drowning <strong>on</strong>e’s sorrows in alcohol).The primary dynamic in the behavior system is that between the problembehaviorstructure and the c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al behavior structure, withengagement in either serving as a c<strong>on</strong>straint up<strong>on</strong> or an alternative toengaging in the other. High involvement in church activities or participati<strong>on</strong>in academic activities should relate negatively to engagement indrug use, or problem drinking, or other problem behaviors, and viceversa. Within either the problem-behavior structure or the c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>albehavior structure. there should be a positive relati<strong>on</strong> am<strong>on</strong>g thevarious behaviors that are included; that is, the different problembehaviors should covary and the different c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al behaviorsshould covary.107


PROBLEM-BEHAVIOR THEORY ANDDEVELOPMENT IN ADOLESCENCEThe logical implicati<strong>on</strong>s in problem behavior theory for development andchange can be drawn by elaborating the noti<strong>on</strong>s of age grading, agenorms, and age expectati<strong>on</strong>s in relati<strong>on</strong> to problem behavior.The logic of applying the same c<strong>on</strong>ceptual framework to development inadolescence rests <strong>on</strong> several key points: that there is stratificati<strong>on</strong> ofsociety in terms of age; that access to valued roles, statuses, andrewards varies with different age strata; that adolescence, especiallyearly adolescence, can be characterized as an age stratum of relativelylimited access to certain valued goals, whether aut<strong>on</strong>omy, status, sex,or mobility; that age strata have associated norms and expectati<strong>on</strong>sthat regulate what behaviors are c<strong>on</strong>sidered to be appropriate; andthat many of the behaviors we have referred to as problem behaviorsare normatively age-graded, that is, the behavior may be permitted oreven prescribed for those who are older, while being proscribed forthose who are younger. Drinking, as <strong>on</strong>e example, is proscribed forthose under legal age but is permitted and even instituti<strong>on</strong>ally encouragedfor those who are bey<strong>on</strong>d that age; sexual intercourse, normativelyacceptable for adults, is a normative departure for a young adolescent,and <strong>on</strong>e that is likely to elicit social c<strong>on</strong>trols.C<strong>on</strong>sensual awareness am<strong>on</strong>g youths of the age-graded norms for suchbehaviors carries with it, at the same time, the shared knowledge thatoccupancy of a more mature status is characterized by actually engagingin such behavior. Thus, engaging in certain behaviors for thefirst time can mark a transiti<strong>on</strong> in status from “less mature” to “moremature,” from “younger” to “older,” or from “adolescent” to “youth”or “adult.”Many of the important transiti<strong>on</strong>s that mark the course of adolescentdevelopment involve behaviors that depart from the regulatory agenorms defining what is appropriate or expected behavior for that ageor stage in life. It is important in this c<strong>on</strong>text to emphasize thatbehavior that departs from regulatory norms is precisely what problembehaviortheory IS meant to account for, and this becomes the basisfor the systematic applicati<strong>on</strong> of problem-behavior theory to developmentalchange in adolescence. By mapping the developmental c<strong>on</strong>ceptof transiti<strong>on</strong> pr<strong>on</strong>eness <strong>on</strong>to the theoretical c<strong>on</strong>cept of problem-behaviorpr<strong>on</strong>eness, it becomes possible to use problem-behavior theory tospecify the likelihood of occurrence of developmental change throughengaging in age-graded, norm-departing, transiti<strong>on</strong>-marking behaviors.EMPIRICAL TESTING OF PROBLEM-BEHAVIORTHEORY IN RELATION TO MARIJUANA USEProblem-behavior theory has been employed in both cross-secti<strong>on</strong>al andl<strong>on</strong>gitudinal studies of marijuana use, in both local and nati<strong>on</strong>al samples,and with respect to both males and females. (See Jessor and Jessor1977, 1978; Jessor et al., in press.) In the c<strong>on</strong>tent of the findings,there is quite impressive coherence, whether c<strong>on</strong>sidering the crosssecti<strong>on</strong>aldifferences between marijuana users and n<strong>on</strong>users, or the108


l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal predictive differences between those likely to begin use inthe near future and those not. A single summarizing dimensi<strong>on</strong> underlyingthe differences between users and n<strong>on</strong>users might be termedc<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>ality-unc<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>ality. With respect to pers<strong>on</strong>ality, theadolescent less likely to engage in marijuana use is <strong>on</strong>e who values andexpects to attain academic achievement, who is not much c<strong>on</strong>cernedwith independence, who treats society as unproblematic rather than asan object for criticism, who maintains a religious involvement and amore uncompromising attitude toward normative transgressi<strong>on</strong>, and whosees little attracti<strong>on</strong> in problem behavior relative to its anticipatednegative c<strong>on</strong>sequences. The adolescent more likely to be involved withmarijuana shows an opposite pattern: a c<strong>on</strong>cern with pers<strong>on</strong>al aut<strong>on</strong>omy,a lack of interest in the goals of c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al instituti<strong>on</strong>s like churchand school, a jaundiced view of the larger society, and a more tolerantview of transgressi<strong>on</strong>.With respect to the envir<strong>on</strong>ment, the youth likely to be involved withmarijuana perceives less parental support, less compatibility betweenparents’ and friends’ expectati<strong>on</strong>s, greater influence of friends relativeto parents, and greater approval of and models for drug use fromfriends. These variables reflect the importance of whether the referenceorientati<strong>on</strong> of a youth is toward parents or peers, and the importanceof the models and reinforcements available in the peer c<strong>on</strong>text. Withrespect to behavior, the adolescent likely to use marijuana is <strong>on</strong>e whois likely to be more involved in other problem behaviors as well andless involved in c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al behavior than his or her n<strong>on</strong>-drug-usingcounterpart.The research findings are generally similar for both males and females,a fact worthy of emphasis. There is also similarity between highschool and college youths, but it is attenuated, particularly in thepers<strong>on</strong>ality system and in the distal structure of the perceivedenvir<strong>on</strong>mentsystem, suggesting that development is not homogeneousthroughout the early-to-late stages of adolescence and youth. Overall,support for the utility of problem-behavior theory as a socialpsychologicalframework for the study of drug use can be found not<strong>on</strong>ly in the research carried out by the Jessors and their colleagues,but in the findings from a wide variety of studies d<strong>on</strong>e by otherinvestigators as well. (For a review of recent studies of marijuanause, see Jessor 1979.)109


Toward a Theory of<strong>Drug</strong> SubculturesBruce D. Johns<strong>on</strong>, Ph.D.The theory of drug subcultures out<strong>line</strong>d below applies theoreticaltraditi<strong>on</strong>s developed by Sutherland (1939), Cohen (1955). Cloward andOhlin (1960). and Wolfgang and Ferricutti (1967) to the phenomena ofn<strong>on</strong>medical drug use. Aspects of this theory are more fully explicatedby Johns<strong>on</strong> (1973) and, to a lesser extent, by Johns<strong>on</strong> and Preble(1978). The c<strong>on</strong>cept of subculture, of course, has a l<strong>on</strong>g and distinguishedhistory in anthropology and sociology (Kluckhohn 1962; Yinger1960; Broom and Selznick 1968, p. 71), but many meanings of thisc<strong>on</strong>cept appear to be too broad for analyzing patterns of drug use.Fine and Kleinman (1979) indicate that the subculture c<strong>on</strong>cept is (1) notsyn<strong>on</strong>ymous with a subsociety or the social structure; (2) not a groupof pers<strong>on</strong>s (primary or peer groups) or a statistical aggregate (i.e.,pers<strong>on</strong>s aged 12 to 18); (3) not homogeneous, static, or closed; and(4) not composed <strong>on</strong>ly of values and central themes. Rather subculturesemerge from, are maintained by, and change over time through acomplex process of interacti<strong>on</strong> involving many pers<strong>on</strong>s and groups thatmay not be directly c<strong>on</strong>nected.The theoretical perspective presented here is not grand theory in themanner of Pars<strong>on</strong>s, Weber, or Durkheim. Rather, it more closelyapproximates what Mert<strong>on</strong> (1957, p. 5), calls “theories of the middlerange, theories intermediate to the minor working hypotheses evolvedin abundance during day-to-day routines of research, and the allinclusivespeculati<strong>on</strong>s comprising a master c<strong>on</strong>ceptual scheme.” Suchtheory “c<strong>on</strong>sists of general orientati<strong>on</strong> toward data, suggesting typesof variables which need somehow to be taken into account, rather thanclear, verifiable statements of relati<strong>on</strong>ships between specified variables”(Mert<strong>on</strong> 1957, p. 9). The perspective presented here emerges frommiddle-range theories in criminology and deviant behavior and focusesup<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly a narrow segment of these fields--that of illicit drug use.Nevertheless, it attempts to present such general theoretical orientati<strong>on</strong>stoward illicit drug use by (1) building from fundamental sociologicalc<strong>on</strong>cepts (values, norms, roles, etc.), (2) describing the c<strong>on</strong>tent110


of such c<strong>on</strong>cepts as found in illicit drug use, (3) analytically linkingthese c<strong>on</strong>cepts for purposes of theory testing, (4) including significantinsights from other theories and empirical findings that have emerged,and (5) indicating unique features of this perspective that are notincorporated in others. Finally, the perspective is distinctly sociologicaland makes little or no attempt to incorporate psychological, biological,or pharmacological theories and insights about drug use, althoughoverlaps with these theories are suggested at some points.For reas<strong>on</strong>s that will become clear, this perspective is most useful inunderstanding patterns of drug use and misuse that occur duringyouth and young adulthood, mainly between the ages of 11 and 25,although some pers<strong>on</strong>s begin earlier and some remain involved at laterages. Moreover, drug-subculture participati<strong>on</strong> is related to the broadAmerican “middle class” culture, the “peer” or youth culture, andvarious other subcultures. The broader framework within which drugsubcultures functi<strong>on</strong> will be de<strong>line</strong>ated first.LINKAGES WITH THE MIDDLE CLASS,PEER CULTURES, AND OTHER SUBCULTURESAn important feature of drug subculture theory includes theoreticallinkages with American “middle class culture,” “peer culture,” andother subcultures (Johns<strong>on</strong> 1973, pp. 6-8). The middle-class culturereflects the broad American culture and defines what adults expectyouths and young adults to do or not to do. This parent cultureexpects youths to avoid tobacco, alcohol in excess, and n<strong>on</strong>medicaldrug use. (Other norms are specified in Johns<strong>on</strong> [1973, p. 6].) Thevalues and c<strong>on</strong>duct norms of the parent culture become internalizedand c<strong>on</strong>tinue to influence youths and young adults even after departurefrom home.The peer culture (also called youth culture) governs patterns ofyouthful behavior and friendship groups (Gans 1962; Yinger 1960).The c<strong>on</strong>duct norms of the peer culture emphasize that (1) the pers<strong>on</strong>must be loyal to friends and attempt to maintain group associati<strong>on</strong>;(2) social interacti<strong>on</strong> with the peer group should occur in locati<strong>on</strong>swhere adult c<strong>on</strong>trols are relatively absent; (3) within such peer groups,a veiled competiti<strong>on</strong> exists for status and prestige am<strong>on</strong>g group participantsand leads to new forms of behavior or operating innovati<strong>on</strong>s(Vaz 1967).The c<strong>on</strong>cepts of peer culture and peer group are closely related. Aparticular pers<strong>on</strong> may have several close friends, the peer group.However, peer groups do not exist in isolati<strong>on</strong>; several peer groupsexhibit behaviors similar to other peer groups because they follow thevalues and c<strong>on</strong>duct norms of the peer culture. Individuals generallyexperience the peer culture as it is mediated through a peer group.Closely related to the peer culture and drug subcultures are othersubcultures organized around different unc<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al behaviors oreven c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al behaviors. Each of these other subcultures hasspecific values and c<strong>on</strong>duct norms governing the central activitiesaround which the group functi<strong>on</strong>s (Cloward and Ohlin 1960) that aredirectly parallel to the drug subculture. Thus, delinquent subculturesemerge from those c<strong>on</strong>duct norms and values which influence behaviors111


promoting the commissi<strong>on</strong> of criminal acts; homosexual subculturesemerge from values and c<strong>on</strong>duct norms regulating interacti<strong>on</strong> betweensexual partners of the same sex; leftist or rightist subcultures followvalues and c<strong>on</strong>duct norms oriented toward political activity. C<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>alsubcultures also exist (e.g., those centered around rock ordisco music, athletic participati<strong>on</strong>, auto racing, etc.).Within the peer culture and other subcultures of unc<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al behavior,there is a c<strong>on</strong>duct norm of veiled competiti<strong>on</strong>. In many middleand lower class peer groups and under a veneer of n<strong>on</strong>competitive goodfellowship and fun, “there is c<strong>on</strong>stant c<strong>on</strong>cealed competiti<strong>on</strong> betweenpeer group members for leadership and status” (Vaz 1967, p. 134).Competiti<strong>on</strong> for status within the peer group frequently leads to experimentati<strong>on</strong>with new behaviors. Such “operating innovati<strong>on</strong>s” if rewardedby the peer group (by increased respect or admirati<strong>on</strong> for the instigator)or copied and repeated by other peer group members, and ifc<strong>on</strong>cealed from adult authorities, frequently “generate their own moralitynorms, standards and rewards” (Dublin 1959). These innovati<strong>on</strong>s,which may not have been permitted at an earlier time, become toleratedand then accepted as normal, and perhaps demanded (a new c<strong>on</strong>ductnorm) of those participating in the peer group. Operating innovati<strong>on</strong>swithin a specific peer group frequently follow a relatively predictablepattern of greater involvement in a specific subculture or experimentaland/or irregular involvement in several subcultures of unc<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>ality.Thus, for many peer groups and for individual participants, theirorientati<strong>on</strong> to c<strong>on</strong>duct norms and values from <strong>on</strong>e or more subculturesmay change over time, and their behavior may change accordingly. Inadditi<strong>on</strong>, as the peer group learns and incorporates subcultural c<strong>on</strong>ductnorms, values, rituals, and argot, the members also reorient thinkingtoward and develop rati<strong>on</strong>alizati<strong>on</strong>s about parental cultural values. Avariety of techniques of neutralizati<strong>on</strong> (Sykes and Matza 1957) may beadopted to denigrate or deny the validity of parent culture c<strong>on</strong>ductnorms (no drugs, no sex before marriage, moderate alcohol use, etc.)and expectati<strong>on</strong>s for c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al behavior.The critical fact is that the c<strong>on</strong>duct norms and values from theseunc<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al subcultures (drug, delinquent, homosexual, etc.) arewidely known within the youth or peer culture (Fine and Kleinman1979; Jessor and Jessor 1977); individuals and specific peer groupsmay orient themselves to any <strong>on</strong>e or a combinati<strong>on</strong> of values and c<strong>on</strong>ductnorms and behave accordingly. For example, a peer group inwhich each pers<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sumes c<strong>on</strong>siderable amounts of alcohol, smokesmarijuana, snorts cocaine, and commits burglary is simultaneouslyfollowing the c<strong>on</strong>duct norms and values of and participating in each ofthese subcultures: peer, alcohol abuse, cannabis use, multiple druguse (defined below), and delinquency. This theory suggests that peerculture participati<strong>on</strong> precedes involvement in several unc<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>alsubcultures. Thus, many statistically significant relati<strong>on</strong>ships betweendrug use and other forms of unc<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al behavior (alcoholism,delinquency, criminality, multiple sex partners, etc.) may exist becauseof a prior involvement in the peer culture and predisposing tendenciestoward unc<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al behavior. Jessor and Jessor (1977), Jessor(1979), Johnst<strong>on</strong> et al. (1978), and Kandel’s (1978b) causal analyses ofthe relati<strong>on</strong>ship between drug use and other problem behaviors showthat neither causes the other(s) and that both are the result of apreexisting tendency toward unc<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al behavior.In additi<strong>on</strong>, many individuals and peer groups have also internalizedvalues and c<strong>on</strong>duct norms from the parent culture which urge avoidance112


of and/or moderati<strong>on</strong> in drug use, alcohol c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong>, criminal activities,and n<strong>on</strong>marital sexual behavior. The c<strong>on</strong>duct norms of theparent culture and various subcultures of unc<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>ality are frequentlyin oppositi<strong>on</strong>; such c<strong>on</strong>flicting standards about appropriatebehavior may lead individuals to shift peer group membership andexperiment or moderate their unc<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al behavior or drug use.THE CONCEPT OF DRUG SUBCULTUREAlthough no definiti<strong>on</strong> of a subculture is widely accepted at this time,an elaborati<strong>on</strong> up<strong>on</strong> Wolfgang’s (1967, p. 146) definiti<strong>on</strong> provides agood starting point; a subculture is “composed of values, c<strong>on</strong>ductnorms, social situati<strong>on</strong>s, role definiti<strong>on</strong>s and performances, sharing,transmissi<strong>on</strong>, and learning of values.” The term “drug subculture”refers to those values, c<strong>on</strong>duct norms, social situati<strong>on</strong>s, argot, rituals,role definiti<strong>on</strong>s, and performances that are associated with the n<strong>on</strong>medicaluse of drugs. Excluded from, although related to, this c<strong>on</strong>cept ofa drug subculture are values and c<strong>on</strong>duct norms governing the medicaluse of drugs; the use of drugs for dieting and sleeping; the c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong>of cigarettes, coffee, and tea; and the social use of alcohol.These are not socially defined as “drugs” by law, social custom, ormost illicit drug users.The most important elements of a subculture are its values and c<strong>on</strong>ductnorms. Values are here understood to be shared ideas about what thesubgroup believes to be true or what it wants (desires) or ought towant. Probably the most important value in a drug subculture, whichprovides a significant disc<strong>on</strong>tinuity (Levi-Strauss 1953, p. 536) fromthe broad American c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al culture, is the intenti<strong>on</strong> or desire toget “high” or to experience euphoria from the n<strong>on</strong>medical c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong>of substances. This value is the organizing focus of the subculturesto be discussed hereafter.C<strong>on</strong>duct norms are also crucial to understanding a subculture. C<strong>on</strong>ductnorms are those expectati<strong>on</strong>s of behavior in a particular social situati<strong>on</strong>that are attached to a status within the group (Wolfgang 1967).C<strong>on</strong>duct norms govern the “central activities around which the group”is organized or functi<strong>on</strong>s and provide “essential requirements for theperformance of the dominant roles” supported by the subculture(Cloward and Ohlin 1960, p. 7). Thus, the dividing <strong>line</strong> betweenmarginal participati<strong>on</strong> and n<strong>on</strong>participati<strong>on</strong> in a drug subculture can berather accurately gauged by whether a pers<strong>on</strong> has used a particulardrug in an intenti<strong>on</strong>al attempt to get high, although pers<strong>on</strong>s whoexpress a definite wish to use the drug(s) may also be included asparticipants.Roles are expectati<strong>on</strong>s (or norms) for appropriate behavior attached toa particular status or social positi<strong>on</strong>. Role performance is the pers<strong>on</strong>’sbehavior as a result of following the c<strong>on</strong>duct norms while an incumbentof a particular status. Within the drug subculture(s), three roles areof central importance: seller, buyer, and user. (These roles will beelaborated later.) Performance of these roles is usually illegal andmay expose the pers<strong>on</strong> to arrest and incarcerati<strong>on</strong>; thus, role performanceis generally covert or hidden.In additi<strong>on</strong> to central values, c<strong>on</strong>duct norms, and roles, drug subculturesfrequently have specialized argot, rituals, and highly valued113


symbolic objects (specific drugs or instruments for administrati<strong>on</strong>).The heroin injecti<strong>on</strong> subculture (defined below) places high value up<strong>on</strong>heroin as the preferred drug and up<strong>on</strong> hypodermic instruments forinjecti<strong>on</strong>, emphasizes drug-taking rituals, and exhibits a highly developedand specialized argot (Agar 1973; Haertzen 1979). Other drugsubcultures may be less developed, but nevertheless exhibit argot,rituals, and symbolic objects that are seldom known outside the subculture,but which are widely known to those participating in the subculture.<strong>Drug</strong> subcultures are seldom static, but change over time. While thecentral value (to get “high”) and central roles (seller, buyer, user)remain relatively unaltered over time, the c<strong>on</strong>duct norms may shiftc<strong>on</strong>siderably in resp<strong>on</strong>se to social pressure from the middle-classculture and pressure from the legal system, fads in drug preferences,and availability of drugs in the illicit market. Moreover, innovati<strong>on</strong>and change are highly valued in most drug subcultures. New drugsare sought out and tried, argot terms are easily adopted and old termsdropped, and the times and places for drug c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong> may change.Of course, individual participants, groups, and demographic segmentsof the populati<strong>on</strong> involved in a given set of subcultural activities maychange greatly during a period of years. Arnold (1970, p. 114)indicates “while subcultures grow out of the interacti<strong>on</strong> of groups ofpeople, they are not themselves those groups” or pers<strong>on</strong>s. Moreover,subcultures are maintained by the c<strong>on</strong>tinuing interacti<strong>on</strong>s of pers<strong>on</strong>s.Because these interacti<strong>on</strong>s tend to be dynamic, subcultures are alsodynamic and c<strong>on</strong>tinuously self-modifying.Subcultural differentiati<strong>on</strong> is comm<strong>on</strong> and changes over time. Since asubculture refers to role relati<strong>on</strong>ships, values, c<strong>on</strong>duct norms, rituals,and argot, subcultural boundaries are always fluid and imprecise. Anattempt to de<strong>line</strong>ate the central c<strong>on</strong>duct norms and values may simplifythe number of subcultures to be described and analyzed. For instance,the multiple-drug subculture (described below) may include subsubculturesfocused around psychedelic drugs (LSD, peyote), pills, orcocaine, which may be useful for other analytical purposes to othersociologists. Any boundaries selected for defining a subculture aresomewhat arbitrary and may not be more correct than other definiti<strong>on</strong>sor boundaries. Most boundaries should be c<strong>on</strong>sidered to have heuristicvalue when and if they assist theoretical and empirical research tounderstand how drug use and abuse is structured and functi<strong>on</strong>s withinthe subculture, and how it relates to n<strong>on</strong>drug subcultures and to thebroader culture.Subcultural participants may observe an elaborate and differentiatedrole structure, set of c<strong>on</strong>duct norms, and argot. For example, <strong>on</strong>eresp<strong>on</strong>dent objected strenuously to a questi<strong>on</strong>naire that included hisfavorite drug (mesca<strong>line</strong>) am<strong>on</strong>g the psychedelics; he had an elaborateset of reas<strong>on</strong>s why mesca<strong>line</strong>’s effects and patterns of use were verydifferent from those of LSD. Subcultural participants tend to ignoregreat similarities in behavior and resp<strong>on</strong>se to the same c<strong>on</strong>duct normsby other users and to emphasize the importance of what seem (tooutsiders) to be small differences in argot, ritual, appearance, andsome behaviors. Hence, subculture definiti<strong>on</strong>s and boundaries canseldom be agreed up<strong>on</strong>, either by sociological analysts or by participants,although the central values, c<strong>on</strong>duct norms, roles, and behaviorsmay provide a useful analytic framework.114


The sociohistorical origins of a particular drug subculture appear to bea product of drug use beginning am<strong>on</strong>g peer groups having certainsociodemographic characteristics and the spread of informati<strong>on</strong> viayouth mobility, and informal communicati<strong>on</strong> channels am<strong>on</strong>g youths(Fine and Kleinman 1979). Mass media coverage of a particular drughas frequently created str<strong>on</strong>g public reacti<strong>on</strong> (Brecher 1972) leading toattempts at c<strong>on</strong>trol or eliminati<strong>on</strong> of n<strong>on</strong>medical use that has later beenassociated with negative c<strong>on</strong>sequences (Lindesmith 1965). The socialhistory and rise of any <strong>on</strong>e particular drug subculture in America arebey<strong>on</strong>d the scope of this overview, but excellent reviews exist (Musto1973; Lindesmith 1965; Brecher 1972; King 1972; Helmer and Vietoriez1974; Nati<strong>on</strong>al Commissi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> Marihuana and <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong> 1973). Twoparticularly critical historical events affect subculture formati<strong>on</strong>:(1) the adopti<strong>on</strong> of a drug by many peer groups within a small segmentof the populati<strong>on</strong>--as with heroin and morphine am<strong>on</strong>g working-classwhites in the 1920s (Street 1953; Musto 1973) and urban blacks in the1950s and 1960s (Helmer and Vietoriez 1974; Preble and Casey 1969)and (2) the expansi<strong>on</strong> of use of a drug(s) into peer groups morerepresentative of the general youth populati<strong>on</strong> as occurred with marijuana,LSD, cocaine, and other substances in the late 1960s and 1970s(Carey 1968; Johnst<strong>on</strong> et al. 1978). When patterns of drug use arelimited to low-income and low-status groups, societal reacti<strong>on</strong> tends tobe punitive, and government pursues a prohibiti<strong>on</strong>ist policy. Whendrug use becomes comm<strong>on</strong> in many segments of the youth populati<strong>on</strong>,public reacti<strong>on</strong> is <strong>on</strong>e of temporary alarm with later adjustment (Becker1967, 1974) and easing of enforcement effects and legal punishments(Johns<strong>on</strong> and Uppal, in press).When the drug-subculture theory was presented by Johns<strong>on</strong> (1973),two different drug subcultures were identified. Both subculturesbegan with marijuana use, but participants in the white drug subcultureused hallucinogens and pills, while black subcultural participantsdisproporti<strong>on</strong>ately used cocaine and heroin. The use of all drugs hasexpanded greatly since 1971, however, and four varieties or subsubcultureswithin the broader drug subculture may be distinguished:(1) the alcohol-abuse subculture, (2) the cannabis subculture, (3) themultiple-drug-use subculture, and (4) the heroin-injecti<strong>on</strong> subculture.These four subcultures are str<strong>on</strong>gly related to each other (Kandel1975, 1978b). generally in a unidimensi<strong>on</strong>al and cumulative fashi<strong>on</strong>(Single et al. 1974). Am<strong>on</strong>g American youths in the early 1970s.experimentati<strong>on</strong> with and increasingly regular use of alcohol precededmarijuana use, which in turn preceded the c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong> of other substances(hallucinogens, sedatives, stimulants, and cocaine), all ofwhich preceded heroin c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong> (except, perhaps, in a few innercity ghetto communities where some youths may have begun heroindirectly [O’D<strong>on</strong>nell and Clayt<strong>on</strong> 1979]). <strong>Drug</strong>-subculture theoryprovides a c<strong>on</strong>ceptual framework for analyzing why and how youthsbecome differentially involved in substance use.Each of these drug subculture varieties or sub-subcultures has numerousand different c<strong>on</strong>duct norms associated with it. Each subcultureemphasizes particular c<strong>on</strong>duct norms (see examples below) that governthe central activities of the group and of individual adherents orparticipants. Moreover, norms shift over time for an individual and apeer group. General types of c<strong>on</strong>duct norms will be identified andthen related to each of the four subcultures menti<strong>on</strong>ed above: (1)experimentati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>duct norms--the subcultural participant is expectedto c<strong>on</strong>sume the focal drug or drugs; (2) maintenance c<strong>on</strong>duct norms--the participant is expected to enjoy the behavior, to repeat the requisite115


ehavior, and to increase the frequency and amount used to the levelcomm<strong>on</strong> in the group; (3) reciprocity c<strong>on</strong>duct norms--when in peergroups, participants are expected to provide others with a porti<strong>on</strong> oftheir drugs either for free or at low cost, but the obligati<strong>on</strong> is reciprocalfor future occasi<strong>on</strong>s; (4) distributi<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>duct norms--the participantis expected to buy the relevant substance, to understand the informaland illegal distributi<strong>on</strong> system, and to engage in drug selling <strong>on</strong> asystematic basis.These general classes of c<strong>on</strong>duct norms are somewhat different in thefour identified drug subcultures that are briefly described here.THE ALCOHOL ABUSE SUBCULTUREAlcohol is a powerful psychoactive substance that is widely and legallyavailable in America. (The same can be said for tobacco, coffee, andtea.) Moreover, alcohol is widely used in the c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al middle-classculture as a beverage and as an agent for promoting social interacti<strong>on</strong>and relaxati<strong>on</strong>. Experimentati<strong>on</strong> with alcohol is the rule rather thanthe excepti<strong>on</strong>. The alcohol-abuse subculture, however, has maintenancenorms that stress the use of alcohol to “get high,” “smashed,” “ripped,”and to promote inebriating c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong>. Reciprocity c<strong>on</strong>duct normsinclude the pooling of m<strong>on</strong>ey to buy alcohol, the obligati<strong>on</strong> to buydrinks for others at some time in the immediate future, and bottlepassing in drinking groups. Distributi<strong>on</strong> norms include purchasingliquor when younger than the legal drinking age, or selling it to theunder-age drinker. For the most part, however, this subculture’sc<strong>on</strong>duct norms governing distributi<strong>on</strong> are not well developed becausealcohol can be easily and legally obtained; during prohibiti<strong>on</strong>, however,illicit distributi<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>duct norms quickly developed.THE CANNABIS SUBCULTUREMarijuana has become increasingly instituti<strong>on</strong>alized in America in thepast decade (Akers 1977; Jessor 1979; Johns<strong>on</strong> and Uppal, in press).Experimental or maintenance c<strong>on</strong>duct norms require the use of marijuana,generally by smoking. Informal pressure from <strong>on</strong>e’s peer groupor best friends has c<strong>on</strong>sistently emerged as a major factor in marijuanaexperimentati<strong>on</strong> (Kandel 1978b) and in the routine and heavy use ofmarijuana or hashish. After initiating use, the participants are expectedto use it <strong>on</strong> a routine basis, frequently <strong>on</strong> a weekly or daily basis; asthe regularity of use increases, the amount c<strong>on</strong>sumed per occasi<strong>on</strong> mayalso increase. The cannabis subculture promotes the sharing of marijuanaand hashish. A joint is frequently shared by many at a party,or where a peer group c<strong>on</strong>gregates. Usually, no m<strong>on</strong>ey is involved insuch sharing but different group members are expected to provide thedrug at various times. Distributi<strong>on</strong> norms expect weekly or morefrequent users to buy their own supply and/or to share with others.Often the buyer of a relatively large amount (an ounce or more) isexpected to give away or sell smaller amounts to friends at cost (Carey1968). Pers<strong>on</strong>s who become regular dealers of cannabis are expectedto give free samples, socialize, and smoke with potential buyers. Ofcourse, marijuana may also be sold as a strictly commercial product,albeit illegal, am<strong>on</strong>g unacquainted pers<strong>on</strong>s.116


THE MULTIPLE-DRUG SUBCULTUREThis subculture grows out of the cannabis subculture and is distinguishedby the use of many substances in additi<strong>on</strong> to cannabis (Singleet al. 1974). The experimental c<strong>on</strong>duct norms expect the participantto try almost any substance to achieve euphoria. Substances such ashallucinogens, barbiturates, other sedatives, stimulants, tranquilizers,inhalants, PCP, cocaine, and, possibly, heroin may also be tried.Even unknown substances may be tried. Maintenance norms expect theparticipant to use small amounts of different substances by sniffing,smoking, or oral c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong>; injecti<strong>on</strong> by hypodermic needle is usuallyavoided. While the regularity of use of a particular drug may beirregular (less than weekly), several different substances may bec<strong>on</strong>sumed within a particular week or <strong>on</strong> a single day. Sharing c<strong>on</strong>ductnorms are important; a pers<strong>on</strong> having a supply of pills or cocaine isexpected to share this supply with friends, who may reciprocate <strong>on</strong>another day with the same or different drugs. The distributi<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>ductnorms expect participants to combine funds, work jointly to obtaindrugs, locate supplies, and use whatever drugs are available withintheir price range. From the selling side, substances such as heroinand cocaine have relatively high ec<strong>on</strong>omic value and are seldom distributedfreely; dealers are not expected to provide free samples or socializeas much as with cannabis selling. Pers<strong>on</strong>s selling other substancesfrequently sell marijuana as well.THE HEROIN-INJECTION SUBCULTUREThis subculture, frequently referred to as the addict subculture,expects participants to c<strong>on</strong>sume heroin via hypodermic injecti<strong>on</strong>s.Maintenance c<strong>on</strong>duct norms expect injecti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> a weekly, daily, ormore frequent basis. While heroin is occasi<strong>on</strong>ally shared with peers,obligati<strong>on</strong>s to reciprocate at a later time or provide some other service(“cop” drugs) are str<strong>on</strong>g. Many participants are expected to selldrugs or heroin or supply “c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s” to other subcultural participants(Lindesmith 1947, 1965; Preble and Casey 1969; Agar 1973;Stephens and Levine 1971; Stephens and McBride 1976; Stephens andSmith 1976; Smith and Stephens 1976; Johns<strong>on</strong> and Preble 1978).The drug subculture perspective holds that participati<strong>on</strong> in the alcoholabusesubculture predisposes <strong>on</strong>e toward participati<strong>on</strong> in the cannabissubculture (Kandel 1976, 1978b). which is almost a prec<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>--am<strong>on</strong>gAmerican youths--for participati<strong>on</strong> in the multiple-drug-use subculture;many heroin-injecti<strong>on</strong> subculture participants have been previously,and c<strong>on</strong>tinue to be, involved in the multiple-drug-use subculture.<strong>Drug</strong>-subculture theory is designed to explain group behavior. Individualbehavior is defined as a functi<strong>on</strong> of following the subculture’svalues, c<strong>on</strong>duct norms, roles, rituals, and argot. The greater apers<strong>on</strong>’s commitment to a drug-using group and to subcultural values,c<strong>on</strong>duct norms, roles, rituals, and argot, the greater the predictabilityof behavior of that individual. While the following secti<strong>on</strong>s occasi<strong>on</strong>allyrefer to an individual, such a pers<strong>on</strong> is c<strong>on</strong>sidered to be an abstractactor who typifies the pattern of initiati<strong>on</strong> to drugs and increasingparticipati<strong>on</strong> according to the c<strong>on</strong>duct norms, roles, rituals, and argotof the specific drug subculture. Because there are many differentlevels of participati<strong>on</strong> in any subculture, and because a given individualholds a variety of roles in many spheres of society and may beexposed to c<strong>on</strong>flicting norms that may limit subcultural commitment, the117


vast majority of specific individuals using drugs may not become increasinglyand successively involved in each of the drug subcultures.USE, ABUSE, DEPENDENCY, AND ADDICTION<strong>Drug</strong>-subculture theory does not employ the c<strong>on</strong>cepts of abuse, dependency. and addicti<strong>on</strong>. These c<strong>on</strong>cepts are primarily seen as labels(Becker 1963; Rubingt<strong>on</strong> and Weinberg 1973) applied to subculturalroles or participants by social-c<strong>on</strong>trol agents and pers<strong>on</strong>s not involvedwith drug use, although terms such as “junkie,” “freak,” “pothead,”and “dope fiend” are frequently used as self-identities by subculturalparticipants. Subculture theory maintains that terms and c<strong>on</strong>ceptsused to describe patterns of drug use will shift over time both withinthe various subcultures and outside them. Subculture theory holdsthat participants tend to define their behavior as “normal” and toproject such patterns up<strong>on</strong> others regardless of how statistically raretheir behavior may be. Thus, drug c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong> episodes that socialc<strong>on</strong>trolauthorities c<strong>on</strong>sider abusive are c<strong>on</strong>sidered normal and areexpected of subcultural participants, especially those in dealer roles.Moreover, as time passes and levels of use increase in many segmentsof the populati<strong>on</strong>, the parent culture and legal instituti<strong>on</strong>s begin--reluctantly--to accept subcultural definiti<strong>on</strong>s. For example, marijuanause <strong>on</strong> a weekly basis was frequently labeled as heavy use in surveysc<strong>on</strong>ducted during the early 1970s, while near-daily use is now beingc<strong>on</strong>sidered as heavy use (Johnst<strong>on</strong> et al. 1978; Jessor 1979; Johns<strong>on</strong>and Uppal, in press). Even with regard to self-labeled “addicts,”research shows patterns of irregular use, lengthy periods of cessati<strong>on</strong>,followed by relapse to daily use. Thus, various commentators (Robins1976; Zinberg 1979; Johns<strong>on</strong> 1978; Johns<strong>on</strong> et al. 1979) have indicateddoubt about what c<strong>on</strong>stitutes opiate addicti<strong>on</strong> or dependency.UTILITY AND LIMITATIONS OFTHE DRUG SUBCULTURE PERSPECTIVEThe strengths of the drug-subculture perspective include the following:(a) It is formulated in terms of norms, values, roles, role behavior,rituals, and argot affecting interacti<strong>on</strong> between peers regarding theintenti<strong>on</strong>al n<strong>on</strong>medical use of drugs. Building from fundamental sociologicalc<strong>on</strong>cepts, the researcher’s effort can be directed toward describing,linking, and analyzing the relati<strong>on</strong>ships of these c<strong>on</strong>cepts. (b) Itprovides a broad c<strong>on</strong>ceptualizati<strong>on</strong> which can incorporate findings andempirical regularities from other studies. (c) It emphasizes the importanceand centrality of the pattern of illegal drug distributi<strong>on</strong> topatterns of drug use, to initiati<strong>on</strong> of other substances, and to othersocial problem behaviors (alcoholism, criminality, etc.). (d) Itaddresses the phenomena of drug abuse at a group level and focusesup<strong>on</strong> those aspects (values, c<strong>on</strong>duct norms, rituals, and argot) thatcannot be explained as the sum of individual behavior, psychologicalstates, or physiological reacti<strong>on</strong>s to drug c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong>. (e) It providesa means of explaining or understanding change in drug use by individuals,groups, and within the subculture itself. Few other perspectives(to the author’s knowledge) present a c<strong>on</strong>ceptual model for explainingbehaviors associated with the illegal drug market and linking them todrug use. (See Langer 1977; Goode 1970.)118


The drug-subculture theory presented by Johns<strong>on</strong> (1973) has receivedlittle commentary or criticism in the professi<strong>on</strong>al literature. Most ofthe empirical relati<strong>on</strong>ships presented there have been uncovered inother studies, but testing of the theoretical aspects has been widelyneglected . Nevertheless, some limitati<strong>on</strong>s have been informally commentedup<strong>on</strong> and await further research in the near future.This perspective is difficult to prove since the critical independentvariables, the c<strong>on</strong>duct norms, cannot be measured directly. That is,expectati<strong>on</strong>s of behavior in a particular situati<strong>on</strong> were not and cannotbe measured directly, although Orcutt (1978), Akers et al. (1979),and Short and Strodtbeck (1965) have made attempts to measure suchnormative orientati<strong>on</strong>s. Johns<strong>on</strong> (1973) <strong>on</strong>ly makes inferences aboutthe c<strong>on</strong>duct norms from the behavior (cannabis, hallucinogen, heroin,or other drug use) which is to be explained. This is a critical problemthat is unlikely to be rectified in the future. Another weakness is thecurrent lack of specificati<strong>on</strong> about why, how, and where subculturesemerge and change through time. There is a distinct need for ethnographicstudies of drug-using peer groups to observe, questi<strong>on</strong>, andanalyze the c<strong>on</strong>scious awareness of expected behavior and unc<strong>on</strong>sciousmotivati<strong>on</strong>s--hypothesized to be due to the c<strong>on</strong>duct norms--directingindividual and group patterns of drug use. Survey research candocument the effects, but a more careful elaborati<strong>on</strong> of the process isneeded.The evidence presented in Johns<strong>on</strong> (1973) is based up<strong>on</strong> a crosssecti<strong>on</strong>alsurvey in which l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal data are needed to test many ofthe critical processes hypothesized. This shortcoming was noted in thebook; some recent l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal studies have presented findings supportingsome hypothesized processes (Single et al. 1974; Single and Kandel1978; Kandel 1978b; Johnst<strong>on</strong> et al. 1978; Jessor and Jessor 1977), butnot others (Ginsberg and Greenley 1978).Jessor (1979) indicates discomfort with the drug-subculture perspectivebecause (a) large proporti<strong>on</strong>s of the youth populati<strong>on</strong>s (frequentlymore than a majority) now use marijuana, making it difficult to distinguishclear subcultural boundaries, and (b) the role of peers in initiatingn<strong>on</strong>users to marijuana use and drug-related role behaviors appearsno different from the role of peers in influencing other behavioraldomains--values, sexual behavior, styles of dress--in which peerinfluence is c<strong>on</strong>siderable. Jessor’s comments appear to equate thec<strong>on</strong>cept of subculture with a subsociety (see Fine and Kleinman 1979).while the subculture perspective out<strong>line</strong>d here does not do so. Inadditi<strong>on</strong>, the mechanism (peer influence) by which pers<strong>on</strong>s are recruitedfor participati<strong>on</strong> in any of the various n<strong>on</strong>c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al subcultures(see above) may be similar, but the c<strong>on</strong>duct norms, values, rituals,argot, and “central activities around which the group” is organized(Cloward and Ohlin 1960) may reflect differentially structured andc<strong>on</strong>ceptually distinct subcultures.A major problem with applying subculture theory to drug use is dissatisfacti<strong>on</strong>with the diffuse and widespread meanings the term “subculture”has acquired. The absence of an accepted definiti<strong>on</strong> for thisc<strong>on</strong>cept, a feature shared with many other sociological c<strong>on</strong>cepts andtheories, however, should not detract from the potential of drugsubculturetheory. Such a theory can alert the researcher and readerto critical c<strong>on</strong>cepts and distincti<strong>on</strong>s, measurable behavior patterns,potentially fruitful hypotheses or relati<strong>on</strong>ships between variables, andlead them to important insights about how and why drug users behavethe way they do.119


Developmental Stages inAdolescent <strong>Drug</strong> InvolvementDenise B. Kandel, Ph.D.INTRODUCTIONA variety of human characteristics pertaining to cognitive, psychological,and physiological functi<strong>on</strong>s have been shown to follow well-defineddevelopmental sequences. Some of the best known of the developmentalstage theories include Piaget’s (1954) hierarchical theory of cognitivestructures and Kohlberg’s (1973) related theory of moral behavior.The appearance of different stages has been postulated to result eitherfrom biological maturati<strong>on</strong> that is under genetic c<strong>on</strong>trol or from theinteracti<strong>on</strong> of the biological organism with the envir<strong>on</strong>ment--physical,social, or cultural.I would like to propose that culturally determined developmental stagescan be observed with respect to drug behavior. However, I advancethis noti<strong>on</strong> not as a formal, grand theory of drug use, but rather as aframework around which to develop specific theories of initiati<strong>on</strong>,progressi<strong>on</strong>, and regressi<strong>on</strong> in drug behavior.Substances that are subject to abuse include not <strong>on</strong>ly the illegal drugs,but those such as alcohol and tobacco that are comm<strong>on</strong>ly used insociety for recreati<strong>on</strong>al purposes, as well as the medically prescribedpsychoactive drugs, such as stimulants and minor tranquilizers. Untilfairly recently, c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s of patterns of sequential or multipledrug use were restricted to a c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> of the illegal drugs.Retrospective studies of the drug histories of heroin addicts, in whichmarijuana use was found to characterize every resp<strong>on</strong>dent, gave riseto the c<strong>on</strong>troversial “stepping st<strong>on</strong>e” theory of drug addicti<strong>on</strong> in whichuse of marijuana was assumed inevitably to lead to the use of harddrugs. especially heroin. The theory is problematic (Goode 1972,1974), and with rare excepti<strong>on</strong>s (see O’D<strong>on</strong>nell and Clayt<strong>on</strong> 1978), fewinvestigators today accept it.However, studies of drug-use patterns in different cohorts of adolescentssuggest that there are at this time in the United States welldefinedstages and sequences in patterns of drug involvement and thatthe so-called legal drugs, such as alcohol and tobacco, must be accepted120


as an integral and crucial part of the sequence. It is most importantto keep in mind that positi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> a particular point in the sequencedoes not indicate that the individual will necessarily progress to otherdrugs higher up in the sequence. Rather, we suggest that the use ofa drug lower in a sequence is a necessary but not a sufficient c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>for progressi<strong>on</strong> to a higher stage indicating involvement with moreserious drugs.This developmental noti<strong>on</strong> of stages in drug use is empirically derivedfrom extensive analyses of cross secti<strong>on</strong>al and l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal data <strong>on</strong>patterns of drug use in adolescence. At least four distinct developmentalstages in adolescent involvement in legal and illegal drugs canbe identified: (1) beer or wine, (2) cigarettes or hard liquor, (3) marijuana,and (4) other illicit drugs. The supporting evidence for thismodel is twofold: (a) results of analyses of hierarchical and sequentialpatterns of drug use, and (b) results of l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal analyses wheredifferent variables identify adolescents at risk who progress from <strong>on</strong>estage to the next.SCALOGRAM ANALYSES OF PATTERNSOF DRUG USEThe first suggesti<strong>on</strong> of stages in drug use came from scalogram analysescarried out <strong>on</strong> a cross secti<strong>on</strong> of New York State adolescents(N=8,206) in public sec<strong>on</strong>dary schools (Single et al. 1974). Guttmanscale analysis is especially well suited for analyzing the ordering ofpatterns of drug use because of its properties of unidimensi<strong>on</strong>ality andcumulati<strong>on</strong>. Since the scale items all measure the same underlyingdimensi<strong>on</strong>, the scale ranking of resp<strong>on</strong>dents indicates not <strong>on</strong>ly howmany but which drugs they have used. Therefore, knowing an individual’sscore <strong>on</strong> a given scale, <strong>on</strong>e can estimate which substances havebeen used, though not the order in which they have been used. Theresults indicated that adolescent drug use behavior fit a valid Guttmanscale. The patterns of lifetime use of drugs could be arranged accordingto a well-defined cumulative and <strong>on</strong>e-dimensi<strong>on</strong>al hierarchical orderwith seven steps. The fit of the data with the Guttman scale modelimplied that youths at any <strong>on</strong>e step have used the drug at that particularlevel as well as all drugs ranked lower, but they have not usedany of the drugs ranked higher. Since these earlier findings werebased <strong>on</strong> data gathered at <strong>on</strong>e time, no time order am<strong>on</strong>g the usagepatterns could be established. Direct evidence was provided by Guttmanscale analyses of drug use resp<strong>on</strong>ses over time (Kandel 1975; Kandeland Faust 1975). Analyses were replicated <strong>on</strong> two different cohorts:(a) a representative panel sample of high school students in New YorkState followed over <strong>on</strong>e school year at a five- to six-m<strong>on</strong>th interval(N=5,468); and (b) a panel sample of seniors who were c<strong>on</strong>tacted fiveto nine m<strong>on</strong>ths following graduati<strong>on</strong> from high school (N =985). Atleast four distinct developmental stages in adolescent involvement inlegal and illegal drugs were identified. These were noted above:(1) beer or wine, (2) cigarettes or hard liquor, (3) marijuana, and(4) other illicit drugs. (See figure 1.) The legal drugs are necessaryintermediates between n<strong>on</strong>use and marijuana. For example, whereas 27percent of the high school students who had smoked and had drunkhard liquor progressed to marijuana within the five-m<strong>on</strong>th followupperiod, <strong>on</strong>ly two percent of those who had not used any legal substancedid so. Marijuana, in turn, was a crucial step <strong>on</strong> the way to other121


FIGURE 1.–Major stages of adolescent Involvement in drug useMajor changes of adolescent involvement in drug use. Probabilities of moving from <strong>on</strong>e stage toanother based <strong>on</strong> changes between Fall 1971 and Spring 1972 in a cohort of New York Statehigh school students, 14 to 18 years old. Youths who started using more than <strong>on</strong>e drug withinthe followup interval were distributed in a sequential order which reproduced the proporti<strong>on</strong>s ofknown exclusive starters of each drug.Reprinted with permissi<strong>on</strong> from D. Kandel. “Stages in Adolescent Involvement in <strong>Drug</strong> Use.” Science 190(1975):912-914.Copyright © 1975 by the American Associati<strong>on</strong> for the Advancement of Science.


illicit drugs. While 26 percent of marijuana users progressed to LSD,amphetamines, or heroin, <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e percent of n<strong>on</strong>users of any drugand four percent of legal users did so. 1 This sequence was found ineach of the four years in high school and in the year following graduati<strong>on</strong>.The same steps were followed in regressi<strong>on</strong> as in progressi<strong>on</strong> inpatterns of use within the followup interval.Except for our own research, no other studies that specifically testthe noti<strong>on</strong> of stages that we have advanced have yet appeared in theliterature. Related analyses include scalogram analyses and analysesof self-reported order of first usage. We carried out additi<strong>on</strong>al scalogramanalyses <strong>on</strong> sets of data besides the New York State sample totest further the applicability of the stage model to other samples ofyouths. We have found the same sequential pattern am<strong>on</strong>g males andfemales and am<strong>on</strong>g adolescents of different ages. Variati<strong>on</strong>s in scalabilityare observed in black as compared to white adolescents (Singleet al. 1974; Jessop et al. 1976; Jessop et al. 1977), although the sameoverall model fits the data in both racial groups.Prior to our studies, <strong>on</strong>ly two scalogram analyses had been reported inthe literature, both inadequate because of methodological or c<strong>on</strong>ceptualI imitati<strong>on</strong>s. Sinnitt et al. (1972) c<strong>on</strong>cluded that drug experiences ofcollege students with alcohol and illicit substances were unidimensi<strong>on</strong>aland cumulative. However, the sample of 33 cases was very small andselected. Loiselle and Whitehead (1971), <strong>on</strong> the other hand, c<strong>on</strong>cludedthat drug use patterns did not fulfill the criteria for unidimensi<strong>on</strong>alityimplied by Guttman scaling. However, questi<strong>on</strong>able decisi<strong>on</strong>s in thestudy must be noted, namely the restricti<strong>on</strong> of the analysis to users ofillicit drugs. Out of a sample of 1,606 high school students, theauthors focused <strong>on</strong> 257 users of any of five drugs (marijuana, stimulants,tranquilizers, glue, and barbiturates) in <strong>on</strong>e analysis (or 16percent of the sample), and <strong>on</strong> 105 marijuana smokers (or seven percent)in another. The skewed marginal distributi<strong>on</strong> of the illicit druguse items is not sufficient methodological justificati<strong>on</strong> for restrictingtheanalyses to users, since techniques are available to correct forsuch skewness. Furthermore, the exclusi<strong>on</strong> of n<strong>on</strong>users of illicitdrugs eliminated a crucial part of the sample required to c<strong>on</strong>siderpatterns of n<strong>on</strong>use and use of various drugs. Indeed, tobacco andalcohol were excluded, although these substances are crucial to ac<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> of processes of drug use.By relying <strong>on</strong> a different criteri<strong>on</strong> for defining usage order, namelyself-reported order of first use, Whitehead and Cabral (1975-76) subsequentlyreached a c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> different from that based <strong>on</strong> the earlierGuttman scaling. Mean order of first use of 10 drugs, includingtobacco and alcohol, in a sample of 902 adolescent users was tobacco,alcohol, marijuana, and other illicit drugs, in that sequence. Asimilar order has been reported by Goldstein and his collaborators(Goldstein et al. 1975) am<strong>on</strong>g college students from an analysis ofself-reported time of initial use of each of eight drugs. A matrix ofpairwise comparis<strong>on</strong>s am<strong>on</strong>g the drugs was created according to theorder of first use for each drug in a pair. Beer and liquor appearedto precede tobacco, followed by marijuana and by other illicit drugs.1 It must be kept in mind that these probabilities of change typify theparticular cohorts that were studied and would probably be somewhatdifferent in different samples c<strong>on</strong>tacted at a different period.123


However, the order of tobacco and liquor was somewhat ambiguous:Am<strong>on</strong>g those students who had used both drugs, the same proporti<strong>on</strong>reported having used each first. Intenti<strong>on</strong>s for future use followedthe hierarchical pattern of use with “the more unusual drugs . . .most often . . . desired <strong>on</strong>ly after acquaintance with the more comm<strong>on</strong>substances” (p. 26). From the l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal data available in theirstudy, the authors presented <strong>on</strong>ly the proporti<strong>on</strong> of each class ofusers who progressed or regressed al<strong>on</strong>g the assumed hierarchy ofdrugs over the four years of college, with no specificati<strong>on</strong> of theparticular drugs used.As we noted earlier, l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal data are required for a definite testof developmental stages in drug behavior. In the absence of otherc<strong>on</strong>tradictory evidence, the l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal analyses of patterns of drugbehavior over time that we have c<strong>on</strong>ducted and the inferential dataprovided by other investigators c<strong>on</strong>stitute to date str<strong>on</strong>g evidence forthe existence of stages in drug use.STAGE-SPECIFIC PREDICTORS OFDRUG INITIATIONFurther evidence for the existence of stages is provided by the findingsthat different social psychological factors predict adolescent initiati<strong>on</strong>into different stages of drug use. We have combined the noti<strong>on</strong> thatadolescent drug use involves sequential stages with a l<strong>on</strong>gitudinalresearch design in which the populati<strong>on</strong> at risk for initiati<strong>on</strong> into eachof the stages could be clearly identified. This has allowed us toassess the relative importance of various factors to predict initialtransiti<strong>on</strong>s into various types of drug behaviors. The socialpsychologicalantecedents of entry into three sequential stages ofadolescent drug use--hard liquor, marijuana, and other illicit drugs--were examined in a two-wave panel sample of New York State publicsec<strong>on</strong>dary students and subsamples of matched adolescent-parent andadolescent-best-schoolfriend dyads (Kandel et al. 1978). Each of fourclusters of predictor variables--parental influences, peer influences,adolescent involvement in various behaviors, and adolescent beliefs andvalues--and single predictors within each cluster assume differentialimportance for each stage of drug behavior. (See figure 2.) Priorinvolvements in a variety of activities, such as minor delinquency anduse of cigarettes, beer, and wine, are most important for predictinghard liquor use. Adolescents’ beliefs and values favorable to the useof marijuana and associati<strong>on</strong> with marijuana-using peers are the str<strong>on</strong>gestpredictors of initiati<strong>on</strong> into marijuana. Poor relati<strong>on</strong>s with parents,feelings of depressi<strong>on</strong>, and exposure to drug-using peers are mostimportant for predicting initiati<strong>on</strong> into illicit drugs other than marijuana.Thus, at the earliest levels of involvement, adolescents who haveengaged in a number of minor delinquent or deviant activities, whoenjoy high levels of sociability with their peers, and who are exposedto peers and parents who drink start to drink themselves. The relati<strong>on</strong>shipwith parental use of hard liquor suggests that these youthslearn drinking patterns from their parents. The use of marijuana ispreceded by acceptance of a cluster of beliefs and values that arefavorable to marijuana use and in oppositi<strong>on</strong> to many standards upheldby adults, by involvement in a peer envir<strong>on</strong>ment in which marijuana isused, and by participati<strong>on</strong> in the same minor forms of deviant behavior124


FIGURE 2.–Percentage of explained variance accountedfor by each successive clusterReprinted with permissi<strong>on</strong> from D. B. Kandel, R. C. Kessler, and R. Z. Margulies,“Antecedents of Adolescent Initiati<strong>on</strong> into Stages of <strong>Drug</strong> Use: A Developmental Analysis,”in L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal Research <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> Use: Empirical Findings and Methodological Issues. ed.D. B. Kandel (Washingt<strong>on</strong>. D.C.: Hemisphere, 1978). Copyright © 1978 by HemispherePublishing Corporati<strong>on</strong>.125


that precede the use of hard liquor. By comparis<strong>on</strong> use of illicitdrugs other than marijuana is preceded by poor relati<strong>on</strong>ships withparents, by exposure to parents and to peers who themselves use avariety of legal, medical, and illegal drugs, by psychological distress,and by a series of pers<strong>on</strong>al characteristics somewhat more deviant thanthose that characterize the novice marijuana or hard liquor user.CONCLUSIONAt this time in history in the United States, adolescents’ involvementin drugs appears to follow certain paths. Beer and wine are the firstsubstances used by youth. Tobacco and hard liquor are used next.The use of marijuana rarely takes place without prior use of liquor ortobacco, or both. Similarly, the use of illicit drugs other than marijuanararely takes place in the absence of prior experimentati<strong>on</strong> withmarijuana.The documentati<strong>on</strong> that different factors are important for differentdrugs provides additi<strong>on</strong>al support for the claim, developed <strong>on</strong> thebasis of Guttman scale analysis, that drug involvement proceeds throughdiscrete stages. The noti<strong>on</strong> of “stage” itself allows a more fruitfulspecificati<strong>on</strong> of the role and structure of different causal factors atdifferent stages of involvement.For example, as regards interpers<strong>on</strong>al influences, we find at differentstages not <strong>on</strong>ly differences in source of influence but also differencesin the aspects of interpers<strong>on</strong>al influences that are important. In theearly stage of drug use, parental behavior seems to be critical inleading the youth to experiment with hard liquor. In later phases ofinitiati<strong>on</strong>, the quality of the parent-child relati<strong>on</strong>ship becomes important,with closeness to parents shielding adolescents from involvementin the most serious forms of drug use. Similarly, there is evidencethat a generalized peer influence, which is important in predictinginitiati<strong>on</strong> to legal drugs and marijuana, is partially supplanted by theinfluence of a single best friend in leading to the initiati<strong>on</strong> of otherillicit drugs. Findings of this kind point to the importance of examiningprofiles of interpers<strong>on</strong>al influences over a series of behaviors, values,and attitudes in order to understand better their dynamic nature.Thus, if <strong>on</strong>e accepts the noti<strong>on</strong> that progressively more serious involvementin drugs underlies the stages we have out<strong>line</strong>d, the data suggestthat the more serious the behavior, the greater the relative importanceof the specific role model provided by <strong>on</strong>e friend in c<strong>on</strong>trast to thesame behavior of the whole group.Similar specificati<strong>on</strong> occurs with respect to the role of participati<strong>on</strong> indeviant behaviors. Participati<strong>on</strong> in various deviant behaviors is mostrelevant in starting to use alcohol, least for illicit drugs. The lessserious the drug, the more its use or n<strong>on</strong>use may depend <strong>on</strong> situati<strong>on</strong>alfactors. By c<strong>on</strong>trast, initiati<strong>on</strong> into illicit drugs other than marijuanaappears to be a c<strong>on</strong>scious resp<strong>on</strong>se to intrapsychic pressures of somesort or other.Many theories of drug dependence offer some c<strong>on</strong>cept of individualpathology as a primary explanati<strong>on</strong>, while others stress social factors.Each of these c<strong>on</strong>cepts may apply to different stages of the process ofinvolvement in drug behavior, social factors playing a more importantrole in the early stages; psychological factors, in the later <strong>on</strong>es.126


The identificati<strong>on</strong> of cumulative stages in drug behavior has importantc<strong>on</strong>ceptual and methodological implicati<strong>on</strong>s for identifying the factorsthat relate to drug use, either as causes or as c<strong>on</strong>sequences. In al<strong>on</strong>gitudinal analytical framework, there should be decompositi<strong>on</strong> of thepanel sample into appropriate subsamples of individuals at a particularstage who are at risk for initiati<strong>on</strong> into the next stage. Since eachstage represents a cumulative pattern of use and c<strong>on</strong>tains fewer adolescentsthan the preceding stage in the sequence, comparis<strong>on</strong>s of usersand n<strong>on</strong>users must be made am<strong>on</strong>g members of the restricted groupthat has already used the drugs at the preceding stage. Otherwise,the attributes identified as apparent characteristics of a particularclass of drug users may actually reflect characteristics important forinvolvement in drugs at the preceding stage(s). The definiti<strong>on</strong> ofstages allows <strong>on</strong>e to define a populati<strong>on</strong> at risk and to isolate systematically,within that populati<strong>on</strong>, those individuals who succumb to thisrisk within a specific time interval.The noti<strong>on</strong> of stage itself is somewhat ambiguous (Wohlwill 1973).Am<strong>on</strong>g developmental psychologists, c<strong>on</strong>troversy exists about whetherthe noti<strong>on</strong> of stages implies that development must necessarily occur ina hierarchical and fixed order, as Piaget, for example, proposes.However, the noti<strong>on</strong> of invariance must be subjected to empirical test(Phillips and Kelly 1975). This is especially important for drug behavior.Indeed, as regards the noti<strong>on</strong> of stages in drug use, two reservati<strong>on</strong>smust be kept in mind. To date, the stages have been identifiedin populati<strong>on</strong>s of American adolescents. The specific sequences areprobably culturally and historically determined. Crosscultural studiesare required in order to determine the extent to which the order thathas been observed is in fact an invariant <strong>on</strong>e. These studies wouldindicate whether or not involvement in illicit drugs is always precededby use of legal drugs, as appears to be the case in the United States,or whether, in certain cultures, involvement in cigarettes, alcohol, andmarijuana proceeds al<strong>on</strong>g parallel and n<strong>on</strong>overlapping paths. Furthermore,while the data show a very clear-cut sequence in the use ofvarious drugs, they do not prove that the use of a particular druginfallibly leads to the use of other drugs higher up in the sequence.Many youths stop at a particular stage without progressing any further.Nor can the findings be interpreted to show that there is somethinginherent in the pharmacological properties of the drugs themselves thatleads inexorably from <strong>on</strong>e to another.The stage theory itself is a recent c<strong>on</strong>ceptualizati<strong>on</strong> of drug behaviorand needs further testing and documentati<strong>on</strong>.127


Self-Esteem andSelf-Derogati<strong>on</strong> Theoryof <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>Howard B. Kaplan, Ph.D.THEORY OVERVIEWWithin the c<strong>on</strong>text of the general theory of deviant behavior presentedbelow, opiate dependence as well as use/abuse of other illicit substances(hallucinogens, barbiturates, amphetamines, marijuana, alcohol) isregarded as an alternative deviant resp<strong>on</strong>se to self-rejecting attitudesgenerated in the course of normative membership-group experienceswhich functi<strong>on</strong> more or less effectively to reduce the experiences ofthe subjectively distressful self-rejecting attitudes. As a theory ofdeviant behavior it would apply <strong>on</strong>ly to drug use/abuse patterns whichdo not c<strong>on</strong>form to the normative expectati<strong>on</strong>s of the pers<strong>on</strong>’s (predeviance)membership group(s) and which derive from the loss of a previousmotivati<strong>on</strong> to c<strong>on</strong>form or from the development of a new motivati<strong>on</strong>to deviate from normative expectati<strong>on</strong>s. The definiti<strong>on</strong> excludes behaviorswhich, although defined as deviant by other groups, are compatiblewith the normative expectati<strong>on</strong>s of the subject’s membership/referencegroups, as well as behaviors to which the pers<strong>on</strong> was motivated toc<strong>on</strong>form but was incapable of so doing because of c<strong>on</strong>flicting expectati<strong>on</strong>sor physical incapacity. The theory, thus, would not be applicablein situati<strong>on</strong>s where, for example, marijuana use was nearlyuniversally observed and/or approved (as <strong>on</strong> a college campus relativelyisolated from extracollege influences) or where the behavior was highlycompatible with other values whether or not it was an already establishedpattern (as where experimentati<strong>on</strong> with illicit drugs in a slumyouth social network is c<strong>on</strong>gruent with the valued attributes of toughnessand adventuresomeness). Normative socializati<strong>on</strong> or social learningtheories would be more appropriate to the explanati<strong>on</strong> of illicitdrug use/abuse in these situati<strong>on</strong>s.The theory c<strong>on</strong>siders the comm<strong>on</strong> factors more or less directly influencingthe adopti<strong>on</strong> of any of a range of deviant patterns, the factorsinfluencing the adopti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>on</strong>e rather than other deviant patterns(e.g., opiate versus hallucinogen use, drug use versus interpers<strong>on</strong>alviolence, property crimes), and factors influencing the c<strong>on</strong>tinuity ofthe deviant pattern.128


The theoretical model is based up<strong>on</strong> the postulate of the self-esteemmotive, whereby, universally and characteristically, a pers<strong>on</strong> is said tobehave so as to maximize the experience of positive self-attitudes, andto minimize the experience of negative <strong>on</strong>es. Self-attitudes refer tothe pers<strong>on</strong>’s (more or less intense) positive and negative emoti<strong>on</strong>alexperiences up<strong>on</strong> perceiving and evaluating his or her own attitubutesand behavior. The model does not apply in the rare instances inwhich a basic c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> for the development of the self-esteem motiveis not present. This c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> is the early and c<strong>on</strong>tinued existence ofstable relati<strong>on</strong>ships between self and significant others in the c<strong>on</strong>textof which the behavior of significant others is predictably c<strong>on</strong>tingentup<strong>on</strong> the resp<strong>on</strong>ses of self. Such a c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> is not present where theresp<strong>on</strong>ses of significant others are either uniform (whether in a punitiveor rewarding directi<strong>on</strong>) or random.Intense self-rejecting attitudes are said to be the end result of ahistory of membership group experiences in which the subject wasunable to defend against, adapt to, or cope with circumstances havingself-devaluing implicati<strong>on</strong>s (that is, disvalued attributes and behaviors,and negative evaluati<strong>on</strong>s of the subject by valued others). Theseencompass a range of variables apparent in other theories includingpeer rejecti<strong>on</strong>, parental neglect, high expectati<strong>on</strong>s for achievement,school failure, physical stigmata, social stigmata (e.g., disvaluedgroup memberships), impaired sex-role identity, ego deficiencies, lowcoping abilities, and (generally) coping mechanisms that are sociallydisvalued and/or are otherwise self-defeating. The likelihood of experiencingcircumstances with self-devaluing implicati<strong>on</strong>s and/or failing topossess effective adaptive/coping/defensive patterns (which wouldforestall or assuage the experience of circumstances with self-devaluingimplicati<strong>on</strong>s) is in turn influenced by complex patterns of interactingsocial (value system, available social support mechanisms, complexity ofthe social system, rate of social change, positi<strong>on</strong>s in the social system,etc.) and <strong>on</strong>togenetic (including c<strong>on</strong>stituti<strong>on</strong>ally given deficits) variables.By virtue of the actual and subjective associati<strong>on</strong> between past membershipgroup experiences and the development of intensely distressfulnegative self-attitudes, the pers<strong>on</strong> loses motivati<strong>on</strong> to c<strong>on</strong>form to, andbecomes motivated to deviate from, membership group patterns (thosespecifically associated with the genesis of negative self-attitudes and,by a process of generalizati<strong>on</strong>, other aspects of the membership groups’normative structures). Simultaneously, the unfulfilled self-esteemmotive prompts the subject to seek alternative (that is, deviant)resp<strong>on</strong>se patterns which offer hope of reducing the experience ofnegative (and increasing the experiences of positive) self-attitudes.Thus, the pers<strong>on</strong> is motivated to seek and adopt deviant resp<strong>on</strong>sepatterns not <strong>on</strong>ly because of a loss of motivati<strong>on</strong> to c<strong>on</strong>form to thenormative structure (which has an earlier associati<strong>on</strong> with the genesisof negative self-attitudes) but also because the deviant patterns representthe <strong>on</strong>ly motivati<strong>on</strong>ally acceptable alternatives that might serveself-enhancing functi<strong>on</strong>s effectively.Which of several deviant patterns is adopted, then, would be a functi<strong>on</strong>of the pers<strong>on</strong>’s history of experiences influencing the visibility andsubjective evaluati<strong>on</strong> of the self-enhancing/self-devaluing potential ofthe pattern(s) in questi<strong>on</strong>. A particular drug use/abuse pattern ismore likely to be adopted, for example, if, due to the greater availabilityof the drug its use was more apparent am<strong>on</strong>g peers at school orin the neighborhood--that is, if the pattern was more visible. The129


subjective likelihood of self-enhancing c<strong>on</strong>sequences of the behaviorwill reflect such variables as the subjectively perceived attitudestoward the illicit drug abuse pattern by members of positive andnegative reference groups (peers, family, authority figures, school),the visibility of more or less prevalent adverse c<strong>on</strong>sequences of use ofthe illicit drug (arrest, loss of c<strong>on</strong>trol, etc.), and the perceivedcompatibility of the c<strong>on</strong>sequences and c<strong>on</strong>comitants of the drug abusepattern with behavior (in)appropriate to (dis)valued social roles.Adopti<strong>on</strong> of the deviant resp<strong>on</strong>se has self-enhancing c<strong>on</strong>sequences if itfacilitates intrapsychic or interpers<strong>on</strong>al avoidance of self-devaluingexperiences associated with the predeviance membership group, servesto attack (symbolically or otherwise) the perceived basis of the pers<strong>on</strong>’sself-rejecting attitudes (that is, representati<strong>on</strong>s of the normative groupstructure), and/or offers substitute patterns with self-enhancingpotential for behavior patterns associated with the genesis of selfrejectingattitudes. Avoidance functi<strong>on</strong>s might be served through thec<strong>on</strong>sequent rejecti<strong>on</strong> of the subject who adopted the drug abuse patternby the normative membership groups in which the self-rejecting attitudeswere developed (resulting in decreased vulnerability to c<strong>on</strong>tinuingself-devaluing experiences), facilitating regressive return to a moredependent state (thus avoiding <strong>on</strong>e’s resp<strong>on</strong>sibilities and the risk offailure to carry them out), the pharmacologic effects of detachment oranesthetizati<strong>on</strong> of self-punitive feelings, etc. Attacks up<strong>on</strong> the normativestructure are symbolized by the illicit nature of the behaviorpattern. Substitute gratificati<strong>on</strong>s may be provided by identificati<strong>on</strong>with a community of users who accept the subject by virtue of his orher c<strong>on</strong>formity to group norms, pharmacologic inducti<strong>on</strong> of feeling inc<strong>on</strong>trol of <strong>on</strong>e’s moods, facilitati<strong>on</strong> of self-enhancing social interacti<strong>on</strong>,replicati<strong>on</strong> of an earlier time (the womb) of feeling more accepted, etc.To the extent that the pers<strong>on</strong> in fact experiences self-enhancingc<strong>on</strong>sequences, is able to defend against any intervening adverse c<strong>on</strong>sequencesof the behavior (anticipated or unanticipated), and does notperceive alternative resp<strong>on</strong>ses with self-enhancing potential the patternis likely to be c<strong>on</strong>firmed. Whether or not these outcomes occur will bea functi<strong>on</strong> of such mutually influencing variables as the nature of thedeviant act, societal resp<strong>on</strong>se to the act, and the pers<strong>on</strong>’s need-valueand adaptive/coping patterns. For example, a highly visible andhighly disvalued act might lead to apprehensi<strong>on</strong> and adjudicati<strong>on</strong> withc<strong>on</strong>sequences of stigmatizati<strong>on</strong>, enforced deviant role enactment, exacerbati<strong>on</strong>of a need to justify the act through c<strong>on</strong>tinued performance ofit, isolati<strong>on</strong> from social c<strong>on</strong>trol, isolati<strong>on</strong> of the subject from legitimateopportunities, and exposure to self-enhancing illegitimate patterns,while at the same time being c<strong>on</strong>gruent with pers<strong>on</strong>al need dispositi<strong>on</strong>(e.g., power) and defense/coping mechanisms (e.g., attack). In sucha case the deviant pattern might become part of the subject’s pers<strong>on</strong>aland (new) social lifestyle, with the pattern being performed as appropriateto the new lifestyle and with gratificati<strong>on</strong> coming from c<strong>on</strong>formancewith the lifestyle. Insofar as the new lifestyle precludes theexperience of self-devaluing life events which were characteristic offormer membership group experiences, the deviant pattern should, afortiori, have self-enhancing c<strong>on</strong>sequences.Or, the deviant pattern may have a low probability of evoking severe(if any) sancti<strong>on</strong>s from membership groups (whether because of lowvisibility or otherwise) but still have self-enhancing c<strong>on</strong>sequences, inwhich case the subject may be expected to perform the pattern inresp<strong>on</strong>se to discrete life events with self-devaluing implicati<strong>on</strong>s. The130


frequency of the deviant pattern becomes a functi<strong>on</strong> of the frequencyof self-devaluing life events and c<strong>on</strong>tinuity of a net aggregate ofgratifying over punishing c<strong>on</strong>sequences of the deviant adaptati<strong>on</strong>.However, cessati<strong>on</strong> of the drug abuse (or other deviant patterns)would be likely to occur if and when self-devaluing outcomes outweighedself-enhancing outcomes. In that case the subject would be likely toexperiment with alternative modes of deviance, since normative patternswould c<strong>on</strong>tinue to be motivati<strong>on</strong>ally unacceptable as l<strong>on</strong>g as they weresubjectively and in fact associated with self-devaluing experiences.But insofar as individual maturati<strong>on</strong> and correlated changes in socioenvir<strong>on</strong>mentalexperiences (including social support systems) reducethe likelihood of self-devaluing experiences, offer new opportunitiesfor self-enhancement, and provide the pers<strong>on</strong> with effective copingmechanisms and a correlated realistic sense of c<strong>on</strong>trol over the envir<strong>on</strong>ment,the illicit drug use is likely to cease in favor of normativeresp<strong>on</strong>se patterns.The pers<strong>on</strong> is likely to relapse into the deviant resp<strong>on</strong>se pattern <strong>on</strong>lyin the face of erosi<strong>on</strong> of pers<strong>on</strong>al and social support mechanisms,pervasive self-devaluing experiences, and a history of self-enhancingc<strong>on</strong>sequences of earlier illicit drug use.Support for the theory is provided by a c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> of the compatibilityof the theory with previous studies <strong>on</strong> deviant behavior (Kaplan1972, 1975b) and by the results of a prospective l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal study ofadolescents which was designed to test several aspects of this theory,including those c<strong>on</strong>cerning the postulate of the self-esteem motive(Kaplan 1975d), hypothesized antecedents of negative self-attitudes(Kaplan 1976a). relati<strong>on</strong>ships between antecedent level of (and increasesin) self-derogati<strong>on</strong> and subsequent adopti<strong>on</strong> of deviant resp<strong>on</strong>ses(Kaplan 1975a, 1976b, 1977b, 1978a), factors said to intervene betweenself-derogati<strong>on</strong> and subsequent deviant resp<strong>on</strong>se patterns (Kaplan1975c, 1977a), and self-enhancing c<strong>on</strong>sequences of deviant resp<strong>on</strong>ses(1978b).SPECIAL POPULATIONSThe theory applies specifically to populati<strong>on</strong>s in which any particulardrug use/abuse pattern under c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> is regarded as deviant.It does not apply to populati<strong>on</strong>s in which the pattern is uniformlyadopted and/or approved.131


The Iowa Theory ofSubstance <strong>Abuse</strong> Am<strong>on</strong>gHyperactive AdolescentsJan L<strong>on</strong>ey, Ph.D.STATEMENT OF THEORYChildhood hyperactivity is believed to affect approximately five percentof elementary school children and to represent perhaps 50 percent ofchildren referred for evaluati<strong>on</strong> to child psychiatrists and psychologists.It is a complex c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, variously defined, and its cause and cureare unknown. Although described by several overlapping terms, someof which presume a subtle organic etiology (e.g., minimal brain dysfuncti<strong>on</strong>or MBD), diagnostic emphasis has centered up<strong>on</strong> the four As:activity (hyperkinetic reacti<strong>on</strong> of childhood), attenti<strong>on</strong> (attenti<strong>on</strong>deficit disorder), aggressi<strong>on</strong> (c<strong>on</strong>duct disorder), and/or achievement(learning disability).Hyperactive children are generally c<strong>on</strong>sidered to be at significant riskfor the development of low self-esteem, academic skill deficits, and avariety of delinquent behaviors--including substance abuse. A bodyof data c<strong>on</strong>nects childhood hyperactivity with subsequent antisocial andalcoholic diagnoses (e.g., Goodwin et al. 1975). That c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong> hasbeen shown to be familial (Cantwell 1972; Morris<strong>on</strong> and Stewart 1971)and is c<strong>on</strong>sidered by some to be genetically determined. Many experts<strong>on</strong> hyperactivity endorse what is often called the primary-sec<strong>on</strong>darytheory (Cantwell 1978; Wender 1971). According to that theory,hyperactivity and a variety of closely related symptoms, such asinattenti<strong>on</strong>, are primary or c<strong>on</strong>stituti<strong>on</strong>al features of the hyperkineticchild’s c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. In medical terms, these primary symptoms areassumed to covary across time and situati<strong>on</strong>s, and they c<strong>on</strong>stitute thecore hyperkinetic syndrome or attenti<strong>on</strong> deficit disorder. Sec<strong>on</strong>daryor resultant symptoms, such as aggressi<strong>on</strong>, are assumed to be theproduct of negative interacti<strong>on</strong>s between the hyperkinetic child and hisor her envir<strong>on</strong>ment: punitive parenting, academic failure, peer rejecti<strong>on</strong>,etc. Thus, antisocial and norm-violating behaviors such assubstance abuse are viewed as sec<strong>on</strong>dary c<strong>on</strong>sequents of severe primaryhyperkinesis. Another popular theory might be called the c<strong>on</strong>duct disordertheory (Barkley, in press; Quay 1979). Prop<strong>on</strong>ents of that132


theory stress the inseparability of hyperactivity and aggressi<strong>on</strong>, andthey maintain that hyperactive children (i.e., children with c<strong>on</strong>ductdisorders) are n<strong>on</strong>compliant, destructive, explosive, aggressive, andantisocial at all ages. Adolescent substance abuse would thus beviewed merely as an age-appropriate expressi<strong>on</strong> of the hyperactiveindividual’s lifel<strong>on</strong>g c<strong>on</strong>duct disorder.The theory of drug use developed at Iowa is derived from <strong>on</strong>goingmultivariate and multisituati<strong>on</strong>al studies of several hundred hyperactiveboys (L<strong>on</strong>ey et al., in press a; L<strong>on</strong>ey et al., in press b). Youngstersin these studies were referred for outpatient psychiatric evaluati<strong>on</strong>between four and 12 years of age. All were diagnosed as having thehyperkinetic syndrome or minimal brain dysfuncti<strong>on</strong>. Each was thentreated either pharmacologically (with a central nervous system stimulant)or psychologically (with behaviorally oriented parent and teacherc<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong>). They are being followed up as adolescents (at 12 to 18years of age) and as young adults (at 21 to 23 years of age).Hyperactive children are often lost to school-based questi<strong>on</strong>nairestudies because of reading disabilities, truancy, early school dropout,and placement in special educati<strong>on</strong> classes. Data from a presumablyvulnerable clinic populati<strong>on</strong> such as ours are therefore well suited foranswering some initial questi<strong>on</strong>s about the attitudinal and behavioralprecursors of experimentati<strong>on</strong> with substances early in the substanceabuse sequence (Kandel 1975). We have used multivariate statisticaltechniques to identify those variables from the referral and earlytreatment periods which predict variati<strong>on</strong> in adolescent behavior and toestimate their relative importance in accounting for that variati<strong>on</strong>.The results of our studies to date suggest that hyperactivity andaggressi<strong>on</strong> are essentially independent (L<strong>on</strong>ey et al. 1978). Childhoodhyperactivity is neither a precursor of adolescent aggressive andself-destructive behavior in general, nor a predictor of teenage substanceabuse in particular. In our data, the anticipated link betweenearly hyperactivity and later delinquency is missing. Although adolescentaggressi<strong>on</strong> is apparently exacerbated by negative envir<strong>on</strong>mentalevents, it does nit appear to be a sec<strong>on</strong>dary result of primary or corehyperactivity. Instead, the link is between early aggressi<strong>on</strong> and laterdelinquency; thus, childhood aggressi<strong>on</strong> is apparently primary (Werry1979). Hyperactive children are not at risk for later illegal substanceuse unless they are also aggressive; aggressive children are at riskfor later illegal substance use whether they are hyperactive or not.Thus, the outcome for any particular group of children c<strong>on</strong>sidered tohave hyperkinetic reacti<strong>on</strong>s, attenti<strong>on</strong> deficit disorders, specific learningdisabilities, or minimal brain dysfuncti<strong>on</strong> syndromes will depend <strong>on</strong>what proporti<strong>on</strong> of the group is also aggressive. And that proporti<strong>on</strong>will depend in turn <strong>on</strong> such factors as: (1) whether children withaggressive temperament and behavior (e.g., irritability, defiance,fighting, cruelty) or with diagnoses of c<strong>on</strong>duct disorder or unsocializedaggressive reacti<strong>on</strong> are included in the group because their aggressivebehaviors and diagnoses are c<strong>on</strong>sidered to be inseparable from or developmentalexpressi<strong>on</strong>s of their hyperactive syndrome; (2) whether selecti<strong>on</strong>criteria favor the inclusi<strong>on</strong> of youngsters who are both hyperactiveand aggressive (e.g., by including children who live in foster andgroup homes) or hinder their inclusi<strong>on</strong> (e.g., by excluding childrenfrom chaotic, punitive, and disadvantaged backgrounds); and (3)whether the circumstances of the study lead, de facto, to an increasedprobability that children will be sampled who are aggressive as well as133


hyperactive (e.g., studies d<strong>on</strong>e in public or tertiary facilities asopposed to private-practice settings).Diagnostic, selecti<strong>on</strong>al, and situati<strong>on</strong>al factors that explicitly or implicitlyfacilitate the inclusi<strong>on</strong> of aggressive children in a so-called “hyperactive”sample will also facilitate the c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> that hyperactive children tendto abuse drugs. Factors biased against the inclusi<strong>on</strong> of aggressivechildren will have the opposite effect. To reduce the danger of suchdiscrepant and c<strong>on</strong>fusing effects, we have suggested that children whoare hyperactive and aggressive be diagnostically and prognosticallyseparated from children who are exclusively hyperactive (Langhorneand L<strong>on</strong>ey 1979).Am<strong>on</strong>g the other predictors of behavioral outcomes am<strong>on</strong>g so-calledhyperkinetic children are the social or envir<strong>on</strong>mental correlates ofaggressi<strong>on</strong>: social class, family compositi<strong>on</strong>, parenting style, urbanresidence, etc. While individual aggressive characteristics, such asrebelliousness, determine a youngster’s susceptibility to illegal druguse, many of these socioecological antecedents of aggressi<strong>on</strong> alsoinfluence the availabIlity of Illegal substances. It is our feeling thatthe interacti<strong>on</strong> of susceptibility and availability explains why exclusivelyhyperactive (n<strong>on</strong>aggressive) youngsters do not abuse drugs. Althoughthey may be susceptible to drug use because of their immaturity andrestlessness, many are also socially awkward and rejected children towhom drugs are less available because of their isolati<strong>on</strong> from the peersettings in which much early drug use is initiated (Jessor and Jessor1977).In our theory, childhood aggressi<strong>on</strong> and childhood hyperactivity areassumed to have different antecedents and different c<strong>on</strong>sequents bothat referral and at followup. If valid, this theory also explains whytreatment with central nervous system stimulants does not lead toimproved adolescent behavior and reduced delinquency (Weiss et al.1975). Although drug treatment reduces childhood inattenti<strong>on</strong> andhyperactivity, behavior outcome and subsequent delinquency are determinedinstead by childhood aggressi<strong>on</strong> and by its ecological antecedents--which are not affected by drug treatment. Thus, drug treatment forchildhood hyperactivity is ineffective in reducing adolescent symptomatologybecause childhood hyperactivity is not the first link in a chainleading to teenage delinquency and deviant behavior (Milich and L<strong>on</strong>ey1979).A complete theory of substance abuse will ultimately describe themultivariate interacti<strong>on</strong> of enduring pers<strong>on</strong>al factors or traits, such asaggressi<strong>on</strong> (Er<strong>on</strong> 1978; Olweus 1978; Robins 1978). with situati<strong>on</strong>alfactors, such as parental modeling, peer pressure, substance availability,and treatment history. A good theory will encompass andestimate the effects of such individual factors as age, sex, and race;such geographical factors as regi<strong>on</strong> and community; and such temporalfactors as year or era--all of which have been little studied am<strong>on</strong>ghyperkinetic children. In so doing, an adequate theory will locatedrug attitudes and use within a matrix of health-threatening andnorm-violating behaviors.In additi<strong>on</strong> to the likelihood that stimulant medicati<strong>on</strong> fails to decreasethe risk of delinquency in general am<strong>on</strong>g hyperkinetic children, c<strong>on</strong>cernhas been expressed about the possibility that treatment with stimulantdrugs further increases the risk that hyperkinetic children will abusedrugs--either pharmacologically (by initiating a dependency that134


children c<strong>on</strong>tinue <strong>on</strong> their own) or psychologically (by creating apredispositi<strong>on</strong> toward chemical soluti<strong>on</strong>s for complex problems). Becauseethical and practical c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s preclude random assignment ofchildren to l<strong>on</strong>g-term treatment groups, investigators interested in thesafety and efficacy of treatment with stimulants have usually had torely <strong>on</strong> naturally occurring diagnostic and treatment groups. Therefore,the great majority of early followup studies (e.g., Laufer 1971;Mendels<strong>on</strong> et al. 1971; Minde et al. 1971; Weiss et al. 1971) werecarried out <strong>on</strong> samples of previously medicated youngsters, withoutc<strong>on</strong>trol groups or systematic comparis<strong>on</strong> data. There are some recentdata (Beck et al. 1975; Denhoff and Stern 1979; Henker et al., inpress) comparing substance use am<strong>on</strong>g previously medicated hyperactivechildren and n<strong>on</strong>hyperactive (and, of course, n<strong>on</strong>medicated) agemates.Although few group differences were found in these data, it is unclearto what degree a negative effect of hyperactivity (or of behaviorproblems in general) might have been canceled out by a positive effectof medicati<strong>on</strong>. Or perhaps a positive effect associated with hyperactivitywas canceled out by a negative effect of medicati<strong>on</strong>. Hechtman etal. (in press) found no greater substance abuse am<strong>on</strong>g essentiallyuntreated hyperactive youngsters and their n<strong>on</strong>hyperactive classmates.Blouin et al. (1978) compared treated and untreated youngsters withina hyperactive sample and found no statistically significant (p < .05)differences between the groups in their use of hard liquor, beer,wine, or marijuana.Another design has involved the comparis<strong>on</strong> of hyperactive and n<strong>on</strong>hyperactiveindividuals within larger groups of psychoeducati<strong>on</strong>al,psychiatric, or neurological referrals. Two such studies (Blouin et al.1978; Schuckit et al. 1978) have yielded numerically intriguing butstatistically insignificant differences in substance use associated withhyperactivity--even though aggressive youngsters were not specificallyexcluded. Likewise, two studies of hyperkinetic children (Cantwell1972; Morris<strong>on</strong> and Stewart 1971) that are widely cited as supportingthe link between hyperactivity and subsequent alcohol use and antisocialbehavior are difficult to interpret because childhood hyperactivity andchildhood aggressi<strong>on</strong> are not separated. More of Goodwin et al.'s(1975) alcoholic adoptees recalled being hyperactive as children thandid n<strong>on</strong>alcoholic c<strong>on</strong>trols, but they also recalled being more aggressiveand shyer. Am<strong>on</strong>g an adolescent group being treated for drug abuse,Schuckit et al. (1978) found that hyperkinetic/antisocial subjects weresignificantly more likely than n<strong>on</strong>hyperkinetic subjects to have beenwarned by a physician that drugs had damaged their health. Thatfinding is difficult to interpret, however, because more of the hyperkineticsubjects probably had c<strong>on</strong>tact with physicians, who may bepr<strong>on</strong>e to attribute symptoms am<strong>on</strong>g hyperkinetic youngsters to drugabuse (Topaz 1971) when in fact the symptoms predated the use ofillegal drugs.Am<strong>on</strong>g drug-treated hyperactive children, good treatment resp<strong>on</strong>seappears to be associated with less use of alcohol (Blouin et al. 1978)and of drugs (Kramer and L<strong>on</strong>ey 1978) at five-year followup. Muchfurther work is obviously going to be required, particularly to separateand specify the effects <strong>on</strong> adolescent drug use of: (1) behavior andlearning problems in general (by comparing hyperactive children withrandomly selected or normal children); (2) hyperkinesis per se (bycomparing hyperkinetic children with other children having behaviorand learning problems); (3) the diagnosis or label “hyperkinetic” (bycomparing diagnosed hyperkinetic children with undiagnosed hyperkineticchildren); (4) drug treatment for hyperkinesis (by comparing medicated135


hvperkinetic children with n<strong>on</strong>medicated hyperkinetic children); and(5) pharmacological resp<strong>on</strong>se to that treatment (by comparing medicatedhyperkinetic children who reso<strong>on</strong>ded well with medicated hyperkineticchildren who did not). Meanwhile, it appears that the risk of substanceuse am<strong>on</strong>g hyperactive youngsters may be neither great nor increasedby early drug treatment.SPECIAL POPULATIONSAlthough our theory is derived from examinati<strong>on</strong> of an “abnormal”sample, it can be removed from a medical c<strong>on</strong>text by c<strong>on</strong>sideringhyperactivity and aggressi<strong>on</strong> as psychological traits rather than aspsychiatric disorders. There is, in fact, c<strong>on</strong>siderable doubt thatchildhood hyperactivity is an authentic medical syndrome (Langhorne etal. 1976; Ross and Ross 1976; Sandberg et al. 1978). A medicalsyndrome should ideally have a specific etiology, a particular patternof symptoms, a predictable resp<strong>on</strong>se to treatment, and a uniformcourse and outcome. Certainly hyperactive children are noted fortheir interindividual heterogeneity and their cross-situati<strong>on</strong>al variability,and questi<strong>on</strong>s of etiology and diagnosis remain unanswered. In psychologicalterms, then, individual susceptibility to subsequent drug use isassociated with childhood aggressi<strong>on</strong>. It is not associated with childhoodhyperactivity, either directly or indirectly (through the effect ofhyperactivity <strong>on</strong> aggressi<strong>on</strong>). Such translati<strong>on</strong> from psychiatric categorizati<strong>on</strong>to psychological quantificati<strong>on</strong> places these findings regardingthe predictors of substance use am<strong>on</strong>g hyperactive children into acomparable framework as studies of predictors of substance use am<strong>on</strong>gchildren in general (J<strong>on</strong>es 1968; Lettieri 1975). However, <strong>on</strong>e genuinelyspecial populati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sists of children who have been treated withstimulant drugs. Within that populati<strong>on</strong>, two special subpopulati<strong>on</strong>sare those children whose clinical resp<strong>on</strong>se has been positive (i.e., whohave shown symptom reducti<strong>on</strong>) and those children whose clinicalresp<strong>on</strong>se has not been positive. So far, it is not clear that drugtreatment per se modifies children’s attitudes in such a way that theprobability of subsequent drug use is affected either positively ornegatively. But our theory, and the findings from which it is derived,suggest that positive drug resp<strong>on</strong>se may reduce the probability ofsubsequent drug abuse by decreasing children’s irritability, touchiness,and sullenness, and by increasing their frustrati<strong>on</strong> tolerance. If so,this would be an effect of medicati<strong>on</strong> up<strong>on</strong> the early temperamental/emoti<strong>on</strong>alaspects of aggressive behavior. Medicati<strong>on</strong> does not appear tohave direct effects <strong>on</strong> any overtly behavioral aspects of aggressi<strong>on</strong>except for substance use.136


Reinforcement and theCombinati<strong>on</strong> of EffectsSummary of a Theory ofOpiate Addicti<strong>on</strong>William E. McAuliffe, Ph.D.Robert A. Gord<strong>on</strong>, Ph.D.The theory summarized here emerged from systematic empirical researchand critical reexaminati<strong>on</strong> of prior literature c<strong>on</strong>cerning opiate addicti<strong>on</strong>(McAuliffe and Gord<strong>on</strong> 1974, 1975, 1979; McAuliffe 1975a,b, 1979;Gord<strong>on</strong> 1979). This effort has resulted in the firm establishment ofeuphoric effects as <strong>on</strong>e of the several major sources of reinforcementderiving directly from opiates even in chr<strong>on</strong>ic addicti<strong>on</strong> (McAuliffe andGord<strong>on</strong> 1974, 1975), and clarificati<strong>on</strong> of the c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s under whicheuphoric effects are available even to many first-time users of opiates(McAuliffe 1975a). Prior to these investigati<strong>on</strong>s, most social scientistsaccorded a relatively restricted role to euphoria (e.g., Lindesmith1947), and this view also found c<strong>on</strong>siderable acceptance am<strong>on</strong>g physicaland medical scientists. Euphoric effects sometimes reported or assumedin the medical literatures were often c<strong>on</strong>sidered atypical. Now, withsuch fundamental issues behind us, it is possible to use a reinforcementtheory to organize and interpret many of the more detailed empiricalphenomena of opiate abuse, where that theory has available to it forexplanatory purposes the full range of effects produced by opiatedrugs. The present digest reflects the current stage of developmentof such a theory. (For a full statement, see McAuliffe and Gord<strong>on</strong>1979.)A BRIEF OVERVIEWTHE CAUSE OF ADDICTIONAccording to our theory, opiate addicti<strong>on</strong> is caused by the extremelypotent reinforcing effects of opiate drugs. These effects c<strong>on</strong>sist ofeuphoria (including the impact effect or “rush”), reducti<strong>on</strong> of withdrawal,and miscellaneous psychotherapeutic and analgesic properties,137


which combine independently to produce a complex schedule of reinforcementfor taking opiates. Opiate use, c<strong>on</strong>sequently, is an operantlyc<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed resp<strong>on</strong>se whose tendency becomes str<strong>on</strong>ger as a functi<strong>on</strong> ofthe quality, number, and size of the reinforcements that follow it.Addicti<strong>on</strong>, in our theory, refers to the strength of the drug-takingresp<strong>on</strong>se and is thus a c<strong>on</strong>tinuous variable, rather than a qualitativelydifferent state. Addicti<strong>on</strong> begins to grow with the first reinforcedopiate-taking resp<strong>on</strong>se. When the opiate-taking resp<strong>on</strong>se has becomepowerful enough, as the result of sufficient reinforcement, the userexperiences an increased desire or “craving” for opiate effects.Craving may, however, be c<strong>on</strong>tingent up<strong>on</strong> the presence of discriminativestimuli that signal to the user that reinforcement for taking opiatesis indeed possible; for example, that he or she is not under opiateblocking by antag<strong>on</strong>ists such as nalox<strong>on</strong>e at the time. An experimentby Mirin et al. (1976, figure 3) found that addicts’ self-reportedintensity of craving rose rapidly when heroin was readily available,fell rapidly under methad<strong>on</strong>e detoxificati<strong>on</strong>, and remained low whenheroin was again made available while the subjects were <strong>on</strong> a blockingregimen receiving naltrex<strong>on</strong>e.A more meaningful definiti<strong>on</strong> of “addicti<strong>on</strong>.” In comm<strong>on</strong> parlance,pers<strong>on</strong>s are said to be “addicted” when they have become physicallydependent or at least seem unable to refrain from using a drug. Weregard these events as merely signalling that a sufficient history ofreinforcement has probably been acquired to impel a high rate of use.In the case of str<strong>on</strong>g physical dependence, the user is c<strong>on</strong>fr<strong>on</strong>ted withthe necessity of resp<strong>on</strong>ding at a minimal rate (which happens to bealso a high rate) if immediate use for whatever reas<strong>on</strong> is to c<strong>on</strong>tinue atall and if a negative reinforcer is to be successfully avoided. In ourtheory, there is no single point at which an individual suddenly becomes“addicted.” Instead, the individual’s addicti<strong>on</strong> develops insidiouslyand varies c<strong>on</strong>tinuously, so that what others seemingly mean whenthey label some<strong>on</strong>e an “addict” is merely a pers<strong>on</strong> with a str<strong>on</strong>g addicti<strong>on</strong>(i.e., a history of reinforced drug taking sufficient to outweighthe more acceptable reinforcers of life, such as are associated with<strong>on</strong>e’s job, family, friends, sex life, and respectability).Physical dependence <strong>on</strong> opiates is neither a necessary nor a sufficientc<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> for the development of addicti<strong>on</strong>. Physical dependencesimply sets the stage for experiencing withdrawal distress, reducti<strong>on</strong>of which c<strong>on</strong>stitutes <strong>on</strong>e of the drug’s powerful reinforcing effects.Other effects (principally euphoria, but including sec<strong>on</strong>dary socialgains, and relief of pain, anxiety, and fatigue) can themselves produceor c<strong>on</strong>tribute to addicti<strong>on</strong>. Most, if not all, street addicts are reinforcedin the early stages of heroin use by effects other than withdrawal,and their drug-taking resp<strong>on</strong>se at that stage must be str<strong>on</strong>genough so that it occurs every day for a few weeks in order for themto develop physical dependence. Since c<strong>on</strong>temporary opiate abusersknow about physical dependence and usually prefer to avoid it, theirdaily use prior to dependence must reflect the existence of an addicti<strong>on</strong>of some strength. We have interviewed heroin users who hadnever been dependent but who were either adamant about wanting toc<strong>on</strong>tinue heroin use despite the risks and severe social pressures orc<strong>on</strong>vinced that they could not stop even though they wanted to. Weand other researchers (Lindesmith 1947; Robins 1974a) have alsointerviewed pers<strong>on</strong>s who had used opiates compulsively <strong>on</strong> a daily basisfor many m<strong>on</strong>ths without ever interrupting l<strong>on</strong>g enough to experiencewithdrawal sickness.138


The distincti<strong>on</strong> between addicti<strong>on</strong> and physical dependence is alsoevident in detoxified addicts who are temporarily free of dependencebut who are still str<strong>on</strong>gly addicted, as witnessed by their expresseddesire for opiates and their dispositi<strong>on</strong> to relapse, and in those medicalpatients who become physiologically dependent without knowing it butwho remain indifferent because they have not developed a str<strong>on</strong>gpsychological attachment to opiates. (See Lindesmith 1947 for examples.)Our theory implies that singling out any particular point in a reinforcementhistory as the stage of “addicti<strong>on</strong>” is more or less arbitrary. Werecognize, however, that there are advantages associated with employingphysical dependence as a tacit operati<strong>on</strong>al criteri<strong>on</strong> of “addicti<strong>on</strong>.”Because the withdrawal syndrome (1) is a salient phenomen<strong>on</strong> thatusually implies a substantial history of prior reinforcement, (2) introducesa potent new reinforcer, and (3) sets a new lower bound <strong>on</strong> therate of c<strong>on</strong>tinued use, the point at which physical dependence appearsserves as a useful peg <strong>on</strong> which to hang a definiti<strong>on</strong> of “addict” thatsignals important changes in lifestyle. This highly visible point dividesopiate users into those with and without such major lifestyle changeswith great efficiency (i.e., low false-positive and false-negative rates).Indeed some addicts date their being “hooked” from the time theyrecognized major changes in their lifestyle, such as intense craving,getting fired from their job, or realizing that they preferred heroin tosex (Hendler and Stephens 1977, p. 41).C<strong>on</strong>venient though it may be, there are important disadvantages associatedwith equating addicti<strong>on</strong> with physical dependence as laymen do,or with making physical dependence a necessary but not sufficientc<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> of addicti<strong>on</strong> in a theory of opiate use (Lindesmith 1947). Byencouraging the noti<strong>on</strong> that physical dependence is necessary in orderfor addicti<strong>on</strong> to be present, <strong>on</strong>e also encourages the seriously misleadingimpressi<strong>on</strong>--according to our theory--that a user is relatively safe asl<strong>on</strong>g as physical dependence is avoided. This c<strong>on</strong>cepti<strong>on</strong> opens neophytesto the insidious features of <strong>on</strong>set underscored by the reinforcementperspective, according to which predependence use is moredangerous than seems apparent because the actual <strong>on</strong>set accrues graduallywith each reinforcement.Clearer recogniti<strong>on</strong> of withdrawal sickness as but another potent sourceof reinforcement should dispel some of the c<strong>on</strong>troversy over whether“addicti<strong>on</strong>” is defined as a physical phenomen<strong>on</strong> or as a psychologicalphenomen<strong>on</strong> and thus also clarify the related issue of whether drugsthat do not entail physical dependency are “addicting.” The distincti<strong>on</strong>between the two c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s is certainly a valuable <strong>on</strong>e, since <strong>on</strong>e addsa potent reinforcer that the other lacks, but the decisi<strong>on</strong> to regard<strong>on</strong>e or the other state as addicti<strong>on</strong> proper is, from our theoreticalstandpoint, basically arbitrary, and hence the theoretical disc<strong>on</strong>tinuitybetween the opiate and n<strong>on</strong>opiate types of chr<strong>on</strong>ic drug use no l<strong>on</strong>gerobtains.The role of psychopharmacological factors. While we grant that anindividual’s pers<strong>on</strong>ality, expectati<strong>on</strong>s, and the setting in which anopiate is used play important roles in the addicti<strong>on</strong> process, we holdthat opiates themselves have intrinsic properties that cause them to bepowerful reinforcers and therefore potently addictive. Experimentalresearch with animals dem<strong>on</strong>strates that pers<strong>on</strong>ality variables, peerpressure, poverty, or other social envir<strong>on</strong>mental factors are not essentialfor the self-administrati<strong>on</strong> of opiates (Schuster and Thomps<strong>on</strong>1969). Moreover, a review (McAuliffe 1975a, pp. 374, 382) of relevant139


esearch showed that normal human subjects in double-blind experimentsunder markedly unfavorable c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s were willing to repeat theexperience caused by their initial doses of opiate drugs, and thatreacti<strong>on</strong>s to the drug effects became increasingly favorable with repeatedadministrati<strong>on</strong>. Thus, in many normal subjects there is sufficientneutrality or favorableness to permit repetiti<strong>on</strong> of the initial dose, andfavorableness tends to snowball in the course of early repetiti<strong>on</strong>.Finally, evidence from studies by Robins and her associates (Robinsand Murphy 1967; Robins et al. 1974a) suggests that the probability ofaddicti<strong>on</strong> in the case of heroin is c<strong>on</strong>siderably greater than that associatedwith other illicit drugs. Although surveys (e.g., O’D<strong>on</strong>nell etal. 1976) show that heroin is the illicit drug least often tried byusers, they also show that the percentage of users who become str<strong>on</strong>glyaddicted and in need of treatment is greater for heroin than for any ofthe other major drugs of abuse (Siegel 1973, p. 1259; O’D<strong>on</strong>nell et al.1976, pp. 67, 79, 126).The role of individual differences. Individual differences do, however,play an important part in the addicti<strong>on</strong> process. Animal studies (Deneau1969; Davis and Nichols 1962) have found that even test animals varysubstantially in their c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ability to opiates, and researchers havebred rats (Nichols and Hsiao 1967) and mice (Erikss<strong>on</strong> and Kiianmaa1971) to produce marked differences in the animals’ willingness toself-administer opiates. Furthermore, humans also vary in the effectsopiates have <strong>on</strong> them and in the particular effects they seek fromopiates, and these variati<strong>on</strong>s appear to have profound effects <strong>on</strong>subsequent drug-related behavior. Heroin addicts, str<strong>on</strong>gly orientedtoward euphoric effects, use large amounts of the drug and evencommit crimes to pay for drugs, whereas physician addicts and iatrogenicaddicts, who typically are not interested in attaining euphoria,usually moderate their doses and rarely turn to crime to finance theirdrug c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong>. These relati<strong>on</strong>ships have led <strong>on</strong>e of us to proposethat there are two distinct forms of opiate addicti<strong>on</strong>: <strong>On</strong>e has euphoriaseeking as a focus, and the other does not (McAuliffe 1979).CONCLUSIONIt is important to stress that operant reinforcement theory is merelythe starting point for our theory of opiate addicti<strong>on</strong>, which attempts tospecify the c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s between and to c<strong>on</strong>vey the relative importanceof the various psychopharmacological and social variables that bringabout initiati<strong>on</strong>, c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong>, and terminati<strong>on</strong> of illicit use of opiates.Our theory differs most from other theories that are based mainly orentirely <strong>on</strong> the avoidance of withdrawal as their source of reinforcement(e.g., Akers 1977; Lindesmith 1947, 1975; Wikler 1965, 1973b) becauseof the major role it reserves for positive reinforcement from euphoria,and because it c<strong>on</strong>siders the overall balance of reinforcement from boththe social envir<strong>on</strong>ment and drugs in motivating abstinence. Those whoc<strong>on</strong>tinue to questi<strong>on</strong> the importance of euphoria (e.g., Akers 1977, p.101) in addicti<strong>on</strong> because it is not always present <strong>on</strong> every shot haveyet to c<strong>on</strong>fr<strong>on</strong>t the difference in criminality between euphoria-seekingaddicts and other addicts as a factor in determining social importance.Although barbiturates also cause physical dependence and severewithdrawal symptoms, and although they were also freely available inSoutheast Asia, serious morbidity from drug use am<strong>on</strong>g U.S. Armyenlisted men was c<strong>on</strong>fined to the chr<strong>on</strong>ic use of heroin, and habituati<strong>on</strong>to barbiturates was infrequent (Siegel 1973, p. 1259; Robins 1974b,140


pp. 26, 34). Clearly, there must be more involved in opiate addicti<strong>on</strong>than physical dependence. Although there is also an extensive psychiatricliterature that emphasizes self-medicating use of opiates to altermoods as a coping mechanism rather than euphoria (e.g., Duncan1977; Khantzian et al. 1974; Powell 1973; Sheppard et al. 1972; Weech1966), euphoria is often menti<strong>on</strong>ed sp<strong>on</strong>taneously in their case historiesbut not elaborated in their explanati<strong>on</strong>s (e.g., Khantzian et al. 1974).Pleasurable experiences of themselves, moreover, have psychotherapeuticvalue, so that self-medicati<strong>on</strong> need not exclude euphoria evenwhen self-medicati<strong>on</strong> does motivate drug use.As we see it, the more distinguishing features of our theory are itsemphasis <strong>on</strong> the intrinsic reinforcement properties of opiates, especiallyeuphoria; the theory’s c<strong>on</strong>cepti<strong>on</strong> of addicti<strong>on</strong> as a c<strong>on</strong>tinuous variableand an insidious process; its attenti<strong>on</strong> to and identificati<strong>on</strong> of therelevant c<strong>on</strong>tingencies and schedules of reinforcement peculiar toopiates and actually governing the behavior of human addicts at variousstages of their careers; and its flexibility in being able to distinguishand accommodate the existence of several different types of addict(weekenders, hardcore addicts, euphoria seekers, and medical addicts).No mere translati<strong>on</strong> of operant c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing theory could accomplishthese various ends.141


Addicti<strong>on</strong> to an ExperienceA Social-Psychological-PharmacologicalTheory of Addicti<strong>on</strong>Stant<strong>on</strong> Peele, Ph.D.OVERVIEWA theory of addicti<strong>on</strong> must be able to explain the following phenomena:(1) the range of substances which are able to fulfill all the criteria foraddictiveness, (2) the variability in the addictiveness of differentdrugs (a) in different cultures and (b) for different individuals in thesame culture, (3) the impact that groups and other social factors have<strong>on</strong> both the addictive use of a drug and withdrawal from it, and(4) variati<strong>on</strong>s in the individual life cycle which influence the individual’slikelihood of being addicted. A theory that accomplishes this will needto take into account all the levels of variables that play a role inhuman functi<strong>on</strong>ing, including biological variables, pers<strong>on</strong>ality, physicaland social envir<strong>on</strong>ment, and cultural and political variables. The keyc<strong>on</strong>cepts for enabling us to c<strong>on</strong>ceptualize all of these variables andtheir interacti<strong>on</strong>s are the experience that an individual derives from adrug and the way in which this experience fits into the entirety of hisor her life.A drug’s chemical structure does not predict the addictive effect thedrug will have <strong>on</strong> an individual. Hence the impossibility of definingaddicti<strong>on</strong> pharmacologically, as a property of a drug (Jaffe 1970a). Wehave now seen that not all people become addicted to narcotics, evenwhen these drugs are administered regularly and in heavy dosages.<strong>On</strong> the other hand, people form addicti<strong>on</strong>s to a range of n<strong>on</strong>narcoticsubstances--from barbiturates, synthetic narcotics, and alcohol t<strong>on</strong>icotine, caffeine, and sedatives. The addictive resp<strong>on</strong>se begins withthe characteristic effect of a drug and is modified by the individual’sreacti<strong>on</strong> to that substance as well as his or her general outlook. Inadditi<strong>on</strong>, setting, groups, and cultural attitudes influence the experiencethe user has with the drug and his or her need for that experience.THE ANALGESIC EXPERIENCEPharmacologists have l<strong>on</strong>g sought to develop a drug that reproducesthe analgesic effects of the narcotics without being addictive. This142


pursuit of the “n<strong>on</strong>addictive analgesic” is based <strong>on</strong> the misunderstandingthat <strong>on</strong>ly a specific molecular structure interacts with the nervoussystem to produce addicti<strong>on</strong> (Peele 1977). Starting with heroin, whichwas developed to replace morphine, the search for a n<strong>on</strong>addictiveanalgesic has uncovered a host of new addictive substances, includingthe barbiturates, the synthetic narcotics such as Demerol and methad<strong>on</strong>e,and the n<strong>on</strong>barbiturate sedatives (Kales et al. 1974).What is evident from this research is that any drug which serves ananalgesic functi<strong>on</strong> can be used addictively. It is, in fact, the experienceof having pain relieved to which the individual becomes addicted.This can be described through reference to the addicti<strong>on</strong> cycle.Pers<strong>on</strong>s who are faced with persistent difficulties and anxieties in theirlives and who are not prepared to cope with them realistically resort toanalgesic drugs for comfort. While enabling them to forget theirproblems and stress, the pain-killing experience engendered by thedrugs actually decreases the ability to cope. This is because suchdrugs depress the central nervous system and the individual’s resp<strong>on</strong>sivecapability. Al<strong>on</strong>g with this, people do not focus <strong>on</strong> their problemswhile intoxicated with a drug, and so the sources of the stress thatled them to take the drug are likely to worsen as a result of havingbeen ignored.Not every<strong>on</strong>e resp<strong>on</strong>ds to the analgesic experience in the same way.Some people find a narcosis tremendously alluring, while others reportthat the sensati<strong>on</strong>s of helplessness are disturbing and distinctly unappealing.Pers<strong>on</strong>s who welcome this experience do not feel able to cometo grips with their problems. They are thus susceptible to the temporaryprotective cloak provided by the drug and are not c<strong>on</strong>cerned forthat time with the reducti<strong>on</strong> in coping capacity that they suffer.It is important to note that the objective stress that a pers<strong>on</strong> facesand his or her reacti<strong>on</strong> to the situati<strong>on</strong> are not the same thing.Settings with which some people cope readily may be overwhelming toothers. Even people in apparently favorable surroundings may findthem intolerable. Self-efficacy and self-esteem are crucial ingredientsin the pers<strong>on</strong>’s makeup that explain these discrepancies. Self-esteemand guilt are also essential to the addicti<strong>on</strong> cycle. Part of the driveto seek the analgesic effect of a drug comes from the drug’s suppressi<strong>on</strong>of the anxiety a pers<strong>on</strong> feels; being intoxicated by this experience,however, exacerbates the pers<strong>on</strong>’s guilt and disrespect for himself orherself, which are str<strong>on</strong>g parts of the motivati<strong>on</strong> to seek intoxicati<strong>on</strong>in the first place.Withdrawal appears in the addicti<strong>on</strong> cycle when the cycle progresses tothe point where the analgesic experience is the major and, indeed, solesource of gratificati<strong>on</strong> for a pers<strong>on</strong>. All other rewards are mediatedby the effects of the drug. To remove the drug from a pers<strong>on</strong>’ssystem is to remove a necessary means of functi<strong>on</strong>ing and, bey<strong>on</strong>dthis, the desire to endure the demands his or her system now c<strong>on</strong>fr<strong>on</strong>ts.Adverse withdrawal symptoms begin with the fact that all drugs havinga measurable impact <strong>on</strong> the human organism will also produce a reverseeffect when removed, since the body must now compensate for theacti<strong>on</strong> of the drug <strong>on</strong> which it has depended. How the individualreacts to this disorientati<strong>on</strong>--and, in particular, how severe the disorientati<strong>on</strong>is--depends <strong>on</strong> the same factors which determined the initialreacti<strong>on</strong> to the drug.143


Let US c<strong>on</strong>sider why hospital patients receiving regular dosages of anarcotic at higher-than-street-level c<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong>s rarely report notinga withdrawal resp<strong>on</strong>se when they return home and cease their use ofthe drug (Zinberg 1974a). As l<strong>on</strong>g as individuals feel they can dealwith their lives, do not think of themselves as addicts, and reenter anenvir<strong>on</strong>ment which does not acknowledge withdrawal and providesstr<strong>on</strong>g alternate gratificati<strong>on</strong>s, they will not experience debilitatingwithdrawal. In the case of the hospital patient, we see that a settingmay temporarily produce a level of discomfort which is comparable tothat which the addicted drug user experiences regularly. Like theaddicted user, the patient may rely <strong>on</strong> drugs in the hospital. Whenthe patient leaves the hospital and the discomfort behind, however,and reengages in meaningful activities, the drug experience loses itsusefulness.The field research which illuminates most clearly the role of setting inaddicti<strong>on</strong> is that surrounding the Vietnam soldier. In Vietnam, facingstress, discomfort, danger, lack of social support, and the absence ofopportunities for c<strong>on</strong>structive effort, many men resorted to narcoticuse. More drastically, of those men who were found to be using anarcotic in Vietnam, 75 percent reported they were addicted in thatsetting. A followup study found that <strong>on</strong>e-third of the drug usersc<strong>on</strong>tinued to use a narcotic when back in the United States. Yet theresearchers found that <strong>on</strong>ly nine percent of the Vietnam addictedgroup showed signs of addicti<strong>on</strong> at home (Robins et al. 1974a). Thesedata show how setting determines whether drug use will be addictiveor not even when amount and type of drug use remain c<strong>on</strong>stant. Forin Vietnam, circumstances modified the appeal of the analgesic experiencefor the individual and the need he had for that experience.COMPLEXITIES IN ADDICTIONUtilizing the experience produced by a drug as the central element inthe definiti<strong>on</strong> of addicti<strong>on</strong> does not obviate the role of a drug’s pharmacologicaleffects. Powerful psychoactive drugs are obviously thesubstances which are most directly capable of producing the experienceto which an individual may become addicted (although they are not the<strong>on</strong>ly causes of such experiences). The nature of a drug’s effects is adeterminant of the type of experience a user will have, and users mayhave genuine preferences for different classes of drugs depending <strong>on</strong>the functi<strong>on</strong> they seek a drug experience to provide. Thus, whiledepressant drugs (those which create analgesic effects), such as thebarbiturates, the narcotics, and alcohol, are major objects for drugaddicti<strong>on</strong>, stimulant drugs are another class of drugs with addictivepotential. For example, laboratory research now indicates that it isnot possible to distinguish qualitatively between the withdrawal producedby stimulant drugs, such as caffeine, and narcotic withdrawal(Goldstein and Kaiser 1969). The mechanism of addicti<strong>on</strong> in the caseof the stimulant experience is the absorpti<strong>on</strong> of the user’s attenti<strong>on</strong> bythe arousal state the drug leads to. This internal stimulati<strong>on</strong>, itseems, makes the drug user less aware of the external stimuli whichcreate tensi<strong>on</strong>. Cigarette smokers have been shown to be more tensethan n<strong>on</strong>smokers but to experience a reducti<strong>on</strong> in tensi<strong>on</strong> from smokingthat n<strong>on</strong>smokers do not report (Nesbitt 1972). In this paradoxicalway, a stimulant can create an analgesic effect for certain individuals.144


In smoking, as in all addicti<strong>on</strong>, the experience c<strong>on</strong>sists of elements inadditi<strong>on</strong> to the drug’s effects. The chief of these is the ritual associatedwith the drug use. A substantial porti<strong>on</strong> of heroin addicts willhave their withdrawal suppressed simply by undergoing the ritual ofinjecti<strong>on</strong>, without receiving any of the drug (Light and Torrance1929). Similarly, cigarette smokers will not resp<strong>on</strong>d totally to nicotinewhich is not taken in through inhalati<strong>on</strong>, even if the alternate methodfor c<strong>on</strong>suming the drug is more efficient (Jarvik 1973). We can understandthese phenomena when we note that with both stimulants anddepressants, it is primarily the overall reassurance of the drug experienceto which the addict is resp<strong>on</strong>ding. Predictable and habitualaspects of the setting in which the drug is c<strong>on</strong>sumed will be as much apart of the addicti<strong>on</strong> as the substance itself.Addicti<strong>on</strong> to a given drug is not c<strong>on</strong>stant from culture to culture. Forexample, debilitating alcoholism is almost unknown in certain ruralMediterranean societies (Blum and Blum 1969). The evidence is that aculture’s attitudes toward a drug influence whether or not the drugwill be abused. In particular, societies which have high alcoholismrates are those in which a premium is placed <strong>on</strong> power but in which itis difficult for <strong>on</strong>e to achieve power. In this cultural c<strong>on</strong>text, alcoholintoxicati<strong>on</strong> leads to fantasies of pers<strong>on</strong>al dominati<strong>on</strong> over other people(McClelland et al. 1972). Behaviors which occur in <strong>line</strong> with thisdrinking are fighting, crime, reckless driving, and other aggressiveand antisocial acts. Compare this to the kind of drinking which occursin a Greek cafe, where the disinhibiti<strong>on</strong> that alcohol produces is usedto enhance social c<strong>on</strong>viviality. Not <strong>on</strong>ly does the social meaning ofalcohol change, but the very processes of thought and feeling which itsets off in the individual can be seen to vary.Placing the power-oriented drinking syndrome in the addicti<strong>on</strong> cycle,we find that individuals who doubt their efficacy drink in order togain the illusi<strong>on</strong> of power. Attempts to dominate others while drunk,however, actually lower social standing and c<strong>on</strong>tribute to a sense offutility and low self-esteem. Drinking may become the <strong>on</strong>e avenue to asatisfactory--if temporary--self-image, and drunkenness becomes apreferred state. In a culture where intoxicati<strong>on</strong> does not producethese feelings and is not taken as an excuse for antisocial behavior,the drinking experience is not <strong>on</strong>e which can serve as the object of anaddicti<strong>on</strong>.SPECIAL POPULATlONSDoctors as a group have often been singled out for their high incidenceof narcotic and other drug use. While many physicians do sufferdebilitating effects from their involvement with a drug, there are alsoindicati<strong>on</strong>s that many physicians use narcotics for l<strong>on</strong>g periods of timewithout showing such negative effects (Winick 1961a). There areseveral factors which might make it less likely for narcotic use am<strong>on</strong>gdoctors to reach an unc<strong>on</strong>trolled stage. These include the status oftheir positi<strong>on</strong>, the meaningfulness of their work, the self-c<strong>on</strong>trolrequired in their training and certificati<strong>on</strong>, and so <strong>on</strong>. Medical doctors,therefore, have come to provide some of the best examples of c<strong>on</strong>trolleduse of narcotics.Recent research has modified this picture in important ways. Whiledoctors obviously have advantages in hiding--and even c<strong>on</strong>trolling--145


their drug involvements, it is now clear that such c<strong>on</strong>trolled use is farfrom excepti<strong>on</strong>al. Investigati<strong>on</strong>s am<strong>on</strong>g both middle-class users andghetto residents using narcotics indicate that the percentage of c<strong>on</strong>trolledusers is high and that these populati<strong>on</strong>s do not differ significantlyfrom medical doctors in this respect (Lukoff and Brook 1974).This special populati<strong>on</strong> that has been uncovered is not defined byoccupati<strong>on</strong>, by ec<strong>on</strong>omic or social status, or by other demographicfactors. It is the group of people who are able to subjugate theirdrug use to other aspects of a productive life. The factors that havebeen shown to enable a pers<strong>on</strong> to do this include a sense of purposeor missi<strong>on</strong> that dictates times when drug use is not appropriate, setsof friends who are not involved in use of the drug, and models forc<strong>on</strong>trolled use either am<strong>on</strong>g peers, status figures, or family (Jacobs<strong>on</strong>and Zinberg 1975).146


A Family Theory of<strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>M. Duncan Stant<strong>on</strong>, Ph.D.THEORETICAL CONSIDERATIONSIn developing a theory of drug abuse, my colleagues 1 and I were facedwith explaining several phenomena in the behavior of drug abuserswhich were not accounted for by existent theories. <strong>On</strong>e of these isthe repetitive, recurrent nature of addicti<strong>on</strong>; related to this is thehigh incidence of treatment dropouts. We were also dissatisfied withthe static theories which predominated in the field--theories which tooklittle or no cognizance of (a) the <strong>on</strong>going behavior in its c<strong>on</strong>text,(b) changes and/or repetitive patterns which occurred during a giventime period, and (c) the interpers<strong>on</strong>al and c<strong>on</strong>textual functi<strong>on</strong>s ofdrug abuse (Stant<strong>on</strong> 1978b). Before proceeding to discussi<strong>on</strong> of atheoretical model, however, there are several c<strong>on</strong>ceptual c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s,stemming from these observati<strong>on</strong>s, which need further elucidati<strong>on</strong>.SYMPTOM CONTEXTA major c<strong>on</strong>cern which, again, has too often been overlooked in thedrug abuse field pertains to the c<strong>on</strong>text of the symptom as this relatesto its genesis and its maintenance. There is a need for viable theoreticalmodels which take into account both the actual symptomatic behaviorand the behavior of others within the symptom-bearer’s interpers<strong>on</strong>alsystem. Symptoms generally do not just “pop up.” They occur withina c<strong>on</strong>text, and most would agree that they serve functi<strong>on</strong>s within thisc<strong>on</strong>text--both for the symptom-bearer and for the other people involved.1 Many of the ideas presented here were developed throuqh a collaborative.effort with a number of colleagues, including Thomas C. Todd,Ph.D.: David B. Heard, Ph.D.: Sam Kirschner, Ph.D.; Jerry I.Kleiman, Ph.D.; David T. Mowatt, Ed. D.; Paul Riley; Samuel M. Scott;and John M. VanDeusen, M.A.C. Jay Haley, M.A., also providedimportant input. A major result of this collaborati<strong>on</strong> has been the c<strong>on</strong>ceptualpaper by Stant<strong>on</strong> et al. (1978).147


In fact, some of these others (e.g., family members) may actually havean investment in maintaining the symptom. C<strong>on</strong>sequently, our formulati<strong>on</strong>sneed to encompass the total “gestalt” of (a) the symptom, (b) thetreatment, (c) those affected by the treatment, and (d) the effectsthese last also have back <strong>on</strong> the treatment endeavor. This is, then, acyclical process, involving numerous homeostatic and feedback mechanisms.<strong>On</strong> this point, Nathan and Lansky (1978), in a recent review ofthe problems in research <strong>on</strong> the addicti<strong>on</strong>s, have stated, “A frequentlyignored issue . . . is that a treatment program may be highly effectivein attaining desired goals while patients are actively involved in theprogram, <strong>on</strong>ly to appear to fail when patients return to n<strong>on</strong>supportiveor destructive envir<strong>on</strong>ments” (p. 82). It is inclusi<strong>on</strong> of these “n<strong>on</strong>supportive”and “destructive” influences which is being stressed here.Treatment does not take place in a vacuum, and if the external variableswhich impinge before, during, and after treatment are not changed, orat least evaluated, both treatment and investigatory efforts operate ata c<strong>on</strong>siderable disadvantage.NONLINEAR CAUSALITYIn some ways we are addressing the issue of causality here. Muchresearch in the drug abuse field has not enjoyed the luxury of havingcomprehensive causal models to give directi<strong>on</strong> to its efforts. Animportant issue surrounding the problem of causality pertains to its<strong>line</strong>ar versus its n<strong>on</strong><strong>line</strong>ar nature. For instance, if <strong>on</strong>e were to regardcausality from a <strong>line</strong>ar standpoint, <strong>on</strong>e would assume that A causes B,or that A and B cause C. A n<strong>on</strong><strong>line</strong>ar, or open systems model, <strong>on</strong> theother hand, would more likely portray the process as a sequence: Aleads to B, B leads to C, and C leads back to A. The behaviors ofthe involved individuals or human systems are sequential and cyclical.We would thus want to look at the comp<strong>on</strong>ents, elements, and specificbehaviors which c<strong>on</strong>stitute the cycle. The addicti<strong>on</strong>/readdicti<strong>on</strong> patternis an example of just such a process. N<strong>on</strong><strong>line</strong>ar causality, whilerequiring a different approach to the ways in which we think aboutsymptoms such as drug abuse, holds c<strong>on</strong>siderable potential for explainingthe addicti<strong>on</strong> process. However, from an operati<strong>on</strong>al standpoint, italso requires a revisi<strong>on</strong> of many of the dependent and independentvariables to be examined.FAMILY LIFE CYCLEIt is helpful to view any family in terms of its place in the familydevelopmental life cycle. Most families encounter a number of similarstages as they progress through life, such as birth of first child,child first attending school, children leaving home, death of a parentor spouse, etc. These are crisis points, which, although sometimesdifficult to get through, are usually weathered without inordinatedifficulty. <strong>On</strong> the other hand, symptomatic families develop problemsbecause they are not able to adjust to the transiti<strong>on</strong>. They become“stuck” at a particular point or stage. Like a broken record, theyrepetitively go through the process without advancing bey<strong>on</strong>d it (Haley1973). This process as it applies to drug users will be discussedbelow.148


DRUG ABUSE AS A FAMILY PHENOMENONWhile the emphasis here will be <strong>on</strong> opiate users under the age of 35, itis my experience and that of my colleagues that most of the patternsand processes described apply to people and families who indulge inheavy, compulsive use of other drugs as well. A number of featureswill be presented, leading to a family homeostatic model of addicti<strong>on</strong>.<strong>On</strong>ly certain of the pertinent references will be cited, and the readeris referred to Stant<strong>on</strong> (1978a, 1979b, 1980) and Stant<strong>on</strong> et al. (1978)for more complete documentati<strong>on</strong>.TRAUMATIC LOSSAccumulating data indicate that a high percentage of drug abusers’families have experienced premature loss or separati<strong>on</strong> during thefamily’s life cycle. The relati<strong>on</strong>ship between drug addicti<strong>on</strong> and(a) immigrati<strong>on</strong> or (b) parent-child cultural disparity appears to beimportant. Alexander and Dibb (1975) and Vaillant (1966b) discoveredthat the rate of addicti<strong>on</strong> for offspring of people who immigrated eitherfrom another country or from a different secti<strong>on</strong> of the United Stateswas c<strong>on</strong>siderably higher (three times so for Vaillant’s sample) than therate for the immigrants themselves. In additi<strong>on</strong>, Vaillant found thatoffspring of immigrants who were born in New York City were atgreater risk for addicti<strong>on</strong> than either their parents or offspring bornin the former culture. Noting the abnormal dependence of addictmothers <strong>on</strong> their children, he suggested that (a) immigrant parents areunder the additi<strong>on</strong>al strain of having to cope with their new envir<strong>on</strong>ment,(b) parental migrati<strong>on</strong> may be correlated with parental instability, and(c) “the immigrant mother, separated as she often is from her ownfamily ties, may be less able to meet the needs of those dependent <strong>on</strong>her and yet experience greater than average difficulty in permittingher child mature independence” (p. 538). It might be added thatimmigrant parents are also faced both with the “loss” of the familythey left in their original culture and their own possible feelings ofguilt or disloyalty for having deserted these other members. In anycase, what appears to happen is that many immigrant parents tend todepend <strong>on</strong> their children for emoti<strong>on</strong>al and other kinds of support,clinging to them and becoming terrified when the offspring reachadolescence and start to individuate.With n<strong>on</strong>-immigrant families of drug abusers, a high proporti<strong>on</strong> showtraumatic, untimely, or unexpected loss of a family member, experiencingmore such early deaths or tragic losses than would be actuariallyexpected (Coleman and Stant<strong>on</strong> 1978). This has led to the idea thatthe high rate of death, suicide, and self-destructi<strong>on</strong> am<strong>on</strong>g addicts isactually a family phenomen<strong>on</strong> in which the addict’s role is to die, or tocome close to death, as part of the family’s attempt to work throughthe trauma of the loss; in a sense, addicts are sacrificial and rathernoble figures who martyr themselves for the sake of their families(Reilly 1976; Stant<strong>on</strong> 1977b; Stant<strong>on</strong> and Coleman 1979).FEAR OF SEPARATIONRelated to this discussi<strong>on</strong> is the intense fear of separati<strong>on</strong> that thesefamilies show (Stant<strong>on</strong> et al. 1978). For instance, addicts do notfuncti<strong>on</strong> well because they are too dependent and not ready to assume149


esp<strong>on</strong>sibility--as if they want to be taken care of. They fear beingseparate or separated. However, closer observati<strong>on</strong> of the wholefamily generally reveals that when addicts begin to succeed--whether<strong>on</strong> the job, in a treatment program, or elsewhere--they are, in asense, heading toward leaving the family, either directly or by developingmore aut<strong>on</strong>omy in general. At this point, some sort of crisisalmost inevitably occurs in the family. <strong>On</strong> the heels of this the addictreverts to some kind of failure behavior and the family problem dissipates.The implicati<strong>on</strong> is that not <strong>on</strong>ly does the addict fear separati<strong>on</strong>from the family, but that the reverse is also true. It is an interdependentprocess in which failure serves a protective functi<strong>on</strong> of maintainingfamily closeness. The family’s need for the addict is greater than orequal to the addict’s need for them, and they cling to each other forc<strong>on</strong>firmati<strong>on</strong> or, perhaps, a sense of “completeness” or “worth.”ADDICT-FAMILY CONTEXTSome corroborati<strong>on</strong> of the noti<strong>on</strong> that addicts are tied into their familiesof origin can be obtained simply by observing how often they c<strong>on</strong>tacttheir parent(s). This is a facet of the drug abuser’s lifestyle whichhas generally been overlooked, since it is not obvious that addicts intheir late twenties and early thirties would still be so involved; theirage. submersi<strong>on</strong> in the drug subculture, frequent changes in residence,possible military service, etc., all seem to imply that they are cut off,or at least distanced, from <strong>on</strong>e or both parents. However, despiteprotestati<strong>on</strong>s of independence, there is increasing evidence that mostaddicts maintain close family ties. Stant<strong>on</strong> (1980) has accumulated 14sources which deal with this idea, and all but <strong>on</strong>e (a poorly designedstudy, it should be noted) support the close-c<strong>on</strong>tact hypothesis. Forinstance, our own data (Stant<strong>on</strong> et al. 1978) from an an<strong>on</strong>ymous surveyof 85 heroin addicts (average age, 28) showed that 66 percent eitherresided with their parents or saw their mothers daily, while 82 percentsaw at least <strong>on</strong>e parent weekly. Further, similar patterns have emergedin Italy and Thailand, where 80 percent of addicts live with theirparents. More recently, Mintz 2 is gathering data in Los Angeles whichappear, at this point, to duplicate the above results, and Perzel andLam<strong>on</strong> (1979) have identified a similar pattern with polydrug abusers,also finding that the frequency of family-of-origin c<strong>on</strong>tact for theabusers was five times that reported for a comparis<strong>on</strong> group of n<strong>on</strong>drugusers. In sum, the accumulating evidence has tended to yield datac<strong>on</strong>sistent with a close addict-family tie hypothesis.FAMILY STRUCTUREThe studies supporting the c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>s in this secti<strong>on</strong> are too numerousto cite here, and the reader is referred to reviews by the author(Stant<strong>on</strong> 1979b,c, 1980) for further documentati<strong>on</strong>. The prototypicdrug abuser’s family--as described in most of the literature--is <strong>on</strong>e inwhich <strong>on</strong>e parent is intensely involved with the abuser, while theother is more punitive, distant, and/or absent. Usually the overinvolvedindulgent, overprotective parent is of the opposite sex from2 J. Mintz, University of California, Los Angeles, and Brentwood VA Hospital.Pers<strong>on</strong>al communicati<strong>on</strong>, August 1979.150


the abuser. This overinvolvement may even reach the point of incest,especially with female abusers. Further, the abusing offspring mayserve a functi<strong>on</strong> for the parents, either as a channel for their communicati<strong>on</strong>,or as a disrupter whose distracting behavior keeps their ownfights from crystallizing. C<strong>on</strong>versely, the abuser may seek a “sick”state in order to assume a childlike positi<strong>on</strong> as the focus of the parents’attenti<strong>on</strong>. C<strong>on</strong>sequently, the <strong>on</strong>set of adolescence, with its threat oflosing the adolescent to outsiders, heralds parental panic. The familythen becomes stuck at this developmental stage and a chr<strong>on</strong>ic, repetitiveprocess sets in, centered <strong>on</strong> the individuati<strong>on</strong>, growing up, and leavingof the drug abuser.It is probably most helpful to view the above process as at least atriadic interacti<strong>on</strong>, involving two adults (usually parents) and theabuser. If the drug-using youth is male, the mother may lavish heraffecti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> him because she is not getting enough from her husband,while the husband retreats because his wife undercuts him--as, forexample, when he tries to discip<strong>line</strong> the s<strong>on</strong> appropriately. This kindof thinking is much more attuned to the system, and <strong>on</strong>ly a few studiesand papers have subscribed to it. In additi<strong>on</strong>, it appears that mostfamily members help to keep the drug abuser in a dependent, incompetentrole, the family thus serving to undermine his or her self-esteem.By staying in role and taking drugs, the abuser helps to maintainfamily stability and homeostasis.COMPARISON WITH OTHERSYMPTOMS OR DISORDERSSince a number of disorders, in additi<strong>on</strong> to drug abuse, show a patternof overinvolvement by <strong>on</strong>e parent and distance/absence by the other,the questi<strong>on</strong> arises as to how drug abusers’ families differ from otherdysfuncti<strong>on</strong>al families. Stant<strong>on</strong> et al. (1978) have tried to clarify thisissue, drawing both from the literature and from their own studies.In brief, the cluster of distinguishing factors for addict families appearsto include the following: (a) There is a higher frequency of multigenerati<strong>on</strong>alchemical dependency--particularly alcohol am<strong>on</strong>g males--plus apropensity for other addicti<strong>on</strong>-like behaviors such as gambling andwatching televisi<strong>on</strong>. (Such practices provide modeling for children andalso can develop into family “traditi<strong>on</strong>s.”) (b) There appears to bemore primitive and direct expressi<strong>on</strong> of c<strong>on</strong>flict, with quite explicit(versus covert) alliances, for example, between addict and overinvolvedparent. (c) Addict parents’ behavior is characterized as “c<strong>on</strong>spicuouslyunschizophrenic” in quality. (d) Addicts may have a peer group orsubculture to which they (briefly) retreat following family c<strong>on</strong>flict--theillusi<strong>on</strong> of independence is greater. (e) Mothers of addicts display“symbiotic” childrearing practices further into the life of the child andshow greater symbiotic needs, than mothers of schizophrenics andnormals. (f) Again, there is a prep<strong>on</strong>derance of death themes andpremature, unexpected, or untimely deaths within the family. (g) Thesymptom of addicti<strong>on</strong> provides a form of “pseudo-individuati<strong>on</strong>” atseveral levels, extending from the individual-pharmacological level tothat of the drug subculture. (See discussi<strong>on</strong> that follows.) (h) Theaforementi<strong>on</strong>ed rate of addicti<strong>on</strong> am<strong>on</strong>g offspring of immigrants isgreater than might be expected, suggesting the importance of acculturati<strong>on</strong>and parent-child cultural disparity in addicti<strong>on</strong>.151


SYMPTOM FUNCTIONIt is legitimate to ask what functi<strong>on</strong>s the symptom of drug abuse mightserve within an interpers<strong>on</strong>al or family system. Stemming from earlierdiscussi<strong>on</strong> of the interdependency and fear of separati<strong>on</strong> that addictfamilies show, drug addicti<strong>on</strong>, especially to heroin, does indeed appearto have many adaptive, functi<strong>on</strong>al qualities in additi<strong>on</strong> to its pleasurablefeatures. The major c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> is that it provides addicts and theirfamilies with a paradoxical resoluti<strong>on</strong> to their dilemma of maintaining ordissolving the family. The drug’s pharmacological effects and thec<strong>on</strong>text and implicati<strong>on</strong>s of its use furnish soluti<strong>on</strong>s to this dilemma atseveral different levels, from individual psychopharmacology to thedrug subculture. These functi<strong>on</strong>s are described below, and, again,rather than listing the various studies up<strong>on</strong> which they are based,refer to the original review by Stant<strong>on</strong> et al. (1978).The Individual-Pharmacological LevelSeveral writers have c<strong>on</strong>ceptualized the addict’s experience of euphoriaas analogous to a symbiotic attachment or fusi<strong>on</strong> with the mother--akind of regressed, infantile satiati<strong>on</strong>. If so, while in this state theaddict can feel “close” to mother or family, and also in some waysappear to them much as a child who is clearly not aut<strong>on</strong>omous. <strong>On</strong> theother hand, heroin blunts the anxiety accompanying separati<strong>on</strong> andindividuati<strong>on</strong>, often causes drowsiness, and in effect allows the addictto be separate, distanced, and self-absorbed while physically present.The drug allows both closeness, or infantile behavior, and distance atthe same time.Aggressive BehaviorWhen an addict succeeds or improves, we have noted that family turmoiloften ensues. The family seems to be covertly urging the addict toremain incompetent and dependent. Heroin, <strong>on</strong> the other hand, hasbeen noted to give a sense of new power, omnipotence, and “triumphantsuccess.” Perhaps more important is the point made by Ganger andShugart (1966), however, that under the influence of heroin, addictsbecome aggressive and assertive toward their families, particularlytheir parents. In so doing they become aut<strong>on</strong>omous, individuated, and“free.” They appear to stand up for themselves, but do not really.This is actually pseudo-individuati<strong>on</strong>, for addicts’ ravings and protestati<strong>on</strong>sare typically discounted by the family. The drug is blamed.Without it they “really aren’t that way.” Through the drug cycle thewhole family becomes engaged in a repetitive reenactment of leavingand returning in which the “leaving” phase is neutralized throughdenial of the possible implicati<strong>on</strong>s of the addict’s assertiveness. Inshort, the family is saying, “You d<strong>on</strong>’t really hate us--you’re justhigh,” and when not influenced by drugs, the addict c<strong>on</strong>curs with,“Yes, I d<strong>on</strong>’t really hate you, but when I’m <strong>on</strong> the drug I can’t c<strong>on</strong>trolmyself.”Heterosexual Relati<strong>on</strong>shipsHeroin may offer a compromise in the area of heterosexual relati<strong>on</strong>ships.Addicts have been noted not to have teenage crushes, to be morelikely than average to engage in homosexual activities, or to be retreatingfrom sexuality. Intense family ties can serve to prevent theaddict from developing appropriate relati<strong>on</strong>ships with spouses or offspring.It may be true that the drug produces a kind of sexual152


experience, which would partially explain the colorfully eroticizedlanguage and loving tenderness that addicts attach to various aspectsof their habit; they seem to be addressing it as a love partner. Sinceit apparently reduces the sex drive also, it can in this way againprovide a soluti<strong>on</strong> to the addict’s dilemma. Through it they can havequasi-sexual experiences without being disloyal to their families, particularlytheir mothers. They do not have to form heterosexual relati<strong>on</strong>shipsbut instead can relate sexually to the drug.The <strong>Drug</strong> SubcultureOther aspects of heroin addicti<strong>on</strong> can help addicts out of their dilemmas,especially those pertaining to extrafamilial systems. Addicts formrelati<strong>on</strong>ships am<strong>on</strong>g members of the drug subculture. They “hustle”and make a lot of m<strong>on</strong>ey to support their habit. Thus they havefriends or peers and are in this way grownup, independent, and“successful.” Paradoxically, however, this is not the case, for themore heroin they shoot, the more helpless, dependent, and incompetentthey are. In other words, they can be successful and competent <strong>on</strong>lywithin the framework of an unsuccessful, incompetent subculture. Itis a limited realm, restricted to people who need help and cannot reallybe expected to functi<strong>on</strong> adequately within society.Abstinence and the Addict RolePreviously, it was noted how the drug may serve as a problem whichkeeps the family together. In this way it transcends its pharmacologicaleffect; it serves more as a symbol of the addict’s incompetence andc<strong>on</strong>sequent inability to leave the family, or the family’s inability torelease the addict. Much has been made of the euphoria in drug addicti<strong>on</strong>,but our experience indicates that this is sec<strong>on</strong>dary to its functi<strong>on</strong>within the family. Given appropriate support, the addict can, forexample, tolerate large decreases in methad<strong>on</strong>e levels. By far thegreatest resistance is in the final step of going from five mg to zero.It is an easy step to take, pharmacologically, and its real significanceis symbolic. <strong>On</strong>ce this step is taken, the addict is no l<strong>on</strong>ger an addictand is making an asserti<strong>on</strong> against the roles played and against themantle of incompetence. Should the family still need some<strong>on</strong>e in thepositi<strong>on</strong> of the addicted <strong>on</strong>e, they can bring almost unbearable pressureto bear--so much so that it may cause the addict to slip <strong>on</strong>ce againinto the addictive cycle.A HOMEOSTATIC MODELThe model presented here is of the n<strong>on</strong><strong>line</strong>ar kind and stems from atheoretical traditi<strong>on</strong> extending at least from the earlier works <strong>on</strong> familyhomeostasis and triadic systems of Jacks<strong>on</strong> (1957) and Haley (1967,1973). This model has been presented in more complete form elsewhere(Stant<strong>on</strong> et al. 1978). In essence, it is proposed that drug addicti<strong>on</strong>be thought of as part of a cyclical process involving three or moreindividuals, comm<strong>on</strong>ly the addict and two parents. These people forman intimate, interdependent, interpers<strong>on</strong>al system. At times the equilibriumof this interpers<strong>on</strong>al system is threatened, such as when discordbetween the parents is amplified to the point of impending separati<strong>on</strong>.When this happens, addicts become activated, their behavior chances,and they create situati<strong>on</strong>s that dramatically focus attenti<strong>on</strong> up<strong>on</strong> themselves.This behavior can take a number of forms. For example,they may lose their temper, come home high, commit a serious crime,153


or overdose <strong>on</strong> drugs. Whatever its form, however, this acti<strong>on</strong> allowsthe parents to shift focus from their marital c<strong>on</strong>flict to a parentaloverinvolvement with the child. In effect, the movement is from anunstable dyadic interacti<strong>on</strong> (e.g., parents al<strong>on</strong>e) to a more stabletriadic interacti<strong>on</strong> (parents and addict). By focusing <strong>on</strong> the problemsof the addict, no matter how severe or life threatening, the parentschoose a course that is apparently safer than dealing with l<strong>on</strong>g-standingmarital c<strong>on</strong>flicts. C<strong>on</strong>sequently--after the marital crisis has beensuccessfully avoided--the addict shifts to a less provocative stance andbegins to behave more competently. This is a new step in the sequence.As the addict dem<strong>on</strong>strates increased competence, indicating the abilityto functi<strong>on</strong> independently of the family--for example, by getting a job,getting married, enrolling in a drug treatment program, or detoxifying--the parents are left to deal with their still unresolved c<strong>on</strong>flicts. Atthis point in the cycle, marital tensi<strong>on</strong>s increase and the threat ofseparati<strong>on</strong> arises. The addict then behaves in an attenti<strong>on</strong>-getting orself-destructive way, and the dysfuncti<strong>on</strong>al triadic cycle is againcompleted.This cycle can vary in its intensity. It may occur in subdued form intreatment sessi<strong>on</strong>s or during day-to-day interacti<strong>on</strong>s and c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>saround the home. For example, a parent hinting at vacati<strong>on</strong>ing withoutthe spouse may trigger a spurt of loud talking by the addict. If thestakes are increased, the cycle becomes more explosive and the acti<strong>on</strong>sof all participants grow more serious and more dramatic, e.g., theparents threatening divorce might well be followed by the addict’soverdosing. Whatever the intensity level, however, we have observedsuch patterns so often that we have almost come to take them forgranted. Viewed from this perspective, the behavior of the addictserves an important protective functi<strong>on</strong> and helps to maintain thehomeostatic balance of the family system.The <strong>on</strong>set of the addicti<strong>on</strong> cycle appears in many cases to occur at thetime of adolescence and is intensified as issues of the addict’s leavinghome come to the fore. This developmental stage heralds difficulttimes for most families and requires that the parents renegotiate theirrelati<strong>on</strong>ship--a relati<strong>on</strong>ship which will not include this child. However,since the parents of the addict are unable to relate to each othersatisfactorily, the family reacts with intense fear when the integrity ofthe triadic relati<strong>on</strong>ship is threatened. Thus we find that most addicts’families become stabilized or stuck at this developmental stage in sucha way that the addict remains intimately involved with them <strong>on</strong> achr<strong>on</strong>ic basis. In additi<strong>on</strong> to staying closely tied to the home, thefailure to separate and become aut<strong>on</strong>omous may take several otherforms, and the child may (a) fail to develop stable, intimate (particularlyheterosexual) relati<strong>on</strong>ships outside the family; (b) fail to becomeinvolved in a stable job, school, or other age-appropriate activity;(c) obtain work which is well below his or her capabilities; (d) becomeinvolved in criminal activities; (e) become an addict.THE ABUSER’S FAMILY OF PROCREATIONC<strong>on</strong>cerning marriage and the family of procreati<strong>on</strong>, it has generallybeen c<strong>on</strong>cluded that the (usually heterosexual) dyadic relati<strong>on</strong>shipsthat abusers, especially addicts, become involved in are a repetiti<strong>on</strong> ofthe nuclear family of origin, with roles and interacti<strong>on</strong> patterns similarto those seen with the opposite-sex parent. (See Stant<strong>on</strong> 1979b and1980 for a review of studies supporting this and subsequent c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>s.)154


In a certain number of these marriages both spouses are addicted,although it is more comm<strong>on</strong> for <strong>on</strong>e or neither or them to be drugdependent at the beginning of the relati<strong>on</strong>ship. If the marital uni<strong>on</strong> isformed during addicti<strong>on</strong>, it is more likely to dissolve after methad<strong>on</strong>etreatment than if initiated at some other time. Also, n<strong>on</strong>addicted wivestend to find their husbands’ methad<strong>on</strong>e program to be more satisfactorythan do addicted wives. Equally important, the rate of marriage formale addicts is half that which would be expected, while the rate formultiple marriages is above average for both sexes. A number ofauthors have noted how parental permissi<strong>on</strong> is often quite tentativefor addicts to have viable marital relati<strong>on</strong>ships. They often flee intomarriage <strong>on</strong>ly to return home, defeated, as a result of parental influenceor “pull.”In our own studies of male addicts (Stant<strong>on</strong> et al. 1978) we have notedthat if the addict had not “checked in” at home recently or if theparents had some other reas<strong>on</strong> to fear they were “losing” him, a crisisoften occurred in their home--often a fight between them--and the s<strong>on</strong>was alerted to it. At that point he was apt to start a fight with hiswife--a move which served two purposes. It showed the parents thatthey had not lost him to marriage, and it gave him an excuse to returnhome to help, since he had “no place else to go.” Usually he succeededin diverting attenti<strong>on</strong> from the problem in the parental home and <strong>on</strong>ceagain functi<strong>on</strong>ed to reduce c<strong>on</strong>flicts between adults.At other times the precipitating event(s) were less obvious and he andhis wife fell into a cycle of periodic altercati<strong>on</strong>s. Their temporalregularity seemed almost servo-c<strong>on</strong>trolled. 3 These appear to be maintenancecycles. They may not have resulted in his moving out, butinstead he would show up with some regularity at his parents’ home tocomplain about c<strong>on</strong>nubial problems. He seemed to be saying, “I justdropped by to let you know that things aren’t going well and youhaven’t lost me.” (In <strong>on</strong>e case, every time the addict’s mother calledhim, he would tell her he had just had a fight with his wife, even ifhe had not--an ingenious way of keeping both systems simultaneouslyintact and pacified.) Marital battles thus became a functi<strong>on</strong>al part ofthe intergenerati<strong>on</strong>al homeostatic system, possessing both adaptive andsacrificial qualities.SINGLE-PARENT FAMILIESIn many drug abuser families-of-origin, <strong>on</strong>e parent (usually father) isabsent. In such cases, <strong>on</strong>e would think that a triadic model (asabove) would not apply, and that a dyadic framework, e.g., <strong>on</strong>eencompassing mother and s<strong>on</strong>, would be more fitting. It would alsoappear to be more parsim<strong>on</strong>ious and less complicated. N<strong>on</strong>etheless, wehave found (Stant<strong>on</strong> et al. 1978) that when the matter is pursuedclosely, a third important member generally pops up as an activeparticipant in the interacti<strong>on</strong>. Usually the triadic system is of a lessobvious form, such as a covert disagreement between mother andgrandmother, or mother and ex-husband. This is c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ant with apoint made emphatically by Haley (1976) that at least two adults are3 In this case, “servo-c<strong>on</strong>trolled” refers to an automatic return to a priorbehavioral state, <strong>on</strong>ce a certain limit (i.e., the end of a time period)is reached.155


usually involved in an offspring’s problem and that clinicians shouldlook for a triangle c<strong>on</strong>sisting of an overinvolved parent-child dyad anda more peripheral parent, grandparent, or parent surrogate. Thus ithas been our experience that in additi<strong>on</strong> to the (male) addict and hismother, the triad may include mother’s boyfriend, an estranged parent,a grandparent, or some other relative. These alternative systemsappear to exhibit patterns and cycles similar to those in which bothparents are present and, again, revolve around interrupti<strong>on</strong> by theabuser of c<strong>on</strong>flicts between adult members. However, achieving separati<strong>on</strong>and independence is even more of an issue in single-parent families,since mother may be left al<strong>on</strong>e with few psychological resources if thedrug abuser departs.156


Self-Esteem Theory of<strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>R. A. Steffenhagen, Ph.D.To be of value a theory must predict as well as explain the phenomenaafter the fact. The self-esteem theory postulates that all behavior ismediated by the individual’s attempt to protect the “self” within thesocial milieu.This theory is a developmental <strong>on</strong>e emanating from an Adlerian approachin which self-esteem is seen as the main psychodynamic mechanismunderlyinq all drug use and abuse. The self-esteem c<strong>on</strong>cept developsout of Adler’s Individual Psychology, more precisely the Psychology ofSelf-Esteem, in which the underlying motive of human behavior is thepreservati<strong>on</strong> of the c<strong>on</strong>cept of the “self” (Ansbacher and Ansbacher1956). The preservati<strong>on</strong> of the c<strong>on</strong>cept of “self” is the most importantvariable in understanding the initiati<strong>on</strong>, c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong>, and cessati<strong>on</strong> ofdrug use, and further explains why the rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> process frequentlyresults in relapse.The theory will not <strong>on</strong>ly account for the initiati<strong>on</strong> into drug use (thesocial milieu) but will determine the course the pattern will take (vis-avisself-esteem) in terms of c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong>, cessati<strong>on</strong>, and/or relapse.The etiology of drug use does not lie in the pers<strong>on</strong>ality of the individual(addicti<strong>on</strong> pr<strong>on</strong>eness) or in family c<strong>on</strong>stellati<strong>on</strong>s (drug use as a behavioralmodel), but in availability, social acceptability, and social pressure.It must be noted that the type of dependency is c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed by theculture. Dependency <strong>on</strong> amphetamines, for example, could not haveexisted before their discovery in the early 1900s, medical use in the1930s, and post-World War II street use. Alcohol (as a social drug)was the main drug of abuse until the post-World War II period in theUnited States, and marijuana was the drug of abuse in India. Today,these two countries are in a state of social change, and the youths ofboth countries are becoming users and abusers of socially unacceptabledrugs--marijuana in the United States and alcohol in India (Cohen1969). Thus, the culture determines the types of drugs available,while social pressure and social acceptability further determine thetype and pattern of use. Social pressure may lead <strong>on</strong>e both into andout of drug abuse. This has become evident in some of the streetgangs in New York City, where youths would become addicted toheroin because of peer pressure and then would later cease as a result157


of the same pressure. A similar situati<strong>on</strong> was true in Vietnam, wheremany of the soldiers who became addicted to heroin were subsequentlycured of their addicti<strong>on</strong>. The reas<strong>on</strong>s for relapse will be discussedlater.The theory incorporates several of Adler’s key c<strong>on</strong>cepts. Self-esteemdoes not emerge full blown at birth but is developed slowly during thesocializati<strong>on</strong> process. The foundati<strong>on</strong> is developed early in life and ispresent at the time the prototype of the pers<strong>on</strong>ality is formed. Thisdoes not mean, however, that self-esteem cannot be changed positivelyor negatively later, since the individual is very much resp<strong>on</strong>sive tosocial pressure. The c<strong>on</strong>cepts which will be elucidated in this paperare (1) inferiority-superiority, (2) social interest, (3) goal orientati<strong>on</strong>,and (4) lifestyle. In the c<strong>on</strong>text of this discussi<strong>on</strong>, the developmentof self-esteem and the social milieu will be looked at to explain howsocial pressures affect the individual.INFERIORITY AND SUPERIORITYParamount to Adler’s Individual Psychology are the c<strong>on</strong>cept(s) ofinferiority and superiority. All children begin life in an inferiorpositi<strong>on</strong>, and much of their early socializati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sists of learning tocope with feelings of inferiority. Exposed to an adult milieu, theyperceive themselves as small and weak, inadequate and inferior.Learning to cope with these inferiority feelings, which dominate thebehavior of all individuals to a lesser or greater degree, becomes thebasis for goal orientati<strong>on</strong>. The uniqueness of human beings stemsfrom their means of dealing with these feelings, their style of life.Coping mechanisms are developed in accord with individual choices (asTillich says, “Man is his choices.”) or goals, which can <strong>on</strong>ly be understoodin relati<strong>on</strong>ship to lifestyle and social milieu. The feelings ofinferiority reflect the extent to which the individual perceives himself/herself as able or unable to obtain goals. The ability to attain goalsis the result of psychological, biological, and sociological factors, whilethe technique chosen to deal with inferiority is the result of a pers<strong>on</strong>’slifestyle.<strong>On</strong> the other hand, expressi<strong>on</strong>s of superiority can become a compensatorymechanism in which the individual’s overt behavior becomes amask for inner feelings of inferiority.SOCIAL INTERESTForemost in the development of a healthy pers<strong>on</strong>ality is the developmentof social interest, because it is <strong>on</strong>ly through social participati<strong>on</strong> thatthe individual can deal with feelings of inferiority and develop highself-esteem. Within the Adlerian paradigm, lack of social interest isalways present in a neurotic pers<strong>on</strong>. Humans are social animals, andmost c<strong>on</strong>scious behavior is spent in c<strong>on</strong>tact with other individuals inthe normal pursuit of work, play, and raising a family. The fundamentalc<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing technique during the socializati<strong>on</strong> process centersaround praise and blame. Praise is good for the ego and helps in thedevelopment of self-esteem when it is given for socially useful acti<strong>on</strong>s.When the mother’s rewards are given for acti<strong>on</strong>s which are sociallyuseless or in such a pampering fashi<strong>on</strong> that the individual <strong>on</strong>ly gets158


ewards for exemplary behavior, feelings of self-worth and good selfesteemdo not develop.Good social interest can be developed <strong>on</strong>ly as a result of otherdirectedness,i.e., a c<strong>on</strong>cern for others. Other-directedness is aprimary phenomen<strong>on</strong> that is healthy in c<strong>on</strong>juncti<strong>on</strong> with socially usefulgoals.GOAL ORIENTATIONGoal orientati<strong>on</strong> is very important to self-esteem theory, becausesuccess and failure can <strong>on</strong>ly be understood subjectively and not objectively;outward symbols of success must be understood in terms of theindividual’s own percepti<strong>on</strong>s. Those who, to others, seem to have theworld by the tail may see themselves as failures (e.g., Marilyn M<strong>on</strong>roe).In this respect, Adler says,In this psychological schema there are two approximatelyfixed points: the low self-estimati<strong>on</strong> of the child who feelsinferior, and the over-life-sized goal which may reach highas god-likeness. Between these two points there rest thepreparatory attempts, the groping devices and tricks, aswell as the finished readiness and habitual attitudes.(Ansbacher and Ansbacher 1956, p. 245)Insecurity in childhood causes the individual to set high goals and todevelop compensatory safeguarding measures: “If I didn’t have thisheadache, I would have d<strong>on</strong>e better” or “If I hadn’t drunk so muchlast night but had studied, I would have d<strong>on</strong>e better <strong>on</strong> my exam.”The individual may well resort to drug abuse as a coping mechanism.Individuals are c<strong>on</strong>stantly striving for superiority; all behavior is aneffort to achieve success (positive situati<strong>on</strong>s) and to overcome obstacles(negative situati<strong>on</strong>s). Motivati<strong>on</strong> is a goal-directed drive; lack ofmotivati<strong>on</strong> is a symptom, not the cause, of neurotic behavior.To cope with over-life-sized goals and low self-esteem, the individualmay turn to drug abuse.LIFESTYLEAdler defined lifestyle as “the wholeness of his individuality” (Adler1933). the guiding <strong>line</strong> of the pers<strong>on</strong>ality. He originally called it“Lebensplan” (life plan), then “Lebenstil” (lifestyle), and finally, styleof life. He further says,In other words the child must have formed a guiding <strong>line</strong>(Leithinie), a guiding image (Leithbild) in the expectati<strong>on</strong>sthus best to be able to orient himself in his envir<strong>on</strong>ment andto achieve satisfacti<strong>on</strong> of his needs, the avoidance of displeasure,and the attainment of pleasure.(Adler 1912, p. 33)Max Weber (1974) was the first to use the c<strong>on</strong>cept of lifestyle to referto a way of life of a subculture--a group-guiding principle. Adler159


used it to refer to the individual’s guiding principle. Thus, lifestylerefers to the individual’s orientati<strong>on</strong> toward social behavior--the guiding<strong>line</strong> of the pers<strong>on</strong>ality, the core around which the pers<strong>on</strong>ality revolves.Lifestyle is the whole which unifies the parts.It is the uniqueness of humans that made Adler call his theoreticdevelopment individual psychology, stressing this uniqueness. Everypers<strong>on</strong> is the same as every other, and every pers<strong>on</strong> is different fromevery other: Culture is the unifying principle.There are two forms of deviant lifestyle--pampered and neglected. Apampered lifestyle results from an overprotective mother who takes allresp<strong>on</strong>sibility for her child’s behavior, preventing the child fromdeveloping a feeling of self-worth from his or her own accomplishments.Here rewarding success does nothing to establish a feeling of self-worth.Approval and reward are seen as coming <strong>on</strong>ly from superior performance.Love is perceived as a resp<strong>on</strong>se to this performance rather than as afeeling for himself or herself as a pers<strong>on</strong>, producing very weak selfesteem.The counterpart of the pampered lifestyle is the neglected lifestyle ofthe impoverished envir<strong>on</strong>ment, in which the individual receives almostno attenti<strong>on</strong> and is left to his or her own devices. The patterns ofdrug abuse as coping mechanisms may vary between these two polarsituati<strong>on</strong>s, for example, heroin addicti<strong>on</strong> am<strong>on</strong>g ghetto youths andmarijuana abuse am<strong>on</strong>g middle-class college youths (Steffenhagen 1974).Today we see marijuana abuse am<strong>on</strong>g lower class youths and heroinaddicti<strong>on</strong> increasing am<strong>on</strong>g the middle/upper middle-class youths,showing that the type of abuse is a functi<strong>on</strong> of the zeitgeist as well asthe availability of the drug, which may account for the kaleidoscopicnature of the present drug scene.SOCIAL MILIEUSutherland’s (1939) differential associati<strong>on</strong> theory of deviance is directlyapplicable to our Adlerian model. Adler was keenly aware of the roleof society in shaping the individual’s behavior. Differential associati<strong>on</strong>has a direct impact up<strong>on</strong> the form deviance will take, given low selfesteem.Sutherland postulates that deviance is learned and that theinternalizati<strong>on</strong> of such behavior is a functi<strong>on</strong> of durati<strong>on</strong>, frequency,intensity, and priority of deviant associati<strong>on</strong>s. It becomes clear in thecollege milieu that the more time an individual spends within a drugusinggroup, the more likely he or she is to use drugs. Becker andStrauss (1956) clearly indicate the role socializati<strong>on</strong> plays in this situati<strong>on</strong>.I menti<strong>on</strong>ed previously how heroin use became a problem inVietnam. Boredom and social stress, which led to a need for a socialrelease of tensi<strong>on</strong>, and the acceptance of heroin by the peer group weresufficient criteria for the development of a drug problem without anyneed for neurotic coping mechanisms. This is an example of individualswith relatively high self-esteem becoming drug abusers as a result of thesocial milieu. The relevance of this to the rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> process willbe discussed in detail later.Peer pressure during adolescence is particularly powerful. The needfor acceptance, while always an important drive, is especially str<strong>on</strong>gduring this formative period, and helps to account for the heavy druguse in the youth subculture. The pressure is not always overt or160


obvious but may be covert or subtle: The fact that an activity maybe the agent around which the group coalesces may provide the impetusfor experimentati<strong>on</strong>. In the case of marijuana, differential associati<strong>on</strong>is particularly important since the individual must associate with usersin order to try the drug and then to obtain a supply. Both thepreference resulting from associati<strong>on</strong> and the necessity prevail.SELF-ESTEEM AND DEVIANCEAs I have said, who will become a drug user or abuser cannot beexplained <strong>on</strong> the basis of any single psychodynamic factor but musttake account of the social milieu. Our postulate is that an individualwith low self-esteem will become a prime target for drug abuse as aresult of the prevalence of drug informati<strong>on</strong>--true or false--providedby the mass media. Behavior accompanying low self-esteem can bestbe explained by the following model:The behavior need not occur singly, but can also occur in combinati<strong>on</strong>.Gross multiple-drug users in college show more emoti<strong>on</strong>al disturbancethan the n<strong>on</strong>using populati<strong>on</strong> (McAree et al. 1969, 1972), indicatingneurosis coupled with drug abuse. In 1974 I further postulated thatdrug abuse (and neurosis) may also move toward the occult, in whichcase the drug use may c<strong>on</strong>tinue or be replaced by the occult support.Participati<strong>on</strong> in the occult may provide an immediate source of power asa coping mechanism or may provide a form of group self-esteem(Lieberman et al. 1973).In the lower class, low self-esteem may take the forms of neurosis,drug abuse, and delinquency. In this milieu, drug abuse and delinquencyare a much more likely pair than drug abuse and occultism.The occult appeals largely to the intellectually curious, especiallycollege students. In both social classes, we may find drug abuse andsuicide pairing together. Alcoholics are much more likely to commitsuicide than are n<strong>on</strong>alcoholics:<strong>Drug</strong> abuse is seen generally as an expressi<strong>on</strong> of the pamperedlife style. Its functi<strong>on</strong> is to safeguard low self-esteem;enabling individuals to shirk resp<strong>on</strong>sibility, while blamingothers and outer circumstances, providing excuses, andenabling them to maintain excessively high goals withoutexpending energy.(Steffenhagen 1974, p. 249)It is important to realize that while low self-esteem is postulated as theunderlying psychodynamic mechanism for drug abuse, it is not asufficient or necessary c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> for initiati<strong>on</strong>. The social milieu canalso provide the impetus (differential associati<strong>on</strong> plus existence of thedrug). The initiati<strong>on</strong> into drug use may then stem from the socialmilieu, but the abuse of the n<strong>on</strong>addicting drugs would be associated161


with low self-esteem. Further, although a cessati<strong>on</strong> may be superimposedby the social structure, relapse would be likely to occur in thecase of low self-esteem. As in the example of Vietnam and heroin, theimpetus came from the social situati<strong>on</strong>--drug use c<strong>on</strong>tinued as l<strong>on</strong>g asthe social situati<strong>on</strong> remained c<strong>on</strong>stant, whereas when the situati<strong>on</strong>changed cessati<strong>on</strong> occurred. We have two possible outcomes: (1) theindividuals with good self-esteem remained drug free whereas (2) thoseindividuals with low self-esteem were likely to relapse since an alreadytried neurotic coping mechanism was within their repertoire.I also postulate that self-esteem is important in determining the effectthat stress will have up<strong>on</strong> the individual. A pers<strong>on</strong> with low selfesteemwill resp<strong>on</strong>d much more negatively to stress than a pers<strong>on</strong> withhigh self-esteem. In the case of the pampered lifestyle the individualmay functi<strong>on</strong> adequately as l<strong>on</strong>g as he or she has the support systemprovided by the family but may quickly resort to neurotic copingmechanisms when this support system is removed--such as by thedeath of parents or by merely going away to college.CONCLUSIONThe following paradigm is offered:Self-esteem + Lifestyle + Pers<strong>on</strong>ality + Goal orientati<strong>on</strong> +Primary group + Social milieu = BehaviorSelf-esteem--high or lowLifestyle--pampered or neglectedPers<strong>on</strong>ality--normal or neurotic (includes inferiority and social interest)Goal orientati<strong>on</strong>--realistic or unrealisticPrimary group--supportive or unsupportiveSocial milieu--excess of definiti<strong>on</strong>s for c<strong>on</strong>formity or devianceThese are not mutually exclusive categories, since they all interrelate,and the lifestyle of the parent is superimposed up<strong>on</strong> the child. Selfesteemis posited not as the apex of the pers<strong>on</strong>ality but the foundati<strong>on</strong>.The theory postulates that the psychodynamic mechanism underlyingdrug abuse is low self-esteem. Self-esteem develops through experientialbehavior involved in mastery, the ability to master situati<strong>on</strong>s andachieve <strong>on</strong>e’s goals. Low self-esteem may result either from settinggoals too high or from not achieving realistic goals because of a lackof c<strong>on</strong>fidence in the ability to attain them. The latter situati<strong>on</strong> mayhappen when a parent or a significant other does everything for thechild, never allowing him or her to develop talents for mastery.William James’ formula, success over pretenti<strong>on</strong>s equaling self-esteem,reflects this situati<strong>on</strong> (James 1890, p. 310). This is not solely aresult of intrapsychic processes but also of the social order. Goalsare set by the individual (the individual’s uniqueness), but they areprovided by the social system. According to Mert<strong>on</strong> (1938), successas defined by the American credo is largely equated with the attainmentof m<strong>on</strong>ey. This is further evidenced by the fact that America is asecular/materialistic culture, as opposed to a religious/spiritual culture(Roszak 1975). The American credo provides the basis for the goals(which sociologists frequently call aspirati<strong>on</strong>s), but the social structure162


does not always provide the means (expectati<strong>on</strong>s) for attainment.However, although many forms of deviance cannot be explained solelywithin the framework of this perspective, e.g., marijuana use am<strong>on</strong>gthe youths of today, nevertheless we must look to the social milieu forpart of the answer.Even when the goals prescribed by the culture are not readily attainabledue to social deprivati<strong>on</strong>, low self-esteem is not inevitable. Theindividual may be able to lower goals appropriately so that attainmentis possible, and thereby achieve satisfacti<strong>on</strong> and develop a feeling ofself-worth.All behavior is goal directed or goal striving--it is the energizing stateof the organism. Since all behavior becomes goal striving, individualsevaluate themselves in terms of their percepti<strong>on</strong>s, their evaluati<strong>on</strong> ofthemselves in terms of achievements. High self-esteem is achievedwhen the evaluati<strong>on</strong> is good and socially useful; when the evaluati<strong>on</strong> isbad or <strong>on</strong> the socially useless side of life, low self-esteem results.Thus, it becomes apparent that self-esteem is the key variable underlyingdrug abuse. If individuals feel inadequate (inferior), they feel aneed to protect their poor self-image, frequently through compensatorymechanisms which create further problems in interpers<strong>on</strong>al relati<strong>on</strong>sand add to the feelings of inferiority.Our theory also helps us explain why Alcoholics An<strong>on</strong>ymous (AA) andSynan<strong>on</strong> are <strong>on</strong>ly rehabilitative 1 and not curative. It is generallyaccepted that alcoholics are never cured but remain functi<strong>on</strong>al <strong>on</strong>ly asl<strong>on</strong>g as they remain active in AA, and a similar c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> seems toprevail for the members of Synan<strong>on</strong> and even Weight Watchers. It isour c<strong>on</strong>tenti<strong>on</strong> that this can be easily explained within the self-esteemtheory, because both of these organizati<strong>on</strong>s do nothing to build individualself-esteem but, rather, build a form of group self-esteem resultingfrom and depending <strong>on</strong> group support cohesi<strong>on</strong> (as the pamperingmother). All of these organizati<strong>on</strong>s provide a socializati<strong>on</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>,and the individual is socialized to remain problem free <strong>on</strong>ly within theframework of the group.With the self-esteem theory we can explain n<strong>on</strong>use, social use, andabuse of drugs as well as why various therapeutic models are or arenot successful. It is possible that an ex-AA member may remainsober, but this can be explained within the c<strong>on</strong>text of the supportsystem or unique circumstances where the drug as a coping mechanismloses its functi<strong>on</strong> as a self-esteem protecting mechanism.1 By rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> I mean that the individual is returned to a state of“normal” functi<strong>on</strong>ing but not cured of the pathology, which can becomereoperative due to trauma.163


Biological, Psychogenic, andSociogenic Factors in<strong>Drug</strong> DependenceW.K. van Dijk, M.D.lNTRODUCTlONIt should be clear from the start that the following c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s arepresented by a clinical psychiatrist. This means that the viewpointfrom which the dependence theme will be inspected is largely determinedby experiences with the treatment and rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> of individuals whohave fallen victim to the problems of abuse of drugs and with thepreventi<strong>on</strong> of relapse.DESCRIPTIONThe state of dependence as a behavioral syndrome is characterized bythe fact that the pers<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cerned cannot live without the drug he orshe is dependent <strong>on</strong>. This inability may take different forms andgrades which depend, am<strong>on</strong>gst other things, <strong>on</strong> the type of drug.With alcohol we sometimes observe that for some reas<strong>on</strong> the alcoholic isable to abstain for days or weeks or even m<strong>on</strong>ths; in case of heroin,however, the ability to stop taking the drug is restricted to a fewhours <strong>on</strong>ly. In either case, there arises after a shorter or l<strong>on</strong>gerinterval, more or less sp<strong>on</strong>taneously, a state of inner tensi<strong>on</strong> in whichthe dependent pers<strong>on</strong> feels an unc<strong>on</strong>trollable craving for the drug.Apart from the type of drug used, these differences are influenced bythe pers<strong>on</strong>ality structure, social factors (including treatment), andThis paper is reprinted with permissi<strong>on</strong> from Prof. Dr. van Dijk’s“Complexity of the Dependence Problem: Interacti<strong>on</strong> of Biological withPsychogenic and Sociogenic Factors,” in Biochemical and PharmacologicalAspects of Dependence and Reports <strong>on</strong> Marijuana Research, ed. H.M.van Praag (Haarlem, The Netherlands: Bohn, 1971), pp. 6-18.164


probably also by the durati<strong>on</strong> of the state of dependence. In somecases dependence is c<strong>on</strong>sistent with a more or less normal way of life,in others it is not. In the latter case the clinical term “addicti<strong>on</strong>” maybe used. It should be noted that this definiti<strong>on</strong> of dependence andaddicti<strong>on</strong> does not include nor exclude physiological mechanisms. Theterms are used descriptively to characterize behavior or ways of living.It is useful not to look at dependence, including addicti<strong>on</strong>, as a separateentity <strong>on</strong>ly, but to keep in mind that it must be regarded as the finalstage of a process. We may roughly sketch the natural history of theuse of a drug leading to dependence as follows.The first stage is the c<strong>on</strong>tact with the drug, which may take place ina medical or a n<strong>on</strong>medical setting. After <strong>on</strong>e or more c<strong>on</strong>tacts theprocess may come to a halt, or it may develop into the sec<strong>on</strong>d stage--experimentati<strong>on</strong>.The stage of experimentati<strong>on</strong> may assume different forms as to itspicture, intensity, and durati<strong>on</strong>, depending <strong>on</strong> several factors whichwill be discussed later. After some time has elapsed, the sec<strong>on</strong>d stagemay come to an end, or it may develop into a stage of socially tolerateduse which from a psychiatric point of view may be called an integratedmode of use, or it may lead to the stage of excessive use.The stage of excessive use often carries several risks and damages,which may be of a physical, psychical, or social (interpers<strong>on</strong>al, ec<strong>on</strong>omic,legal, moral, etc.) nature. What is to be regarded as a risk or adamage depends largely <strong>on</strong> the prevailing social habits and customs, <strong>on</strong>the ec<strong>on</strong>omic and historical situati<strong>on</strong>, etc. The psychiatrist is inc<strong>line</strong>dto speak of excessive use when there is the threat of an impairment ofsocial, psychological, or physical functi<strong>on</strong>ing; certainly this stage willbe so labelled if actual damage in <strong>on</strong>e field or other can be dem<strong>on</strong>stratedas being related to the drug habit.The stage of excessive use as part of a process should be distinguishedfrom incidental excessive use and from periodical excessive use causedby psychiatric factors like recurring depressi<strong>on</strong> or epilepsy. It is amore or less c<strong>on</strong>tinuous state.In a percentage of cases this phase may develop into the syndrome ofaddicti<strong>on</strong>, which is a more or less terminal state. <strong>On</strong> the other hand,stopping the excessive use, or a return to integrated use may occur.The development of the use of a drug leading to dependence is recapitulatedin figure 1.Addicti<strong>on</strong>, which is the extreme form of dependence and which may bec<strong>on</strong>sidered an illness, is a syndrome showing three main features:1. It is damaging to the individual.2. It is relatively aut<strong>on</strong>omous. By this we mean that, whatevercomplex interplay of factors may have led to the phases of excessiveuse and addicti<strong>on</strong>, <strong>on</strong>ce the boundary has been passed, a relativelystable state has come into being, which is more or less independentof the primary generating causes and c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. As for thetreatment, relative aut<strong>on</strong>omy means that in the majority of casesmerely removing the initiating factors is not sufficient; specialmeasures have to be taken, aiming at treating the addicti<strong>on</strong> syndromeas such.165


FIGURE 1.–Stages in process of drug use3. The addicti<strong>on</strong> syndrome is self-perpetuating, sp<strong>on</strong>taneous recoverybeing excepti<strong>on</strong>al. <strong>On</strong> the c<strong>on</strong>trary, if no help is offered there is atendency to further deteriorati<strong>on</strong>.The courses the process can take in the individual case, the successivestages, and the final state show great variety, depending <strong>on</strong> the druginvolved, the pers<strong>on</strong>ality structure of the user, and the social c<strong>on</strong>text.During the development of dependence several changes in the featuresof the stages of drug use occur. They may be described as in figure 2.FIGURE 2.—Some shifts in characteristicsof stages of drug use166


ETIOLOGYGENERATING FACTORSExtensive research has so far failed to show <strong>on</strong>e simple cause whichinitiates the process of pathological use of drugs. <strong>On</strong> the c<strong>on</strong>trary,both research findings and clinical work have made it clear that acomplexity of causes and c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s gives rise to the drug-taking process.We may summarize these factors as follows.Pharmacological Effects of the <strong>Drug</strong>We may roughly locate the various drugs <strong>on</strong> a scale. <strong>On</strong> <strong>on</strong>e side arethe drugs with a str<strong>on</strong>g addictive acti<strong>on</strong> (e.g., heroin and morphine),<strong>on</strong> the other are those to which an addictive power can scarcely beascribed (e.g., aspirin, chlorpromazine, laxatives, or even petrol orvinegar). The addictive property of a drug depends <strong>on</strong> the somaticand psychic influences it exercises; in what way these are c<strong>on</strong>nectedwith the chemical structure is largely unknown. All of the addictivedrugs have an influence <strong>on</strong> the feeling or mode of experience of theuser.For practical reas<strong>on</strong>s we may divide them according to their effects:sedating, stimulating, and psychedelic. In all cases, the drug istaken for its desired acti<strong>on</strong>. What in a given case is regarded asdesired depends <strong>on</strong> the following factors.Pers<strong>on</strong>al Factors of the UserIn this dimensi<strong>on</strong>, too, we can make a scale with at its extremes pers<strong>on</strong>swho have a str<strong>on</strong>g dispositi<strong>on</strong> toward excessive use and addicti<strong>on</strong> andthose who are scarcely susceptible to it. The former type can be seento inc<strong>line</strong> toward a dysfuncti<strong>on</strong>al use even of n<strong>on</strong>addictive or hardlyaddictive drugs. Often with these pers<strong>on</strong>s a triad of features can befound.1. Feelings of discomfort, tensi<strong>on</strong>, and displeasure may easily arise inthem, as a result of mild frustrati<strong>on</strong>s or even “sp<strong>on</strong>taneously.”2. These unpleasant feelings are very intense or nearly unbearable.3. They find it impossible to master, sublimate, and canalize suchfeelings.The drug may be used to seek relief from the state of tensi<strong>on</strong>. Tothe group of pers<strong>on</strong>s not disposed toward abuse bel<strong>on</strong>g those in ourculture who are impervious to alcohol, tobacco, tea, coffee, sweets,etc.The pers<strong>on</strong>al factors should not be c<strong>on</strong>sidered to form an invariableand stable system. Age, for example, is an important modifyingfactor; during puberty and adolescence the risk appears to be increased.Physical and psychiatric diseases with a debilitating influence shouldalso be kept in mind as predisposing factors.167


Social Meaning and Value of a<strong>Drug</strong> and <strong>Drug</strong> TakingAl<strong>on</strong>g this dimensi<strong>on</strong>, factors like the drug being accepted in theculture or being c<strong>on</strong>sidered alien to it play a role (cf. the acceptanceor rejecti<strong>on</strong> of alcohol or opiates in some Eastern and Western cultures).Furthermore, the ritualizati<strong>on</strong> of the use of a drug, and the socialnorms, habits, and sancti<strong>on</strong>s governing it, including the legal regulati<strong>on</strong>sand jurisdicti<strong>on</strong>; the load of sensati<strong>on</strong> and thrill; the significanceof a drug as a symbol of sturdy, competitive, aggressive masculinity,or of a n<strong>on</strong>committal, n<strong>on</strong>aggressive attitude and mode of behavior; thefuncti<strong>on</strong> of a drug or of using drugs as a symbol and a signifier ofdifferences between groups, classes, and generati<strong>on</strong>s: to the groupthe user is the insider and the n<strong>on</strong>user is the outsider, while, c<strong>on</strong>versely,the user is the outsider in society in general; the functi<strong>on</strong> ofa drug or of drug taking is a symbol of a progressive, n<strong>on</strong>authoritarianattitude, etc.The social meaning of a drug and of drug taking is not <strong>on</strong>ly importantas an incentive to take drugs, but also as a factor which may have animportant influence <strong>on</strong> the effect of a drug. Here lies a c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>with the general pharmacological problem of the placebo effect and ofthe difference between reactors and n<strong>on</strong>reactors.Envir<strong>on</strong>mental Influences <strong>on</strong> the UserThese can be divided into positive factors, leading to a favorablereacti<strong>on</strong> of the pers<strong>on</strong>, and negative <strong>on</strong>es, causing too great, toosmall, or inadequate stress. This dimensi<strong>on</strong> is of course c<strong>on</strong>nectedwith the sec<strong>on</strong>d <strong>on</strong>e, above. The same social situati<strong>on</strong> and influencemay be experienced by <strong>on</strong>e pers<strong>on</strong> as a positive stimulus and as tooheavy a burden by another, which he or she may try to get rid of bymeans of the use of drugs.In each individual case the drug-using process starts and developsfrom the interplay of factors from the dimensi<strong>on</strong>s menti<strong>on</strong>ed above;figure 3 summarizes these.Factors Maintaining the ProcessAfter C<strong>on</strong>tact with the <strong>Drug</strong>After this brief discussi<strong>on</strong> of the generating factors of the drug-usingprocess, some short remarks will be made about the factors maintainingthe process after c<strong>on</strong>tact with the drug has been made. This pertainsto the questi<strong>on</strong> why dependence c<strong>on</strong>tinues, in spite of its unfavorableeffects. We can study these factors best in the case of addicti<strong>on</strong> withits self-perpetuating character. There is as yet no adequate explanati<strong>on</strong>for this remarkable feature of addicti<strong>on</strong>. In my opini<strong>on</strong>, however,there are str<strong>on</strong>g indicati<strong>on</strong>s that an important pathogenic part isplayed by the mechanism of vicious circles.A vicious circle may be described as a circular process in which acause generates a result, which in its turn maintains or reinforces theinitial cause. In addicti<strong>on</strong> we may distinguish four vicious circles.Pharmacological Vicious CirclePharmacological investigati<strong>on</strong>s have shown that the repeated use ofdrugs may cause a change in metabolism. This change may manifest168


1. Pharmacological properties:Str<strong>on</strong>gly addictive drugs(e.g., morphine, heroin)2. Pers<strong>on</strong>al factors:Str<strong>on</strong>g dispositi<strong>on</strong> toexcessive useN<strong>on</strong>-addictive drugs(e.g., aspirin, laxatives, vinegar)No dispositi<strong>on</strong> toexcessive use3. Social meaning and value: <strong>Drug</strong> accepted or rejected, ritualizati<strong>on</strong>,social norms and sancti<strong>on</strong>s (including legal regulati<strong>on</strong>s,police acti<strong>on</strong>s and jurisdicti<strong>on</strong>), symbolic significance4. Envir<strong>on</strong>mental influences:Negative factorsPositive factorsFIGURE 3.–Operating factors in the etiologyof the process of drug useitself in the phenomen<strong>on</strong> of tolerance (after prol<strong>on</strong>ged use an increaseof the dose is needed to attain the same effect) and the withdrawalsyndrome (a sudden interrupti<strong>on</strong> may cause unpleasant and evenserious physical and psychological signs and symptoms). In sometypes of dependence, the persistent need for the drug and the inclinati<strong>on</strong>to increase the dose may be explained by these phenomena.C<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> of the use, however, maintains the metabolic change,which in its turn is resp<strong>on</strong>sible for the need to use the drug again.Cause and effect influence each other by means of pharmacologicalmechanisms. This is the reas<strong>on</strong> we speak of a pharmacological viciouscircle, illustrated in figure 4.Cerebral Vicious CircleIn some cases the quantitatively and qualitatively excessive use of adrug may have a direct damaging influence <strong>on</strong> those cerebral functi<strong>on</strong>sthat form the basis for regulati<strong>on</strong> and integrati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the behaviorallevel. The outcome is a weakening of the strength of the ego. Thismeans that the pers<strong>on</strong>al psychical powers to regulate and c<strong>on</strong>trol theuse are reduced. This, in its turn, implies that the motives leadingto the use of the drug get the opportunity to assert themselves moreeasily. Because of this mutual relati<strong>on</strong>ship of cause and effect <strong>on</strong>e canalso speak of a vicious circle. See figure 5.Psychic Vicious CircleThis refers to the effects of dysfuncti<strong>on</strong>al use in the mental field. Inthis case, feelings of guilt and shame, the unpleasant noti<strong>on</strong> thatdecreasing or abstaining from use would be better, and the disagreeableperspective of the future, etc., play an important role. The easiestand most effective way to get rid of these annoying feelings is to takethe drug, and in this way a vicious circle is started. Moreover, wemay point at the infantomimetic effect of the use of drugs. By this ismeant a regressi<strong>on</strong> to a more infantile form of behavior with an increasein the affective and instinctive aspects of behavior and a decrease in169


FIGURE 4.–Pharmacological vicious circleFIGURE 5.–Cerebro-ego-weakening vicious circle170


the c<strong>on</strong>trolling and synthetic functi<strong>on</strong>s of the ego. Describing thisprocess in psychoanalytical terms, we may ascertain a shift from thereality principle to the pleasure-unpleasure principle and an increasingrelative predominance of the primary over the sec<strong>on</strong>dary process.Since cause and effect influence each other to and fro, we may assessagain a vicious circle in the psychic level. (See figure 6.)Social Vicious CircleThis circular process is based <strong>on</strong> the fact that drug addicti<strong>on</strong> hassocial c<strong>on</strong>sequences, which in their turn reinforce the use of thedrug. The social sequelae may be described as dysfuncti<strong>on</strong>, and,finally, a disintegrati<strong>on</strong> within the groups the addict is (or was)functi<strong>on</strong>ing in. This process has harmful effects <strong>on</strong> the addict. Wemay <strong>on</strong>ly menti<strong>on</strong> the reproaches of the spouse and other members ofthe family, the quarrels, the disdain and withdrawal of friends andacquaintances, the tensi<strong>on</strong>s and c<strong>on</strong>flicts in the occupati<strong>on</strong>al sphere,and, finally, the dropping out from society. This isolati<strong>on</strong> and rejecti<strong>on</strong>engender in the subject negative feelings, which foster an attitude ofletting <strong>on</strong>eself go into the state of being an addict. This means afixati<strong>on</strong> of the role behavior that goes with it and a reinforcement ofthe identificati<strong>on</strong> with a drug-using subculture. As an instance of thelatter we may point to the fact that severe penal measures againstmarijuana users may tip the balance and change an unstable and riskysituati<strong>on</strong> into a fixed harmful <strong>on</strong>e.The social vicious circle is illustrated in figure 7.General RemarksAfter this brief discussi<strong>on</strong> of the principle of the four vicious circles,some general remarks may be added.1. In some drugs, e.g., alcohol, all vicious circles menti<strong>on</strong>ed arepresent, whereas, in others, they are not. In marijuana, forinstance, the pharmacological and the cerebral circuits are lacking,as far as we can see at present.2. The original pharmacological, cerebral, psychological, and socialfactors which give rise to the vicious circles are by no meansrestricted to the state of addicti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly. They may already bedem<strong>on</strong>strated in the earlier stages of the process of drug use.What we can see, however, is that the more the process moves tothe stage of excessive use, the more the generating factors arebecoming circular. This shift from a <strong>line</strong>ar to a circular mechanismis c<strong>on</strong>nected with a developing disequilibrium between the operatingfactor <strong>on</strong> the <strong>on</strong>e hand, and the capacity to keep up with thisoperating factor or its effects <strong>on</strong> the other.Finally, when a shift has taken place from <strong>line</strong>ar to circular andwhen the quantitative influence of the circuits has risen to acritical level, the addictive state has been attained and will bemaintained.3. In this progress from more <strong>line</strong>ar to more circular acti<strong>on</strong> thefactors menti<strong>on</strong>ed do not work separately. In earlier stages theymay either cooperate and intensify or, c<strong>on</strong>versely, counterbalanceand reduce <strong>on</strong>e another. In the later phases they mostly reinforce171


FIGURE 6.–Psychic vicious circleFIGURE 7.–Social vicious circle172


<strong>on</strong>e another, which explains the relative aut<strong>on</strong>omy and the selfperpetuatingnature of addicti<strong>on</strong>.4. An example of the fact that the factors involved may counterbalanceeach other may be found in the therapeutic field. It is oftenpossible in the process of treatment to switch over a heroin addictto methad<strong>on</strong>e and to keep the patient <strong>on</strong> a stable dose of it bysupportive psychic and social measures. In some cases, the patientsthemselves try to decrease their dosage, whereas in a n<strong>on</strong>medicalsetting methad<strong>on</strong>e gives rise to addicti<strong>on</strong> with increasing doses inmost cases. Here we see that the social and pers<strong>on</strong>al factors areable to reduce the pharmacological vicious circle.173


A Theory of OpioidDependenceAbraham Wikler, M.D.Psychoanalytical theories of addicti<strong>on</strong> virtually ignored the specificpharmacological acti<strong>on</strong>s of the drug of addicti<strong>on</strong> but stressed theimportance of alleged intrapsychic “impulses” and “archaic l<strong>on</strong>gings.”Thus, Rado (1933) stated, “. . . not the toxic agent, but the impulseto use it, makes an addict out of a given individual.” Fenichel (1945)wrote, “. . . the origin and nature of addicti<strong>on</strong> are not determined bythe chemical effect of the drug but by the psychological structure ofthe patient. ” Be this as it may, the author is not aware of any data<strong>on</strong> the results of psychoanalytical therapy in the treatment of addicts;indeed, apart from the prohibitive cost of such therapy, it would seemthat in view of the prevalence of psychopathy (sociopathy) and thinkingdisorder am<strong>on</strong>g detoxified opioid addicts (Hill et al. 1960; M<strong>on</strong>roe etal. 1971), psychoanalytical therapy would be futile. Furthermore, thefact that rats and m<strong>on</strong>keys, equipped with intravenous cannulas forself-injecti<strong>on</strong>, will readily take and maintain themselves <strong>on</strong> morphine,amphetamines, cocaine, and pentobarbital (Schuster and Thomps<strong>on</strong>1969) casts some doubt <strong>on</strong> the necessity of such psychoanalyticalvariables for the genesis of addicti<strong>on</strong>.Regardless of theoretical speculati<strong>on</strong>s about the role of pers<strong>on</strong>ality,most writers have agreed that it is the “euphoria” produced by morphinethat impels the user to repeat the experience and to relapse after l<strong>on</strong>gperiods of abstenti<strong>on</strong>, for whatever reas<strong>on</strong>. “Euphoria” is defined byMcAuliffe and Gord<strong>on</strong> (1974) as “a subjectively pleasurable feelingproduced by taking an opiate drug,” and may be assigned a numericalrating <strong>on</strong> the Hill-Haertzen MBG scale (feeling happy, clear-headed,less discouraged, full of energy, etc.--cited by Jasinski [1973]). However,<strong>on</strong> the basis of interviews of 60 to 70 opioid addicts, LindesmithThis paper is reprinted with permissi<strong>on</strong> from Dr. Wikler’s “Opioid Antag<strong>on</strong>istsand Dec<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing in Addicti<strong>on</strong> Treatment,” in <strong>Drug</strong> Dependence--Treatment and Treatment Evaluati<strong>on</strong>, Skandia Internati<strong>on</strong>al Symposia,eds. H Bostrom, T. Larss<strong>on</strong>, and N. Ljungstedt (Stockholm: Almqvist& Wiksell Internati<strong>on</strong>al, 1975), pp. 157-182.174


(1947) c<strong>on</strong>tended that “euphoria” disappears <strong>on</strong>ce the subject hasbecome physically dependent, and that the user becomes an addict,and regards himself as such, when he makes a cognitive c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>between administrati<strong>on</strong> of the drug and relief of withdrawal distress.This has been c<strong>on</strong>tested recently by McAuliffe and Gord<strong>on</strong> (1974), whoreported that 98 percent of 64 opioid addicts stated they experienced“euphoria” (initial “rush” followed by “<strong>on</strong> the nod”) after each selfinjecti<strong>on</strong>of opioid drug (usually heroin) despite l<strong>on</strong>g-c<strong>on</strong>tinued dailyuse, and claim they have dem<strong>on</strong>strated that “despite the developmentof tolerance chr<strong>on</strong>ic opioid addicts do experience euphoria followinginjecti<strong>on</strong>s, and that their desire for euphoria appears to be a majoractor in the explanati<strong>on</strong> of their behavior.” (Presumably, “behavior”includes relapse.) It may be questi<strong>on</strong>ed just how tolerant (and physicallydependent) McAuliffe and Gord<strong>on</strong>’s subjects were, since theamounts of drug they took were estimated in street terms and dependencewas judged merely by asking other addicts about the individual,looking for extensive old and new scarring (“needle tracks”) andasking the addicts themselves. Furthermore, Wikler (1952) observedthat there was often a wide discrepancy between subjective reportsmade by an addict and his objective behavior. Thus, after intravenousinjecti<strong>on</strong> of 30 mg of morphine, the subject reported he was full of“pep,” then went “<strong>on</strong> the nod” and had to be aroused to explain whathe meant by “pep.” After several days <strong>on</strong> multiple, escalating dosesof morphine (given <strong>on</strong> demand), reports of “pep” decreased markedly(though the “thrill” or “rush” persisted) and the subject becameincreasingly dysphoric (guilt, hostility). Although the subject was atliberty to disc<strong>on</strong>tinue taking morphine at any time (with appropriatetreatment to minimize withdrawal distress) he c<strong>on</strong>tinued to escalate thedose and frequency of injecti<strong>on</strong>s and developed a high degree oftolerance and physical dependence. Wikler (1952) c<strong>on</strong>cluded that withthe development of physical dependence, a new, pharmacological needwas acquired, the gratificati<strong>on</strong> of which (by injecti<strong>on</strong> of morphine)served to maintain addicti<strong>on</strong> despite the waning of initial “euphoria.”It should be noted that this need is appetitive (gratificati<strong>on</strong> of it isaccomplished by getting more and more of the reinforcer, morphine),not aversive (gratificati<strong>on</strong> of it is accomplished by getting less andless of the reinforcer, e.g., electric shock). Dysphoria (hypoch<strong>on</strong>driasis)in opioid-tolerant and physically dependent subjects has alsobeen observed by Haertzen and Hooks (1969) and by Martin et al.(1973). In an experimental study <strong>on</strong> six ex-addicts involving a tendayperiod of self-injecti<strong>on</strong> of heroin (earned by operating a counter),Mirin et al. (1976) observed that initially increased scores <strong>on</strong> “elatedmood” as well as decreased scores <strong>on</strong> “anxiety” and “somatic c<strong>on</strong>cern”tended to return to base<strong>line</strong> with c<strong>on</strong>tinued self-administrati<strong>on</strong> ofheroin, while c<strong>on</strong>comitantly, belligerence and negativism increased overbase<strong>line</strong>. Babor et al. (1976) found that the patients showed a tendencyto express more hostility after higher doses of heroin. It appears,therefore, that the comm<strong>on</strong>sense interpretati<strong>on</strong> of relapse, namely thequest for “euphoria,” is open to questi<strong>on</strong>.In 1948, Wikler proposed that in man relapse is due to evocati<strong>on</strong> bydrug-related envir<strong>on</strong>mental stimuli (“bad associates,” neighborhoodswhere opioids are illegally available) of fragments of the opioidabstinencesyndrome that had become classically c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed to suchstimuli during previous episodes of addicti<strong>on</strong>. As elaborated furtherover the years (Wikler 1961, 1965, 1973a,b,c) and presented in figure1, this hypothesis may be stated as follows. Reinforcement of opioidself-administrati<strong>on</strong> and of physiological events immediately precedingsuch self-administrati<strong>on</strong> is c<strong>on</strong>tingent up<strong>on</strong> the prior existence of175


FIGURE 1.–C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed theory of addicti<strong>on</strong> and relapse


“needs” (or “sources of reinforcement”) which are reduced by thepharmacological effects of the drug (e.g., heroin). The processes ofaddicti<strong>on</strong> and relapse may be divided into two successive phases,namely, “primary” and “sec<strong>on</strong>dary” pharmacological reinforcement. Inthe cases of young pers<strong>on</strong>s with prevailing moods of hypophoria andanxiety and with str<strong>on</strong>g needs to bel<strong>on</strong>g to some identifiable group,self-administrati<strong>on</strong> of heroin is often practiced in resp<strong>on</strong>se to thepressure of a heroin-using peer group in a social envir<strong>on</strong>ment in whichsuch a peer group exists. In primary pharmacological reinforcement,the pharmacological effects of heroin (miosis, respiratory depressi<strong>on</strong>,analgesia, etc.) are c<strong>on</strong>ceived as reflex resp<strong>on</strong>ses to the receptoracti<strong>on</strong>s of the drug, but its “direct” reinforcing properties are ascribedto acceptance by the peer groups and reducti<strong>on</strong> of hypophoria andanxiety.With repetiti<strong>on</strong> of self-administrati<strong>on</strong> of heroin, tolerance developsrapidly to the direct pharmacological effects of the drug and physicaldependence begins (dem<strong>on</strong>strable by administrati<strong>on</strong> of narcotic antag<strong>on</strong>istsafter <strong>on</strong>ly a few doses of morphine, heroin, or methad<strong>on</strong>e; see Wikleret al. 1953). The prevailing mood of the heroin user is now predominantlydysphoric, and withholding of heroin now has as its reflexc<strong>on</strong>sequence the appearance of signs of heroin abstinence (mydriasis,hyperpnea, hyperalgesia, etc.), which generate a new need, experiencedas abstinence distress. Because of previous reinforcement of heroinself-administrati<strong>on</strong>, the heroin user engaged in “hustling” for opioids--i.e., seeking “c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s,” earning or stealing m<strong>on</strong>ey, attempting tooutwit the law--which eventually becomes self-reinforcing, thoughinitially at least, it is maintained by acquiring heroin for selfadministrati<strong>on</strong>.In this stage, the “indirect” reinforcing properties ofheroin are attributed to its efficacy in suppressing abstinence distress.“<strong>On</strong> the street,” the heroin user who is both tolerant and physicallydependent frequently undergoes abstinence phenomena before he isable to obtain and self-administer the next dose. Given certain moreor less c<strong>on</strong>stant exteroceptive stimuli (street associates, neighborhoodcharacteristics, “strung out” addicts or leaders, “dope” talk) that aretemporally c<strong>on</strong>tiguous with such episodes, the cycle of heroin abstinenceand its terminati<strong>on</strong> can become classically c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed to such stimuli,while heroin-seeking behavior is operantly c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed. So<strong>on</strong>er orlater, the heroin user is detoxified, either in a hospital or in a jail.The well-known “acute” heroin-abstinence syndrome which is of relativelyshort durati<strong>on</strong> (about two to four weeks) is followed by the“protracted” abstinence syndrome which, in the case of morphineaddicti<strong>on</strong>, has been found to last about 30 weeks (Martin 1972). Atleast during this period, the detoxified heroin user may be said tohave still another new need. If, then, he is returned to his homeenvir<strong>on</strong>ment, he is exposed to the phase of sec<strong>on</strong>dary pharmacologicalreinforcement. In resp<strong>on</strong>se to the c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed exteroceptive stimulialready described, he may exhibit transient c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed abstinencechanges, experienced as yet another new need, namely “narcotichunger” or “craving.” Previously reinforced “hustling” is also likelyto appear now as a c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed resp<strong>on</strong>se (self-reinforcing) to these’same exteroceptive stimuli and lead to acquisiti<strong>on</strong> and self-administrati<strong>on</strong>of heroin. The reinforcing properties of heroin, ascribed to its efficacyin suppressing c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed abstinence distress, generate further selfadminstrati<strong>on</strong>of the drug with reestablishment of physical dependenceas in the “indirect” stage of primary pharmacological reinforcement,and the cycle of renewed c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing, detoxificati<strong>on</strong>, and sec<strong>on</strong>darypharmacological reinforcement with relapse is repeated again. Also, in177


the phase of primary pharmacological reinforcement, certain of theinteroceptive acti<strong>on</strong>s of opioids, not involved in the suppressi<strong>on</strong> ofabstinence phenomena, can acquire c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed properties, inasmuch asin a tolerant and physically dependent individual, they are oftenfollowed by abstinence phenomena before terminati<strong>on</strong> of the latter bythe next dose. Hence, in the phase of sec<strong>on</strong>dary pharmacologicalreinforcement, the usual effects of an opioid, administered for whateverreas<strong>on</strong>, may be followed by c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed abstinence phenomena, c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>edabstinence distress, and c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed hustling leading to selfadministrati<strong>on</strong>of heroin (relapse). Other interoceptive events canlikewise acquire the property of evoking c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed self-administrati<strong>on</strong>of opioids. For example, anxiety is frequently associated with theopioid-abstinence syndrome, and probably the two phenomena aremediated, in part, by the same central nervous system pathways.Hence, the occurrence of anxiety for whatever reas<strong>on</strong> l<strong>on</strong>g after detoxificati<strong>on</strong>may result in relapse.If it is accepted that c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing factors (classical and operant) andprotracted abstinence play an important role in relapse, then addicti<strong>on</strong>must be regarded as a disease sui generis, and regardless of antecedentetiological variables (e.g., premorbid pers<strong>on</strong>ality), its specificfeatures must be eliminated by appropriate procedures. As Wikler(1965) pointed out, mere detoxificati<strong>on</strong>, with or without c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>alpsychotherapy and enforced abstenti<strong>on</strong> from self-administrati<strong>on</strong> ofopioids, will not prevent relapse when the former addict returns to hishome envir<strong>on</strong>ment or other envir<strong>on</strong>ments where the c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed stimuliare present (drugs readily available; “pushers” and active addicts).What is needed in treatment after “detoxificati<strong>on</strong>” is active extincti<strong>on</strong>of both classically c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed abstinence and operantly c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>edopioid self-administrati<strong>on</strong>. This would require repeated elicitati<strong>on</strong> ofc<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed abstinence and repeated self-administrati<strong>on</strong> of opioidsunder c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s that prevent the reinforcing effects of opioids (producti<strong>on</strong>of “euphoria,” reestablishment of physical dependence).Under such c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s, c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed abstinence should eventually disappearand self-administrati<strong>on</strong> of opioids should eventually cease. Withthe introducti<strong>on</strong> of the orally effective, l<strong>on</strong>g-acting opioid antag<strong>on</strong>ist,cyclazocine, by Martin et al. (1966), it became possible to prevent thereinforcing effects of opioids by daily administrati<strong>on</strong> of cyclazocine. Ifformer addicts are maintained <strong>on</strong> blocking doses of an antag<strong>on</strong>ist for asufficient length of time (e.g., over 30 weeks) to permit disappearanceof protracted abstinence, and if active extincti<strong>on</strong> procedures arecarried out during this period (Wikler 1973d), then administrati<strong>on</strong> ofthe antag<strong>on</strong>ist may be disc<strong>on</strong>tinued, with the expectati<strong>on</strong> that relapsewill be much less likely to recur.178


THEORIES ON<strong>On</strong>e’sRelati<strong>on</strong>shipto Society


The Social Bases of<strong>Drug</strong>-Induced ExperiencesHoward S. Becker, Ph.D.Scientists no l<strong>on</strong>ger believe that a drug has a simple physiologicalacti<strong>on</strong>, essentially the same in all humans. Experimental, anthropological,and sociological evidence has c<strong>on</strong>vinced most observers that drugeffects vary greatly, depending <strong>on</strong> the physiology and psychology ofthe pers<strong>on</strong>s taking them, <strong>on</strong> their state when they ingest the drug,and <strong>on</strong> the social situati<strong>on</strong>. We can understand the social c<strong>on</strong>text ofdrug experiences better by showing how the nature of the experiencedepends <strong>on</strong> the amount and kind of knowledge available to the pers<strong>on</strong>taking the drug. Since distributi<strong>on</strong> is a functi<strong>on</strong> of the social organizati<strong>on</strong>of the groups in which drugs are used, drug experiences differwith differences in social organizati<strong>on</strong>. This paper will focus primarily<strong>on</strong> the illegal use of drugs for pleasure--and especially the use of LSDand marijuana--but will also discuss the use of medically prescribeddrugs by patients, and the involuntary ingesti<strong>on</strong> of drugs by victimsof chemical warfare. 2<strong>Drug</strong> effects vary from pers<strong>on</strong> to pers<strong>on</strong> and place to place becausethey almost always have more than <strong>on</strong>e effect. People may c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>allyfocus <strong>on</strong> and recognize <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e or a few of these effects, ignoringthe others as irrelevant. For example, most people think the effect ofaspirin is to c<strong>on</strong>trol pain; some know that it also reduces fever; fewthink of gastric irritati<strong>on</strong> as a typical effect, although it is. Thus1 This paper, prepared by Jean B. Wils<strong>on</strong> and reviewed by Howard S.Becker, c<strong>on</strong>sists of material taken from two previously published articleswritten by Dr. Becker. (1) “C<strong>on</strong>sciousness, Power and <strong>Drug</strong> Effects,”Journal of Psychedelic <strong>Drug</strong>s 6 (1974): 67-76. Reprinted with permissi<strong>on</strong>of STASH, Inc. Copyright © 1974. (2) “History, Culture and SubjectiveExperience: An Explorati<strong>on</strong> of the Social Bases of <strong>Drug</strong>-InducedExperiences,” Journal of Health and Social Behavior 8(1967):163-176.Reprinted with permissi<strong>on</strong> of the American Sociological Associati<strong>on</strong>.2 Material in this paragraph was taken from “C<strong>on</strong>sciousness, Power and<strong>Drug</strong> Effects,” p. 67. See footnote 1.180


users are likely to focus <strong>on</strong> the “beneficial” effects they seek and toignore others. 2DRUG EFFECTS, KNOWLEDGE,AND SOCIAL STRUCTUREWhen people take drugs, their subsequent experience is likely to beinfluenced by their ideas and beliefs about the drug (Becker 1967).What they know about the drug influences the way they use it, theway they interpret its manifold effects and resp<strong>on</strong>d to them, and theway they deal with the aftereffects. C<strong>on</strong>versely, what they do notknow also affects their experience, making both certain interpretati<strong>on</strong>sand acti<strong>on</strong>, based <strong>on</strong> that missing knowledge, impossible. (I use“knowledge” to refer to any ideas or beliefs about a drug that any<strong>on</strong>ec<strong>on</strong>cerned in its use, e.g., illicit drug sellers, physicians, researchers,or lay drug users, believes to have been tested against experience andthus to carry more weight than mere asserti<strong>on</strong>s of faith.) 2DOSAGEMany drug effects are dose related. The drug has <strong>on</strong>e set of effectsif you take X amount and quite different if you take 5X. Similarly,the effects vary depending <strong>on</strong> the means of taking a drug. How muchof a drug you take and how you take it depend <strong>on</strong> what you havelearned from sources you c<strong>on</strong>sider knowledgeable and trustworthy. 3Most people know, for instance, that the usual dose of aspirin tabletsis two and that they should be swallowed. <strong>On</strong> the other hand, fewpeople have readily available knowledge about the vast majority ofdrugs prescribed by doctors or about those illicitly obtained, such asLSD. Pers<strong>on</strong>s planning to take a drug (for whatever reas<strong>on</strong>) eitherresort to trial-and-error experimentati<strong>on</strong> or rely <strong>on</strong> sources theyc<strong>on</strong>sider reliable (scientists, physicians, or more experienced drugusers). These sources can usually tell the prospective user how muchto take and how to take it to achieve whatever the desired effect maybe (to c<strong>on</strong>trol blood clotting time, to get high, or whatever). 3With the understanding thus acquired, users take an amount whoseeffect they can more or less accurately predict. They usually findthis predicti<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>firmed, though the accuracy of c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al knowledgeneeds to be known. In this way, their access to knowledgeexerts a direct influence <strong>on</strong> their experience, allowing them to c<strong>on</strong>trolthe physiological input to that experience. 3This analysis supposes that users have complete c<strong>on</strong>trol over theamount they take. This is not always true, since a user may wish totake more than the physician will prescribe or a pharmacist sell. <strong>On</strong>2 Material in these paragraphs was taken from “C<strong>on</strong>sciousness, Power and<strong>Drug</strong> Effects,” p. 67. See footnote 1.3 Material in these paragraphs was taken from “C<strong>on</strong>sciousness, Power and<strong>Drug</strong> Effects.” pp. 67-68. See footnote 1.181


the other hand, doctors ordinarily prescribe and pharmacists sellamounts larger than recommended for <strong>on</strong>e-time use, so that users cantake more than they are “supposed to.” They can also purchasedrugs illicitly or “semilicitly” (e.g., from a friendly neighborhoodpharmacist). 3MAIN EFFECTSSocial scientists have shown how the definiti<strong>on</strong>s drug users apply totheir experience affect that experience. Pers<strong>on</strong>s suffering opiatewithdrawal will resp<strong>on</strong>d as “typical” addicts if they interpret theirdistress as opiate withdrawal, but not if they blame the pain <strong>on</strong> someother cause (e.g., recovery from surgery). Marijuana users mustlearn to interpret its subtle effect as being different from ordinaryexperience and as pleasurable before they “get high” (Becker 1953).Native Americans and Caucasians interpret peyote experiences differently(Aberle 1966), and LSD trips have been experienced as c<strong>on</strong>sciousnessexpansi<strong>on</strong>, transcendental religious experience, mock psychosis,or being high (Blum and Associates 1964). In short, users bring tobear, in interpreting their experience, knowledge and definiti<strong>on</strong>sderived from participati<strong>on</strong> in particular social groups. 4SIDE EFFECTSSide effects are not a medically or pharmacologically distinct categoryof reacti<strong>on</strong>s to drugs. Rather, they are effects not desired either bythe user or the pers<strong>on</strong> administering the drug. Both side effects andmain effects are thus socially defined categories. Mental disorientati<strong>on</strong>might be an unwanted side effect to a physician but a desired maineffect for an illicit drug user. 4A drug user’s knowledge, if adequate, lets him or her identify unwantedside effects and deal with them in a self-satisfactory way. Usersc<strong>on</strong>centrating <strong>on</strong> a desired main effect may not observe an unpleasantside effect or may not c<strong>on</strong>nect it with use of the drug. They interprettheir experience most adequately if those who prepare them for thedrug’s main effects likewise teach them the likely side effects and howto deal with them. Illicit drug users typically teach novices the sideeffects to look out for, give reassurance about their seriousness, andgive instructi<strong>on</strong>s in how to avoid or overcome them. 5LSDThe peculiar effects that lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD-25) has <strong>on</strong>the mind were discovered in 1938 by Albert Hoffman, who synthesizedthe drug in 1943. Following World War II, it came into use in3 Material in this paragraph was taken from “C<strong>on</strong>sciousness, Power and<strong>Drug</strong> Effects,” pp. 67-68. See footnote 1.4 Material in these paragraphs was taken from “C<strong>on</strong>sciousness, Power and<strong>Drug</strong> Effects,” pp. 68-69. See footnote 1.5 Material in this paragraph was taken from “C<strong>on</strong>sciousness, Power and<strong>Drug</strong> Effects,” p. 69. See footnote 1.182


psychiatry, both as a method of simulating psychosis for clinical studyand as a means of therapy (Unger 1966), and has been the subject ofc<strong>on</strong>troversy ever since. At <strong>on</strong>e extreme, Timothy Leary c<strong>on</strong>siders itsuse so beneficial that he has founded a new religi<strong>on</strong> in which it is themajor sacrament. At the other extreme, psychiatrists, police, andjournalists allege that LSD is extremely dangerous, that it producespsychosis, and that pers<strong>on</strong>s under its influence are likely to commitacts dangerous to themselves and others that they would not otherwisecommit.In spite of the great interest in the drug, I think it is fair to saythat the evidence of its danger is by no means decisive (Cohen 1960;Cohen and Ditman 1962, 1963; Frosch et al. 1965; Hoffer 1965; Rosenthal1964; Ungerleider et al. 1966). If the drug does prove to be thecause of a b<strong>on</strong>a fide psychosis, it will be the <strong>on</strong>ly case in whichany<strong>on</strong>e can state with authority that they have found the unique causeof any such phenomen<strong>on</strong>.But if we refuse to accept the explanati<strong>on</strong>s of others, we are obligatedto provide <strong>on</strong>e of our own. In what follows, I will c<strong>on</strong>sider the reportsof LSD-induced psychoses and try to relate them to what is known ofthe social psychology and sociology of drug use. By keeping in mindwhat is known of the influence that knowledge and social orientati<strong>on</strong>have <strong>on</strong> the effects--both main effects and side effects--that a druguser experiences, I hope to add both to our understanding of thecurrent c<strong>on</strong>troversy over LSD and to our general knowledge of thesocial character of drug use. In particular, I will make use of acomparis<strong>on</strong> between LSD use and marijuana use. The early history ofmarijuana use c<strong>on</strong>tains the same reports of “psychotic episodes” nowcurrent with respect to LSD. But reports of such episodes disappearedat the same time as the number of marijuana users increased greatly.I must add a cauti<strong>on</strong>ary disclaimer. I have not exhaustively examinedthe literature <strong>on</strong> LSD. What I have to say about it is necessarilyspeculative with respect to its effects; what I have to say about thec<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s under which it is used is also speculative, but is based inpart <strong>on</strong> interviews with a few users.The physiological effects of drugs can be ascertained by standardtechniques of physiological and pharmacological research. In c<strong>on</strong>trast,the subjective changes produced by a drug can be ascertained <strong>on</strong>ly byasking the subject how he or she feels. People who take drugs forrecreati<strong>on</strong>al purposes do so because they wish to experience just thosesubjective effects which they would either ignore or define as noxiousside effects if they were taking a drug for medicinal reas<strong>on</strong>s. Andbecause the use of drugs to induce a change in c<strong>on</strong>sciousness seems tomany immoral, drug users come to the attenti<strong>on</strong> of sociologists aslawbreakers.Nevertheless, some sociologists, anthropologists, and social psychologistshave investigated the problem of drug-induced subjective experiencein its own right. Taking their findings together the followingc<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>s seem justified (Becker 1963; Blum and Associates 1964; 66 Material <strong>on</strong> this page was taken from “History, Culture and SubjectiveExperience: An Explorati<strong>on</strong> of the Social Bases of <strong>Drug</strong>-Induced Experiences.”See footnote 1 .183


Lindesmith 1947; Metzner et al. 1965; Aberle 1966; Schachter and Singer1962; Nowlis and Nowlis 1956). (1) Many drugs, including those usedto produce changes in subjective experience, have a great variety ofeffects, and the user may be unaware of some of them, or may notrecognize them as attributable to use of the drug. (2) The effects ofthe same drug may be experienced differently by different people orby the same people at different times. (3) Since recreati<strong>on</strong>al userstake drugs in order to achieve some subjective state not ordinarilyavailable to them, they expect and are most likely to experience thoseeffects which are different from ordinary patterns. Thus, distorti<strong>on</strong>sin percepti<strong>on</strong> of time and space and shifts in judgment of the importanceand meaning of ordinary events are the most comm<strong>on</strong>ly reported effects.(4) Any of a great variety of effects may be singled out by the useras desirable or pleasurable. Even effects which seem to the uninitiatedto be uncomfortable, unpleasant, or frightening--perceptual distorti<strong>on</strong>sor visual and auditory hallucinati<strong>on</strong>s--can be defined by users as agoal to be sought (Becker 1963). (5) How people experience theeffects of a drug depends greatly <strong>on</strong> the way others define thoseeffects for them (Becker 1963; Blum and Associates 1964; Lindesmith1947; Metzner et al. 1965; Aberle 1966; Schachter and Singer 1962;Nowlis and Nowlis 1956). If others whom users believe to be knowledgeablesingle out certain effects as characteristic and dismiss others,they are likely to notice those same effects as characteristic of theirown experience. If certain effects are defined as transitory, usersare apt to believe that those effects will go away.The scientific literature and, even more, the popular press frequentlystate that recreati<strong>on</strong>al drug use produces a psychosis. What writersseem to mean by “psychosis” is a mental disturbance of some unspecifiedkind, involving hallucinati<strong>on</strong>s, an inability to c<strong>on</strong>trol <strong>on</strong>e’s streamof thought, and a tendency to engage in socially inappropriate behaviorIn additi<strong>on</strong>, and perhaps most important, psychosis is thought to be astate that will last l<strong>on</strong>g bey<strong>on</strong>d the specific event that provoked it.Verified reports of drug-induced psychoses are scarcer than <strong>on</strong>e mightthink (Cohen 1960; Cohen and Ditman 1962, 1963; Frosch et al. 1965;Hoffer 1965; Rosenthal 1964; Ungerleider et al. 1966; Bromberg 1939;Curtis 1939; Nesbitt 1940). Nevertheless, let us assume that thesereports represent an interpretati<strong>on</strong> of something that really happened.What kind of event can we imagine to have occurred that might havebeen interpreted as a “psychotic episode”?The most likely sequence of events is this. An inexperienced user hascertain unusual subjective experiences, which he or she may or maynot attribute to having taken the drug, such as a distorted percepti<strong>on</strong>of space, so that it is difficult to climb stairs. The user’s train ofthought may be so c<strong>on</strong>fused that it is impossible to carry <strong>on</strong> a normalc<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>. The user may suspect that the way he or she sees orhears things is quite different from the way others see and hear them.Whether or not the user attributes what is happening to the drug, theexperiences are apt to be upsetting. <strong>On</strong>e of the ways we know thatwe are normal human beings is that our perceptual world seems to be 66 Material <strong>on</strong> this page was taken from “History, Culture and SubjectiveExperience: An Explorati<strong>on</strong> of the Social Bases of <strong>Drug</strong>-Induced Experiences.”See footnote 1.184


pretty much the same as other people’s. If this is no l<strong>on</strong>ger true--ifwe find our subjective state so altered that our percepti<strong>on</strong>s are nol<strong>on</strong>ger like other people’s, we may think we have become insane. Thisis precisely what may happen to the inexperienced drug user. Moreover,this interpretati<strong>on</strong> implies that the change is irreversible or, atleast, that normality is not going to be restored easily. The drugexperience, perhaps originally intended as a momentary entertainment,now looms as a momentous event which will disrupt <strong>on</strong>e’s life, possiblypermanently. Faced with this c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>, the user develops a fullblownanxiety attack, but it is an attack caused by the reacti<strong>on</strong> to thedrug experience rather than a direct c<strong>on</strong>sequence of the drug itself.(It is interesting that, in published reports of LSD psychoses, acuteanxiety attacks appear as the largest category of untoward reacti<strong>on</strong>s[Frosch et al. 1965; Cohen and Ditman 1963; Ungerleider et al. 1966;Bromberg 1939].) Of course, l<strong>on</strong>g-time users may have similar experiencesif they take a higher dosage than they are used to or becauseillicitly purchased drugs may vary greatly in strength.The scientific literature does not report any verified cases of peopleacting <strong>on</strong> their distorted percepti<strong>on</strong>s so as to harm themselves orothers, but such cases have been reported in the press. If usershave, for instance, stepped out of a sec<strong>on</strong>d story window, deludedinto thinking it <strong>on</strong>ly a few feet to the ground (Cohen 1960; Hoffer1965). it would be because they had failed to make the necessarycorrecti<strong>on</strong> for the drug-induced distorti<strong>on</strong> rather than because of ananxiety attack. Experienced users assert, however, that such correcti<strong>on</strong>scan be made and that they can c<strong>on</strong>trol their thinking and acti<strong>on</strong>sso as to behave appropriately (Becker 1963).Thus the most likely interpretati<strong>on</strong> we can make of the drug-inducedpsychoses reported is that they are either severe anxiety reacti<strong>on</strong>s toan event interpreted and experienced as insanity, or failures of theuser to correct for the perceptual distorti<strong>on</strong>s caused by the drug.While there are no reliable figures, it is obvious that a very largenumber of people use recreati<strong>on</strong>al drugs, primarily marijuana and LSD.<strong>On</strong>e might suppose, then, that a great many people would have disquietingsymptoms and that many would decide they had g<strong>on</strong>e crazy andthus have a drug-induced anxiety attack. But while there must bemore such occurrences than are reported in the professi<strong>on</strong>al literature,it is unlikely that there are any large number. Since the psychoticreacti<strong>on</strong> stems from a definiti<strong>on</strong> of the drug-induced experience, theexplanati<strong>on</strong> of this paradox must lie in the availability of competingdefiniti<strong>on</strong>s of the subjective states produced by drugs.Competing definiti<strong>on</strong>s come to users from other users who are known tohave had sufficient experience with the drug to speak with authority.New users know that the drug does not produce permanent disablingdamage in all cases, for they can see that other users do not sufferfrom it. The questi<strong>on</strong> remains, of course, whether the drug may notproduce damage in some cases, however rare, and whether a particularpers<strong>on</strong> may be <strong>on</strong>e of those cases. 66 Material <strong>on</strong> this page was taken from “History, Culture and SubjectiveExperience: An Explorati<strong>on</strong> of the Social Bases of <strong>Drug</strong>-Induced Experiences.”See footnote 1.185


When users experience disturbing effects, other users typically assurethem that the change in their subjective experience is neither rare nordangerous. They may, for instance, know of an antidote for thefrightening effects. They talk reassuringly about their own experiences,“normalizing” the frightening symptom by treating it as temporary.They maintain surveillance over affected users, preventing anyphysically or socially dangerous activity. They show them how toallow for the perceptual distorti<strong>on</strong> the drug causes and how to manageinteracti<strong>on</strong> with n<strong>on</strong>users. They redefine the experience the novice ishaving as desirable rather than frightening, as the end for which thedrug is taken (New York City Mayor’s Committee <strong>on</strong> Marihuana 1944;Becker 1963). What they say carries c<strong>on</strong>victi<strong>on</strong>, because the novicecan see that it is not some idiosyncratic belief but is instead culturallyshared. He or she thus has an alternative to defining the experienceas “going crazy,” and may decide that it was not so bad after all.We do not know how often this mechanism comes into play or howeffective it is in preventing untoward psychological reacti<strong>on</strong>s. However,in the case of marijuana, at least, the paucity of reported cases ofpermanent damage coupled with the undoubted increase in use suggeststhat it may be effective.For such a mechanism to operate, a number of c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s must be met.First, the drug must not produce permanent damage to the mind.Sec<strong>on</strong>d, users of the drug must share a set of understandings--aculture--which includes, in additi<strong>on</strong> to material <strong>on</strong> how to obtain andingest the drug, definiti<strong>on</strong>s of the typical effects, the typical courseof the experience, the impermanence of the effects, and a descripti<strong>on</strong>of methods for dealing with some<strong>on</strong>e who suffers an anxiety attackbecause of drug use or attempts to act <strong>on</strong> the basis of distorted percepti<strong>on</strong>s.Third, the drug should ordinarily be used in group settings, whereother users can present the definiti<strong>on</strong>s of the drug-using culture tothe pers<strong>on</strong> whose inner experience is so unusual as to provoke use ofthe comm<strong>on</strong>sense category of insanity. <strong>Drug</strong>s for which technologyand custom produce group use should produce a lower incidence of“psychotic episodes.”The last two c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s suggest, as is the case, that marijuana, surroundedby an elaborate culture and ordinarily used in group settings,should produce few psychotic episodes. I will discuss evidence <strong>on</strong> thispoint later.Users suffering from drug-induced anxiety may also come into c<strong>on</strong>tactwith n<strong>on</strong>users who will offer definiti<strong>on</strong>s, depending <strong>on</strong> their ownperspective and experience, that may validate the diagnosis of “goingcrazy” and thus prol<strong>on</strong>g the episode, possibly producing relativelypermanent disability. These n<strong>on</strong>users include family members andpolice, but most important am<strong>on</strong>g them are psychiatrists and psychiatricallyoriented physicians.”6 Material <strong>on</strong> this page was taken from “History, Culture and SubjectiveExperience: An Explorati<strong>on</strong> of the Social Bases of <strong>Drug</strong>-Induced Experiences.”See footnote 1.186


Medical knowledge about the recreati<strong>on</strong>al use of drugs is spotty.Little research has been d<strong>on</strong>e or--as in the case of LSD--its c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>sare not clear, and what is known is not at the fingertips of physicianswho do not specialize in the area. Psychiatrists are not anxious totreat drug users, so few of them have accumulated any clinical experiencewith the phenomen<strong>on</strong>. Nevertheless, a user who develops severeand unc<strong>on</strong>trollable anxiety will probably be brought to a psychiatrichospital, to an emergency room where a psychiatric resident will becalled, or to a private psychiatrist (Ungerleider et al. 1966).Physicians, c<strong>on</strong>fr<strong>on</strong>ted with a case of drug-induced anxiety and lackingspecific knowledge of its character or proper treatment, rely <strong>on</strong> a kindof generalized diagnosis. They reas<strong>on</strong> that people probably do not usedrugs unless they are suffering from a severe underlying pers<strong>on</strong>alitydisturbance; that use of the drug may allow repressed c<strong>on</strong>flicts tocome into the open where they will prove unmanageable; that the drugin this way provokes a true psychosis; and, therefore, that the patientc<strong>on</strong>fr<strong>on</strong>ting them is psychotic. Furthermore, even though the effectsof the drug wear off, the psychosis may not, for the repressed psychologicalproblems it has brought to the surface may not recede.<strong>On</strong> the basis of such a diagnosis, the physician hospitalizes the patientfor observati<strong>on</strong> and prepares, where possible, for l<strong>on</strong>g-term therapydesigned to repair the damage d<strong>on</strong>e to the psychic defenses or to dealwith the c<strong>on</strong>flict. Both hospitalizati<strong>on</strong> and therapy are likely to reinforcethe definiti<strong>on</strong> of the drug experience as insanity, for in both thepatient will be required to “understand” that he or she is mentally illas a prec<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> for return to the world (Szasz 1961).Physicians, then, do not treat the anxiety attack as a localized phenomen<strong>on</strong>,to be treated in a symptomatic way, but as an outbreak of aserious disease heretofore hidden. They may thus prol<strong>on</strong>g the seriouseffects bey<strong>on</strong>d the time they might have lasted had the user insteadcome into c<strong>on</strong>tact with other users. This analysis, of course, isfrankly speculative; what is required is more study of the way physicianstreat cases of the kind described and, especially, comparativestudies of the effects of treatment of drug-induced anxiety attacks byphysicians and by drug users.A number of variables, then, affect the character of drug-inducedexperiences. It remains to show that the experiences themselves areapt to vary according to when they occur in the history of use of agiven drug in a society. In particular, it seems likely that the experienceof acute anxiety caused by drug use will so vary.Let us suppose that some<strong>on</strong>e in a society discovers, rediscovers, orinvents a drug which has the ability to alter subjective experience indesirable ways. This becomes known to increasing numbers of people,and the drug itself simultaneously becomes available, al<strong>on</strong>g with theinformati<strong>on</strong> needed to make its use effective. Use increases, but usersdo not have a sufficient amount of experience with the drug to form astable c<strong>on</strong>cepti<strong>on</strong> of it. No drug-using culture exists, and there isthus no authoritative alternative with which to counter the possible 66 Material <strong>on</strong> this page was taken from “History, Culture and SubjectiveExperience: An Explorati<strong>on</strong> of the Social Bases of <strong>Drug</strong>-Induced Experiences.”See footnote 1.187


definiti<strong>on</strong>, when and if it comes to mind, of the drug experience asmadness. “Psychotic episodes” occur frequently.But individuals accumulate experience with the drug and communicatetheir experiences to <strong>on</strong>e another. C<strong>on</strong>sensus develops about thedrug’s subjective effects, their durati<strong>on</strong>, proper dosages, predictabledangers and how they may be avoided. All these points become mattersof comm<strong>on</strong> knowledge, available to the novice user as well as theexperienced <strong>on</strong>e. A culture exists. “Psychotic episodes” occur lessfrequently in proporti<strong>on</strong> to the growth of the culture.Is this model a useful guide to reality? The <strong>on</strong>ly drug for which thereis sufficient evidence to attempt an evaluati<strong>on</strong> is marijuana. Eventhere the evidence is equivocal, but it is c<strong>on</strong>sistent with the model.Marijuana first came into use in the United States in the 1920s andearly 30s. and all reports of psychosis associated with its use datefrom approximately that period (Bromberg 1939; Curtis 1939; Nesbitt1940)--before there was a fully formed drug-using culture. Thesubsequent disappearance of reports of psychosis thus fits the model.It is, of course, a shaky index, for it depends as much <strong>on</strong> the reportinghabits of physicians as <strong>on</strong> the true incidence of cases, but it isthe <strong>on</strong>ly thing available.The psychoses described also fit the model, insofar as there is anyclear indicati<strong>on</strong> of a drug-induced effect. The best evidence comesfrom the 31 cases reported by Bromberg. Where the detail givenallows judgment, it appears that all but <strong>on</strong>e stemmed from the pers<strong>on</strong>’sinability to deal with either the perceptual distorti<strong>on</strong> caused by thedrug or with the panic created by the thought of losing <strong>on</strong>e’s mind(Bromberg 1939, pp. 6-7).The evidence cited is extremely scanty, which leaves the final questi<strong>on</strong>,then, whether the model can be used to interpret current reports ofLSD-induced psychosis. Are these episodes the c<strong>on</strong>sequence of anearly stage in the development of an LSD-using culture? Will thenumber of episodes decrease while the number of users rises, as themodel leads us to predict?We cannot predict the history of LSD by direct analogy to the historyof marijuana, for a number of important c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s may vary, andevidence <strong>on</strong> a number of important factors is still highly inc<strong>on</strong>clusive.For example, there is a great deal of c<strong>on</strong>troversy as to whether or notLSD has any dem<strong>on</strong>strated causal relati<strong>on</strong> to psychosis, apart from thedefiniti<strong>on</strong>s users impose <strong>on</strong> their experience. My own opini<strong>on</strong> is thatwhile LSD may be more powerful in its effects than other drugs thathave been studied, the cases in the literature support the belief thatmost of the psychotic episodes are panic reacti<strong>on</strong>s to the drug experienceoccasi<strong>on</strong>ed by the users’ belief that they have lost their minds,or further disturbances am<strong>on</strong>g people already quite disturbed.Is there an LSD-using culture? Here again, discussi<strong>on</strong> must be tentative.It appears likely, however, that such a culture is in an early 66 Material <strong>on</strong> this page was taken from “History, Culture and SubjectiveExperience: An Explorati<strong>on</strong> of the Social Bases of <strong>Drug</strong>-Induced Experiences.”See footnote 1.188


stage of development, and that users who are part of that culture arehelped to cope with their experiences. For example, the noti<strong>on</strong> that a“bad trip” can be brought to a speedy c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> by taking Thorazinehas spread. Knowledge of other safeguards is also becoming morewidely known. Insofar as this emergent culture spreads so that mostor all users share the belief that LSD does not cause insanity, theknowledge about dosage, effects, and so <strong>on</strong>, as well as the incidenceof “psychoses” should drop markedly or disappear. 6<strong>On</strong> the other hand, the ease with which LSD can be taken may negatethe helpful influence of an LSD culture. No special paraphernalia isnecessary, no special technique. A sugar cube can be swallowedwithout instructi<strong>on</strong>. C<strong>on</strong>sequently it is possible that many people willtake the drug without having acquired the presently developing culturalunderstanding, that many users will be people with no previous experienceof recreati<strong>on</strong>al drug use, and that they will take it without thepresence of supportive, experienced users. Changing mores aboutyouth use may add to the number of people who take the drug withoutbeing indoctrinated in the new cultural definiti<strong>on</strong>s, in which case thenumber of episodes may go up. 6We have been talking of drug use in which taking the drug is a matterof choice and in which the desired effect is a subjective <strong>on</strong>e. Butpeople also delegate c<strong>on</strong>trol of their drug use to others, most comm<strong>on</strong>lyto physicians. When people take drugs prescribed to them by doctors,they do not rely <strong>on</strong> trial and error or a drug culture for knowledgec<strong>on</strong>cerning dosage, main effects, and side effects, but usually <strong>on</strong> thedoctor. While the doctor wants to alleviate some dangerous c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>the patient is suffering from, doctor’s and patient’s desires do notnecessarily coincide. Moreover, the doctor may not give patientssufficient informati<strong>on</strong> to anticipate the effects a drug may have, withthe result that patients are sometimes unnecessarily frightened or maysuffer dangerous reacti<strong>on</strong>s without c<strong>on</strong>necting them with the drug.The doctor may not give patients all the informati<strong>on</strong> he or she has forfear that the patient will disobey orders (Lennard 1972). Sometimesthe doctor does not have adequate informati<strong>on</strong> about the experience thedrug will produce. In either case, the drug experience is amplifiedand the chance of serious pathology increases. The patient, notknowing what is likely to happen, cannot recognize the event when itoccurs and cannot resp<strong>on</strong>d adequately or present the problem to anexpert who can provide an adequate resp<strong>on</strong>se. 7CONTROL BY EXTERNAL AGENTSPeople sometimes find themselves required to ingest drugs involuntarily.In some instances, the agent administers the drug believing it to befor the good of the patient, as when a doctor gives medicine to ababy. who cannot resist. Or the agent may administer drugs “for the6 Material in these paragraphs was taken from “History, Culture andSubjective Experience: An Explorati<strong>on</strong> of the Social Bases of <strong>Drug</strong>-Induced Experiences.” See footnote 1 .7 Material in this paragraph was taken from “C<strong>on</strong>sciousness, Power and<strong>Drug</strong> Effects,” pp. 71-72. See footnote 1.189


good of the community,” as when people with tuberculosis or leprosyare medicated to prevent them from infecting others (Roth 1963). 8But sometimes the external agent’s purposes c<strong>on</strong>flict directly withthose of the user, as when people find themselves the victims ofchemical warfare. Those who administer drugs to involuntary usersare either indifferent about providing recipients with any knowledgeabout it or actively attempt to prevent them from getting that knowledge.Where destructi<strong>on</strong> or incapacitati<strong>on</strong> of the target populati<strong>on</strong> isthe aim, the agent may try to c<strong>on</strong>ceal the fact that a drug is beingadministered. In this way, the agent hopes to prevent the taking ofcountermeasures and, in additi<strong>on</strong> to the drug’s specific physiologicaleffects, create panic at the <strong>on</strong>slaught of the unknown. 8CONCLUSIONIf drug experiences somehow reflect or are related to social settings,we must specify the settings in which drugs are taken and the specificeffect of those settings <strong>on</strong> the experiences of the participants. Thisanalysis suggests that it is useful to look at the role of power andknowledge in those settings: knowledge of how to take the drugs andwhat to expect when <strong>on</strong>e does, and power over their distributi<strong>on</strong>, theacquisiti<strong>on</strong> of informati<strong>on</strong> about them, and the decisi<strong>on</strong> to take or notto take them. The need for further research extends both to the licitand illicit use of drugs, to the danger of taking drugs for recreati<strong>on</strong>alpurposes (including “prescribed” drugs), into the profit orientati<strong>on</strong> ofpharmaceutical manufacturers, and to the sometimes inadequate knowledgeand sometimes ambivalent motives of doctors who share or do notshare their knowledge with their patients. 98 Material in these paragraphs was taken from “C<strong>on</strong>sciousness, Powerand <strong>Drug</strong> Effects,” pp. 74-75. See footnote 1.9 Material in this paragraph was taken from “C<strong>on</strong>sciousness, Power and<strong>Drug</strong> Effects,” p. 75. See footnote 1.190


<strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong> asLearned BehaviorCalvin J. Frederick, Ph.D.Although there are recognized physiological factors involved in hardcoreaddicti<strong>on</strong>, the sine qua n<strong>on</strong> for drug abuse/addicti<strong>on</strong> is to be found inlearning theory. A variety of comp<strong>on</strong>ents, such as cultural envir<strong>on</strong>ment,availability, exposure to drug use patterns, and self-perceivedneeds, c<strong>on</strong>tribute to the acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of a drug habit. The fact thatphysical relief occurs in the addictive cycle cannot be separated fromthe psychological aspects which accompany it. The impact of profoundrelief adds appreciably to the learning process. What was so satisfyingduring the initial period of tensi<strong>on</strong> reducti<strong>on</strong> will be likely to repeatitself under similar circumstances <strong>on</strong> the next occasi<strong>on</strong>. A learningframework can explain not <strong>on</strong>ly drug abuse/addicti<strong>on</strong> but other relatedbehaviors as well. This has been noted previously by the author(1972, 1973), by Frederick and Resnik (1971), and by Frederick et al.(1973).The learning theory paradigm tends to follow a particular sequence.When an intense stimulus situati<strong>on</strong> remains relatively unchanged, it wil<strong>line</strong>vitably be followed by anxiety, a state which requires diminuti<strong>on</strong>.In terms of traditi<strong>on</strong>al reinforcement theory, anxiety is a sec<strong>on</strong>daryreinforcer, since the attainment of the goal object (drug) possesses itsown reinforcing properties. This occurs because, through past experience,drug ingesti<strong>on</strong> has become associated with a primary drive state,such as a physiological need or imbalance. For our purposes here,any stimulus c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> which c<strong>on</strong>tributes to this drive state is sufficientto support the noti<strong>on</strong> of drug abuse as learned behavior. The resp<strong>on</strong>sethat follows is likely to become progressively more prominent as aspecific act which brings results, since it evokes dramatic attenti<strong>on</strong>al<strong>on</strong>g with a need for drug ingesti<strong>on</strong>. The ensuing tensi<strong>on</strong> reducti<strong>on</strong>from the drug brings relief and reinforcement of the act which inducedthe administrati<strong>on</strong> of the drug in the first place. As this process isrepeated, the sequence of events is shortened in time because thedecrease in tensi<strong>on</strong> reducti<strong>on</strong> becomes so powerfully reinforced, andevery point in the sequence becomes an associative cue for the ultimaterelief. With each reinforcement, the act of substance abuse becomesstrengthened, and the likelihood of its recurrence under similar c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>sis increased. In cases when the tensi<strong>on</strong> is particularly acute,such an act may be learned very quickly. The paradigm looks like191


this: stimulus situati<strong>on</strong> (stress, shame, guilt) tensi<strong>on</strong> (anxiety)addictive acts (drug seeking/receiving/ingesting) tensi<strong>on</strong> reducti<strong>on</strong>stimulus situati<strong>on</strong>--and the cycle repeats itself.Other authors (Wikler 1965, 1973b; Jaffe 1970a; Crowley 1972), havealso commented up<strong>on</strong> the learning comp<strong>on</strong>ents inherent in drug abuse.A descripti<strong>on</strong> of the theoretical c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> of each of the majorelements in this treatise can not <strong>on</strong>ly illustrate the theory but canmake each of the five elements of drug abuse--initiati<strong>on</strong> of use, c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong>,shift or transiti<strong>on</strong> from use to abuse, cessati<strong>on</strong>, and relapse--more understandable, especially to the therapist. <strong>Drug</strong> abuse isexpressed as a ratio of destructive factors to c<strong>on</strong>structive factorsoperating in the pers<strong>on</strong>ality. These factors are multiplicative functi<strong>on</strong>sof each other as they c<strong>on</strong>tribute to drug behavior. This may beillustrated as follows:Ba = Pd x Md x Hd x RdPC x MC x Hc x Rcor Ba =destructive factorsc<strong>on</strong>structive factorswhereBa =Pd =Md =Hd =<strong>Drug</strong> addicti<strong>on</strong> or abusePers<strong>on</strong>ality comp<strong>on</strong>ents that are weak and destructiveMotivati<strong>on</strong> or strength of drive state toward destructive,undesirable behaviorHabits as a functi<strong>on</strong> of the number of reinforcementsassociated with drug-taking behaviorRd =Risk-takingstimuli associated with drug ingesti<strong>on</strong>Pc =MC =Hc =Rc =Pers<strong>on</strong>ality comp<strong>on</strong>ents that are str<strong>on</strong>g and c<strong>on</strong>structiveMotivati<strong>on</strong> or strength of drive state toward c<strong>on</strong>structive,desirable behaviorHabits as a functi<strong>on</strong> of the number of reinforcementsassociated with favorable resp<strong>on</strong>ses to stressRisk-taking stimuli associated with c<strong>on</strong>structive resp<strong>on</strong>sesLet the value of 1.0 be c<strong>on</strong>sidered the point where drug addicti<strong>on</strong> orabuse will definitely occur; zero represents the value where no likelihoodof such behavior obtains. As the proporti<strong>on</strong> moves upward from theequally weighted value of 50 percent (0.50). the probability of drugabuse, thereby, increases as the value of 1.0 is approached. C<strong>on</strong>versely,the likelihood of drug abuse occurring decreases proporti<strong>on</strong>atelyas the numerical value approaches zero. Each of the variableslisted in the formula will possess its own weights, according to pastexperience and those influences currently operating in the life of theindividual.Since destructive and c<strong>on</strong>structive factors in drug addicti<strong>on</strong> or abusemay be expressed illustratively as a ratio, str<strong>on</strong>g pers<strong>on</strong>ality andmotivati<strong>on</strong>al variables predominate as c<strong>on</strong>structive forces in the denominator,while habits and motivati<strong>on</strong> are equal in both the numerator anddenominator. In order to show the learning principles involved, let us192


assume that there is an equal chance for the growth of destructive andc<strong>on</strong>structive factors which c<strong>on</strong>tribute to the development of drug-relatedbehavior. A 50-percent probability represents this situati<strong>on</strong> numerically.This may be shown by substituting arbitrary values for each of thevariables in the formula, as follows:When the risk-taking aspects of the destructive factors increase evenslightly, there is a growth in the likelihood that drug abuse willdevelop. This will obtain even when other factors remain the same asthose in the situati<strong>on</strong> noted above, with a 50-percent probability in thelevel of occurrence. This change may be dem<strong>on</strong>strated by increasingthe risk factor (Rd) by <strong>on</strong>e point in the formula, since the ratio valuenow becomes 0.60, which is closer to 1.0 than is 0.50.C<strong>on</strong>versely, when the c<strong>on</strong>structive aspects of risk are strengthened by<strong>on</strong>e point in the formula, the likelihood of drug abuse developingdecreases, inasmuch as the resulting proporti<strong>on</strong> of 0.42 is closer tozero than is 0.50.Obviously, when the other factors in the equati<strong>on</strong> change throughreinforcement or n<strong>on</strong>reinforcement, the ratio changes accordingly. If<strong>on</strong>e or more of the variables is weakened through n<strong>on</strong>reinforcement,the scales are tipped in either a destructive or c<strong>on</strong>structive directi<strong>on</strong>,depending up<strong>on</strong> the total value of the proporti<strong>on</strong>. For purposes ofsimplificati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>on</strong>ly the risk factor has been varied here to illustratethe importance of a single value in the ratio. Moreover, factors otherthan those noted may be involved, although these seem to be the mostprominent, especially if envir<strong>on</strong>mental influences are subsumed underthose listed. Risk-taking behavior, in particular, is likely to beresp<strong>on</strong>sive to envir<strong>on</strong>mental stimuli, for example. The abuser/addictshould be aware of the increase in risk-taking behavior as a destructiveforce since mere geographic placement into an old, familiar envir<strong>on</strong>mentcan often stimulate the recurrence of a previous drug problem. Thisis due to the strength of past associati<strong>on</strong>s as they c<strong>on</strong>tribute to oldhabits of drug use.Substitute medicati<strong>on</strong>s, such as methad<strong>on</strong>e, may alter the balance ofdestructive factors in the behavioral equati<strong>on</strong> by reducing anxiety anda tendency toward depressi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> a tentative basis. Frederick et al.(1973) report that clinical depressi<strong>on</strong> recurs during methad<strong>on</strong>e abstinence,and, hence, the abuser/addict and the therapist should becognizant of this fact as well as of the temporary palliative effects ofdrug substitutes like methad<strong>on</strong>e. This must be taken into account inthe readjustment process of a therapy program. Substitutes in effectiveliving can be supplied, rather than replacing <strong>on</strong>e drug withanother, particularly at a point in treatment when the habit has begunto lose strength. The relearning process affects every facet of thetreatment program through the same principles by which abuse/addicti<strong>on</strong>develops and c<strong>on</strong>tinues.193


The strengthening of the addictive act is not merely a direct functi<strong>on</strong>of the number of reinforcements. Reinforcement of drug resp<strong>on</strong>ses <strong>on</strong>an intermittent basis can lead to greater c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing and more resistanceto extincti<strong>on</strong> than can reinforcement of every resp<strong>on</strong>se. Anticipati<strong>on</strong>of the receipt of a drug can stimulate further drug-seeking/receivingbehavior. In extinguishing the drug resp<strong>on</strong>se, the intermittentreinforcement principle holds for punishment as well as for reward.<strong>On</strong>ce drug abuse/addicti<strong>on</strong> has begun, it c<strong>on</strong>stitutes a punishing stateof affairs when the pers<strong>on</strong> goes without the drug. Not receiving thisstr<strong>on</strong>g reinforcer <strong>on</strong> each anticipated occasi<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tributes both to thedrive to use it and to its suppressi<strong>on</strong> and extincti<strong>on</strong>. Going “coldturkey” exemplifies this. The heightened drive state increases thelikelihood of the addictive resp<strong>on</strong>se when the strength of the habitremains c<strong>on</strong>stant or is even slightly reduced, depending up<strong>on</strong> thevalue of the drive state. N<strong>on</strong>reinforcement of the habit, which istantamount to punishment, causes it to weaken. After the drivereaches its peak, a decrement occurs in the strength of the habit,resulting in a diminuti<strong>on</strong> of the addictive resp<strong>on</strong>se. General reinforcementprinciples are employed to account for the various facets of thedevelopment of drug usage for purposes of clarificati<strong>on</strong>, although it isevident that intermittent reward and punishment also operate in c<strong>on</strong>certwith the theory as out<strong>line</strong>d. The entire treatment spectrum, includingadministrati<strong>on</strong> of substituted medicati<strong>on</strong>s, milieu therapy, and psychotherapy,is governed by these principles, as well.194


Psychosocial Theory of<strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>A Psychodynamic ApproachHerbert Hendin, M.D.A psychodynamic approach to psychosocial problems seeks to explainthe interrelati<strong>on</strong> between social and psychological variables in producingadaptive and maladaptive behavior. It relies <strong>on</strong> psychodynamic studyof a representative number of individuals to assess the meaning ofthese variables.Psychosocial theory without a psychodynamic base has increasinglytended to reduce emoti<strong>on</strong>al illness to the c<strong>on</strong>sequences of such socialfactors as poverty, sex, and race. Ec<strong>on</strong>omic determinism, sexism, andracism, however, cannot explain the great variati<strong>on</strong>s in the abilities ofpeople to deal with the problems of class, sex, and race. The psychologyof a c<strong>on</strong>siderable number of any group must be evaluated tounderstand the actual impact of caste or class <strong>on</strong> the character andadaptati<strong>on</strong> of the rich or the poor. <strong>On</strong> the basis of work with PuertoRican families, Oscar Lewis (1966) gave us an illuminating picture ofthe “culture of poverty.” Yet any<strong>on</strong>e who works with poor Hispanic,poor white, and poor black families quickly becomes familiar with howdifferent the culture of poverty is in each of these groups, let al<strong>on</strong>ehow varied the individual and family resp<strong>on</strong>se is to the fact of poverty.In the case of the drug problem, social variables ranging from sexualactivity to associati<strong>on</strong> with friends who use drugs have been shown tobe related to drug use (Kandel 1973; Jessor and Jessor 1975). Friends,sexual activity, and drug use, however, are all part of an individual’stotal adaptati<strong>on</strong>, and their interrelated significance for this adaptati<strong>on</strong>must be understood in order to establish any meaningful psychosocialperspective.Psychodynamic investigati<strong>on</strong> employing unstructured interview sessi<strong>on</strong>sthat rely <strong>on</strong> free associati<strong>on</strong>s, associative linkages, transference reacti<strong>on</strong>s,omissi<strong>on</strong>s, dreams, and fantasies provides a uniquely sensitivemethod for establishing individual and family dynamics (Hendin 1964;Hendin et al. 1965). Early psychodynamic studies of drug abuse,however, ignored social and even familial factors and viewed the abuserin a psychodynamic vacuum. All types of drug craving were seen as195


epresenting a single disease (Rado 1933) characterized as an impulsedisorder in which the “ego is subjugated” by an “archaic need for oralgratificati<strong>on</strong>” (Rado 1926, 1933; Fenichel 1945). In the past twodecades we have become aware of the adaptive or defensive functi<strong>on</strong>sof drug use and abuse (Ausubel 1961; Alien and West 1968; Cuarner1966; Wieder and Kaplan 1969; Hendin 1975). We have also come torealize that heroin, marijuana, LSD, and amphetamines appeal to differentkinds of people according to the specific psychopharmacologicaleffects of each drug (Wieder and Kaplan 1969; Hendin 1973a,b, 1974a,b,1975; Milkman and Frosch 1973). Mixed drug abuse also has its ownparticular effects and appeal (Hendin 1973b, 1974c). Psychodynamicemphasis, nevertheless, has been too often c<strong>on</strong>fined to determining theregressive state produced by each drug and establishing parallelsbetween the regressive state and specific phases of childhood development(Wieder and Kaplan 1969).We view drug use as part of the individual’s attempt to deal withneeds and c<strong>on</strong>flicts, relati<strong>on</strong>s with others, and the social envir<strong>on</strong>mentin which he or she lives. Since all of these vary with age and stageof life, <strong>on</strong>e would expect drugs to be used and abused for differentpurposes at different points in the life cycle. A comparis<strong>on</strong> of ourstudy of adolescent drug abusers and their families with our study ofcollege students who were drug abusers tends to support this c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>.COLLEGE STUDENTSOur study of drug-abusing college students showed that c<strong>on</strong>flicts overperformance and competiti<strong>on</strong> were pervasive am<strong>on</strong>g college studentswho were marijuana abusers (Hendin 1973a). The same students whoadvocated a competiti<strong>on</strong>-free world saw their own success or failure interms of murderous aggressi<strong>on</strong> or intolerable humiliati<strong>on</strong>. Most retreatedfrom activities that engaged them because they wished to be free ofsuch painfully intense feelings, and they found relief in less challengingactivities. No survey of drug incidence or evaluati<strong>on</strong> of students’marks would reflect the numbers of students who withdrew from whatthey wanted most, to pursue activities with which they were lessengaged and by which they were not challenged.Amphetamine abuse was particularly comm<strong>on</strong> am<strong>on</strong>g college women. Itserved the functi<strong>on</strong> of helping these women move in directi<strong>on</strong>s thatthey thought they should go but to which their actual inner feelingswere opposed (Hendin 1974b). Most comm<strong>on</strong>ly, that directi<strong>on</strong> wasacademic success which they felt was expected by their family andtheir own image of themselves; in some cases it was a marriage thatthey thought they should enter into but to which they were inwardlyopposed. <strong>On</strong>e of them dreamed of herself as a puppet. Another whodreamed of herself as a mari<strong>on</strong>ette saw amphetamines as necessary tomove her strings and to keep her performing.It is interesting to note that while college women were using amphetaminesto help increase their achievement levels, college men were usingmarijuana to help ease or withdraw from competitive pressures. Thisdifference appears to be c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ant with the psychosocial changes weare witnessing in the roles of the sexes.196


Heroin, too, served specific dynamic functi<strong>on</strong>s for the college men whoused it. Most of them came to do so during the course of a relati<strong>on</strong>shipwith a young woman, when heroin was used as a protecti<strong>on</strong> against theintensity of the involvement (Hendin 1974a). Pleasure remained undertheir c<strong>on</strong>trol; they did not become close to the woman or let her bethe major source of pleasure--they saw it as safer to be high <strong>on</strong>heroin. Because it provided a check <strong>on</strong> their involvement, heroin wasoften necessary for them to have a relati<strong>on</strong>ship with a woman at all.Psychedelics and indiscriminate drug abuse were favored by youngpeople who wished to fragment themselves as a way of escaping thesense of c<strong>on</strong>stricti<strong>on</strong> and entrapment they had developed in theirrelati<strong>on</strong>ships with their families. Two of them dreamed of themselvesas jigsaw puzzles, reflecting their feeling that they could escape <strong>on</strong>lyby being torn apart or disassembled (Hendin 1973b, 1974c).ADOLESCENTSMost drug abuse begins in adolescence. Since a major adaptive task ofadolescence is a change in the individual’s relati<strong>on</strong>ship to his or herfamily, <strong>on</strong>e would expect the family to be the arena in which thec<strong>on</strong>flicts that center around drug abuse are expressed.The parents’ difficulties in accepting the changes in relati<strong>on</strong>ships withadolescent children have been shown to c<strong>on</strong>tribute to the problems(Zinner and Shapiro 1974, 1975). Families are most aware of thedrug-abusing youngsters’ anger, defiance, destructiveness and perhapsmost infuriated by their provocativeness--agreements violated, promisesbroken, and the like. For some youngsters the anger they feel towardtheir families is open, often unc<strong>on</strong>trollable, and frequently frighteningto the youngster. Marijuana in particular may be used by youngstersto help them subdue their rage (Allen and West 1968; Hendin 1973a;Hendin et al., in press).Less accessible to the awareness of these youngsters and their familiesis their need for the parents’ support and approval. The youngsters’defiance and provocativeness serve to force the parents to treat themlike young children who have to be watched and c<strong>on</strong>trolled, lockingthem into an angry, dependent relati<strong>on</strong>ship. And despite all the angerof the drug abusers toward their families and despite their insistence<strong>on</strong> a desire for freedom, moving out of their homes and away from thisdependent relati<strong>on</strong>ship with their parents is extremely difficult forthem.Ackerman’s (1958) early family studies highlighted the c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> ofthe delinquent child (often the scapegoat) to the family situati<strong>on</strong>.Stant<strong>on</strong> and his coworkers (1978) have developed and applied thisc<strong>on</strong>cept to their work with drug-abusing youngsters and their families.Our own recent studies of adolescent drug abusers, in which theirn<strong>on</strong>drug-abusing siblings were used as c<strong>on</strong>trols, show how familydynamics make it more or less likely for a particular youngster toexpress his or her difficulties through drug abuse (Hendin et al., inpress).Early childhood experiences play a critical role in determining latervulnerability to drug abuse (Hartmann 1969; Pittel et al. 1971). Bythe time a child reaches adolescence, parents may have resolved the197


problems that troubled their marriage earlier or that interfered withtheir interacti<strong>on</strong> with a particular child. Unfortunately, the youngsterwill have already suffered the c<strong>on</strong>sequences and may, in a sense, makethe parents c<strong>on</strong>tinue to pay for old injuries.Parental resp<strong>on</strong>se to the youngster’s difficulties must be distinguishedfrom the parent’s c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> to the origin of those difficulties.Failure to make this distincti<strong>on</strong> can lead to the mistaken assumpti<strong>on</strong>that the family’s need for a drug-abusing youngster is resp<strong>on</strong>sible forthe drug abuse. In some cases the drug abuser brings the familycloser together; in others, the family does better when the childleaves home.What drug abusers derive from their families becomes part of their ownadaptati<strong>on</strong> which they express both in and outside the family, andwhich they will c<strong>on</strong>tinue to use after leaving the family. It is necessaryto understand some of the features of this adaptati<strong>on</strong> to be in a positi<strong>on</strong>to understand what role drug abuse serves for an individual.Solely in terms of the difficulties they create for themselves at home,at school, and with the police, and the ways in which they damagetheir present and future prospects in life, the drug-abusing youngsterscould be characterized as self-destructive. Many of the drug abusersare aware of some desire to harm themselves directly, if <strong>on</strong>ly throughthe use of drugs. Although most speak at first of their drug use as ac<strong>on</strong>flict-free source of pleasure, in time they express somewhat moreambivalence. A young man who claimed to be joyfully high <strong>on</strong> marijuanawhenever he could eventually indicated that he was wasting his lifeaway being st<strong>on</strong>ed, and that marijuana took away his ambiti<strong>on</strong> anddrive and made him unable to express himself. A young woman indicatedthat she took drugs with a “let something happen to me” attitude.A young man who claimed his intermittent use of heroin was <strong>on</strong>ly asource of pleasure to him dreamed of it as a mixture of milk andpois<strong>on</strong>.Although virtually all of the drug-abusing young women we have seenhad sexual relati<strong>on</strong>s, n<strong>on</strong>e of them took precauti<strong>on</strong>s to prevent pregnancy.All of them eventually revealed a great deal of c<strong>on</strong>flict andguilt over sexual activity which, when combined with their failure touse c<strong>on</strong>tracepti<strong>on</strong> and a tendency to be involved in relati<strong>on</strong>ships thatexposed them to abuse or danger, suggested a self-destructive qualityto their sexual behavior.If the chances taken with regard to pregnancy were a reflecti<strong>on</strong> of theself-destructiveness of the young women, the chances taken with carsand motor bikes were a comparable measure for the young men.Accidents were frequent; <strong>on</strong>e of the young men we studied was killedwhen he crashed his motor bike into a truck. Sometimes being st<strong>on</strong>edor drunk when driving c<strong>on</strong>tributed to the accidents, but even in suchcases, it <strong>on</strong>ly reinforced an already existing recklessness. Driving forthese youngsters has an aggressive quality--going over the speedlimit, cutting off cars--but the risks some took and the frequency withwhich they had accidents suggest a self-destructive quality as well.A grandiose illusi<strong>on</strong> of invulnerability to injury often accompanies suchbehavior, and a grandiose self-image frequently serves to alleviate thedepressi<strong>on</strong> and low self-esteem that accompany the self-destructivebehavior of drug-abusing young men (Guarner 1966; Hendin et al., inpress).198


Grandiosity encouraged the sense that magical transformati<strong>on</strong> withouteffort was possible; the use of drugs to transform their mood helpedsupport this belief. <strong>On</strong>e young drug abuser whose current life was anightmare believed he was destined for some special fate that wouldmake itself evident in time. Another talked of his special luck, believingthat unusual things, both good and bad, happened to him morethan to others.For some of the young people we studied, drug abuse was sec<strong>on</strong>daryto other delinquent behavior--usually some form of larceny. Some ofthe drug-abusing youths occasi<strong>on</strong>ally stole m<strong>on</strong>ey to buy drugs, butsuch behavior was not central to their adaptati<strong>on</strong> as it was for thedelinquent youngsters. C<strong>on</strong>versely many delinquent youngsters werenot drug (or alcohol) abusers.Many drug-abusing youngsters are c<strong>on</strong>scious of their rage and frustrati<strong>on</strong>with their families. To some extent their drug abuse is a way ofmaking their emoti<strong>on</strong>s more tolerable. Delinquent youngsters moreoften use their behavior as a way of expressing their frustrati<strong>on</strong>without being aware of what they feel.Some youngsters, however, see drug abuse itself primarily as a delinquentact and they, too, are often unaware that their abuse hasanything to do with their families, so profoundly have they pushedtheir rage at them out of their c<strong>on</strong>sciousness. These young people areinvariably unable to deal with their parents directly and are bound insimultaneous needs to defy their parents and to punish themselves fortheir rebelli<strong>on</strong>.<strong>Drug</strong>s provide these young people with both crime and punishment,while removing their defiance away from the direct presence of theirparents. <strong>On</strong>e young man would “let his mind float away” and c<strong>on</strong>centrate<strong>on</strong> music he liked whenever his father berated him. Afterwardhe went out and took whatever drugs he could buy. While he neverc<strong>on</strong>nected his drug abuse with his anger toward his father, he oftendreamed of it as a crime for which he would be punished. He had adream in which a riot was going <strong>on</strong> in another part of town while hewas shooting heroin. He was afraid that somehow he would be arrestedal<strong>on</strong>g with the rioters. <strong>Drug</strong>s were clearly his way of rioting, ofdiverting the crime of rebelli<strong>on</strong> to the crime of drug abuse, and offocusing his destructive potential <strong>on</strong> himself.The expectati<strong>on</strong> that he would be arrested was revelatory of the appealof drugs for him and typical of the group. Jail signified to theseyoung men a c<strong>on</strong>crete way of locking up their rage. <strong>Drug</strong>s permittedthem both to c<strong>on</strong>tain their rage and to express it in a way that gavethem a sense of defiance, however self-damaging that defiance mightbe. Often young people who are most in trouble with the police overdrugs are those for whom the need for crime and punishment is moresignificant than the need for drugs (Hendin 1975).Individual and social distress are linked in psychosocial pathology bythe destructiveness and self-destructiveness that are comm<strong>on</strong> to all ofthe barometers of psychosocial stress. Failure to understand this hasled to c<strong>on</strong>fusi<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cerning the subject of correlati<strong>on</strong>s or inversecorrelati<strong>on</strong>s between <strong>on</strong>e form of psychosocial pathology and another.Suicide will be attributed to alcoholism or drug abuse because of thehigh frequency of alcoholics and drug abusers am<strong>on</strong>g those who killthemselves. A young man may narcotize his depressi<strong>on</strong> in alcohol and199


drugs for years before deciding to kill himself. He may even drink ordrug himself to death. In either case, although it may be physiologicallyaccurate, it is psychologically inaccurate to attribute his suicideto alcoholism or drug abuse.There are a limited number of ways in which psychosocial pathologycan express itself--crime, sexual deviancy, suicide, drug or alcoholabuse, etc. The early traumas that predispose to such pathologycreate a vulnerability that is often not specific to a particular disturbanceand is subject to a variety of psychosocial influences.<strong>On</strong>ce young people have become entrenched in a particular adaptati<strong>on</strong>like drug abuse, however, it is not easy for them to give up the imagethey have of themselves and the role they have created. <strong>On</strong>e youngman was trying to move away from his drug abuse and the nicknamehe had at school of “burned-out Billy.” He spoke of the rigid divisi<strong>on</strong>of every<strong>on</strong>e in his school into “jocks,” “freaks,” or “greasers.”Billy had been lifting weights lately and thought if people at schoolknew about it they would make fun of him and claim he was a jock,He related a dream in which he was standing <strong>on</strong> the street wearing hisfootball shirt, when some guys who supplied him with drugs came byin a car. They put him in the car, yelled “jock” at him, beat him up,and as the dream ended, threw him out of the car. After relating thedream, Billy spoke of a fellow he liked who was a good football player--a nice fellow, and not a typical jock. The dream revealed the internalc<strong>on</strong>flict involved in identifying with people whom Billy now admired,adopting a new role, and surrendering his past image as a drug abuser.That he was making the effort was significant, and it seemed likelythat he would succeed.200


Toward a Sociologyof <strong>Drug</strong> UseIrving F. Lukoff, Ph.D.Illicit substance use would appear to be a fruitful arena in which touse sociology to provide us with the insights needed to understand avast and changing panorama. In very recent history, illicit drug usehas engaged most of our youths, at least some of the time, and substantialsegments of the adult populati<strong>on</strong>. The issue to be discussed hereis whether sociology has c<strong>on</strong>tributed to our understanding of substanceuse, particularly the illicit substances proscribed by society.It is necessary to specify precisely what is meant by a sociology ofdrug abuse. Although we will refer to the “licit” substances, ourmain task is to review what sociology has to c<strong>on</strong>tribute to our understandingof the use of a range of illicit substances. These include averitable pharmacopeia of substances: narcotics of various types;marijuana and hashish; cocaine; methaqual<strong>on</strong>e; methad<strong>on</strong>e; inhalants;PCP; and illicitly used prescripti<strong>on</strong> drugs, including a wide array oftranquilizers, barbiturates, amphetamines, and similar compounds.Most of our discussi<strong>on</strong>, however, will focus <strong>on</strong> heroin and marijuanabecause much more is currently known about the users of these substances.Not <strong>on</strong>ly is there a vast array of substances people use, there is alsoa very marked selectivity as to who uses which kinds of substances.When LSD was being used by middle-class, college-age youths it wasalmost unknown in ghetto communities, where the drug users preferredheroin and marijuana. Patterns of drug use are generally not random;that is, the rates will vary sometimes by social class, other times byethnicity, and almost always by age, since most illicit drug use isc<strong>on</strong>centrated am<strong>on</strong>g adolescents and younger adults. Any effort atexplanati<strong>on</strong> must note that the use of different substances variesacross populati<strong>on</strong> groups. Further, usage patterns appear to gothrough various changes, partly because substances may becomeunavailable but also because trends abound in drug-using cultures asin other aspects of society.Except for marijuana and alcohol, the rates of sustained use of mostother substances are rare events. This creates an additi<strong>on</strong>al problem,that of obtaining sufficient subjects for detailed investigati<strong>on</strong>s in mostresearch strategies.201


The variability just described, in choice of substances used and thedifferent segments of society using them, raises a fundamental issue--<strong>on</strong>e that is not often c<strong>on</strong>fr<strong>on</strong>ted. That is, whether drug use is aphenomen<strong>on</strong> that can be directly explained or whether it is an epiphenomen<strong>on</strong>,an encrustati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> a more basic set of behaviors. <strong>On</strong>e way thisis often expressed is whether heroin use causes crime or vice versa orif marijuana use leads to the “hang loose” pattern associated withheavy users (Suchman 1968). There are indeed efforts to describemore elaborate patterns of behavior that cluster with the use of particularsubstances, that is, lifestyles or typologies, but the implicati<strong>on</strong>s ofthis perspective are not often clearly drawn (Nurco and Lerner 1974).The theoretical significance of this distincti<strong>on</strong> is, of course, that what<strong>on</strong>e is endeavoring to understand shifts radically. If <strong>on</strong>e views heroinor LSD or marijuana as the focus for understanding, as an unalloyeddependent variable, then explanati<strong>on</strong>s take <strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>e form. This assumpti<strong>on</strong>explains the focus <strong>on</strong> the primary group, particularly the role offriendship networks and attendant processes. <strong>On</strong> the other hand, ifheroin use attracts individuals who are already <strong>on</strong> the path to systematicdeviance and social disengagement, explanati<strong>on</strong>s take another form.The classic thesis of Lindesmith (1947) serves to illustrate this dilemma.He established as a c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> for a theory of narcotic use that it mustnot be idiosyncratic, nor limited to particular cultures or groups. Butthe use of opiates in very diverse settings involves not <strong>on</strong>ly individualswho are immersed in very different social systems, it even involvesdifferent forms of opiate use and generally engages individuals ofdifferent ages. The diversity that is implicit in this must lead, then,to a theory that is able to abstract social-structural comm<strong>on</strong>alities invery different systems (perhaps insurmountable at this point in time)or <strong>on</strong>e that reduces to an explanati<strong>on</strong> that is primarily focused <strong>on</strong>properties of the substance. In Lindesmith’s case, this becomes thephenomen<strong>on</strong> of withdrawal and the percepti<strong>on</strong> of users that they can<strong>on</strong>ly relieve their symptoms by engaging in the use of the drug. Thislatter explanati<strong>on</strong> cannot be c<strong>on</strong>sidered a sociological <strong>on</strong>e, irrespectiveof any merits it may have.The issue noted earlier, whether heroin use causes criminal behavior,takes <strong>on</strong> very different meanings, depending <strong>on</strong> whether heroin isviewed as a discrete behavior that can be isolated from other aspectsof a pers<strong>on</strong>’s life history or is instead simply an attribute of thepatterned behavior of individuals (NIDA 1976).Another questi<strong>on</strong> is whether it is possible to integrate all substanceuse into a single theory. Just as we noted that even a particularsubstance may be, from <strong>on</strong>e point of view, an epiphenomen<strong>on</strong>, the widearray of substances that are used also presents problems for any<strong>on</strong>ewho would attempt to include them in a single theoretical framework.SOME SOCIOLOGICAL PERSPECTIVESThe sociological theories that are most often cited are derived fromformulati<strong>on</strong>s that were designed to provide insight into delinquencyand criminal behavior. We review them in some detail because theyilluminate the sociological questi<strong>on</strong>s that may be raised. They alsodirect us to the questi<strong>on</strong>s that remain to be answered. The formulati<strong>on</strong>of Mert<strong>on</strong>’s essay <strong>on</strong> “Social Structure and Anomie” (1957, pp. 131-160)is probably the most frequently cited theory. The key feature, and202


perhaps the primary reas<strong>on</strong> for the theory’s attractiveness, is that itis an effort to specify how features of the social structure that areexternal to the individual actors produce observable patterns of behavior(Stinchcombe 1975, pp. 11-33). As with any effort at sociologicalexplanati<strong>on</strong>, it does not endeavor to account for all varieties of idiosyncraticresp<strong>on</strong>ses. The theoretical objective is to understand differentrates of behavior that are observed in socially important entities suchas sex, class, and ethnic groups.In his well-known formulati<strong>on</strong>, Mert<strong>on</strong> posits two systems: culturallyprescribed goals for achievement, and instituti<strong>on</strong>ally organized modesfor achieving these goals. The feature of this formulati<strong>on</strong> that c<strong>on</strong>cernsus is that despite the abundant citati<strong>on</strong> of this theory (Cole 1975), itillustrates another of Mert<strong>on</strong>’s observati<strong>on</strong>s made elsewhere, namely,that there is a disjuncti<strong>on</strong> between theory and empirical research(1957, pp. 131-160). While there are efforts to use at least porti<strong>on</strong>sof the theory (as in Jessor 1979 and Jessor et al. 1968), the basicformulati<strong>on</strong> is incompatible with most research strategies. <strong>On</strong>e doesnot generally observe instituti<strong>on</strong>al norms but obtains individual percepti<strong>on</strong>sof these norms, except where legal norms are invoked (Waldorfand Daily 1975). Nor does <strong>on</strong>e readily obtain informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> instituti<strong>on</strong>alaccess; <strong>on</strong>e infers them, in most instances, from resp<strong>on</strong>dents’ reports.While these may reflect larger cultural and structural facts, as Mert<strong>on</strong>and Jessor suggest, it is not altogether clear that <strong>on</strong>e can trace individualpercepti<strong>on</strong>s to larger systems except as they appear to be c<strong>on</strong>sistentwith the assumpti<strong>on</strong>s of the theory. For example, lower class adolescentsmay often see schools as hostile and irrelevant envir<strong>on</strong>ments forthem. <strong>On</strong>e may interpret this as reflecting a reality that blocks asignificant route for the achievement of culturally prescribed successgoals. However, this is not an unambiguous interpretati<strong>on</strong>. It isequally plausible to view the same informati<strong>on</strong> as a resp<strong>on</strong>se to muchmore limited spheres--such as cognitive ability or a resp<strong>on</strong>se to familyand peer groups--that socialize lower class youths in ways that areinc<strong>on</strong>gruent with the demands of educati<strong>on</strong>al or occupati<strong>on</strong>al systems.We do not argue for this latter interpretati<strong>on</strong>, nor is it an “unsociological”<strong>on</strong>e. But it illustrates how the same informati<strong>on</strong> may be variouslyinterpreted and embedded at different levels of abstracti<strong>on</strong>. Thelinkages between theory and fact are simply ambiguous without otherinformati<strong>on</strong>, which is often not available.The derivati<strong>on</strong>s from Mert<strong>on</strong>’s theory, however, are also troublesome.Mert<strong>on</strong> views drug use (and he appears to have heroin addicts inmind) as a sort of “retreatism,” in which individuals eschew culturallyprescribed goals for achievement and are barred from or reject accessto the goals that facilitate success. The rejecti<strong>on</strong> of both goals andmeans encompasses not <strong>on</strong>ly drug addicts, but alcoholics, psychotics,outcasts, and vagab<strong>on</strong>ds. The use of opiates, which are depressants,is c<strong>on</strong>sistent with the theme that addicts have little incentive to participatein the activities of the day-to-day world, both its cultural prescripti<strong>on</strong>sand the instituti<strong>on</strong>ally approved routes for achievement.Unfortunately, the facts that have accumulated <strong>on</strong> addicts, most ofthem subsequent to the formulati<strong>on</strong> of the theory (1949), are not easilyrec<strong>on</strong>ciled with the retreatist theme (Lukoff 1972; Lukoff and Brook1974; Waldorf and Daily 1975). Life is almost frenetic for addicts. Inorder to survive they must keep out of the way of the police, raisethe c<strong>on</strong>siderable funds they require, and keep abreast of where drugsmight be obtained.203


An extensi<strong>on</strong> of Mert<strong>on</strong>’s formulati<strong>on</strong> is the theory of Cloward andOhlin (1960). with its focus <strong>on</strong> the structure of opportunities. Theyposit a more elaborate organizati<strong>on</strong> or criminal activity, in which youngsterswho are recruited into crime achieve some of the culturallyprescribed rewards associated with achievement. But those who havefailed in both the c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al route and the criminal <strong>on</strong>e are doublefailures and prime candidates for drug use. The significance of thisformulati<strong>on</strong> is that it also locates heroin use am<strong>on</strong>g the structures thatare external to the individual. It appears to comport with the factthat minority youths, who are assumed, to have little access to mobilityin the ranks of organized crime, have higher rates of drug use thando lower class white youths, who presumably have such access.It is difficult to document the distributi<strong>on</strong> of various forms of organizedcrime, or the recruitment of youngsters into these circles, except inillustrative or anecdotal ways (White 1943). The body of findings wewill review later suggests that addicts are derived from the samematrix found in n<strong>on</strong>addicted delinquents and that there is little todistinguish them from n<strong>on</strong>users. And although addicts generallycommit fewer violent crimes than n<strong>on</strong>addicted criminals, they must bequite good at various forms of hustling and criminal activity in orderto survive (Lukoff 1972; Preble and Miller 1977; NIDA 1976).Despite their failure to explain drug use, the theories of Mert<strong>on</strong> andof Cloward and Ohlin c<strong>on</strong>tinue to be influential. The problems inspecifying universal norms, or reas<strong>on</strong>ably coherent structures thatallocate individuals al<strong>on</strong>g different paths, are not unique to theseformulati<strong>on</strong>s. But they are the major efforts that have as their goalsthe identificati<strong>on</strong> of socially structured alternatives within which individualspresumably act out their lives and shape the opti<strong>on</strong>s available tothem (Stinchcombe 1975). While c<strong>on</strong>tingent and subcultural patternsmay c<strong>on</strong>tribute to different modes of expressi<strong>on</strong>, they still attempt tospecify the broad out<strong>line</strong>s that direct pers<strong>on</strong>s’ lives.Much research <strong>on</strong> drug use would appear to examine derivative themesthat are useful for organizing much of our knowledge. We make noeffort to review all of the research in this brief paper, <strong>on</strong>ly thatporti<strong>on</strong> that directs us to alternative structural sources for understandingdrug use and deviance.SOCIAL LOCATIONMost investigati<strong>on</strong>s, even those that are descriptive or primarily epidemiological,without any clear theoretical agenda, generally examinesubstance use rates by age, sex, social class, and race/ethnicity(Abels<strong>on</strong> et al. 1977; Johnst<strong>on</strong> et al. 1979; O’D<strong>on</strong>nell et al. 1976).Social class and race/ethnicity serve as surrogates for socially significantstructural parameters. Where patterned differences emerge, theyappear to reflect the different propensities these groups have for drugexperimentati<strong>on</strong>. The theoretical issue, at first glance, is to comprehendhow social locati<strong>on</strong> affects individuals located differentially withinsociety.But illicit substance use is very volatile, even over relatively shorthistorical epochs. Currently, heroin use is c<strong>on</strong>centrated in black andHispanic communities, which appears to suggest that both lower socioec<strong>on</strong>omicstatus and bel<strong>on</strong>ging to disadvantaged minorities provide204


important clues to the attracti<strong>on</strong> of heroin use. However, at the turnof the century, opiate use, in various forms, was found primarily inwhite, middle-class females as a result of therapeutic use (Ball 1970;Ball and Bates 1970; Commissi<strong>on</strong> of Inquiry into the N<strong>on</strong>-Medical Use of<strong>Drug</strong>s 1973). In Britain, heroin users roughly match the class distributi<strong>on</strong>of the larger society, and blacks are underrepresented (Commissi<strong>on</strong>of Inquiry into the N<strong>on</strong>-Medical Use of <strong>Drug</strong>s 1973). A closerexaminati<strong>on</strong> of heroin use in ghetto communities in the United Statesreveals a more complex relati<strong>on</strong>ship of stratificati<strong>on</strong> to heroin use.Vaillant (1966b) c<strong>on</strong>trasted Lexingt<strong>on</strong> addicts against their own communitiesand observed they were better educated than their comparableage-mates in the same tracts. In a survey of an urban ghetto community,it was found that reported heroin use was associated with highersocioec<strong>on</strong>omic status, although this, as we will see, was a spuriousrelati<strong>on</strong>ship (Lukoff and Brook 1974; Lukoff 1977). Thus, heroinusers are not necessarily drawn from the most impoverished segmentsof the communities, where use is currently c<strong>on</strong>centrated (Nurco 1979;Robins 1975a). Nor, as their educati<strong>on</strong> and intelligence suggest, arethey necessarily those who should appear to be doomed to the marginsof society (Ball and Bates 1970). <strong>On</strong>ly when c<strong>on</strong>trasted against thelarger society do socioec<strong>on</strong>omic status and lower educati<strong>on</strong> appear to berelated to heroin use. This, however, appears to be the wr<strong>on</strong>g wayto examine the informati<strong>on</strong>. Instead, the relevant c<strong>on</strong>trast wouldappear to be to examine heroin users against the backdrop of theirown communities. Then the picture shifts substantially.Because heroin use is a relatively rare event, most general populati<strong>on</strong>surveys report too few users for reliable estimates. Thus, cauti<strong>on</strong> isnecessary in interpreting trends. In a study of selective serviceregistrants, O’D<strong>on</strong>nell and his colleagues (1976), when examiningreported narcotic use by cohorts, showed that there was a dec<strong>line</strong>am<strong>on</strong>g blacks in the later cohort, with an accompanying increase am<strong>on</strong>gwhites. In a survey of blacks in Harlem, Brunswick and Boyle (1979)examined rates by cohorts and observed a dec<strong>line</strong> in initiati<strong>on</strong> intoheroin use am<strong>on</strong>g younger members of their panel. Although it bearsrepeating that cauti<strong>on</strong> should be used, such trends do suggest howephemeral heroin or other narcotic use might be in historic perspective,and that the clues to its use might be elsewhere than in the simplematter of gross c<strong>on</strong>trasts by class or race observable in any <strong>on</strong>eepoch.The dynamic nature of drug use trends is even clearer for marijuana.When Becker (1963) investigated marijuana use two decades ago, it waslargely c<strong>on</strong>fined to inner city blacks and jazz musicians. Currently,marijuana competes with alcohol as the most popular drug, especiallyam<strong>on</strong>g the young (Jessor and Jessor 1977; Johnst<strong>on</strong> et al. 1978; Kandel1978a). Jessor and Jessor (1978), in reviewing marijuana trends,observe that there is a declining significance of such factors as “urbanicity,”race, and socioec<strong>on</strong>omic status. Even sex differences aredeclining, although they appear to persist for heroin use. “At thelevel of the demographic envir<strong>on</strong>ment then there has been a trendtoward homogenizati<strong>on</strong> as far as variati<strong>on</strong> in marijuana use is c<strong>on</strong>cerned”(Jessor and Jessor 1978, p. 341). It is increasingly smoked in publicsettings; legal penalties in many places have been reduced; sancti<strong>on</strong>s,where they exist, are often not invoked for possessi<strong>on</strong> of small quantitiesfor pers<strong>on</strong>al use. Even when sancti<strong>on</strong>s were punitive, marijuanause c<strong>on</strong>tinued to increase in popularity, both for those who have evertried it and am<strong>on</strong>g the proporti<strong>on</strong> who use it with reas<strong>on</strong>able frequency.Thus, normative systems are often <strong>on</strong>ly marginally effective, and they205


are subject to rapid change as the larger community begins to accommodatethe persistent and pervasive use of the substance.If most of the usual indicators of social locati<strong>on</strong> show declining significance,<strong>on</strong>e persistent feature of marijuana use c<strong>on</strong>tinues to be important:The vast majority of users are young. And increasingly, the age of<strong>on</strong>set of marijuana use appears to be declining (Abels<strong>on</strong> et al. 1977;Johnst<strong>on</strong> et al. 1979). Because of the relatively short time in whichmarijuana use has become popular, it is possible that current youthfuland young adult users will c<strong>on</strong>tinue to use it as they become older.The same persistent relati<strong>on</strong>ship to age is present am<strong>on</strong>g heroin users.Almost all users start when young, at least in the United States experience(Brunswick and Boyle 1979; Lukoff 1972; Nurco 1979; Robins1975a). As cohorts of adults advance in age, the largest proporti<strong>on</strong>who were addicted aband<strong>on</strong> heroin use. Winick (1964) estimates thetypical durati<strong>on</strong> of addicti<strong>on</strong> to be just over eight years. Althougholder addicts exist, the heroin-using populati<strong>on</strong> is still weighted towardthose who are relatively young.Thus, the <strong>on</strong>e unambiguous associati<strong>on</strong> with drug use, <strong>on</strong>e that appearsto persist, at least in Western cultures, is the relati<strong>on</strong>ship of drug useto youthfulness (Braucht et al. 1973). Most of those who experimentwith illicit drugs are young; those who become addicted, where thereis informati<strong>on</strong>, decrease or cease drug use with advancing age. Structuralvariables such as social class and race/ethnicity are much moreambiguously related to drug use, as our review of trends suggests.We exclude the misuse of medically prescribed drugs because theywould appear to present very different c<strong>on</strong>figurati<strong>on</strong>s.SOCIALIZATIONThe identificati<strong>on</strong> of social norms assumes that behavior is transmittedto actors who, depending <strong>on</strong> circumstances, tend to adhere to appropriatebeliefs and c<strong>on</strong>comitant behaviors. This explains the emphasis<strong>on</strong> socializati<strong>on</strong> in the research literature, although sometimes <strong>on</strong>ly the“end product,” the beliefs themselves, is identified and assumed tohave been somehow transmitted (Jessor et al. 1968; Mert<strong>on</strong> 1957). Thesearch for antecedents of pers<strong>on</strong>ality, rooted in family childrearingpractices, overlaps with the effort to identify how cultural values andnorms are communicated to the young (Brook et al. 1977a,b, 1978;Lukoff 1977).But socializati<strong>on</strong> is not limited to the family. Other agencies of socialc<strong>on</strong>trol also c<strong>on</strong>tribute, sometimes with perspectives that are at variancewith those of the family. The most heavily investigated area has beenthe impact of peer groups (Becker 1963; Braucht et al. 1973; Feldman1968) and the attendant mechanisms that shape the choice of friendsand influence the accommodati<strong>on</strong> to the behaviors and values of peers.This raises two theoretical issues. The first is the identificati<strong>on</strong> ofthe countervailing forces that influence the dec<strong>line</strong> of parental legitimacy,as well as of other agencies that promote c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al behavior.The sec<strong>on</strong>d issue is the way in which adolescents develop a peerculture with alternate value systems and goals (Becker 1963; Feldman1968; O’D<strong>on</strong>nell et al. 1976; Whyte 1943).206


There is <strong>on</strong>e thing, however, which is not altogether c<strong>on</strong>gruent withthe above statement. The literature <strong>on</strong> family socializati<strong>on</strong> of adolescentshas two foci, and many variati<strong>on</strong>s within each. First, there is a focus<strong>on</strong> the models family members provide for the use of drugs, tobacco,alcohol, or even medically prescribed, mood-altering drugs (Brook etal. 1977a, 1978; Kandel et al. 1978). Here, the assumpti<strong>on</strong> is thatchildren will emulate their parents’ use regardless of the choice ofsubstance. From this perspective, although substances may change,there should be a c<strong>on</strong>tinuity across generati<strong>on</strong>s. The findings aregenerally c<strong>on</strong>sistent with this assumpti<strong>on</strong>, although less powerful than<strong>on</strong>e might expect. This may be an artifact because rates of reporteduse by family members whether obtained from adolescents or fromparents are generally very low compared to the rates of usage of illicitdrugs by adolescents. Alcohol, of course, differs in this respect fromillicit substances (Braucht et al. 1973). The direct modeling of parents’behaviors is unlikely to explain a great deal of the usage by youngerindividuals where rates of use dec<strong>line</strong> rapidly after the mid-twenties(Abels<strong>on</strong> et al. 1977).The other focus is the examinati<strong>on</strong> of various forms of childrearing aswell as the quality of the parent-child relati<strong>on</strong>ship, i.e., whetherthere is warmth and affecti<strong>on</strong> between them. These studies generallyindicate that parental rules are related to adolescent drug use (Brooket al. 1977a, 1978). More proscriptive orientati<strong>on</strong>s are associated withlower rates of drug use. In additi<strong>on</strong>, adolescents who report positively<strong>on</strong> their parents also tend to have lower rates of involvement withillicit substances (Gerstein 1976). This is an important researchdirecti<strong>on</strong> that has its own utility.From the perspective laid out at the beginning of this paper, however,these are intervening processes. Since the purpose of this paper is toidentify aspects of the social structure that ultimately affect adolescentsand young adults, it is necessary to recast the issue in order to attemptto understand what it is about the social structure that may result invariati<strong>on</strong>s in the form of socializati<strong>on</strong>.Although not ordinarily viewed in the c<strong>on</strong>text of socializati<strong>on</strong>, age of<strong>on</strong>set of drug use serves as a surrogate index for an important dimensi<strong>on</strong>of socializati<strong>on</strong>, namely, the unfettering of the b<strong>on</strong>ds of socialc<strong>on</strong>trol. Early <strong>on</strong>set reflects the premature segmentalizing, or insulati<strong>on</strong>,of youthful activities from the normative system of the adultcommunity. Those who start young are more likely to persist insubstance use and other forms of deviance and to resist the blandishmentsof treatment (Commissi<strong>on</strong> of Inquiry into the N<strong>on</strong>-Medical Use of<strong>Drug</strong>s 1973; Lukoff 1972; NIDA 1976). Referring to narcotics users,Nurco (1979, p. 321) states, “The earlier the <strong>on</strong>set of deviant behaviors,the more malignant the process invoked and the more ominous theprognosis. . . . The younger the age of <strong>on</strong>set the more intense andcommitted the addictive career.”The early <strong>on</strong>set of drug use and other forms of deviance means thatindividuals are less likely to complete school, to have a history ofsustained employment, or to engage in other adolescent or young adultactivities that facilitate passage to adult status. In this sense, theirsocializati<strong>on</strong> is truncated, and they are less prepared to assume therequirements of adult roles of their communities. They are <strong>on</strong>ly marginallyc<strong>on</strong>nected to the adult worlds of their respective communities.Adolescent lifestyles, c<strong>on</strong>gregating with peers, avoiding employment207


and family relati<strong>on</strong>ships may persist until the pers<strong>on</strong> is quite advancedin age (Preble and Miller 1977).Equally important is the fact that, as Robins (1979) has noted, any.form of deviance, particularly am<strong>on</strong>g the young, forecasts other formsof deviance, including alcohol c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong>, school deportment, delinquency,and early sexual promiscuity. There are several possibleimplicati<strong>on</strong>s, but the <strong>on</strong>e that c<strong>on</strong>cerns us here is that the roots ofdeviance are shared by many forms of problem behavior. The formthat problem behavior takes, while it may in part reflect pers<strong>on</strong>aldispositi<strong>on</strong>s, is primarily a resp<strong>on</strong>se to the encounters with otherindividuals, the peer cultures of adolescents and young adults.The l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal rec<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong> of substance use by Robins (1975a),from premilitary usage through Vietnam and after discharge, dramaticallyillustrated how easy access to heroin inflated use rates substantially.Almost all soldiers in Vietnam would presumably have had easy accessto heroin, but not all of them used it. But those who scored high <strong>on</strong>preservice deviance were about four times as likely to initiate use asthose who were low in deviance. These findings underscore that whileproximal settings, where drugs are plentiful, markedly affect rates ofuse, earlier histories also exert a powerful influence. The fact thatam<strong>on</strong>g heroin users there is often a history of delinquency prior to the<strong>on</strong>set of use is c<strong>on</strong>sistent with these findings (Lukoff 1972; NIDA1976).Despite the addictive potential of heroin, for some individuals involvementis <strong>on</strong>ly experimental or sporadic; others appear to cease usewithout the assistance of treatment or to accommodate the goals oftreatment programs. Although scarcely studied, the informati<strong>on</strong> thatis available indicates that such individuals are less alienated or disengagedfrom family and work, and less intensively immersed in drugusinggroups (Lukoff 1974; Robins 1979; Zinberg 1979).Because marijuana is a comm<strong>on</strong> recreati<strong>on</strong>al drug for so many pers<strong>on</strong>s,the factors involved in its use are more diverse than those for heroinuse. It appears necessary, for example, to distinguish between pers<strong>on</strong>sin a late-<strong>on</strong>set, sporadic-use group and pers<strong>on</strong>s in an early-<strong>on</strong>set,frequent-use (generally daily) group. For those in the first group,use is c<strong>on</strong>fined to specific social c<strong>on</strong>texts in which it is simply acultural trend, much like tastes in music or clothing (i.e., the use isgoverned by proximal variables reflecting aspects of the current socialmilieu). In the sec<strong>on</strong>d group, use can be predicted from antecedentvariables, such as perceived or actual parental roles and the quality offamilial relati<strong>on</strong>ships (Jessor and Jessor 1977; Jessor et al. 1968;Braucht et al. 1973; Brook et al. 1977a, 1978; Lukoff 1977).As marijuana use moved from vanguard users who adopted the drugwhen it was still subject to heavy penalties, it appears to have alsoattracted individuals who, in varying degrees, were less likely to beengaged in subcultures that held perspectives divergent from those ofthe larger society.Although c<strong>on</strong>cepts used in the many investigati<strong>on</strong>s reflect the generalanarchy in a great deal of social research, <strong>on</strong>e trend appears to persist,namely, that youthful <strong>on</strong>set of marijuana use is associated with aslackening of parental c<strong>on</strong>trols, early rebelliousness, and the presenceof a wide array of behaviors inc<strong>on</strong>gruent with the expectati<strong>on</strong>s of thefamily, i.e., adult c<strong>on</strong>trols are markedly attenuated so that the208


disc<strong>on</strong>tinuity between generati<strong>on</strong>s is exacerbated (Braucht et al. 1973;Jessor and Jessor 1977; Kandel 1978a).GENERATIONAL DISJUNCTIONS<strong>Drug</strong> use, at least for the committed user, is always more than simplya preference for a particular substance, or <strong>on</strong>ly a habituati<strong>on</strong> that canbe slaked by repeated use of the drug. It is immersed in a morecoherent lifestyle pattern, <strong>on</strong>e that involves values and goals andpatterns of relati<strong>on</strong>ships. It is, therefore, part of a process of theemergence of cultural systems that are innovative, at least by thestandards of the communities from which drug users derive. Thus,the questi<strong>on</strong> implied earlier: How do variant lifestyles emerge in whichdrug use becomes a comp<strong>on</strong>ent element? If the family and the otheragencies of social c<strong>on</strong>trol were c<strong>on</strong>sistently effective, there would belittle illicit drug use because it has not been a major feature of adultlifestyles.Socializati<strong>on</strong> implies some form of inculcati<strong>on</strong> of basic adaptive strategiesof younger people, an activity ordinarily c<strong>on</strong>signed to the family,schools, churches. But this process is never wholly successful andcompetiti<strong>on</strong> can come from other sources, the most comm<strong>on</strong> being agemates.There is evidence, however, that the mere associati<strong>on</strong> withothers who use drugs, while a necessary feature of drug use, certainlyduring initiati<strong>on</strong>, is not sufficient to explain drug use. Andrews andKandel (1979) have dem<strong>on</strong>strated that there is a presocializati<strong>on</strong> processin the sense that those who initiate use have already acquired theattitudes that facilitate drug use. Jessor and Jessor (1977) note thatwhile marijuana users almost always are associated with a network ofusers, there are also individuals who choose not to use drugs. Am<strong>on</strong>gthose who have experimented with heroin and remain in close associati<strong>on</strong>with heroin users there are many who pull back.Vaillant (1966b) who observed that heroin users were overrepresentedby native-born offspring of migrant parents--not the children ofmigrants who had been brought up elsewhere before coming to urbanareas--hypothesized that there was a cultural disparity between thegenerati<strong>on</strong>s that appeared to increase susceptibility to heroin use.Lukoff and Brook (1974) observed that reported heroin users in aghetto community were disproporti<strong>on</strong>ately derived from the highersocioec<strong>on</strong>omic groups within the community, but that this was a functi<strong>on</strong>of the higher socioec<strong>on</strong>omic standing of the native-born when comparedto migrants. The key element, then, was the migrant-native status,with the native-born overrepresented am<strong>on</strong>g the users of heroin. Inthe same investigati<strong>on</strong> there was also a corresp<strong>on</strong>dence of viewpointstoward childrearing that accounted for the generati<strong>on</strong>al differences.Migrants in all four ethnic groups, American black, black British WestIndians, whites, and Puerto Ricans, subscribed to more proscriptiveand c<strong>on</strong>trolling orientati<strong>on</strong>s toward children than did native-bornmembers of those groups. Although reported heroin use differedbetween the groups, the same c<strong>on</strong>sistent relati<strong>on</strong>ship appeared: familiesthat were less proscriptive, even am<strong>on</strong>g migrants, reported higherrates of heroin use and closer c<strong>on</strong>tact with users of heroin.The socializati<strong>on</strong> studies cited earily appear to be c<strong>on</strong>sistent with theabove findings (Braucht et al. 1973; Commissi<strong>on</strong> of Inquiry into theN<strong>on</strong>-Medical Use of <strong>Drug</strong>s 1973; Gerstein 1976). Insofar as parental209


ideologies are oriented toward greater c<strong>on</strong>trol and m<strong>on</strong>itoring of children,drug initiati<strong>on</strong> appears to dec<strong>line</strong>. At another extreme, whenheroin users have been studied, generally retrospectively, thereappears to be markedly disturbed family backgrounds in which thefamilies of origin are often abusive or unable to m<strong>on</strong>itor the activitiesof their children effectively (Commissi<strong>on</strong> of Inquiry into the N<strong>on</strong>-MedicalUse of <strong>Drug</strong>s 1973; Robins 1975a; Zinberg 1979).Whether we speak of the markedly deviant lifestyle of the heroin addictor the more “laid back” patterns of the middle-class psychedelic user,both patterns can <strong>on</strong>ly evolve when youth cultures operate with relativefreedom, in isolati<strong>on</strong> from the agencies of social c<strong>on</strong>trol. This alsopresumes that the usual socializati<strong>on</strong> mechanisms, including but notlimited to the family, have declining legitimacy. There appear to beseveral possible causes for this situati<strong>on</strong>.There is, first, the diversity of the urban social envir<strong>on</strong>ment. C<strong>on</strong>trastthis with individuals from rural backgrounds in which there are fewcompeting cultural systems. Thus, not <strong>on</strong>ly is the adherence to communitynorms more difficult, but there are attractive alternative systemsthat can be observed and to which <strong>on</strong>e can often gain access.There is also the increasing isolati<strong>on</strong> of the family. It is not just thatmore families are headed by single parents, since this has not beenunequivocally associated with heroin use (Lukoff and Brook 1974). Itis more likely that the networks of family support systems are smaller,and, by the very nature of the urban envir<strong>on</strong>ment, even when present,are less likely to affect young people. <strong>On</strong>e does not often encounteran aunt, uncle, or cousin who can report to <strong>on</strong>e’s parents, as happensin smaller communities or in rural areas. In additi<strong>on</strong>, more of theactivities formerly c<strong>on</strong>fined to the family are now performed elsewhere,from preschool through a l<strong>on</strong>ger and more extended schooling periodwhere primary adult groups have minimal impact.Larger social changes are difficult to link to various forms of youthfulrebelIi<strong>on</strong>. We can <strong>on</strong>ly note that it is in urban areas that traditi<strong>on</strong>alsegregati<strong>on</strong> norms began to lose hold. With the rejecti<strong>on</strong> of the adultswho accommodated the restricti<strong>on</strong>s imposed <strong>on</strong> blacks, the legitimacy ofthe c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al society of the ghettos also dec<strong>line</strong>d in significance.Am<strong>on</strong>g middle-class, mainly white youths, the disparity between voicedvalues and reality attracted increasing attenti<strong>on</strong>, whether it was thecivil rights struggle, or oppositi<strong>on</strong> to a war for which they could findno justificati<strong>on</strong>. This often was translated by many young people intoa rejecti<strong>on</strong> of the entire middle-class value system. With the declininglegitimacy of the usual agents of social c<strong>on</strong>trol, the possibility forinnovati<strong>on</strong>, always present am<strong>on</strong>g young people, appears to haveescalated. It is in these c<strong>on</strong>texts that drug use increased, from anactivity engaged in by <strong>on</strong>ly a few, to <strong>on</strong>e that has become, at least formarijuana, a normal part of the youth culture.Parental ideologies toward children appear to be implicated. Several ofthe investigati<strong>on</strong>s cited earlier note that parental orientati<strong>on</strong>s towardchildrearing appear to be c<strong>on</strong>sistently related to the initiati<strong>on</strong> of druguse. The ideology of self-determinati<strong>on</strong> of children is another factor.An outcome of urban sophisticati<strong>on</strong>, it is not so prevalent in smalltowns and rural communities, nor is it shared by migrants from moretraditi<strong>on</strong>al cultures, though it is so<strong>on</strong> incorporated in the ideologies oftheir descendants. This is often accompanied by a declining willingnessto enforce c<strong>on</strong>trols and m<strong>on</strong>itor the activities of children and is often210


accompanied by more extensive use of surrogate guardians. When therewards, as perceived by the children, appear more exciting and challengingelsewhere, the opti<strong>on</strong>s provided by the family appear to dec<strong>line</strong>in influence. And so a greater receptivity to encounters with peerswould seem to be a c<strong>on</strong>sequence of the lessened “internalizati<strong>on</strong>” ofnorms and values derived from the family.We have <strong>on</strong>ly roughly sketched in some possible sources of the way inwhich youth cultures appear to have greater priority in the evoluti<strong>on</strong>of new values and behaviors, with illicit substance use an importantcomp<strong>on</strong>ent of these activities. The form it takes, from the “hangloose” orientati<strong>on</strong> described by Suchman, or the “hippie” culture of the1960s, or the “cool cat” of the ghettos, depends <strong>on</strong> subcultural formswithin the communities and the kinds of values and activities, oftenderived from the adult culture, but profoundly transformed in theprocess, that are available. In this brief paper we cannot explore thisarea in detail, but it appears that the choice of adaptive styles, whileat variance with the community’s system of values, is in importantaspects a facet of that system.CONCLUSIONIn this paper we argue that the key social structural feature associatedwith drug use is found in the <strong>on</strong>e unambiguous associati<strong>on</strong>, that ofillicit substance use with young people. In fact, the evidence seemsto point to a lowering of the age at which individuals commence theuse of illicit substances (Abels<strong>on</strong> et al. 1977; Johnst<strong>on</strong> et al. 1979).Other structural features such as social class or ethnic group membership,while clearly associated with many aspects of drug use, whenexamined historically and even in the short period of the past fewdecades, are seen to be <strong>on</strong>ly ephemerally related. It appears that theindigenous cultures are shaping forces, but they do not play a decisiverole. What we have said appears to be true for the United States,and perhaps for western Europe, but it does not hold for narcoticsuse by medical practiti<strong>on</strong>ers or by Middle Eastern rural dwellers.We also advance the view that it is less useful to speak of drug useal<strong>on</strong>e, because those who are heavily invested in drug use are also partof more integrated lifestyles, different in the ghettos than <strong>on</strong> the campuses,but at variance with many aspects of c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al adult culture.We suggest that marijuana in particular, since it is used by the majorityof young people, may be peripheral for many. But for those whostart when young and use with reas<strong>on</strong>able frequency, the evidence isc<strong>on</strong>sistent with the theme that illicit substance use is not an isolatablephenomen<strong>on</strong>, but must be understood in a larger c<strong>on</strong>text. And wherethere is informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> who uses drugs there appears to be a processof disengagement from c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al values and norms that precedesinitiati<strong>on</strong>. We suggest the sources of the rapid escalati<strong>on</strong> of drug useare located in the forces that influence the declining legitimacy ofc<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al norms and values and agents of social c<strong>on</strong>trol <strong>on</strong> the <strong>on</strong>ehand, and in the structural forces that increase the opportunities foryounger people to operate with greater freedom outside the c<strong>on</strong>fines ofthe usual c<strong>on</strong>trol mechanisms. In this sense, drug use and the attendantcultural prescripti<strong>on</strong>s represent a process of social change.211


Achievement, Anxiety,and Addicti<strong>on</strong>Rajendra K. Misra, D. Phil.<strong>Drug</strong> abuse is a resp<strong>on</strong>se to fear of failure; it helps us to withdrawfrom the pressures of achievement by inducing and maintaining a senseof apathy toward the standards of excellence in society. Tensi<strong>on</strong>s andstress of lifestyle in urban and developed societies are marked bypressure for achieving goals that subscribe to the so-called “approved”quality of life.CULTURAL PERSPECTIVE<strong>Drug</strong> abuse, or at least its impact, seems to be more comm<strong>on</strong> in thetechnologically developed societies than in the developing <strong>on</strong>es. Industrializedcultures are quite regimented in terms of their standards ofexcellence. There are definite, clearly identified criteria for goalattainment. Quality of life is measurable. The indicators of happinessare c<strong>on</strong>crete and specific. In the United States, for instance, thestandards of excellence are more visual and substantive than, say, inIndia, where about 70 percent of the populati<strong>on</strong> live in rural areasand depend <strong>on</strong> agriculture for a living. In India, belief in (a) thetransmigrati<strong>on</strong> of the soul, (b) the birth-rebirth cycle, and (c) thegoal of life being the ability to break away from the birth-rebirthprocess and merge with the Supreme Being do not encourage preoccupati<strong>on</strong>with earthly, material things. The quality of life is relativelyvague in its beginning and ending. Standards of excellence are fewerthan in the developed nati<strong>on</strong>s. Pressures for achievement are relativelymild; penalties for failure, few. Blended with this sociocultural ethosare the religious sancti<strong>on</strong>s against taking bhang (hashish) or smokingmarijuana, except during the specified religious festivals, when drugsare often a part of the ritual.In any culture, celebrati<strong>on</strong> is marked by (relatively) inhibited expressi<strong>on</strong>of emoti<strong>on</strong>s (usually love and anger). Social and cultural systemsbuild in occasi<strong>on</strong>s for celebrati<strong>on</strong> of the basic historical and religioustraditi<strong>on</strong>s. Two features of any celebrati<strong>on</strong> are food and emoti<strong>on</strong>, theassumpti<strong>on</strong> being that the chores and routines of day-to-day livingtend to restrain eating and expressi<strong>on</strong>. An average Indian lunches <strong>on</strong>212


a paratha (shallow, fried, layered bread made with whole wheat flour)and curried potatoes. An average American grabs a sandwich andwashes it down with a soft drink. Emoti<strong>on</strong>al expressi<strong>on</strong> is alsorestrained. Smiles are closer to courtesy than to feelings. Self-c<strong>on</strong>troland restrained expressi<strong>on</strong> day after day and week after week programus somewhat for an almost computerized lifestyle. Even televisi<strong>on</strong>comedy shows sandwich “canned” laughter in between the scenes as ifto remind the audience about the humor.Celebrati<strong>on</strong>s acquire special significance against this backdrop of dryand dreary lifestyle. We have to plan to relax. It is not uncomm<strong>on</strong>for people to go <strong>on</strong> a strict diet before going <strong>on</strong> a vacati<strong>on</strong> so thatthey can eat without much guilt. Even more important is the expressi<strong>on</strong>of emoti<strong>on</strong>s. The recent mushrooming of the “pop” therapy methods(encounter groups, marath<strong>on</strong>s, self-improvement techniques, stressmanagement, and so forth) illustrates our obsessi<strong>on</strong> with inhibitedexpressi<strong>on</strong>.ACHIEVEMENT ANXIETYIn the developing countries, however, because of relatively less pressurefor achievement, celebrati<strong>on</strong>s are observed more frequently andfor l<strong>on</strong>ger durati<strong>on</strong>. Methods of relaxati<strong>on</strong> usually c<strong>on</strong>sist of visitingwith friends and going to movies. In a developed nati<strong>on</strong> like theUnited States, people just do not have time for much relaxati<strong>on</strong>. AnAmerican, creatively enough, treats living and working as syn<strong>on</strong>ymous.The weekends are planned and filled as tightly as are the weekdays.Relaxati<strong>on</strong> is not “doing nothing”; it is another kind of work. Weekendgolfers, painters, and vacati<strong>on</strong>ers love to achieve standards of excellencein their relaxati<strong>on</strong> ventures. It is not enough to feel that “myvacati<strong>on</strong> was relaxing”; I also want to feel, prove, and publicize that“my vacati<strong>on</strong> was better than yours.”We do not mind trading relaxati<strong>on</strong> for tensi<strong>on</strong>: Borrowing m<strong>on</strong>ey to go<strong>on</strong> a vacati<strong>on</strong> is a good example of this. Doing something rather thannothing is the hallmark of relaxati<strong>on</strong>. Frequently, <strong>on</strong>e is as tensea out seeking relief as <strong>on</strong>e is about achieving work goals. Relaxati<strong>on</strong>must be achieved, here and now. A sense of immediacy encouragessearch for time-saving techniques for achieving peace and tranquility.<strong>Drug</strong> abuse emerges as a natural corollary to this way of life. In thespeed-oriented culture of the United States, for instance, drug abuseis a handy device for “getting away from it all” (Misra 1975). Chemicalaids for feeling “fresh and relaxed” are so widely publicized throughthe media that it is extremely difficult to resist the temptati<strong>on</strong> for thisshortcut to happiness.The vast range of data in the media, including advertisements forautomobiles, homes, food, vacati<strong>on</strong>s, and so <strong>on</strong>, describes and perhapseven sets the goals we are expected to achieve to qualify as “leading agood life.” The focus is <strong>on</strong> what, not how, to attain in order to havea feeling of achievement, a sense of satisfacti<strong>on</strong>.Availability of opti<strong>on</strong>s causes anxiety. Different goals are perceived interms of their potential value to satisfy our needs. Do we buy anautomobile to get from <strong>on</strong>e place to the other? Maybe. But also toacquire status, power, and prestige. It is not easy to decide <strong>on</strong> thekind of car we want to buy, essentially because there are so many to213


choose from. The situati<strong>on</strong> is the same in many other areas: cereals,bread, cheese, vacati<strong>on</strong>, home, and so forth. Any time you decide infavor of <strong>on</strong>e goal over the other, the latter will look better (at leastmost of the time). We must then try to c<strong>on</strong>vince ourselves that theopti<strong>on</strong> we chose was indeed superior to the <strong>on</strong>e we did not. Industrialcultures encourage a rat race for status and identity, with every<strong>on</strong>estriving hard to “be somebody.”SUMMARY<strong>Drug</strong> use is initiated as a time-saving device to cope with the stress ofachieving standards of excellence. Chemically induced relaxati<strong>on</strong> issimple and quick. The ease and speed with which feelings of reliefcan be attained encourages the c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> of drug use. Initially,drugs are used to escape from the pressures of achievement, butgradually, the thrill becomes the goal, marking the c<strong>on</strong>versi<strong>on</strong> of useinto abuse. Cessati<strong>on</strong> of drug abuse is an awfully slow process becauseit involves changes in <strong>on</strong>e’s lifestyle. The whole area of goal-settingbehavior must be addressed before the chemically c<strong>on</strong>venient copingstrategies, nothing more than acts of slow suicide, can be c<strong>on</strong>trolled.PROBLEM BEHAVIORS<strong>Drug</strong>s do seem to have the advantage of calming down our anxietyabout achievement. However, the process by which this is d<strong>on</strong>e hasalso an important negative effect insofar as it induces a sense ofdefiant indifference. In most cases, excessive use (abuse) of drugsincreases our level of c<strong>on</strong>fidence. (<strong>On</strong>e pers<strong>on</strong> under the influence ofLSD believed he could fly: He jumped out of a 17th floor window anddied.) This, however, is a compensati<strong>on</strong> for the underlying achievementanxiety, which was initially a resp<strong>on</strong>se to our feelings of inadequacy.It is no w<strong>on</strong>der, then, that drug abusers have a higher proclivity forengaging in antisocial behaviors. The so-called “morning after” effectreflects a sense of depressi<strong>on</strong> and remorse for the night before.Depressi<strong>on</strong> leads to anxiety, which, in turn, leads to increased needfor chemical relief, and so goes the process of strengthening theanxiety which was the cause for initiating the abuse of drugs in thefirst place.We tend to overlook the fact that drug abuse is a resp<strong>on</strong>se to our fearof failure: It starts as a “little break” from the pressures of achievementbut then, over a period of time, becomes a goal in itself. Westart using drugs when we are emoti<strong>on</strong>ally upset. A temporary feelingof relief is all we desire. <strong>On</strong>ce the effect wears off, we are backagain in the jungle of competitive culture, and <strong>on</strong>ce again, we resortto chemical aids to have a feeling of thrill and happiness. The processc<strong>on</strong>tinues until achieving the thrill becomes our goal. The need fortemporary relief is transformed into the ultimate goal of achieving astate of nothingness.In a lifestyle marked by hed<strong>on</strong>ism, a sense of c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> for othersbecomes the least important of all needs. The most crucial need is fora child-like, impulsive happiness (Clements and Simps<strong>on</strong> 1978). Lawand order tend to be perceived as evil forces in society. Thus, therelati<strong>on</strong>ship between problem behaviors and drug abuse is as predictableas water boiling at 100°C.214


The Natural History of<strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>Lee N. Robins, Ph.D.INTRODUCTIONThe first step in discussing the natural history of drug abuse has tobe to offer a definiti<strong>on</strong> of what we mean by drug abuse. By “drugs”we will mean <strong>on</strong>ly illicitly used psychoactive drugs--that is, eitherthose bought through illegal channels or obtained legally but used bypers<strong>on</strong>s for whom they were not prescribed or in quantities largerthan prescribed or for purposes other than those for which they wereprescribed. By “abuse” we mean all such illicit use up to the point ofaddicti<strong>on</strong>. The reas<strong>on</strong> for selecting this definiti<strong>on</strong> of “abuse” is primarilya practical <strong>on</strong>e. Stopping short of addicti<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>forms to the definiti<strong>on</strong>sof substance or drug abuse in ICD-9 and DSM-III, where “abuse”is used to categorize problems with drugs which do not encompassdrug dependence.While our separati<strong>on</strong> of “abuse” from dependency c<strong>on</strong>forms with ICD-9and DSM-III,’ we will not require social or health problems resultingfrom use, as these sources do when they define abuse. Because weare discussing <strong>on</strong>ly the illicit use of drugs, <strong>on</strong>e could justifiably arguethat any use c<strong>on</strong>stitutes abuse. But a more telling reas<strong>on</strong> for notattempting to distinguish abuse from use is that most of the studies <strong>on</strong>which we will draw have not made this distincti<strong>on</strong>. Further, sinceabuse inevitably must be preceded by use, use would play a part inthe natural history of abuse as a predisposing factor in any case.This paper is extracted from “The Natural History of <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>,”presented at a symposium <strong>on</strong> treatment evaluati<strong>on</strong> in drug abuse, 19thScandinavian Psychiatric C<strong>on</strong>gress, Uppsala, Sweden, June 15, 1979.The work was supported in part by USPHS grants DA 00013, DA 000259,and MH 31302.1 ICD-9 is the 9th revisi<strong>on</strong> of the Internati<strong>on</strong>al Classificati<strong>on</strong> of Diseaseby the World Health Organizati<strong>on</strong>; DSM-II I is the American PsychiatricAssociati<strong>on</strong>’s Diagnostic and Statistical Manual.215


Having decided that our review will encompass any use of illicit drugsshort of addicti<strong>on</strong>, we still need to decide whether drug abuse thusdefined has a natural history to describe. Unlike schizophrenia, whichis a rare disorder but <strong>on</strong>e which is recognizable in every culture andin every historical period, drug abuse has emerged as a series of“epidemics” of abuse of different drugs affecting different age, sex,and socioec<strong>on</strong>omic groups at different historical times and in differentcountries. As the groups affected vary, the natural history mayvary, just as the natural history of measles differs in adults andchildren, and in children who are chr<strong>on</strong>ically undernourished as comparedwith those who are well fed. The particular drug or drugsabused may each have its own natural history of abuse, as well. Totake an analogy from the infectious diseases, to attempt to talk about anatural history of drug abuse may be equivalent to trying to describethe natural history of “infecti<strong>on</strong>,” rather than the natural history ofparticular infectious diseases. As both agent and host vary over timeand place, our descripti<strong>on</strong> may be accurate <strong>on</strong>ly for a particular momentin time and a particular locati<strong>on</strong>. Thus while we can describe thenatural history of schizophrenia with some c<strong>on</strong>fidence as a rare disorderhaving its <strong>on</strong>set in young adulthood, and having a chr<strong>on</strong>ic course ifuntreated, there is no such simple descripti<strong>on</strong> of the natural history ofdrug abuse.Recognizing these limitati<strong>on</strong>s, we will n<strong>on</strong>etheless attempt to fashi<strong>on</strong> anatural history by summarizing what is known about the circumstancesof initiati<strong>on</strong>, which groups are most vulnerable to drug abuse, motivati<strong>on</strong>sfor use, how drugs are taken, to what extent dosages tend toincrease, and finally, we will attempt to interpret these findings byasking to what extent the natural history of drug abuse suggests thatit is a disorder for which those with antisocial pers<strong>on</strong>alities are particularlyat risk.In order to present this picture, we will draw <strong>on</strong> a variety of studies,but many of our illustrati<strong>on</strong>s will come often from our own study ofVietnam veterans, because it is the largest study so far of pers<strong>on</strong>swho have been involved in more than casual use of illicit drugs.A BRIEF HISTORICAL NOTEFew drugs have been illicit from the moment of their discovery orsynthesis. Generally drugs have been defined as illegal <strong>on</strong>ly as evidencefor problems resulting from their use appeared. Many drugsnow illegal have enjoyed a period of legal popularity with the upperand middle classes. As their legal status changed, so did their clientele.Those drugs now valued for their ability to create illicit pleasureshave previously been used to relieve physical pain, as cough medicines,as cures for diarrhea, as sleeping poti<strong>on</strong>s, as health-giving “t<strong>on</strong>ics,”as means of improving daily work performance, and even as cures fordependence <strong>on</strong> other drugs.After World War I, in the United States the Harris<strong>on</strong> Act marked amajor attempt to make psychoactive drugs illegal. With this effortthere came a reducti<strong>on</strong> in their prescripti<strong>on</strong> by physicians and adec<strong>line</strong> in their use by the middle class. Use became c<strong>on</strong>centrated invarious “outsider” groups; such as musicians and minority groups.Since World War II, drug use has become much more widespread. Itspread first within the segregated black ghettoes of the United Statesand from there to urban middle-class college students. From them it216


spread to their younger siblings, and to working-class youths andrural populati<strong>on</strong>s. Over the course of the last 30 years, the tendencyhas been for larger and larger groups to become involved and for ageof initiati<strong>on</strong> to dec<strong>line</strong>.In many parts of the world where the older patterns of use by middleclassand rural populati<strong>on</strong>s were less forcibly suppressed by legalsancti<strong>on</strong>s, this new pattern of use by urban youths has been superimposed<strong>on</strong> the traditi<strong>on</strong>al pattern. In South America, for instance,urban high school and college students are using marijuana just aschildren in Europe and America do, but at the same time the cocachewing in the Bolivian highlands c<strong>on</strong>tinues, with little communicati<strong>on</strong>between the two drug cultures.With the spread of illicit drug use to middle-class youths, there hasoccurred an enormous increase in drug research, most of it focusing<strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong> this newer postwar pattern. As a result, our ability to describethe “natural history of drug abuse” is in general <strong>on</strong>ly an ability todescribe the present historical phase. While this limitati<strong>on</strong> must makeus w<strong>on</strong>der about the generalizability of our c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>s, we are fortunatein having available a number of large, well-executed studies thatprovide documentati<strong>on</strong> of the current drug abuse phenomena that isprobably more complete than that available for any other topic ofcurrent psychiatric interest.STUDIES OF THE “NEW” DRUG ABUSEAm<strong>on</strong>g the studies that are most important are those by Lloyd Johnst<strong>on</strong>(1973), which followed tenth graders until a year past high schoolgraduati<strong>on</strong>. They were then asked about their drug use in theirsenior year of high school and their use in the following year. Johnst<strong>on</strong>is currently doing a similar study beginning with five cohorts of highschool seniors each being followed for five years (Johnst<strong>on</strong> et al.1977).Another extremely important study was d<strong>on</strong>e by O’D<strong>on</strong>nell et al. in1976. A large sample of men 20 to 30 was selected from military draftregistrati<strong>on</strong>s, located, and interviewed about their lifetime drug experiences.There have been many studies of school populati<strong>on</strong>s. Am<strong>on</strong>g the mostinteresting are Kellam et al.'s followup of black first-grade students inChicago to age 17 (in press), in which they look for predictors in firstgrade of later drug use. Kandel et al. (1978) did a survey in highschools throughout New York State, and followed their resp<strong>on</strong>dents fivem<strong>on</strong>ths later. Their particular interest was in the respective roles ofparents and peers in introducti<strong>on</strong> to illicit drug use. The Jessors (1977)did a four-year followup study of both high school and college students,in which they were able to watch the emergence of drug use year byyear. Smith (1977) has been following fourth to twelfth graders afterfour years. Mellinger and Mannheimer are studying the development ofdrug use in college students (cited in Smith 1977).Our own work has covered two populati<strong>on</strong>s, young blacks and Vietnamveterans. The study of young black men in the mid-1960s was thefirst n<strong>on</strong>patient, n<strong>on</strong>student survey of drug abuse (Robins and Murphy1967). Later we studied a large sample of Vietnam veterans who had217


served in Vietnam at the height of the availability of heroin there, anda matched n<strong>on</strong>veteran c<strong>on</strong>trol group (Robins et al. 1977).Our c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>s about the natural history of drug abuse stem mainlyfrom these studies. Thus we will be describing the drug experienceof young people in the United States during the 1960s and 1970s.VULNERABILITY TO DRUG USE<strong>Drug</strong> abuse has spread remarkably in the United States, so that currentestimates of the number of high school seniors who have usedsome illicit drug are over 60 percent (Johnst<strong>on</strong> et al. 1977). As theproporti<strong>on</strong> approaches 100 percent, it becomes impossible to identify an<strong>on</strong>vulnerable segment. At this time, however, it is still possible tofind some descriptors of pers<strong>on</strong>s who are more likely to use illicit drugs,and particularly those more likely to use them early, or to use themmore frequently than average, or to use a greater variety of drugsthan average.It is clear that the characteristics of the “new” drug users are verydifferent from the characteristics of the former users. The formerusers tended to be middle-aged or older women who had a high rate ofvisiting doctors, and who were well integrated into the “establishment.”Young users of illicit drugs differ from them in terms of their demographiccharacteristics, their family settings, and the kinds of peoplewith whom they associate. Since World War II, young drug users havetended to be urban, male, minority-group members, particularly blackand Spanish-American. It has been thought that these young peoplewere from the lowest social stratum, perhaps because impressi<strong>on</strong>s werebased <strong>on</strong> those pers<strong>on</strong>s who sought treatment <strong>on</strong>ly after becomingchr<strong>on</strong>ically unemployed. Since drug use is especially comm<strong>on</strong> am<strong>on</strong>gminority groups, users necessarily include pers<strong>on</strong>s of lower classbackgrounds. However, neither the minority-group nor the majoritygroupusers come from particularly ec<strong>on</strong>omically disadvantaged familiesrelative to their own groups, perhaps reflecting the high cost ofdrugs. The parents of drug abusers, if not poor, do have more thantheir share of broken marriages, and tend to have a history of excessuse of alcohol and psychotherapeutic drugs. The friends of users arethemselves users, and support the use of drugs, which makes it easyfor the n<strong>on</strong>user to obtain the drugs and to find encouragement fortheir use.<strong>On</strong>e of the most striking findings of these studies is the brief agespan in which the <strong>on</strong>set of illicit drug use typically occurs. Theperiod of risk begins in the teens and ends by the mid-twenties. Asthe number of drug users in this age group has increased, there hasbeen a ripple effect to other age brackets, with greatest increase injust younger and just older groups, but first use remains unusualbefore age 13 or after 25.The pers<strong>on</strong>al characteristics of those particularly liable to use drugshave been obtained by comparing using with n<strong>on</strong>using adolescents inthe same schools. <strong>On</strong>e of the characteristics looked at from time totime is IQ. The IQ of drug users tends to be good to superior, quitedifferent from that reported for the typical delinquent, whose IQ isslightly below normal. Despite their good IQs, prospective drug userstend to be underachievers in school. They report a lack of motivati<strong>on</strong>218


to do well at school; they are not particularly interested in going <strong>on</strong>to college; and they generally d<strong>on</strong>’t like school very much. In earlystudies of drug-abusing students, it was hypothesized that they hadserious pers<strong>on</strong>al problems that motivated them to seek escape fromreality. There seems to be little evidence for this view. In fact,rather than being maladjusted isolates, drug abusers tend to be moresociable than average. This would seem necessary if they are to haveaccess to drugs through friends. <strong>On</strong> the other hand, there is someevidence from Kandel et al.'s work that they have more depressivesymptoms than n<strong>on</strong>users (1978). which suggests that at least occasi<strong>on</strong>allydrugs may be used to treat such feelings.The behavior of drug abusers prior to the <strong>on</strong>set of drugs resemblesthat of mild delinquents. They tend to be sexually active at a veryyoung age; they tend to have committed a number of minor sociallydisapproved acts, such as getting into fights, truancy, getting drunkat a young age, and smoking early. Few have held full-time jobs atthe time they take up drug abuse. If they delay drug use until theyenter college, those in the humanities or social sciences seem morevulnerable than those in the hard sciences and mathematics. Thebelief system of those vulnerable to drug use has clearly been n<strong>on</strong>c<strong>on</strong>formist.They are generally areligious, not greatly attached to home,and generally tolerant of deviance in others. They do not, for instance,voice str<strong>on</strong>g disapproval of shoplifting or truancy.The characteristics we have described not <strong>on</strong>ly tell us which childrenwho have not yet used drugs are particularly liable to become drugusers, but they also predict the timing of use--those with these characteristicstend to use at a younger age than those without them--andthe frequency of use--those who have these characteristics tend to usemore heavily than children without these characteristics even whenboth use drugs.Most of the results that we have presented so far come from studies ofhigh school and college populati<strong>on</strong>s. These findings apply principallyto the use of marijuana, since that is the <strong>on</strong>ly drug used with sufficientfrequency to be well studied in such general populati<strong>on</strong>s. It isinteresting, therefore, to compare these results with our results fromthe Vietnam veteran study, in which we were studying men with easyaccess in Vietnam not <strong>on</strong>ly to marijuana but also to narcotics. Westudied a sample of about 1,000 Army enlisted men at ten m<strong>on</strong>ths aftertheir return from Vietnam, and we then reexamined a selected twothirdsof them when they had been back in the States three years.All had left Vietnam during the m<strong>on</strong>th of September 1971. We interviewed96 percent of our target sample the first time, and 94 percentof that part of the selected sample that we intended to interview thesec<strong>on</strong>d time. We matched these veterans with a group of n<strong>on</strong>veteranschosen from draft registrati<strong>on</strong>s, in order to see whether the same usepatterns held for men who did not serve in Vietnam. At the time weinterviewed the veterans for the sec<strong>on</strong>d time, most were 23 to 24 yearsof age. In figure 1, we look at preservice predictors of their druguse during the sec<strong>on</strong>d and third years after their return from Vietnam.As figure 1 shows, social class was unimportant in predicting drug usein veterans, as it had been in studies of students. <strong>On</strong> the otherhand, other demographic variables, including growing up in an innercity, being black, and entering the service at a very young age wereall related to drug use. Early drug use, that is, before the age atwhich they entered service (i.e., age 18 or younger), also predicted219


FIGURE 1.–Preservice predictors of any illicitdrug use by veterans 1973-1974From D.E. Smith, S.M. Anders<strong>on</strong>. M. Buxt<strong>on</strong>, N. Gottlieb, W. Harvey,and T. Chung, eds., A Multicultural View of <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>--Proceedingsof the Nati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong> C<strong>on</strong>ference,1977, p.77. (Cambridge. Mass.:Hall/Schenkman, 1978). Copyright © 1978. Reprinted with permissi<strong>on</strong> ofthe publisher.220


drug use at ages 23 and 24. The best predictor of all was deviantbehavior before service. The deviant behavior scale was made up offive behaviors: truanting, expulsi<strong>on</strong> or dropping out of high school,getting arrested, fighting, and getting drunk before age 15. Wecombined the predictive variables--demographic, drug use, and deviance--intowhat we called a “youthful liability scale.” This scale didan excellent job of predicting drug use. We also found that it didvery well for n<strong>on</strong>veterans in the same age period.Our study c<strong>on</strong>firmed the findings of school studies that broken homesand parental alcoholism and drug use predicted veterans’ drug use.However, we found that these family variables added nothing to our“youthful liability scale.” Apparently coming from this kind of familyhelped to explain the preservice deviance and early exposure to drugswhich in turn predicted drug use in the twenties, but it had no directeffect <strong>on</strong> drug use at that age.We found very little else that was predictive of drug abuse in thetwenties, although those who had seen a doctor for a nervous ormental difficulty before going into service and who had not worked fulltime had somewhat increased rates of drug use.The youthful liability scale predicted use of each of the drugs studied.We studied use of four major types of drugs: marijuana, amphetamines,barbiturates, and heroin. Heroin users had a higher youthful liabilityscore than did users of any other class of drugs. For drugs otherthan heroin, increased scale scores were associated with a greaterfrequency of use, but am<strong>on</strong>g heroin users, there was no variati<strong>on</strong> byfrequency. Use of heroin at any level was associated with a very highscore.There have now been a large number of studies showing that illicitdrug use typically starts with marijuana, and that approximately <strong>on</strong>ehalfof the marijuana users then try some other drug. If there is <strong>on</strong>ly<strong>on</strong>e drug that is going to be used, it is almost always marijuana. Thisis true in almost every study that we have seen, including the Vietnamveterans. When veterans used a single drug, it was marijuana in nineout of ten cases. Since marijuana is typically the first drug of abuse,it has been called “the stepping st<strong>on</strong>e to drug addicti<strong>on</strong>.” This nominati<strong>on</strong>has raised endless discussi<strong>on</strong> as to whether marijuana use “causes”the use of other drugs. Those who say “no” point to the half whouse marijuana and never go <strong>on</strong> to anything else. Those who say “yes”point to the fact that the use of other drugs rarely occurs in theabsence of marijuana use. At present marijuana use seems to be anecessary but not a sufficient c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> for the progressi<strong>on</strong> to otherdrugs.The “stepping st<strong>on</strong>e” hypothesis is clearly wr<strong>on</strong>g if it is taken toimply that when marijuana users go <strong>on</strong> to other drugs, they drop theiruse of marijuana. In our experience and that of most other studies, itappears that as new drugs are tried, the drug repertoire grows,rather than experiencing the displacement of <strong>on</strong>e drug by another;Use of the less popular drugs, therefore, implies the use of manydrugs. Am<strong>on</strong>g both our veterans and our n<strong>on</strong>veterans, there is astr<strong>on</strong>g negative correlati<strong>on</strong> between the frequency with which a particulardrug is used and the number of other drugs used during the sametime period.221


Those marijuana users who go <strong>on</strong> to other drugs are almost exclusivelythose who have used marijuana frequently and who began its useearly. Most Vietnam veterans who used marijuana several times a weekused other drugs as well. Most of those who used marijuana morerarely used nothing else. There is also the fact that the earliermarijuana is used, the more likely it is that there will be other drugsused as well. Marijuana use beginning at age 20 or later in our sampleof young black men (Robins and Murphy 1967) was typically infrequent,mild, and involved use of no other drugs at all.Heroin is a drug that is used infrequently, and thus heroin userstypically use many other drugs as well. This phenomen<strong>on</strong> may havec<strong>on</strong>tributed to heroin’s reputati<strong>on</strong> as an especially dangerous drug.To find out whether heroin’s bad name is largely explained by itsplace late in the sequence of adding new drugs, we compared <strong>on</strong> anumber of adult variables the outcomes of veterans who used heroinwith the outcomes of other veterans, holding c<strong>on</strong>stant the number ofother drugs used at all, specific other drugs used regularly, and theiryouthful liability scale scores, since this scale predicted general adjustmentas well as drug use. When we c<strong>on</strong>trolled <strong>on</strong> these factors, wefound that heroin use was associated with an increase in adjustmentproblems such as crime, alcoholism, violence, unemployment, andmarital breakup, but the increase in such problems accounted for byheroin was no greater than the increase accounted for by the use ofamphetamines or barbiturates, similarly studied. Thus the especiallybad reputati<strong>on</strong> of heroin seems due more to the kinds of people whouse it and the large number of other drugs they use al<strong>on</strong>g with itthan to properties of the drug itself.INTERPRETING THE RESULTSSo far, I have tried to describe what we know about the naturalhistory of drug abuse up to the point of addicti<strong>on</strong>, with due recogniti<strong>on</strong>that this descripti<strong>on</strong> is very much a product of <strong>on</strong>e historical era,and that there is variati<strong>on</strong> by locati<strong>on</strong>, populati<strong>on</strong>, and availability ofthe drugs even within this era. There are important subpopulati<strong>on</strong>s ofabusers, such as those overusing prescribed drugs and drug-abusingdoctors and nurses that I have not included here at all, in part becausethey have not been as fully studied.To summarize these findings, we find that drug use occurs disproporti<strong>on</strong>atelyin young people with average or better than average IQs,who come from minority groups, are urban, who have disaffecti<strong>on</strong> forschool, and who are critical of the c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al social mores of theirtimes; that the earlier drug use begins, the more serious it is; thatuse typically progresses al<strong>on</strong>g quite easily describable <strong>line</strong>s, beginningwith marijuana use, which in itself is predicted by the use of alcoholand cigarettes; and that those who become frequent and heavy marijuanausers have a greatly increased liability of progressi<strong>on</strong> to otherdrugs, although they do not give up the use of marijuana as they addnew drugs. We have also found that many of the reported characteristicsof heroin do not really seem to be distinctive. Heroin of thequality recently available <strong>on</strong> the street does not seem to differ fromother drugs in its liability to frequent use or daily use, althoughregular users of it do more often perceive themselves as dependentthan do users of other drugs, even though they seem able to give itup as readily. To what extent their opini<strong>on</strong> reflects heroin’s bad222


eputati<strong>on</strong> rather than their pers<strong>on</strong>al experience of craving is hard tosay.Having described the natural history of drug abuse in the UnitedStates in the 1970s. there remains the difficult issue of trying tounderstand the implicati<strong>on</strong>s of these findings. Is drug abuse simply<strong>on</strong>e part of the general pattern of deviant behavior that we call “c<strong>on</strong>ductdisorder” when it occurs in children and “antisocial pers<strong>on</strong>ality”when it occurs in adults? Or is it simply <strong>on</strong>e expressi<strong>on</strong> of adolescentrebelli<strong>on</strong> and deviance am<strong>on</strong>g many others? If so, then what we describeas the “natural history of drug abuse” may have little to do witheffects of exposure to drugs but may instead be a descripti<strong>on</strong> of thecourse of development of juvenile deviance or adolescent rebelli<strong>on</strong>.The progressi<strong>on</strong> to the use of a variety of drugs and then the c<strong>on</strong>sequentwithdrawal from drug use may parallel the general pattern ofdevelopment of adolescent deviance, followed by a dec<strong>line</strong> in deviancewith maturati<strong>on</strong>. To throw some light <strong>on</strong> that questi<strong>on</strong>, we first needto say what the characteristic pattern of development of adolescentdeviance is and how closely drug abuse follows the same pattern.In an earlier study (Robins 1966) exploring the development of theantisocial pers<strong>on</strong>ality, we discovered that it is primarily a male phenomen<strong>on</strong>,that it usually begins in the early school years with schoolfailure and truancy, progresses by adolescence into drinking excessively,dropping out of school, and delinquency. Our study and other studiesof delinquents find their typical IQ score to be slightly below normal,usually in the low 90s. There seems to be some associati<strong>on</strong> withminority group membership. Parents of deviant children often have ahistory of antisocial behavior themselves, particularly of excessivedrinking and crime. Childhood deviance encompasses a variety ofjuvenile problem behaviors which are all highly intercorrelated, andeach is independently correlated with each of the adult behaviors thatare part of antisocial pers<strong>on</strong>ality (Robins 1978). No single childhoodbehavior appears necessary to the development of antisocial pers<strong>on</strong>ality,and the variety of childhood deviant behaviors is a better predictorthan is the occurrence of any specific type of behavior. The typicaladult antisocial pattern includes chr<strong>on</strong>ic unemployment, marital breakup,multiple arrests, excessive drinking, and irresp<strong>on</strong>sibility toward sexualpartners and children. Like the childhood behaviors, these adultoutcomes are highly intercorrelated. Often they terminate in middleage.Can we see drug abuse as part of this general process? Clearly thereare both differences and similarities. Occasi<strong>on</strong>al or mild drug useseems clearly not to be part of the antisocial pers<strong>on</strong>ality. It encompassestoo large a proporti<strong>on</strong> of youth, and has few adverse c<strong>on</strong>sequences.While more serious abuse of drugs resembles generaladolescent deviance in its c<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong> in urban male minority groupsfrom broken homes and its associati<strong>on</strong> with adolescent delinquency,school dropout, and early drinking, it does not occur disproporti<strong>on</strong>atelyin pers<strong>on</strong>s from impoverished families or in children with lower thanaverage IQs, or in those with early school failure and truancy. Itssex distributi<strong>on</strong> is not so <strong>on</strong>e-sided as is the distributi<strong>on</strong> of delinquencyor adult antisocial pers<strong>on</strong>ality.In adolescence and adult life, the correlates of serious drug abuse arevery similar to those of antisocial pers<strong>on</strong>ality. Those who use drugsheavily have higher than expected rates of adult arrest, unemployment,marital breakup, alcohol problems, and child neglect. <strong>Drug</strong> abusers223


often seem to improve with aging, as do those with antisocial pers<strong>on</strong>ality,although their recovery may well be earlier--probably between 25 and30 rather than in the fourth decade. Further, those young peoplewho have the predictors and course typical of antisocial pers<strong>on</strong>ality areindeed likely to abuse illicit drugs, just as they tend to smoke anddrink more than average.Thus the present picture is a c<strong>on</strong>fusing <strong>on</strong>e. Certainly there is someoverlap between antisocial pers<strong>on</strong>ality and serious drug abuse, butthere are also striking differences. The most reas<strong>on</strong>able positi<strong>on</strong> atthe present time seems to be that drug abuse can be part of antisocialpers<strong>on</strong>ality, but that most drug abusers probably do not have thatsyndrome, since the typical drug abuser is so different in terms ofIQ, social class, history of elementary school problems, and very earlyterminati<strong>on</strong>.The fact that the preuse history of drug abusers is more favorablethan that of pers<strong>on</strong>s with antisocial pers<strong>on</strong>ality, and yet the adultoutcomes are often equally disastrous, leaves us with the possibilitythat it is exposure to drugs itself that may be harmful, in additi<strong>on</strong> toany underlying effects of the predispositi<strong>on</strong> of the drug user. Whilethis is an important c<strong>on</strong>cern, the good recovery of Vietnam veteransshows that any harm that the drugs may engender need not be permanentor irreversible, if the supply of drugs again c<strong>on</strong>tracts. I amafraid that the implicati<strong>on</strong>s of these findings are that we must c<strong>on</strong>tinueto rely <strong>on</strong> supply c<strong>on</strong>trol as a chief preventive measure, until we canprovide some other explanati<strong>on</strong> for the adverse outcomes of those whobecome frequent users of illicit drugs.224


A Theory of <strong>Drug</strong> DependenceBased <strong>on</strong> Role, Access to,and Attitudes Toward <strong>Drug</strong>sCharles Winick, Ph.D.Why is a theory of drug dependence needed? Most theories help us tounderstand a specific situati<strong>on</strong> or substance. But we now find dependence<strong>on</strong> a wide range of substances am<strong>on</strong>g so many different groupsand even countries that a heuristic theory must improve our ability tounderstand the whole spectrum of dependence. With the c<strong>on</strong>tinuingdevelopment of new substances of dependence, it seems foolhardy todevelop a theory of drug dependence that is linked to any <strong>on</strong>e chemical.Dependence involves taking a substance over a specific period of timeat a specific minimal rate; the time and rate needed for dependencevary with the substance. We generally follow the World Health Organizati<strong>on</strong>definiti<strong>on</strong> of dependence as a state of psychic or physical dependence,or both, <strong>on</strong> a drug, arising in a pers<strong>on</strong> following administrati<strong>on</strong>of that drug <strong>on</strong> a periodic or c<strong>on</strong>tinuous basis (Eddy et al. 1965).Any proposed theory should explain the differential incidence of drugdependence <strong>on</strong> populati<strong>on</strong> subgroups in a manner which does not rely<strong>on</strong> individual pers<strong>on</strong>ality factors. The large number of different kindsof people who have become drug dependent makes it unlikely that theyshare specific pers<strong>on</strong>ality traits. Where such pers<strong>on</strong>ality traits havebeen identified, they usually apply to a wide range of activities and d<strong>on</strong>ot explain why pers<strong>on</strong>s with such traits become drug dependentrather than, for example, join a chess club, although both drug dependentsand chess players may share the same pers<strong>on</strong>ality characteristics(Winick 1957).Our three-pr<strong>on</strong>ged theory suggests that the incidence of drug dependencewill be high in those groups in which there is--1. Access to dependence-producing substances;2. Disengagement from proscripti<strong>on</strong>s against their use; and3. Role strain and/or role deprivati<strong>on</strong>.225


A role is a set of expectati<strong>on</strong>s and behaviors associated with a specificpositi<strong>on</strong> in a social system. A role strain is a felt difficulty in meetingthe obligati<strong>on</strong>s of a role. By role deprivati<strong>on</strong>, we mean the reacti<strong>on</strong> tothe terminati<strong>on</strong> of a significant role relati<strong>on</strong>ship.A role approach can help to minimize fruitless debates over whether<strong>on</strong>e specific factor is more important than another in the genesis ofdrug dependence, because role is a sufficiently dynamic c<strong>on</strong>cept tosubsume a number of other dimensi<strong>on</strong>s. The role approach is c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>antwith modern medical thinking about the effect of stress <strong>on</strong> genesis ofdisease and the integrati<strong>on</strong> of c<strong>on</strong>cepts of psychosomatic disease.Medicine is moving away from allopathic treatment as it integrates thepublic health view of the pers<strong>on</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>ing in a specific envir<strong>on</strong>ment.Instead of having to say that people become drug dependent in orderto meet their pers<strong>on</strong>ality needs, we are suggesting that it is possibleto locate the structural sources of role strain and deprivati<strong>on</strong> withinthe social system. We hypothesize that all points of taking <strong>on</strong> newroles or all points of being tested for adequacy in a role are likely tobe related to role strain and thus to a greater incidence of drugdependence in a group. We also hypothesize that incompatible demandswithin <strong>on</strong>e role, such as between two roles in the same role set, arelikely to lead to a greater incidence of drug dependence. The amountof role strain is a functi<strong>on</strong> of various factors, so that the larger thevolume of properties of a role set, the greater the potential for strain.Role strain is positively correlated with the ambiguity of role obligati<strong>on</strong>s(Snoek 1966). the inc<strong>on</strong>sistency of role obligati<strong>on</strong>s, the distributi<strong>on</strong> ofpower and interest within the role set, the visibility of different roleswithin the role set, and the kind of c<strong>on</strong>formity (attitudinal, behavioral,doctrinal) required by different roles within the role set (Coser 1961).The three pr<strong>on</strong>gs of the theory which are out<strong>line</strong>d above deal with thegenesis of dependence and are relevant to the use of psychoactivesubstances such as marijuana, LSD, amphetamines, barbiturates,peyote, and opiates.<strong>On</strong>e clear applicati<strong>on</strong> of the theory is to pers<strong>on</strong>s whose drug of choiceis heroin. Heroin users are likely to be pers<strong>on</strong>s whose substance useis overdetermined and who have a multiplicity of problems and difficulties,whereas users of other substances are more likely to take themfor specific problems (Blum and Associates 1969). Heroin users aretherefore pers<strong>on</strong>s who are especially likely to experience role difficulties.Because of its history in this country, heroin is typically regardedwith cauti<strong>on</strong> by most people and access to it is not easy.<strong>On</strong>ce we have located the sources of role strain in a society, we canspecify those role situati<strong>on</strong>s which are likely to show a high incidenceof drug dependence. It ought to be possible for us to identify positi<strong>on</strong>sin the social structure which are more vulnerable than others to rolestrain and/or role deprivati<strong>on</strong>. We can also cite role sets within astatus which tend to place a pers<strong>on</strong> in a structural positi<strong>on</strong> of increasedstrain.A theory of drug dependence should enable us to predict (1) whichsubgroups in a populati<strong>on</strong> will be most likely to become dependent and(2) which individuals in a subgroup will be most likely to become dependent.There are always many people who are at risk and who arerecreati<strong>on</strong>al or experimental users but who do not become drug226


dependent. A valid theory should help to explain such occurrences,without relying <strong>on</strong> tenuous pers<strong>on</strong>ality characteristics which may bereflecting drug use rather than c<strong>on</strong>tributing to its etiology.This theory has the merit of explaining the genesis and c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> ofdrug dependence when there is an endemic situati<strong>on</strong>, such as prevailedin the United States in the 1940s and 1950s. and when we could expectthat there will be fairly identifiable characteristics of those who getinvolved with drugs. It also can clarify the genesis and c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong>of drug dependence if there is an epidemic or even a pandemic, asprevailed in the late 1960s, and when so many people are becomingdependent that there is a much broader base of pers<strong>on</strong>s at risk.The theory helps to clarify the initiati<strong>on</strong> of use and its c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong>and expansi<strong>on</strong> into dependence. To the extent that all three pr<strong>on</strong>gsof the theory are met, there will be a greater likelihood of use merginginto dependence. If <strong>on</strong>ly two pr<strong>on</strong>gs are met, there will be a lesserlikelihood of a user becoming dependent. The threshold to dependenceis more likely to be crossed when all three pr<strong>on</strong>gs are operative.In terms of the proposed theory, addicti<strong>on</strong> is regarded as <strong>on</strong>e type ofdependence, and there would seem to be no need for a special theoryof addicti<strong>on</strong>. The relatively diluted street drugs available in the last15 years make addicti<strong>on</strong> a less significant dimensi<strong>on</strong> of dependencethan was the case in the 1930s. Also, the widespread dependence <strong>on</strong>physiologically n<strong>on</strong>addicting substances like marijuana and cocaine andthe prevalence of polydependence would appear to have made addicti<strong>on</strong>less important in the large drug “scene.”The theory regards drug abuse as another dimensi<strong>on</strong> of drug dependence.Although the noti<strong>on</strong> of abuse may have relevance to legislative,public relati<strong>on</strong>s, or funding c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s, it does not seem necessaryas an explanatory variable.SOME APPLICATIONS OF THE THEORY IN AMERICAIn order to get a direct test of the predictive ability of our theory,we developed a role inventory for adolescents. There is good reas<strong>on</strong>to expect that the adolescent years will be heavily complicated becauseof the ambiguity of the status of adolescents in our society, who havelost the role of children but are not yet able to assume an adult role.The 20 items in the inventory measure three dimensi<strong>on</strong>s of the adolescentrole:1. The adolescent’s ability to handle the opti<strong>on</strong>s and possibilities, realand imaginary, open to him or her.2. Positi<strong>on</strong>ing <strong>on</strong>eself am<strong>on</strong>g <strong>on</strong>e’s peers.3. Handling the changes in <strong>on</strong>e’s body.Each subject also answered a number of questi<strong>on</strong>s about family, school,lifestyle, eating and drinking habits, and degree of use of a variety ofpsychoactive substances (Winick 1974c).This role inventory was administered to 1,311 high school juniors inthe metropolitan New York area. Juniors were used because they227


would be unlikely to have the role-adjustment problems of either graduatingseniors or entering students.Scores <strong>on</strong> the role inventory were translated into a maximum of 100,with a relatively high score indicating comfort and a minimum of rolec<strong>on</strong>flict and/or deprivati<strong>on</strong>. The students in the lowest quartile of therole inventory were regarded, in terms of our theory, as high risks interms of use of marijuana; the other three-fourths of the studentswere c<strong>on</strong>sidered low risks. We found that the proporti<strong>on</strong> of high-riskadolescents using marijuana at least <strong>on</strong>ce a week or more for at leastfour weeks during the preceding year was 11 percent. However, <strong>on</strong>lytwo percent of the low-risk group had used marijuana <strong>on</strong>ce weekly ormore for at least four weeks during the preceding year; the differencebetween the two groups was statistically significant (X 2 =49, df=1,p < 0.001).In additi<strong>on</strong> to such specific tests of the theory, we can infer thepresence in drug-dependent pers<strong>on</strong>s of such role variables from sec<strong>on</strong>daryanalysis of data collected for other purposes. For example, althoughthe age at which a young pers<strong>on</strong> is allowed to work varies from Stateto State, we find that almost without excepti<strong>on</strong> it is an age at whichthere is a peak incidence of new cases of drug dependence (U.S.Department of Labor 1966). Thus, in New York, where the youngpers<strong>on</strong> may leave school and begin working at 16, the age of 16 hasl<strong>on</strong>g been the age at which <strong>on</strong>e is most susceptible to beginning regularuse of heroin. At the time when glue sniffing was a serious problem,the incidence of glue sniffing was highest am<strong>on</strong>g youngsters leavingsixth grade and entering junior high school (Winick and Goldstein1965). Comparable 12-year-olds who were in an eight-year elementaryschool displayed far less glue sniffing.Johnst<strong>on</strong> (1973), in <strong>on</strong>e of the very few studies to follow a large(2,200) sample of adolescent males for some years, found that therewas a clear and positive relati<strong>on</strong>ship between negative attitudes towardthe Vietnam war, negative attitudes toward government, and the use ofmarijuana, hallucinogens, and amphetamines. We can interpret negativeattitudes toward the war and government as dimensi<strong>on</strong>s of role strain.Seven out of ten of the resp<strong>on</strong>dents said they thought marijuana wouldbe easy to obtain. Proscripti<strong>on</strong>s against drug use are less salientam<strong>on</strong>g young people than am<strong>on</strong>g other groups.Many other existing studies of drug dependence am<strong>on</strong>g young peoplecan be c<strong>on</strong>structively interpreted in terms of our theory of role strain/deprivati<strong>on</strong>, access, and attitudes. These include studies of delinquents(Cloward and Ohlin 1960); Chicago heroin addicts (Finest<strong>on</strong>e 1957):Colorado marijuana users (Jessor and Jessor 1973); and New York Cityaddicts (Chein et al. 1964). If we look at these studies, the datathey provide are compatible with our theory, although all these studieswere c<strong>on</strong>ducted independently of our theory.A large-scale study of the life cycle of addicti<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cluded that itsgenesis was c<strong>on</strong>centrated during the years of late adolescence andearly adulthood because of the role strain stemming from decisi<strong>on</strong>sabout sex, adult resp<strong>on</strong>sibility, social relati<strong>on</strong>ships, family situati<strong>on</strong>s,school, and work, as well as from role deprivati<strong>on</strong> resulting from theloss of familiar patterns of behavior (Winick 1964).There are many potentially hazardous c<strong>on</strong>sequences of role disc<strong>on</strong>tinuityand a lack of order and sequence in the cultural training of a pers<strong>on</strong>228


moving al<strong>on</strong>g a life cycle (Benedict 1938). Americans have increasinglybeen deprived of significant role-related ritual experiences that help inthe achievement of an emoti<strong>on</strong>al state that could bridge the gap betweenold and new. The role-related ritual helped to give meaning to thec<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> of <strong>on</strong>e phase of the life cycle and the commencement ofanother, providing a sense of community and publicly affirming thesubject’s social and pers<strong>on</strong>al identity and the move from <strong>on</strong>e age andstatus group to another. As modern American rites of passage havebecome more subdued, people have had a lesser role identity and lessopportunity to develop a sense of self. Insufficiently graded sequencesof role positi<strong>on</strong>s through which people move may be dysfuncti<strong>on</strong>al andcould be related to the <strong>on</strong>set of drug dependence (Winick 1968).STUDIES OF SPECIAL GROUPSIt is possible to apply our theory to a variety of special groups whichhave had a high incidence of drug dependence: Native Americans,soldiers in Vietnam, college students, jazz musicians, physicians, andnurses.NATIVE AMERICANSA study of Menomini Indians c<strong>on</strong>cluded that the members of a tribe mostdrawn to peyote had difficulty in developing role relati<strong>on</strong>ships eitherwith the tribe or the world outside (Spindler 1952).SOLDIERS IN VIETNAMAnother situati<strong>on</strong> providing data relevant for our theory can be derivedby analysis of the experience of the American troops in Vietnam. Astudy of Army enlisted men in Vietnam c<strong>on</strong>cluded that approximately 35percent of this group tried heroin at least <strong>on</strong>ce during their “hitch.”Fully 20 percent of the troops were “strung out,” or dependent <strong>on</strong> thedrug during their year of service (Robins 1973). While serving inVietnam, the soldiers had (1) access to heroin, which was cheap andfreely available; (2) disengagement from negative proscripti<strong>on</strong>s aboutits use because many of the natives as well as other soldiers werealready using it; and (3) severe role strain because of boredom,homesickness, uneasiness, the ambiguity of our role in Vietnam, thelack of a clearly defined “fr<strong>on</strong>t,” and the enormous oppositi<strong>on</strong> to thewar in the United States, all of which combined to make the strain sosevere that tours of duty there were limited to <strong>on</strong>e year.COLLEGE STUDENTSThere are a number of studies of drug use am<strong>on</strong>g college studentswhich, although they were c<strong>on</strong>ducted for other purposes, lend themselvesto interpretati<strong>on</strong> in terms of our theory. Certainly at manycolleges there is a high degree of access to drugs and emancipatedattitudes toward their use, which means that two of our three c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>sare met. <strong>Drug</strong> use is favored by those students, we would argue,who are experiencing role strain and/or role deprivati<strong>on</strong>. Am<strong>on</strong>g thec<strong>on</strong>tributors to role strain am<strong>on</strong>g the young are the current c<strong>on</strong>fusi<strong>on</strong>over the mascu<strong>line</strong> and feminine roles, the dec<strong>line</strong> in clothing as an229


indicator of age-graded role expectati<strong>on</strong>s, the role competitivenessinduced by the large numbers of young people seeking similar goals,disillusi<strong>on</strong> about c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al roles, loss of positive role models in massmedia, and c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> of the noti<strong>on</strong> that many of our role models inpublic life are less than admirable (Winick 1973).Suchman (1968), in a survey of a large West Coast university, foundthat marijuana use was correlated positively with reading undergroundnewspapers, negative reacti<strong>on</strong>s to educati<strong>on</strong>, respect for the “hippie”way of life, approval of “getting around the law, and other dimensi<strong>on</strong>sof a “hang loose” ethic, which we can interpret as a special case ofthe larger phenomen<strong>on</strong> of role strain.In a survey of almost 8,000 college students throughout the country,Groves (1974) found a positive correlati<strong>on</strong> between marijuana, psychedelics,opium, and methamphetamine use and counterculture attitudes.The latter may be interpreted as reflecti<strong>on</strong>s of what we would c<strong>on</strong>siderrole strain.The recurrent finding that the incidence of drug dependence and useis higher am<strong>on</strong>g liberal arts than engineering students and higheram<strong>on</strong>g undergraduates than graduate students can be interpreted interms of role theory (Marra 1967). The liberal arts and undergraduatestudents are less explicitly role oriented and experience more rolestrain than the engineer-to-be or graduate student, who has made acareer commitment which she or he is pursuing with a certain degreeof awareness of what lies ahead.Similarly, the finding that drug use is more comm<strong>on</strong> am<strong>on</strong>g studentsliving off campus and not with their families than am<strong>on</strong>g dormitoryresidents or students living with their families can be understood interms of the greater role strain to which the off-campus students aresubjected (McKenzie 1969).MUSICIANSThe theory has helped to explain the genesis and c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> of drugdependence am<strong>on</strong>g jazz musicians (Winick 1960, 1961b, 1962b). Jazzmusicians tend to have liberated attitudes toward drugs, and theyoften perform in places where drugs are freely available. The occupati<strong>on</strong>involves massive role strain, because of uncertainty over employment,the need for improvisati<strong>on</strong>, and c<strong>on</strong>tinually changing musicalstyles. <strong>Drug</strong> dependence am<strong>on</strong>g jazz musicians has c<strong>on</strong>sistently peakedat times when role deprivati<strong>on</strong> threatened performers, such as thetransiti<strong>on</strong> from Dixieland to swing (1930-35), from swing to bop (1945-49). and from jazz to rock (1954-58). Musicians who became drugusers tended to be those who felt threatened by the shift from <strong>on</strong>ekind of music to another. The same kind of phenomen<strong>on</strong> could befound am<strong>on</strong>g rock musicians as they moved from rhythm and blues(1955-57) to the British sound in the early 1960s to folk rock (1965-67)to hard rock (1970-71) and “crossover” music (1977-78).PHYSICIANSPhysicians have l<strong>on</strong>g been identified as an occupati<strong>on</strong>al group with ahigh rate of drug dependence (Winick 1961a). Physicians have accessto drugs of dependence and tend to have emancipated attitudes toward230


their use. Physicians who become dependent may even have magical oromnipotent attitudes toward drugs. (“Because I am a doctor, I willknow when to stop.”)Interview studies with 315 drug-dependent physicians c<strong>on</strong>cluded thatmedical specialties which traditi<strong>on</strong>ally involve c<strong>on</strong>siderable role strain,such as psychiatry and surgery, have a disproporti<strong>on</strong>ately high rateof drug dependence. Also overrepresented am<strong>on</strong>g addicted physiciansare those in career c<strong>on</strong>tingencies that are likely to produce role strain:last year of residency, year before taking board examinati<strong>on</strong>s, inabilityto handle overwork, c<strong>on</strong>flict between a humanitarian and entrepreneurshipview of medicine, ambivalence about being a physician, and c<strong>on</strong>flictbetween demands of the professi<strong>on</strong> and of a spouse. Role deprivati<strong>on</strong>figures in another group of addict physicians, those who are movingfrom <strong>on</strong>e type of practice to another, failing specialty boards, movingtheir office, leaving <strong>on</strong>e specialty for another, facing retirement, orare c<strong>on</strong>cerned about the effect of illness or their ability to practice.NURSESAbout <strong>on</strong>e percent of the approximately 650,000 American nurses aredrug dependent. Nurses have access to drugs because they administerthem to patients and c<strong>on</strong>trol their use in hospitals. They are relativelyemancipated in terms of attitudes toward their use because of familiaritywith their analgesic properties. In an interview study of 195 drugdependentnurses, role strain and deprivati<strong>on</strong> were significant c<strong>on</strong>tributorsto the beginning of the dependence (Winick 1974a). Am<strong>on</strong>g therole strain factors which emerged were extreme fatigue, physicalailments making for work difficulties, quarrels with coworkers, disagreementsbetween the nurse’s c<strong>on</strong>cepti<strong>on</strong> of her job and urgencies of thework situati<strong>on</strong>, c<strong>on</strong>flict between demands of a family situati<strong>on</strong> and thejob. and pressures arising from c<strong>on</strong>flicting demands of the nurse’srole. Am<strong>on</strong>g the role deprivati<strong>on</strong> factors found in the drug-dependentnurses were uneasiness about leaving bedside nursing for a promoti<strong>on</strong>to supervisor, the loss of a significant pers<strong>on</strong>al relati<strong>on</strong>ship (viadeath, a child moving out, or divorce), facing retirement, or leaving afamiliar situati<strong>on</strong>.FOREIGN EPIDEMICSThe theory has been successfully used to clarify the reas<strong>on</strong>s for ahuge increase in drug dependence in the three countries which haveexperienced the most thoroughly documented post-World War II epidemics:Japan, Switzerland, and Sweden.The amphetamine epidemic which swept Japan between 1945 and 1955and involved more than 2,000,000 people centered <strong>on</strong> groups such asartists, Korean emigres, young male delinquents, and ec<strong>on</strong>omicallymarginal pers<strong>on</strong>s who had been dislocated from their jobs and othermoorings by post-War social change (Brill and Hirose 1969). We suggestthat such pers<strong>on</strong>s were resp<strong>on</strong>ding to role strain and/or deprivati<strong>on</strong>.In Japan, methamphetamine was available without prescripti<strong>on</strong> in 1945in large quantities. The drugs were promoted actively for their moodelevatingproperties by manufacturers. The situati<strong>on</strong> in Japan meetsthe three criteria of access, freedom from negative proscripti<strong>on</strong>s, androle strain and/or deprivati<strong>on</strong>. The groups that did become drugdependent were usually vulnerable to role strain and/or deprivati<strong>on</strong>.231


So<strong>on</strong> after World War II, drug dependence to analgesic compoundsc<strong>on</strong>taining phenacetin, caffeine, and a hypnotic drug became a severeproblem in the German-speaking part of Switzerland (Kielholz andBattegay 1963). Some 80 percent of these cases were women whotended to fall into two groups: working housewives experiencing rolestrain because of the multiple demands posed by their jobs, housework,and raising children, and single women who experienced role deprivati<strong>on</strong>as a result of moving into urban areas from the country in order tobecome piecework employees of the watch and textile factories. Incommunities with such factories, about <strong>on</strong>e percent of the populati<strong>on</strong>was dependent <strong>on</strong> these substances. Because Switzerland is the homeof some of the world’s largest pharmaceutical manufacturers, the analgesicsubstances were not <strong>on</strong>ly easily available but were advertised asharmless. All three elements of our theory are relevant to the Swisssituati<strong>on</strong>.A third foreign example is provided by Sweden, which had some 200,000amphetamine users around 1959, when widespread n<strong>on</strong>medical use ofvarious amphetamines began (Goldberg 1968). The users tended to besingle or divorced adults, from homes where the parents were divorced(41 percent as against three percent in the normal populati<strong>on</strong>); nomadicand disaffected youths; and others whose life situati<strong>on</strong>s posed problemsof role strain or deprivati<strong>on</strong>. There was relative acceptance of amphetamines,which were easily available. All three pr<strong>on</strong>gs of our theory arerelevant to the Swedish epidemic.CESSATION OF DRUG DEPENDENCEThe theory suggests that a populati<strong>on</strong> or subgroup will tend to ceasedrug dependence when (1) access to the substances dec<strong>line</strong>s, (2) negativeattitudes to their use become salient, and (3) role strain and/ordeprivati<strong>on</strong> are less prevalent. If all three of these trends are operative,the rate of drug dependence will dec<strong>line</strong> more rapidly than if<strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e or two trends are relevant.Several examples illustrate the dynamics of cessati<strong>on</strong>. Of the largenumber of soldiers who were addicted in Vietnam, <strong>on</strong>ly seven percenthave been addicted at any time since their return to America (Robins1973).If we explain the genesis of the relatively high rate of Vietnam heroinuse in terms of our theory, can we use the same theory to explain itsrelative n<strong>on</strong>resumpti<strong>on</strong> by the soliders? Yes, because when theyreturned to this country, the soldiers came to a situati<strong>on</strong> in which(1) a major law enforcement effort had made drugs relatively inaccessibleand expensive, (2) there was a str<strong>on</strong>g feeling of disapproval ofheroin and growing acceptance of the negative proscripti<strong>on</strong>s about it,and (3) less role strain because the soldiers were out of Vietnam andusually no l<strong>on</strong>ger in uniform.Perhaps the single most successful treatment program for drug addicts,in terms of recovery rates, was the Musicians’ Clinic (Winick andNyswander 1961). <strong>On</strong>e reas<strong>on</strong> that it was so successful is that itfaced and dealt with the musicians’ role c<strong>on</strong>flicts about their work.The very existence of the clinic, which was widely publicized, alsoc<strong>on</strong>tributed to an atmosphere in which musicians’ attitudes toward druguse became less accepting.232


In Japan, the drug epidemic ended in a few years because when thedangers of the situati<strong>on</strong> became clear, Japanese authorities acteddecisively to c<strong>on</strong>trol the availiability of amphetamines, change attitudestoward their use, and assist those users who needed treatment. Theenormous boom in the Japanese ec<strong>on</strong>omy and the stabilizati<strong>on</strong> of thesociety further helped to minimize role dislocati<strong>on</strong>s and, thus, in termsof our hypothesis, pr<strong>on</strong>eness to drug dependence.Similarly, in Switzerland, the drug epidemic ended in the 1960s becausethe Swiss acted to educate the public <strong>on</strong> the possible hazards of thesesubstances, made access to them more difficult, and provided treatmentfor those already afflicted. The educati<strong>on</strong> and treatment effort wasquite successful for a number of reas<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>on</strong>e of which was that therole c<strong>on</strong>flicts of the high-risk populati<strong>on</strong> were faced.Because the Swedish Government has d<strong>on</strong>e little to deal with the availabilityof drugs, favorable attitudes toward their use, or role c<strong>on</strong>flictsam<strong>on</strong>g its populati<strong>on</strong>, drug dependence still c<strong>on</strong>tinues there as asevere problem.RELAPSEHere or abroad, a pers<strong>on</strong> may, of course, cease drug dependence,stop using for some period of time, and then relapse. The reas<strong>on</strong>s forrelapse, in terms of this theory, would reflect the pers<strong>on</strong>’s inability tosustain the role of the n<strong>on</strong>user. Each period of abstinence may representa trying out of the n<strong>on</strong>user’s role. It is likely that the mostcomm<strong>on</strong> pattern of cessati<strong>on</strong> of drug dependence involves experimentati<strong>on</strong>with the n<strong>on</strong>user’s role until it is c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ant with other aspects ofthe pers<strong>on</strong>’s life.An earlier formulati<strong>on</strong> of the theory argued that drug-dependentpers<strong>on</strong>s “matured out” when there was a lessening of the role pressureswhich had led to the beginning of regular drug use (Winick 1962a).The process of “maturing out” was slow and typically involved a stopstartpattern of drug use until the pers<strong>on</strong> felt comfortable with therole of the n<strong>on</strong>user. This was the most frequently found manner ofcessati<strong>on</strong> of drug dependence, and there is reas<strong>on</strong> to believe that it isstill the most prevalent form of terminati<strong>on</strong> of regular drug use.In the original study which led to the formulati<strong>on</strong> of the “maturingout” theory, based <strong>on</strong> a nati<strong>on</strong>al sample, the mean age of “maturingout” was 35 (Winick 1962a). A study of Puerto Ricans who weredependent <strong>on</strong> opiates c<strong>on</strong>cluded that those who “matured out” did so atthe mean age of 33 (Ball and Snarr 1969). An analysis of the phenomen<strong>on</strong>in New York City c<strong>on</strong>cluded that pers<strong>on</strong>s listed in the NarcoticsRegister who “matured out” did so at a mean age of 34 (Snow 1974).This narrow clustering of age at “maturing out” in different samples atdifferent times suggests that there are underlying regularities in theprocess. Ethnicity, sex, residence, access to and salience of drugs,attitudes toward drugs in an area, and the extent to which n<strong>on</strong>drugrelatedroles are plausible and reinforced, c<strong>on</strong>tribute to cessati<strong>on</strong> ofdrug use, as does the extent to which the user experiences less rolestrain and/or deprivati<strong>on</strong>.233


LINKAGES WITH OTHER PROBLEM BEHAVIORThe drug-dependent pers<strong>on</strong> may or may not be involved with othersocial problem behavior, such as crime. In recent years, a substantialproporti<strong>on</strong> of those who become drug dependent have also been involvedwith a larger pattern of deviant activity, over and above their illegalpurchase of drugs. During the 1950s, such a pattern was less comm<strong>on</strong>.The relati<strong>on</strong>ship between drug use and other forms of deviance is afuncti<strong>on</strong> of socioec<strong>on</strong>omic status, life changes, anchorages in the“square” culture, a pers<strong>on</strong>’s place in the life cycle, and many otherfactors. <strong>Drug</strong>s serve many different purposes for people, and thesepurposes c<strong>on</strong>tribute substantially to whether or not the use is part ofa larger antisocial stance.NEGATIVE CASESA theory should be able to explain negative cases, and we can use ourtheory to explain why Army officers serving in Vietnam were virtuallyuninvolved with heroin. In terms of the three pr<strong>on</strong>gs of our theory,Army officers were (1) easily able to get heroin, (2) accepting of thec<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al negative proscripti<strong>on</strong>s about its use because most werecareerists for whom a heroin record would have meant a serious setbackto their futures, and (3) relatively unlikely to experience role strainbecause they were generally volunteers and Vietnam service was almosta prerequisite for rapid promoti<strong>on</strong> and desirable staff assignments.Therefore, two of the three requirements of our theory were not met,and it is not surprising that Army officers largely ignored the availabilityof heroin, whereas enlisted men were far less likely to do so.The low rate of drug dependence am<strong>on</strong>g pharmacists and veterinariansmay be explained by the relative lack of role strain am<strong>on</strong>g them, eventhough they have easy access to drugs and have few negative proscripti<strong>on</strong>sabout them. Similarly, there is hardly any drug dependenceam<strong>on</strong>g certain medical specialties, such as dermatology and radiology,for related reas<strong>on</strong>s. Dermatologists and radiologists have relativelyroutinized practices, with few of the stresses of the psychiatrist orthe peaks and valleys of the surge<strong>on</strong>. A number of other negativecases have been developed.ADVANTAGES OF THE THEORYThe proposed theory has the merit of parsim<strong>on</strong>y and applicability to abroad range of situati<strong>on</strong>s, cultures, and pers<strong>on</strong>s. It can help toexplain and clarify an unfolding or developing situati<strong>on</strong> and predict anupcoming problem. It appears relevant to practically all instances ofdrug dependence except for those which are iatrogenic. The theoryalso has direct implicati<strong>on</strong>s for therapy and public health. It lendsitself to operati<strong>on</strong>al definiti<strong>on</strong>s and combines c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> of therealities of availability and the marketplace, attitudinal dimensi<strong>on</strong>s, andthe central dimensi<strong>on</strong> of role (Winick 1974b). The theory may shedlight <strong>on</strong> historical situati<strong>on</strong>s, current populati<strong>on</strong>s, and it possessespredictive value.234


It lends itself to many different policy and planning purposes, sheddinglight <strong>on</strong> rates of drug dependence in the general populati<strong>on</strong> and am<strong>on</strong>gspecial groups. It is practical in the sense that Paul F. Lazarsfeldmeant when he said that there is little that is as practical as a goodtheory.The most reas<strong>on</strong>able way to deal with drug dependence is an effectiveprogram of preventi<strong>on</strong>. We can identify role strain or deprivati<strong>on</strong>situati<strong>on</strong>s and pay special attenti<strong>on</strong> to methods of handling the associatedproblems. Assuming that society c<strong>on</strong>tinues the prevalent viewthat drug dependence is undesirable, it should be possible to anticipatesituati<strong>on</strong>s likely to be related to high rates of drug dependence and toact in order to deal appropriately with them. C<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> highriskgroups which can be identified as such in terms of role can helpto minimize the hazards of gearing our community programs to specificsubstances.235


The Social Setting as aC<strong>on</strong>trol Mechanism inIntoxicant UseNorman E. Zinberg, M.D.An individual’s decisi<strong>on</strong> to use an intoxicant, the effects it has <strong>on</strong> theuser, and the <strong>on</strong>going psychological and social implicati<strong>on</strong>s of that usedepend not <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong> the pharmaceutical properties of the intoxicant (thedrug) and the attitudes and pers<strong>on</strong>ality of the user (the set), but also<strong>on</strong> the physical and social setting in which such use takes place (Huxley1970; Weil 1972; Zinberg and Roberts<strong>on</strong> 1972). This theoretical positi<strong>on</strong>has been so widely accepted in the last two years as to become almosta truism, but, though lip service is paid to the importance of all threevariables (drug, set, and setting), the influence of the setting <strong>on</strong>intoxicant use and <strong>on</strong> the user is still little understood (Zinberg andDeL<strong>on</strong>g 1974; Zinberg et al. 1975).Even those who make use of this theoretical c<strong>on</strong>struct in analyzing thepatterns of drug use and treating users fail to realize the importantrole played by the setting (both physical and social) as an independentvariable in determining the impact of use. When a drug is administeredin a hospital setting, for example, the effect is very different fromthat experienced by a few people sitting around in a living roomlistening to records. Not <strong>on</strong>ly is there a vast difference between theactual physical locati<strong>on</strong>s, but different social attitudes are involved.In the hospital, the administrati<strong>on</strong> of opiates subsumes the c<strong>on</strong>cepts ofinstituti<strong>on</strong>al structure of therapy and licitness. In the living room,there is a flavor of dangerous adventure, antisocial activity, illicitpleasure, and the c<strong>on</strong>siderable anxiety that accompanies all three.C<strong>on</strong>sidering these differences, it is not surprising that few patients inhospital settings experience c<strong>on</strong>tinued drug involvement after itstherapeutic necessity is past (O’Brien 1978; Zinberg 1974a), whilemany of the living-room users express an intense and c<strong>on</strong>tinued interestin the drug experience.The role of the setting c<strong>on</strong>tinues to be minimized because of the greaterpreoccupati<strong>on</strong> either with the pharmaceutical properties, with thepers<strong>on</strong>al health hazards of the drug itself, or with the pers<strong>on</strong>alitydeteriorati<strong>on</strong> of those who have not been able to c<strong>on</strong>trol their use(Zinberg 1975; Zinberg and Harding 1979). These preoccupati<strong>on</strong>s236


obscure from the scientific community, as well as from the public, theprecise ways in which the setting influences both use itself and theeffects of use, acting either in a positive way to help to regulate useor in a negative way to weaken c<strong>on</strong>trol.This paper defines the mechanisms of c<strong>on</strong>trol developed within thesocial setting, which I call social sancti<strong>on</strong>s and rituals, and the theorybehind their operati<strong>on</strong>. Then it discusses and gives illustrati<strong>on</strong>s ofthe process of social learning by which these mechanisms become activein c<strong>on</strong>trolling use.SOCIAL CONTROLS--SANCTIONS AND RITUALSSocial sancti<strong>on</strong>s are the norms defining whether and how a particulardrug should be used. They include both the informal (and oftenunspoken) values and rules of c<strong>on</strong>duct shared by a group and theformal laws and policies regulating drug use (Zinberg et al. 1977;Maloff et al. 1979). For example, two of the sancti<strong>on</strong>s or basic rulesof c<strong>on</strong>duct that regulate the use of our culture’s favorite drug, alcohol,are “Know your limit” and “D<strong>on</strong>’t drive when you’re drunk.” Socialrituals are the stylized, prescribed behavior patterns surrounding theuse of a drug. These patterns of behavior may apply to the methodsof procuring and administering the drug, the selecti<strong>on</strong> of the physicaland social setting for use, the activities undertaken after the drug hasbeen administered, and the ways of preventing untoward drug effects.Rituals thus serve to buttress, reinforce, and symbolize the sancti<strong>on</strong>s.In the case of alcohol, for example, the statement “Let’s have a drink,”by using the singular term “a drink,” automatically exerts c<strong>on</strong>trol.Social c<strong>on</strong>trols (rituals and sancti<strong>on</strong>s), which apply to all drugs, notjust alcohol, operate in different social c<strong>on</strong>texts, ranging all the wayfrom very large social groups, representative of the culture as awhole, down to small, discrete groups (Harding and Zinberg 1977).Certain types of special-occasi<strong>on</strong> use involving large groups of people--beer at ball games, drugs at rock c<strong>on</strong>certs, wine with meals, cocktailsat six--despite their cultural diversity, have become so generallyaccepted that few, if any, legal strictures are applied even if suchuses technically break the law. For example, a policeman will usuallytell young people with beer cans at an open-air c<strong>on</strong>cert “to knock itoff” but will rarely arrest them, and in many States the police reacti<strong>on</strong>would be the same even if the drug were marijuana (Newmeyer andJohns<strong>on</strong> 1979). The culture as a whole can inculcate a widespreadsocial ritual so thoroughly that it is eventually written into law, justas the socially developed mechanism of the morning coffee break hasbeen legally incorporated into uni<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tracts. The T.G.I.F. (ThankGoodness It’s Friday) drink may not be far from acquiring a similarstatus. Small-group sancti<strong>on</strong>s and rituals tend to be more diverse andmore closely related to circumstances. N<strong>on</strong>etheless, some caveats maybe just as firmly upheld, such as: “Never smoke marijuana until afterthe children are asleep,” “<strong>On</strong>ly drink <strong>on</strong> weekends,” “D<strong>on</strong>’t shoot upuntil the last pers<strong>on</strong> has arrived and the doors are locked.”The existence of social sancti<strong>on</strong>s or rituals does not necessarily meanthat they will be effective, nor does it mean that all sancti<strong>on</strong>s orrituals were devised as mechanisms to aid c<strong>on</strong>trol. “Booting” (thedrawing of blood into and out of a syringe) by heroin addicts seeminglylends enchantment to the use of the needle and therefore opposes237


c<strong>on</strong>trol. But it may <strong>on</strong>ce have served as a c<strong>on</strong>trol mechanism whichgradually became perverted or debased. Some old-time users, atleast, have claimed that booting originated in the (err<strong>on</strong>eous) beliefthat by drawing blood in and out of the syringe, the user could tellthe strength of the drug that was being injected.More important than the questi<strong>on</strong> of whether the sancti<strong>on</strong> or ritual wasoriginally intended as a c<strong>on</strong>trol mechanism is the way in which theuser handles c<strong>on</strong>flicts between sancti<strong>on</strong>s. With illicit drugs, the mostobvious c<strong>on</strong>flict is that between formal and informal social c<strong>on</strong>trols,that is, between the law against use and the social group’s c<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>ingof use. The teenager attending a rock c<strong>on</strong>cert is often pressured intotrying marijuana by his or her peers, who insist that smoking isacceptable at that particular time and place and will enhance the musicalenjoyment. The push to use may include a c<strong>on</strong>trol device, such as,“Since Joey w<strong>on</strong>’t smoke because he has a cold, he can drive,” therebyh<strong>on</strong>oring the “D<strong>on</strong>’t drive after smoking” sancti<strong>on</strong>. Nevertheless, thedecisi<strong>on</strong> to use, so rati<strong>on</strong>ally presented, c<strong>on</strong>flicts with the law and maymake the user w<strong>on</strong>der whether the police will be benign in this instance.Such anxiety interferes with c<strong>on</strong>trol. In order to deal with the c<strong>on</strong>flictthe user will probably come forth with more bravado, exhibiti<strong>on</strong>ism,paranoia, or antisocial feeling than would be the case if he or she hadpatr<strong>on</strong>ized <strong>on</strong>e of the little bars set up al<strong>on</strong>gside the c<strong>on</strong>cert hall forthe selling of alcohol during intermissi<strong>on</strong>. It is this kind of mentalc<strong>on</strong>flict that makes c<strong>on</strong>trol of illicit drugs more complex and difficultthan the c<strong>on</strong>trol of licit drugs across a wide range of pers<strong>on</strong>alitytypes.The existence and applicati<strong>on</strong> of social c<strong>on</strong>trols, particularly in thecase of illicit drugs, does not always lead to moderate, decorous use,and yet it is the reigning cultural belief that c<strong>on</strong>trolled use is orshould be always moderate and decorous. This requirement of decorumis perhaps the chief reas<strong>on</strong> why the power of the social setting toregulate intoxicant use has not been more fully recognized and exploited.The cultural view that the users of intoxicants should always behaveproperly stems from the moralistic attitudes toward such behavior thatpervade our culture, attitudes that are almost as marked in the case oflicit drugs as in the case of illicit drugs. Yet <strong>on</strong> some occasi<strong>on</strong>s--at awedding celebrati<strong>on</strong> or during an adolescent’s first experiment withdrunkenness--less-than-decorous behavior is culturally acceptable.Though we should never c<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>e the excessive use of intoxicants, ithas to be recognized that when such boundary breaking occurs, itdoes not signify a breakdown of overall c<strong>on</strong>trol. Unfortunately, theseoccasi<strong>on</strong>s of impropriety, particularly following the use of illicit drugs,are often taken by moralists to prove what they see as the ultimatetruth: that in the area of drug use there are <strong>on</strong>ly two possible typesof behavior--abstinence or unchecked excess leading to addicti<strong>on</strong>.Despite massive evidence to the c<strong>on</strong>trary, many people c<strong>on</strong>tinue unshakenin this belief.Such a stolid stance affects negatively the development of a rati<strong>on</strong>alunderstanding of c<strong>on</strong>trolled use. Two facts in particular are overlooked.First, the most severe alcoholics and addicts, who cluster at<strong>on</strong>e end of the spectrum of drug use, do not use as much of theintoxicating substance as they could. Some aspects of c<strong>on</strong>trol alwaysoperate. Remarkably few people--particularly some pers<strong>on</strong>ality theoristswho think inhibiti<strong>on</strong> against c<strong>on</strong>trol stems from an actual defect insome aspect of pers<strong>on</strong>ality (Zinberg 1975)--recognize this fact, however,because it is obscured by the appearance of great excess. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, at238


the other end of the spectrum of drug use, as the careful interviewingof ordinary citizens has shown, highly c<strong>on</strong>trolled users and evenabstainers express much more interest in and preoccupati<strong>on</strong> with theuse of intoxicants than is generally acknowledged. Whether to use,when, with whom, how much, how to explain why <strong>on</strong>e does not use--these questi<strong>on</strong>s occupy an important place in the emoti<strong>on</strong>al life ofalmost every citizen. Yet hidden in the American culture lies a deepseatedaversi<strong>on</strong> to acknowledge this preoccupti<strong>on</strong>. As a result, ourculture plays down the importance of the many social mores--sancti<strong>on</strong>sand rituals--that enhance our capacity to c<strong>on</strong>trol use. Thus the wholeissue becomes muddled. Both the existence of c<strong>on</strong>trol <strong>on</strong> the part ofthe most compulsive users and the interest in drugs and the quality ofdrug use (the questi<strong>on</strong>s of with whom, when, and how much to use)<strong>on</strong> the part of the most c<strong>on</strong>trolled users are ignored. We are left withl<strong>on</strong>gings for that utopian society where no <strong>on</strong>e would need drugseither for their pleasant or for their unpleasant effects, either forrelaxati<strong>on</strong> and good fellowship or for escape and torpor.But since such idealized abstinence is socially unacceptable and impossible,the culture’s reigning model of extreme decorum overemphasizesthe pharmaceutical powers of the drug or the pers<strong>on</strong>ality of the user.It inculcates the view that <strong>on</strong>ly a disordered pers<strong>on</strong> would not live upto the cultural standard, or that the quantity or power of the drug isso great that the standard cannot be upheld. To think this way andthus to ignore the social setting requires c<strong>on</strong>siderable psychologicallegerdemain, for, as in most other areas of living, people can rarelyremain indefinitely <strong>on</strong> so decorous a course. Intoxicant use tends tovary with <strong>on</strong>e’s time of life, status, and even geographical locati<strong>on</strong>.Many adolescents who have made heavy use of intoxicants slow downappreciably as they reach adulthood and change their social setting(their friends and circumstances), while some adults, as they becomemore successful, may increase their intoxicant use. A man born andbred in a dry part of Kansas may change his use significantly after amove to New York City. The effects <strong>on</strong> intoxicant use of such variati<strong>on</strong>sin social circumstances have certainly been perceived, but theyare not usually incorporated into a sound theoretical understanding ofhow the social setting influences the use and c<strong>on</strong>trol of intoxicants.The history of the use of alcohol in America provides a striking exampleof the variability of intoxicant use and its c<strong>on</strong>trol (Ade 1931; Bac<strong>on</strong>1969). First, it illustrates the social prescripti<strong>on</strong>s that define thesocial c<strong>on</strong>cept of c<strong>on</strong>trol and, sec<strong>on</strong>d, it shows that the time span ofthese c<strong>on</strong>trol variati<strong>on</strong>s can be as l<strong>on</strong>g as a major historical epoch.Five social prescripti<strong>on</strong>s that define c<strong>on</strong>trolled or moderate use ofalcohol--and these may apply to other intoxicants as well--have beenderived from a study of alcohol use in many different cultures. Allfive of these c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s encourage moderati<strong>on</strong> and discourage excess(Zinberg and Fraser 1979).1. Group drinking is clearly differentiated from drunkenness and isassociated with ritualistic or religious celebrati<strong>on</strong>s.2. Drinking is associated with eating or ritualistic feasting.3. Both of the sexes, as well as different generati<strong>on</strong>s, are includedin the drinking situati<strong>on</strong>, whether they drink or not.239


4. Drinking is divorced from the individual effort to escape pers<strong>on</strong>alanxiety or difficult (even intolerable) social situati<strong>on</strong>s. Further,alcohol is not c<strong>on</strong>sidered medicinally valuable.5. Inappropriate behavior when drinking (violence, aggressi<strong>on</strong>, overtsexuality) is absolutely disapproved, and protecti<strong>on</strong> against suchbehavior is offered by the sober or the less intoxicated. Thisgeneral acceptance of a c<strong>on</strong>cept of restraint usually indicates thatdrinking is <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e of many activities and thus carries a low levelof emoti<strong>on</strong>alism. It also shows that drinking is not associated witha male or female “rite de passage” or sense of superiority.The enormous changes in alcohol use that have occurred since thecol<strong>on</strong>ial period in America illustrate the importance of these socialprescripti<strong>on</strong>s in c<strong>on</strong>trolling the use of alcohol.Pre-Revoluti<strong>on</strong>ary America, though veritably steeped in alcohol, str<strong>on</strong>glyand effectively prohibited drunkenness. Families drank and ate togetherin taverns, and drinking was associated with celebrati<strong>on</strong>s and rituals.Tavernkeepers were people of status; keeping the peace and preventingexcesses stemming from drunkenness were grave duties. Man<strong>line</strong>ss orstrength was measured neither by the extent of c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong> nor byviolent acts resulting from it. Pre-Revoluti<strong>on</strong>ary society, however,did not abide by all the prescripti<strong>on</strong>s, for certain alcoholic beverageswere viewed as medicines: “Groaning beer” was c<strong>on</strong>sumed in largequantities by pregnant and lactating women.With the Revoluti<strong>on</strong>ary War, the industrial revoluti<strong>on</strong>, and the expansi<strong>on</strong>of the fr<strong>on</strong>tier, an era of excess dawned. Men were separatedfrom their families, which left them to drink together and with prostitutes.Alcohol was served without food, was not limited to specialoccasi<strong>on</strong>s, and violence resulting from drunkenness grew. In the faceof increasing drunkenness and alcoholism, people began to believe (asis the case with some illicit drugs today) that it was the powerful,harmful pharmaceutical properties of the intoxicant itself that madec<strong>on</strong>trolled use remote or impossible.The increase in moderati<strong>on</strong> that appeared at the end of the nineteenthcentury was interrupted in the early 1900s by the Volstead Act, whichushered in another era of excess. We are still recovering from thespeakeasy ambience of Prohibiti<strong>on</strong> in which men again drank togetherand often with prostitutes, food was replaced with alcohol, and thedrinking experience was colored with illicitness and potential violence.Although repeal provided relief from excessive and unpopular legal c<strong>on</strong>trol,the society was left floundering without an inherited set of socialsancti<strong>on</strong>s and rituals to c<strong>on</strong>trol use.SOCIAL LEARNINGToday this vacuum has been largely filled. In most sectors of oursociety, informal alcohol educati<strong>on</strong> is readily available. Few childrengrow up without an awareness of a wide range of behaviors associatedwith the use of alcohol, learned from that most pervasive of media,televisi<strong>on</strong>. They see cocktail parties, wine at meals, beer at ballgames, homes broken by drink, drunks whose lives are wrecked, andall the advertisements in which alcohol lends glamor to every occasi<strong>on</strong>.240


Buttressed by movies, the print! media, observati<strong>on</strong> of families andfamily friends, and often by a sip or watered-down taste of the grownups’porti<strong>on</strong>, young people gain an early familiarity with alcohol.When, in a peer group, they begin to drink and even, as a rite ofpassage, to overdo it; they know what they are about and what thesancti<strong>on</strong>s are. The process of finding a “limit” is a direct expressi<strong>on</strong>of “Know your limit.” <strong>On</strong>ce that sancti<strong>on</strong> has been experientiallyinternalized--and our culture provides mores of greater latitude foradolescents than for adults--they can move <strong>on</strong> to such sancti<strong>on</strong>s as “Itis unseemly to be drunk” and “It is all right to have a drink at theend of the day or a few beers <strong>on</strong> the way home from work, or in fr<strong>on</strong>tof the televisi<strong>on</strong>, but d<strong>on</strong>’t drink <strong>on</strong> the job” (Zinberg et al. 1977).This general descripti<strong>on</strong> of the learning or internalizati<strong>on</strong> of socialsancti<strong>on</strong>s, while neat and precise, does not take account of the variati<strong>on</strong>sfrom individual to individual that result from differences inpers<strong>on</strong>ality, cultural background, and group affinity. Specific sancti<strong>on</strong>sand rituals are developed and integrated in varying degrees withdifferent qroups (Edwards 1974). Certainly a New York child from arich, sophisticated family, brought up <strong>on</strong> Saturday lunch with adivorced oarent at The “21” Club, will use drinks in a different wayfrom the small-town child who vividly remembers accompanying a parentto a sporting event where alcohol intake acted as fuel for the excitementof unambivalent partisanship. Yet <strong>on</strong>e comm<strong>on</strong> denominatorshared by young people from these very different social backgroundsis the sense that alcohol is used at special events and bel<strong>on</strong>gs tospecial places.This kind of educati<strong>on</strong> about drug use is social learning, absorbedinchoately and unc<strong>on</strong>sciously as part of the living experience (Zinberg1974b). The learning process is impelled by an unstated and oftenunc<strong>on</strong>scious recogniti<strong>on</strong> by young people that this is an area of emoti<strong>on</strong>alimportance in American society, and, therefore, knowledge aboutit may be quite important in future social and pers<strong>on</strong>al development.Attempts to translate this informal process into the formal drug educati<strong>on</strong>courses, chiefly intended to discourage any use, of the late 1960sand early 1970s have failed. Formal educati<strong>on</strong>, paradoxically, hasstimulated drug use <strong>on</strong> the part of many young people who were previouslyundecided, while c<strong>on</strong>firming the fears of those who were alreadyexcessively c<strong>on</strong>cerned. Is it possible, <strong>on</strong>e might ask, for formal educati<strong>on</strong>to codify social sancti<strong>on</strong>s and rituals in a reas<strong>on</strong>able way for thosewho have somehow been bypassed by the informal process? Or, does thereigning cultural moralism, which has pervaded all such courses, precludethe possibility of discussing reas<strong>on</strong>able informal social c<strong>on</strong>trolsthat may, of course, c<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>e use? So far, these questi<strong>on</strong>s remainunanswered. It will be impossible even to guess at the answers untilour culture has accepted the use not <strong>on</strong>ly of alcohol but of other intoxicantssufficiently to allow teachers to explain how they can be usedsafely and well. Teaching safety is not intended to encourage use; itsmain focus is the preventi<strong>on</strong> of abuse. Similarly, the primary purposeof the few good sex educati<strong>on</strong> courses in existence today is to teach theavoidance of unwanted pregnancy and venereal disease, not the encouragementor the avoidance of sexual activity per se.Whatever happens to formal educati<strong>on</strong> in these areas, the naturalprocess of social learning will inevitably go <strong>on</strong>, for better or worse.The power of this process is illustrated by two recent and extremely241


important social events: the use of psychedelics in the United Statesin the 1960s and the use of heroin during the Vietnam War.Following the Timothy Leary “Tune In, Turn <strong>On</strong>, and Drop Out”slogan of 1963, the use of psychedelics became a subject of nati<strong>on</strong>alhysteria--the “drug revoluti<strong>on</strong>.” These drugs, known then as psychotomimetics(imitators of psychosis), were widely believed to be thecause of psychosis, suicide, and even murder (Mogar and Savage 1954;Robbins et al. 1967). Equally well publicized were the c<strong>on</strong>tenti<strong>on</strong>sthat they could bring about spiritual rebirth, mystical <strong>on</strong>eness withthe universe, and the like (Huxley 1970; Weil 1972). Certainly therewere numerous cases of not merely transient but prol<strong>on</strong>ged psychosesfollowing the use of psychedelics. In the mid-sixties, psychiatrichospitals like the Massachusetts Mental Health Center and Bellevuewere reporting as many as <strong>on</strong>e-third of their admissi<strong>on</strong>s resulting fromthe ingesti<strong>on</strong> of these drugs (Robbins et al. 1967). By the late sixties,however, the rate of such admissi<strong>on</strong>s had dec<strong>line</strong>d dramatically.Initially, many observers c<strong>on</strong>cluded that this dec<strong>line</strong> was due to feartactics--the warning about the various health hazards, the chromosomebreaks and birth defects, which were reported in the newspapers.These stories proved later to be false. In fact, although psychedelicuse c<strong>on</strong>tinued to be the fastest growing drug use in American through1973, the dysfuncti<strong>on</strong>al sequelae virtually disappeared (Nati<strong>on</strong>al Commissi<strong>on</strong><strong>on</strong> Marihuana and <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong> 1973). What then had changed?It has been found that neither the drugs themselves nor the pers<strong>on</strong>alitiesof the users were the most prominent factors in those painfulcases of the sixties. A retrospective study of the use of such drugsbefore the early sixties has revealed that although resp<strong>on</strong>ses to thedrugs varied widely, they included n<strong>on</strong>e of the horrible, highly publicizedc<strong>on</strong>sequences of the mid-sixties. Another book, entitled LSD:Pers<strong>on</strong>ality and Experience (Barr et al. 1972), describes a study ofthe influence of pers<strong>on</strong>ality <strong>on</strong> psychedelic drug experience that wasmade before the drug revoluti<strong>on</strong>. It found typologies of resp<strong>on</strong>se tothe drugs but no <strong>on</strong>e-to-<strong>on</strong>e relati<strong>on</strong>ship between untoward reacti<strong>on</strong>and emoti<strong>on</strong>al disturbance. And Howard S. Becker in his propheticarticle of 1967 compared the then current anxiety about psychedelics toanxiety about marijuana in the late 1920s when several psychoses werereported. Becker hypothesized that the psychoses came not from thedrug reacti<strong>on</strong>s themselves but from the sec<strong>on</strong>dary anxiety generatedby unfamiliarity with the drug’s effects and ballo<strong>on</strong>ed by media publicity.He suggested that such unpleasant reacti<strong>on</strong>s had disappeared when theeffects of marijuana became more widely known, and he correctlypredicted that the same things would happen with the psychedelics.The power of social learning also brought about a change in the reacti<strong>on</strong>sof those who expected to gain insight and enlightenment from theuse of psychedelics. Interviews have shown that the user of the early1960s, with great hopes and fears and a sense of total unfamiliaritywith what might happen, had a far more extreme experience than theuser of the 1970s. who had been exposed to a decade of interest inpsychedelic colors, music, and sensati<strong>on</strong>s. The later user, who mightremark, “Oh, so that is what a psychedelic color looks like,” had beenthoroughly prepared, albeit unc<strong>on</strong>sciously, for the experience andresp<strong>on</strong>ded accordingly, within a middle range.The sec<strong>on</strong>d example of the enormous influence of the social setting andof social learning in determining the c<strong>on</strong>sequences of drug use comesfrom Vietnam. Current estimates indicate that at least 35 percent of242


enlisted men used heroin, and 54 percent of these became adicted to it(Robins et al. 1977). Statistics from the U.S. Public Health Servicehospitals active in detoxifying and treating addicts showed a recidivismrate of 97 percent, and some observers thought it was even higher.<strong>On</strong>ce the extent of the use of heroin in Vietnam became apparent, thegreat fear of Army and Government officials was that the maxim “<strong>On</strong>cean addict, always an addict” would operate, and most of the expertsagreed that this fear was entirely justified. Treatment and rehabilitati<strong>on</strong>centers were set up in Vietnam, and the Army’s slogan thatheroin addicti<strong>on</strong> stopped “at the shore of the South China Sea” washeard everywhere. As virtually all observers agree, however, thoseprograms were total failures. Often people in the rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> programsused more heroin than when they were <strong>on</strong> active duty (Zinberg1972).Nevertheless, as the study by Robins et al. (1977) has shown, mostaddicti<strong>on</strong> did indeed stop at the South China Sea. For addicts wholeft Vietnam, recidivism was approximately 10 percent after they gotback home to the United States--virtually the reverse of the previousU.S. Public Health Service figures. Apparently it was the abhorrentsocial setting of Vietnam that led men who ordinarily would not havec<strong>on</strong>sidered using heroin to use it and often to become addicted to it.But evidently they associated its use with Vietnam, much as hospitalpatients who are receiving large amounts of opiates for painful medicalc<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s associate the drug with the c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. The returnees werelike those patients (menti<strong>on</strong>ed earlier) who, having taken opiates torelieve a physiological disturbance, usually do not crave the drugafter the c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> has been alleviated and they have left the hospital.Returning to the first example--psychedelic drug use in the 1960s--itis my c<strong>on</strong>tenti<strong>on</strong> that c<strong>on</strong>trol over use of these drugs was establishedby the development in the counterculture of social sancti<strong>on</strong>s and ritualsvery like those surrounding alcohol use in the culture at large. “<strong>On</strong>lyuse the first time with a guru” was a sancti<strong>on</strong> or rule that told neophytesto use the drug the first time with an experienced user whocould reduce their sec<strong>on</strong>dary anxiety about what was happening byinterpreting it as a drug effect. “<strong>On</strong>ly use at a good time, in a goodplace, with good people” was a sancti<strong>on</strong> that gave sound advice tothose taking a drug that would sensitize them so intensely to theirinner and outer surroundings. In additi<strong>on</strong>, it c<strong>on</strong>veyed the messagethat the drug experience could be simply a pleasant c<strong>on</strong>sciousnesschange, a good experience. The specific rituals that developed toexpress these sancti<strong>on</strong>s--just when it was best to take the drug, how,with whom, what was the best way to come down, and so <strong>on</strong>--variedfrom group to group, though some spread from <strong>on</strong>e group to another.It is harder to document the development of social sancti<strong>on</strong>s and ritualsin Vietnam. Most of the early evidence indicated that the drug wasused heavily in order to obscure the actualities of the war, with littlethought of c<strong>on</strong>trol. Yet later studies showed that many enlisted menused heroin in Vietnam without becoming addicted (Robins and Helzer1975). More important, although 95 percent of heroin-addicted Vietnamreturnees did not become readdicted in the United States, 88 percentdid use heroin occasi<strong>on</strong>ally, indicating that they had developed somecapacity to take the drug in a c<strong>on</strong>trolled way (Robins et al. 1977).Some rudimentary rituals, however, do seem to have been followed bythe men who used heroin in Vietnam. The act of gently rolling thetobacco out of an ordinary cigarette, tamping the fine white powderinto the opening, and then replacing a little tobacco to hold the powder243


in before lighting up the opium joint seemed to be followed all over thecountry, even though the units in the north or in the highlands hadno direct c<strong>on</strong>tact with those in the Delta (Zinberg 1971). To whatextent this ritual aided c<strong>on</strong>trol is, of course, impossible to determine.Having observed it many times, however, I can say that it was almostalways d<strong>on</strong>e in a group and thus formed part of the social experienceof heroin use. While <strong>on</strong>e pers<strong>on</strong> was performing the ritual, the otherssat quietly and watched in anticipati<strong>on</strong>. It would be my guess thatthe degree of socializati<strong>on</strong> achieved through this ritual could have hadimportant implicati<strong>on</strong>s for c<strong>on</strong>trol.Still, the development of social sancti<strong>on</strong>s and rituals probably occursmore slowly in the secretive world of illicit drug use than with the useof a licit drug like alcohol, and it is hard to imagine that any coherentsocial development occurred in the incredible pressure cooker of Vietnam.Now the whole experience has receded so far into history that it isimpossible to nail down what specific social learning might have takenplace to be passed <strong>on</strong>. But certainly Vietnam illustrates the power ofthe social setting to influence large numbers of apparently ordinarypeople to engage in drug activity that was viewed as extremely deviantand to limit that activity to that setting. Vietnam also showed thatheroin, too, despite its tremendous pharmaceutically addictive potential,is not universally or inevitably addictive.Further study of various patterns of heroin use, including c<strong>on</strong>trolleduse, in the United States c<strong>on</strong>firms the less<strong>on</strong>s taught by the history ofalcohol use in America, the use of psychedelics in the 1960s. and theuse of heroin during the Vietnam War. The social setting, with itsformal and informal c<strong>on</strong>trols, its capacity to develop new informal socialsancti<strong>on</strong>s and rituals, and its transmissi<strong>on</strong> of informati<strong>on</strong> in numerousinformal ways, is a crucial factor in the c<strong>on</strong>trolled use of any intoxicant.This does not mean, however, that the pharmaceutical properties ofthe drug or the attitudes and pers<strong>on</strong>ality of the user count for littleor nothing. As I stated at the beginning of this essay, all threevariables--drug, set, and setting--must be included in any validtheory of drug use. In every case of use it is necessary to understandhow the specific characteristics of the drug and the pers<strong>on</strong>ality of theuser interact and are modified by the social setting and its c<strong>on</strong>trols.244


THEORIES ON<strong>On</strong>e’sRelati<strong>on</strong>shipto Nature


Addicti<strong>on</strong> to PleasureA Biological and Social-PsychologicalTheory of Addicti<strong>on</strong>Nils Bejerot, M.D.INTRODUCTIONIn my experience, the debate <strong>on</strong> the nature of addicti<strong>on</strong> has been to<strong>on</strong>arrowly limited to lead to a general theory that can explain the variedand complicated phenomena which these c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s present.The earliest explanati<strong>on</strong>s were that the soul of the individual waspossessed by the devil or by satanic forces. In medical circles in thefirst half of the nineteenth century it was believed that dependencewas associated with the digestive system (opium eaters and theirsevere opium hunger). From the viewpoint of cultural history, we cantrace the development of this alimentary theory in the psychoanalyticalc<strong>on</strong>cept of oral fixati<strong>on</strong>.When the subcutaneous injecti<strong>on</strong> needle was introduced in 1856, physiciansthought that the addicti<strong>on</strong> problem could be eliminated as amedical complicati<strong>on</strong>. During the American Civil War, however, it wasfound that subcutaneous injecti<strong>on</strong>s led to dependence more rapidly thanoral administrati<strong>on</strong>, and thousands of wounded soldiers were afflictedby an addicti<strong>on</strong> which remained even after the physical injury and painhad completely disappeared. Because of this, morphinism was for atime called the “soldiers’ disease” or the “army disease” in the UnitedStates (O’D<strong>on</strong>nell and Ball 1966).During the twentieth century, the development of tolerance and physicaldependence has played an important part and has obscured the mechanismof addicti<strong>on</strong> (Fishman 1978). Before describing these interpretati<strong>on</strong>sand theories, I will give a simple example of what I mean by thedevelopment of a dependence.246


NICOTINISM AS A MODEL DEPENDENCEThe malignant addicti<strong>on</strong>s are so emoti<strong>on</strong>ally charged and subject to somany c<strong>on</strong>tradictory explanatory models that it is difficult to discussthem without a c<strong>on</strong>tinual risk of misunderstanding. We need to examinean addicti<strong>on</strong> which is not emoti<strong>on</strong>ally enflamed, is not surrounded bysocial sancti<strong>on</strong>s, which is well known and of comm<strong>on</strong> occurrence indifferent societies and groups, and, in additi<strong>on</strong>, presents all therelevant phenomena of dependence. I c<strong>on</strong>sider that nicotinism is asimple and good example of the development of dependence.The dotted <strong>line</strong> A-B in figure 1 shows a young pers<strong>on</strong> who has not yetcome in c<strong>on</strong>tact with tobacco. In time, tobacco makes its entry (B).We can immediately state that the young debutant has neither an innateneed for nicotine nor a nicotine craving. No psychological or sociologicalanalyses are required to show that the totally decisive reas<strong>on</strong> whya child smokes a cigarette for the first time is purely and simplycuriosity.Nicotine is a fairly str<strong>on</strong>g stimulant. An ordinary cigarette c<strong>on</strong>tains<strong>on</strong>ly about 1.5 mg nicotine, but this is a large dose for some<strong>on</strong>e who isnot used to smoking or snuffing tobacco and who has perhaps half theweight of an adult.The debutant in our example feels giddy, suffers from nausea andheadache, and may even vomit. In spite of the discomfort, the comm<strong>on</strong>pattern is that the beginner obstinately coughs through <strong>on</strong>e pack ofcigarettes after the other (B-C). This may in itself seem strange,since the beginner at this stage has still not developed a craving fornicotine or a dependence up<strong>on</strong> it. The reas<strong>on</strong> for c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> of theinitial smoking is usually that the individual wishes to imitate olderfriends and adults, and in this way to appear more grown up andself-c<strong>on</strong>fident than he or she really is.VOLUNTARY PHASEOur young smoker still has complete voluntary c<strong>on</strong>trol over nicotinec<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong>. It is no problem at all to refrain from smoking a cigarettewhen this fits his or her (usually unc<strong>on</strong>scious) goal. At this stage,the smoker has aims other than to satisfy a craving for nicotine, asthis has not yet been established. Some smokers remain their wholelives in this stage, which may suitably be described as the voluntaryphase (B-C in figure 1). These pers<strong>on</strong>s may sometimes take a cigaretteinstead of a biscuit with coffee, since this reduces their intake ofcalories. Or they may smoke a cigarette in order to have something todo with their hands in company where they do not really feel at home,or perhaps just to make an impressi<strong>on</strong> and appear to be sunk in thoughtwhen they really want to hide their shyness, etc. Typical of thevoluntary phase is that there is some motive for smoking other than tosatisfy a still n<strong>on</strong>existent craving for nicotine.Voluntary smokers are not to be regarded as nicotinists in this definiti<strong>on</strong>alsystem. I would describe them as incidental smokers. Thecharacteristic factor in the voluntary phase is that the will and comm<strong>on</strong>senseare in c<strong>on</strong>trol of the drug effects and emoti<strong>on</strong>s. It is the individual’s“independent will” which steers behavior.247


FIGURE 1.–Diagrammatic presentati<strong>on</strong> of thedevelopment of nicotinismTHE DEPENDENCE PHASEIf <strong>on</strong>e is unwise enough to c<strong>on</strong>tinue for a time to inspire nicotine,certain elements in the nervous system are stimulated, and the individualbegins to learn how to appreciate the pleasant stimulant effectof nicotine. Through learning, a direct c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing, the individualglides imperceptibly into a nicotine dependence. A craving for nicotine--orrather for the effects of nicotine--begins to develop.As far as I know, there are no investigati<strong>on</strong>s into whether it requires10, 50, or 100 packs of cigarettes before an individual glides into amanifest nicotine dependence. The phenomen<strong>on</strong> appears to be anordinary pharmacological dose-resp<strong>on</strong>se relati<strong>on</strong>, with variables such asthe size of the dose (the nicotine c<strong>on</strong>tent of the cigarettes), the intensityof the dose (how often <strong>on</strong>e smokes), the durati<strong>on</strong> of smoking, andthe individual variati<strong>on</strong>s which always occur in a biological material.It is characteristic for the phase of dependence that the craving fornicotine resembles the character and force of a natural drive. Anotherway of expressing this is that the acquired craving for satisfacti<strong>on</strong>which has developed from the effects of nicotine is in c<strong>on</strong>trol of the“will,” which adjusts to the craving in a similar way as to innatebiological drives. In psychoanalytic terminology <strong>on</strong>e would say thatthe forces in the “id” have taken c<strong>on</strong>trol over the forces of the “ego”and “superego.”If smoking is forbidden during lectures and meetings, the nicotinist,without great distress, can delay smoking until there is a pause, inthe same way as he or she can delay the satisfacti<strong>on</strong> of sexual needs.The nicotinist would also, with a certain amount of effort, be able to248


manage a full-time job at a petrol stati<strong>on</strong> where smoking would beimpossible during the working day. The impossibility of satisfying thenicotine craving in such a situati<strong>on</strong> will reduce the abstinence andmake it more endurable, in rather the same way as a seaman can moreeasily repress his sexual craving amid the storms at sea than amid thebrothels in port.When nicotinists are in a situati<strong>on</strong> where the satisfacti<strong>on</strong> of the nicotinecraving does not give rise to any inc<strong>on</strong>venience, they c<strong>on</strong>sume therequired dose. If c<strong>on</strong>firmed nicotinists are unable to administer acouple of milligrams of nicotine every 20th to 30th minute during theirwaking hours, they feel that something essential is missing. In myopini<strong>on</strong>, the process described here represents the general dynamics ofhow a drug dependence arises.If for any reas<strong>on</strong> the nicotinist in this situati<strong>on</strong> wants to stop smoking,it is, as we know, always an uncertain venture; and this is thecase even if the individual is aware of the serious c<strong>on</strong>sequences ofsmoking <strong>on</strong> health.THE IMPORTANCE OF THE BASIC PERSONALITYThe basic pers<strong>on</strong>ality is not without significance for the development ofnicotinism or for whether the individual will eventually overcome thedependence. It is not, however, of any decisive importance in whatsort of brain nicotinism lies. Let us take as a hypothetical experimentthat nicotinism afflicts an individual without any physical, mental,erotic, social, ec<strong>on</strong>omic, or other problem. If the individual’s soleproblem is nicotinism, this will not make it essentially easier for him orher to stop smoking than it would for any<strong>on</strong>e else.When the nicotinists, both those who are free from problems and thoseoverwhelmed by them, disc<strong>on</strong>tinue their administrati<strong>on</strong> of nicotine, theyenter into the same kind of abstinence state, characterized by str<strong>on</strong>gand frequent waves of intense nicotine hunger. We know from experiencethat most of them quickly relapse into their smoking habit. <strong>On</strong>eyear after an ambitious treatment program for smokers, about 75percent of them have relapsed, even in the case of well-motivatedgroups.If we have a singularly determined and str<strong>on</strong>g-willed pers<strong>on</strong>, who, inadditi<strong>on</strong>, has definitely determined to stop smoking, we know that thewaves of str<strong>on</strong>g nicotine hunger will in time dec<strong>line</strong> in strength andfrequency. After a few m<strong>on</strong>ths, they will have almost disappeared,but even years later--when the ocean, so to speak, is as smooth as amirror--there may still arise isolated, str<strong>on</strong>g swells of nicotine sucti<strong>on</strong>,particularly in situati<strong>on</strong>s where previously the individual always begansmoking, for instance, while playing bridge or after a good meal. Itshows that smoking is often supported by several reinforcing c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ingfactors. Even these late swells fade away in time, but I haveheard of several ex-smokers who have experienced them several yearsafter a free interval, and after more than ten years of totalabstinence.249


DEPENDENCE MECHANISM AND THELENGTH OF THE ABSTINENCE PERIODIf a previously heavy smoker undergoes such a prol<strong>on</strong>ged period ofabstinence that even the late abstinence effects have ceased manyyears previously, the individual has still not recovered, but is <strong>on</strong>ly anabstinent nicotinist with a latent nicotinism for the rest of his or herlife. If the abstinent nicotinist, after 10, 15, or 20 years, smokes afew cigarettes through a desire “to see how it feels now,” this willalmost without excepti<strong>on</strong> lead to c<strong>on</strong>tinued smoking--after a certainthreshold c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong> is exceeded. After a short period, the individualusually reverts to the same advanced pattern of c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong>, andthe length of the intermediary abstinence period seems to be of sec<strong>on</strong>daryimportance (E-F, figure 1).In reality, nicotinism seems to be an “incurable” c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> in the sensethat a very l<strong>on</strong>g period of total abstinence does not cure nicotinedependence. There can be no return to the youthful, innocent relati<strong>on</strong>to tobacco or to the previous learning period, the voluntary phase,when experimentati<strong>on</strong> with tobacco and sporadic or regular smokingwas under full voluntary c<strong>on</strong>trol.DEPENDENCE: A CONDITION IN ITSELFThere is nothing remarkable about the mechanisms surrounding nicotinism;they follow the well-known laws of learning theories. In myopini<strong>on</strong>, this shows very clearly that nicotinism represents a drugdependence and also that dependence is not a symptom, but a c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>of its own.Smoking the first cigarette is a result (symptom) of youthful curiosity.A couple of decades later, smoking perhaps a pack of cigarettes a dayis not a late symptom or expressi<strong>on</strong> of the curiosity of those earlyyears or a need to imitate older friends; it is a c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> of its own--anicotine dependence. A dialectical change has taken place, a changein quality from the voluntary phase to the phase of dependence.CHEMICALLY INDUCED ADDICTIONSAs we know, a large number of chemical substances may give rise todrug dependence of varying strength. Comm<strong>on</strong> to them all is the factthat they give pleasant effects in <strong>on</strong>e way or another. Often it maybe the subtle and, for the experimenter, perhaps completely unc<strong>on</strong>sciouseffects which are decisive for the development of dependence. <strong>Drug</strong>sthat give more unpleasant than pleasant sensati<strong>on</strong>s are obviouslyunsuitable as intoxicants or as a source of enjoyment. Even thoughcertain drugs, specifically alcohol, tobacco, and cannabis, may <strong>on</strong> firstc<strong>on</strong>tact seem repellent or uninteresting, the individual may n<strong>on</strong>ethelesspersist because of cultural pressure and learn to appreciate the euphoricqualities which were not initially discernible. Other drugs seem togive pleasant effects from the first dose (if the dose is of adequatesize). These are caffeine, amphetamine, cocaine, and morphine.From the aspects of biology and learning theory, it seems that there isno difference, in principle, between caffeinism, nicotinism, alcoholism,and what is usually called drug addicti<strong>on</strong>. <strong>On</strong> the other hand, drugs250


vary greatly in the intensity of the euphoria they provide, the toxiceffects, and the subsequent ability of the user to functi<strong>on</strong> socially.Some may be enjoyed daily throughout life without noticeable injuriouseffects (caffeine), others give rise to marked complicati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong>ly afterprol<strong>on</strong>ged c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong>, while a third group may result in rapid dependenceand entail severe complicati<strong>on</strong>s (heroin and cocaine).The social acceptance of different types of drug use varies greatly indifferent cultures and circles within cultures. Risk groups and initiati<strong>on</strong>mechanisms vary greatly also for different drugs and differentc<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. We will return to this later.PHYSICAL DEPENDENCE: ONLY A COMPLICATIONUntil the 1970s. pharmacologists had stubbornly held that it is thedirect pharmacological effects of certain drugs up<strong>on</strong> the nervous system,and the vegetative reacti<strong>on</strong>s when these drugs are withdrawn (abstinencesyndrome) which c<strong>on</strong>stitute addicti<strong>on</strong>. “Physical dependence”was c<strong>on</strong>ceived as an essential comp<strong>on</strong>ent in the c<strong>on</strong>cept of addicti<strong>on</strong>.During recent years, however, even pharmacologists and neurophysiologistsare inc<strong>line</strong>d to agree that addicti<strong>on</strong> has a more general importthan pharmacological effects and vegetative reacti<strong>on</strong>s to them (Olds andMilner 1954). The development of tolerance and the irrelevance ofvegetative phenomena for dependence may be illustrated by a couple ofexamples.The newborn infant of an opiate-dependent mother may be <strong>on</strong> theverge of death from the severe vegetative abstinence reacti<strong>on</strong>s (vomiting,diarrhea, etc.), but such a child is not, and has never been, anaddict, since it has not learned to appreciate the euphoric effects ofopiates, but has <strong>on</strong>ly been exposed to the development of tolerance.If, in an intramural milieu, we were to give a group of people methad<strong>on</strong>e(a morphine substitute with prol<strong>on</strong>ged effects) mixed in their food,they would, after a m<strong>on</strong>th, be completely saturated with (tolerant of)opiates, and this without their becoming aware of it themselves. Theywould, of course, notice the c<strong>on</strong>stipati<strong>on</strong> and the lack of sexual appetite,but would not suspect the secret administrati<strong>on</strong> of drugs. An intravenousinjecti<strong>on</strong> of heroin in this situati<strong>on</strong> would be without effect,either in regard to euphoria or pharmacologic toxicity (overdose). Ifin this situati<strong>on</strong> the methad<strong>on</strong>e administrati<strong>on</strong> were suddenly stopped,the individuals would so<strong>on</strong> become very ill and might think that theyhad food pois<strong>on</strong>ing. If, instead, the doses were reduced gradually,they could recover from tolerance in a m<strong>on</strong>th without knowing thatthey had had maximal doses of opiates in their bodies and a fullydeveloped tolerance. (The methad<strong>on</strong>e blockade treatment of heroinistsis based <strong>on</strong> these principles.)In the same way, the risk for the development of dependence is smallwhen patients suffering from pain are given morphine in adequatedoses. The euphoric effects are “neutralized” by the pain and anxiety,and the patient is relieved of a great deal of suffering. If morphineis given in an inadequate way, the patient may experience a pleasurablemorphine reacti<strong>on</strong>. If, in additi<strong>on</strong>, he or she is then told what hadproduced the pleasant effects, the basis would be laid for dependenceas a complicati<strong>on</strong> of the medical treatment.251


PLEASURE AS A BIOLOGICAL PREFERENCE SYSTEMIt is well known that animal behavior is steered by a number of internaland external factors--genetic and acquired, persistent and incidental.Hunger, thirst, sexual craving, aggressi<strong>on</strong>, fear, self-preservati<strong>on</strong>,and the ability of the individual and the race to adjust and surviveare well-known steering factors.Unc<strong>on</strong>sciously it seems that all observable internal and external c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>sand previous memories and acquired knowledge are weighed,together with c<strong>on</strong>stituti<strong>on</strong>al resources and current physiological c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s,in deciding behavior at each moment. Thus a thirsty animalseeks a source of water, but if it suspects danger, the animal willendure its thirst or find a safer place in which to satisfy it.All stimuli, schematically speaking, must be experienced either aspleasant, unpleasant, or indifferent. In this way, everything can bereduced to pleasure or pain, and the balance between these experiencesseems to steer behavior.Neurophysiologists have analyzed the mechanisms of pleasure in themid-brain and limbic system. Olds and Milner (1954) applied electricstimulati<strong>on</strong> to the pleasure center of the hypothalamus of rats whichwere able to tramp <strong>on</strong> a pedal and receive an electric current. Thiswas obviously quite pleasurable and resulted in str<strong>on</strong>g repetitivebehavior. The males stimulated themselves up to 5,000 times a dayuntil they fell down, unc<strong>on</strong>scious, from exhausti<strong>on</strong>. They did noteven give themselves time to drink, eat, or take an interest in femalesin heat. This phenomen<strong>on</strong> may be seen as the biological archetype foraddicti<strong>on</strong>. Not <strong>on</strong>ly the social and pharmacological factors, but thepsychological factors had been eliminated here, and addicti<strong>on</strong> appearsas a fixati<strong>on</strong> in a m<strong>on</strong>ot<strong>on</strong>ous stimulati<strong>on</strong> of the pleasure centers with arepetitive behavior of enormous persistence as a result. The behaviorexperienced is so pleasurable that, if interrupted, it is desired againwith the force and character of a natural drive. This direct stimulati<strong>on</strong>of the pleasure mechanisms and fixati<strong>on</strong> to a repetitive behavior maybe seen as the simplest model for addicti<strong>on</strong>.ADDICTION WITHOUT DRUGSFreud, <strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>e occasi<strong>on</strong>, described masturbati<strong>on</strong> as “the primary addicti<strong>on</strong>”and compared it with drug dependence. This seems to be verysharp sighted and relevant. Sexuality may be seen as a biological,endogenic, and very potent pleasure system which normally dominatesthe efforts and pleasure seeking of animals and humans during l<strong>on</strong>gperiods of their lives.Numerous exogenic stimuli may, in various ways, lead to str<strong>on</strong>g feelingsof pleasure and through learning give rise to a c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing whichdirects the future pleasure-seeking behavior of the individual in a waysimilar to natural drives, and is str<strong>on</strong>gly reminiscent of sexuality.When this is brought about by means of drugs we call it drug addicti<strong>on</strong>,but the phenomen<strong>on</strong> may also be initiated in many other ways. As anexample of an addicti<strong>on</strong> without drugs we may take gambling, which ischaracterized by all the elements that occur in a drug addicti<strong>on</strong> exceptthat the stimulati<strong>on</strong> is derived from a game. Other c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s that252


seem to have a similar basic mechanism are pyromania, kleptomania,anorexia nervosa, and overeating. In a more general model it seemsthat even nail biting, neurodermatitis, phobia, compulsive neuroses,perhaps paranoia querulans, and many other disturbances fit into thispattern. They have in comm<strong>on</strong> that a great discomfort is reduced oreliminated for a time through certain thought patterns or behavior,and in this way they provide a pleasurable gain. Thoughts may insuch c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s fill the same functi<strong>on</strong> as acti<strong>on</strong>.DRUG ADDICTION: A CHEMICAL LOVEThe pleasure mechanism may be stimulated in a number of ways andgive rise to a str<strong>on</strong>g fixati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> repetitive behavior. Stimulati<strong>on</strong> withdrugs is <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e of many ways, but <strong>on</strong>e of the simplest, str<strong>on</strong>gest,and often also the most destructive.When str<strong>on</strong>gly euphoric drugs are given to experimental animals, itseems that all of them c<strong>on</strong>tinue to seek the drugs, providing that theyhave learned to appreciate them and that they are not in a state ofexhausti<strong>on</strong> caused by the drug (as, for instance, <strong>on</strong> prol<strong>on</strong>ged overstimulati<strong>on</strong>with central nervous system stimulants and associateddehydrati<strong>on</strong>, etc.). From the biological viewpoint, it therefore seemsto be normal to c<strong>on</strong>tinue with chemical pleasure stimulati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>ce it hascommenced and the behavior has been learned. In humans, <strong>on</strong> theother hand, it is regarded as abnormal, “deviant”,or morbid to c<strong>on</strong>tinuewith intoxicating behavior, while the biologically atypical behavior--torefrain from pleasure or to use the drug “with restraint”--issocialIy recommended, accepted, or tolerated.If the pleasure stimulati<strong>on</strong> becomes so str<strong>on</strong>g that it captivates anindividual with the compulsi<strong>on</strong> and force characteristic of naturaldrives, then there exists what I would describe as an addicti<strong>on</strong>. Thisaddicti<strong>on</strong> usually--but not inevitably--is expressed in addictive behavior,that is, a specific, repetitive pleasure stimulati<strong>on</strong> with lack of motivati<strong>on</strong>to change this behavior, even if the individual realizes that it isextremely injurious. Addicti<strong>on</strong> may easily become even str<strong>on</strong>ger thanthe instinct for self-preservati<strong>on</strong>.A pseudomotivati<strong>on</strong> for treatment is a very comm<strong>on</strong> phenomen<strong>on</strong> inaddicti<strong>on</strong>. The individual seeks help and treatment of troublesomesomatic, psychic, social, and many other kinds of complicati<strong>on</strong>s toaddicti<strong>on</strong> without really being prepared to give up the special sourceof pleasure that causes the addicti<strong>on</strong>. In the more advanced andsocially unaccepted addicti<strong>on</strong>s (alcoholism, heroinism, anorexia nervosa,etc.), addicts usually act as full-time defense lawyers for their addicti<strong>on</strong>,and usually succeed in hiding their deepest aims from relatives,physicians, psychologists, social workers, attorneys, and judges, in acunning defensive game around the protecti<strong>on</strong> of their addicti<strong>on</strong>.The simplest way of regarding a drug addicti<strong>on</strong> is to see it as fallingin love with specific, pleasurable sensati<strong>on</strong>s (or the means to preventpain). The lack of “treatment motivati<strong>on</strong>” and h<strong>on</strong>esty in regard todependence is often interpreted as a sign of a primary characterdisturbance. I do not c<strong>on</strong>sider this to be peculiar, however, ascomm<strong>on</strong>sense is usually put aside by the str<strong>on</strong>g pleasure fixati<strong>on</strong> inlove and in addicti<strong>on</strong>.253


DEPENDENCY: IN THE MEMORYFalling in love is a learned phenomen<strong>on</strong> and is located in the memoryand not in gross physiological and vegetative reacti<strong>on</strong>s (although thememory functi<strong>on</strong>s do have their special physiological base). This isalso the case with drug dependence. I will illustrate these memorymechanisms with a couple of banal examples.Suppose that a motorcyclist is out with his fiancee, has an accident,strikes his head <strong>on</strong> the road, and loses his memory for a while. Hewould be completely at a loss if his fiancee entered the hospital wardwith a bunch of flowers. Since he could not remember that he hadseen her before, he could not, of course, be in love with her.I have myself seen an elderly nicotinist who suffered from seniledementia after more than 60 years of intensive smoking. <strong>On</strong>e daywhen the patient received his daily two packs of cigarettes from hisrelatives, he refused them indignantly, with the explanati<strong>on</strong> that hehad never been a smoker. When the relatives protested he said, “Youmust have mixed me up with some<strong>on</strong>e else.” He never asked forcigarettes again. When the memory is extinguished, the dependencedisappears.A DEFINITIONif, after this discussi<strong>on</strong>, we were to try to formulate a definiti<strong>on</strong> ofthe c<strong>on</strong>cept of addicti<strong>on</strong>, it should cover active and passive, directand indirect, c<strong>on</strong>structive and destructive addicti<strong>on</strong>s. It could begiven the following general form: An emoti<strong>on</strong>al fixati<strong>on</strong> (sentiment)acquired through learning, which intermittently or c<strong>on</strong>tinually expressesitself in purposeful, stereotyped behavior with the character and forceof a natural drive, aiming at a specific pleasure or the avoidance of aspecific discomfort.Addicti<strong>on</strong> may take many forms and may occur in different phases.(a) The currency of addicti<strong>on</strong>: In manifest addictive behavior, addicti<strong>on</strong>is suitably described as active. If the individual throughcounterforces (treatment, social c<strong>on</strong>trol, fear of complicati<strong>on</strong>s,sancti<strong>on</strong>s, etc.) sacrifices the specific stimulati<strong>on</strong> and remainsabstinent, the addicti<strong>on</strong> is, for the time, passive. If the sentimentdisappears completely through dec<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing (reducti<strong>on</strong> or absenceof stimulati<strong>on</strong> in resp<strong>on</strong>se to the behavior), rec<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing, loss ofmemory, or cerebral damage, the addicti<strong>on</strong> is extinguished.(b) The stimuli of addicti<strong>on</strong>: If the stimulati<strong>on</strong> occurs with the helpof drugs, a drug addicti<strong>on</strong> is present. If it occurs through otherpleasurable exogenic stimuli, behavior such as gambling, ars<strong>on</strong>,kleptomania, and overeating may arise. The addicti<strong>on</strong>s which havearisen from pleasure stimulati<strong>on</strong> may be called direct and will differfrom those that arise from very unpleasant experiences--as phobias,compulsive neuroses, paranoid reacti<strong>on</strong>s, nail biting, and anorexianervosa. Since the stereotyped behavior in these cases serves toeliminate discomfort, they may be called indirect addicti<strong>on</strong>s.254


(c) The relevance of addicti<strong>on</strong>: If addicti<strong>on</strong> causes a deteriorati<strong>on</strong> inthe health of the individual and/or the ability to functi<strong>on</strong> socially,it may be described as destructive; if it increases these qualities,it is c<strong>on</strong>structive. Am<strong>on</strong>g c<strong>on</strong>structive addicti<strong>on</strong>s we can includethe creative obsessi<strong>on</strong> of scientists, authors, artists, and politicians,also the extreme attainment fixati<strong>on</strong> of successful athletes andbusinesspers<strong>on</strong>s.According to these definiti<strong>on</strong>s, every<strong>on</strong>e has a number of addictivebehaviors. Many sacrifice their lives for their destructive addicti<strong>on</strong>s;others receive the Nobel Prize for their c<strong>on</strong>structive <strong>on</strong>es.SPECIAL POPULATIONSAddicti<strong>on</strong> of the therapeutic type is the <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e of the malignantforms of addicti<strong>on</strong> in which women are as numerous as men and mayeven be somewhat overrepresented. Anxious, asthenic, neurotic, andeasily stressed pers<strong>on</strong>alities run a greater risk.Addicti<strong>on</strong> of the professi<strong>on</strong>al type usually afflicts physicians who wereoriginally very ambitious and had unrealistically high expectati<strong>on</strong>sabout their careers. They became disappointed when they realizedthat they would never reach the goal they had aimed at (Pescor 1942)and fell into drug abuse through self-treatment of somatic problems.Addicti<strong>on</strong> of the epidemic type is always a breach of norms and istherefore str<strong>on</strong>gly associated with groups at risk for norm breaking,such as active criminals, bohemians, young people, etc. The more theabuse spreads, the less of a breach of norms it becomes, and thegreater will be the proporti<strong>on</strong> of ordinary youths who enter the riskz<strong>on</strong>e and are finally drawn into addictive behavior. Finally, an epidemicmay in this way change into an endemic, as marijuana smoking has nowd<strong>on</strong>e in a large part of the United States (Johns<strong>on</strong> 1973).Addicti<strong>on</strong> of the cultural type threatens, in principle, the whole populati<strong>on</strong>.In most cultures, women are protected by the norms in regardto intoxicated behavior. Am<strong>on</strong>g men, the group with the greatest riskc<strong>on</strong>sists of those who have plenty of time, m<strong>on</strong>ey, access to alcohol,and so <strong>on</strong>. The high-risk groups for alcoholism are authors, artists,musicians, entertainers, diplomats, commercial travelers, seamen, andpeople working in restaurants.The various addictive behaviors still cannot be explained by a singlemodel, but they can be explained by a combinati<strong>on</strong> of general biologicaland social psychological models.255


Methad<strong>on</strong>e MaintenanceA Theoretical PerspectiveVincent P. Dole, M.D.Marie E. Nyswander, M.D.The Methad<strong>on</strong>e Maintenance Research Program (Dole and Nyswander1965, 1966; Dole et al. 1966) began in 1963 with pharmacological studiesc<strong>on</strong>ducted <strong>on</strong> the metabolic ward of the Rockefeller University Hospital.<strong>On</strong>ly six addict patients were treated during the first year, but theresults of this work were sufficiently impressive to justify a trial ofmaintenance treatment of heroin addicts admitted to open medical wardsof general hospitals in the city.The dramatic improvements in social status of patients <strong>on</strong> this programexceeded expectati<strong>on</strong>s. The study started with the hope that heroinseekingbehavior would be stopped by a narcotic blockade but itcertainly was not expected that we would be able to retain more than90 percent of the patients and that almost three-fourths would besocially productive and living as normal citizens in the community after<strong>on</strong>ly six m<strong>on</strong>ths of treatment. Prior to admissi<strong>on</strong>, almost all of thepatients had supported their heroin habits by theft or other antisocialactivities. Further handicapped by the ostracism of the community,slum backgrounds, minority group status, school dropout status,pris<strong>on</strong> records, and antisocial compani<strong>on</strong>s, they had seemed poorprospects for social rehabilitati<strong>on</strong>.The unexpected resp<strong>on</strong>se of these patients to a simple medical programforced us to reexamine some of the assumpti<strong>on</strong>s that we brought to thestudy. Either the patients that we admitted to treatment were quiteexcepti<strong>on</strong>al, or we had been misled by the traditi<strong>on</strong>al theories ofThis paper, prepared by Jack E. Nels<strong>on</strong> and reviewed by MarieNyswander, is based largely <strong>on</strong> an article written by Dr. Nyswanderand Dr. Vincent P. Dole, “Methad<strong>on</strong>e Maintenance and Its Implicati<strong>on</strong>for <str<strong>on</strong>g>Theories</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Narcotic Addicti<strong>on</strong>,” Research Publicati<strong>on</strong>s of the Associati<strong>on</strong>for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease, 49-359-66, 1968.Material from this article is reprinted with the permissi<strong>on</strong> of the Associati<strong>on</strong>for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease.256


addicti<strong>on</strong> (Terry and Pellens 1928). If, as is generally assumed, ourpatients’ l<strong>on</strong>g-standing addicti<strong>on</strong> to heroin had been based <strong>on</strong> weaknessesof character--either a self-indulgent quest for euphoria or aneed to escape reality--it was difficult to understand why they soc<strong>on</strong>sistently accepted a program that blocked the euphoric acti<strong>on</strong> ofheroin and other narcotic drugs, or how they could overcome thefrustrati<strong>on</strong>s and anxieties of competitive society to hold resp<strong>on</strong>siblejobs.Implicit in the maintenance programs is an assumpti<strong>on</strong> that heroinaddicti<strong>on</strong> is a metabolic disease, rather than a psychological problem.Although the reas<strong>on</strong>s for taking the initial doses of heroin may bec<strong>on</strong>sidered psychological--adolescent curiosity or neurotic anxiety--thedrug, for whatever reas<strong>on</strong> it is first taken, leaves its imprint <strong>on</strong> thenervous svstem. This phenomen<strong>on</strong> is clearly seen in animal studies:A rat, if addicted to morphine by repeated injecti<strong>on</strong>s at <strong>on</strong>e to twom<strong>on</strong>ths of age and then detoxified, will show a residual tolerance andabnormalities in brain waves in resp<strong>on</strong>se to challenge doses of morphinefor m<strong>on</strong>ths, perhaps for the rest of its life. Simply stopping the drugdoes not restore the nervous system of this animal to its normal,preaddicti<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. Since all studies to date have shown a closeassociati<strong>on</strong> between tolerance and physical dependence, and since thediscomfort of physical dependence leads to drug-seeking activity, apersistence of physical dependence would explain why both animals andhumans tend to relapse to use of narcotics after detoxificati<strong>on</strong>. Thismetabolic theory of relapse obviously has different implicati<strong>on</strong>s fortreatment than the traditi<strong>on</strong>al theory that relapse is due to moralweakness.Whatever the theory, all treatment should be measured by results.The main issue, in our opini<strong>on</strong>, is whether the treatment can enableaddicts to become normal, resp<strong>on</strong>sible members of society, and if amedicati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tributes to this result it should be regarded as usefulchemotherapy. Methad<strong>on</strong>e. like sulfanilamide of the early antibioticdays. undoubtedly will be supplanted by better medicati<strong>on</strong>s, but thesuccess of methad<strong>on</strong>e maintenance programs has at least established theprinciple of treating addicts medically.The efficacy of methad<strong>on</strong>e as a medicati<strong>on</strong> must be judged by itsability or failure to achieve the pharmacological effect that is intended--namely, eliminati<strong>on</strong> of heroin hunger and heroin-seeking behavior, andblockade against the euphoriant acti<strong>on</strong>s of heroin. The goal of socialrehabilitati<strong>on</strong> of criminal addicts by a treatment program is a muchbroader objective; it includes the stopping of heroin abuse, but is notlimited to this pharmacological effect. Failures in rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> programstherefore must be analyzed to determine whether they are due tofailures of the medicine, or to inability of the therapists to rehabilitatepatients who have stopped heroin use. Individuals who have stoppedheroin use with methad<strong>on</strong>e treatment but who c<strong>on</strong>tinue to steal, drinkexcessively, or abuse n<strong>on</strong>narcotic drugs, or are otherwise antisocial,are failures of the rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> program but not of the medicati<strong>on</strong>.When the Food and <strong>Drug</strong> Administrati<strong>on</strong> asks for proof of efficacy of anew drug it is the pharmacological efficacy that is in questi<strong>on</strong>. Forexample, diphenylhydantoin is accepted as an efficacious drug forpreventi<strong>on</strong> of epileptic seizures. Whether or not the treated epilepticsobtain employment or otherwise lead socially useful lives is not relevantto the evaluati<strong>on</strong> of this drug as an efficacious drug for preventi<strong>on</strong> ofepileptic seizures or as an antic<strong>on</strong>vulsant. Similarly with methad<strong>on</strong>e.257


With thousands of patients now living socially acceptable lives withmethad<strong>on</strong>e blockade and with many more street addicts waiting foradmissi<strong>on</strong>, the questi<strong>on</strong> as to whether these patients are excepti<strong>on</strong>al isno l<strong>on</strong>ger a practical issue. The theoretical questi<strong>on</strong>, however, remains:Is addicti<strong>on</strong> caused by an antecedent character defect, and does themaintenance treatment merely mask the symptoms of an addictive pers<strong>on</strong>ality?The psychogenic theory of addicti<strong>on</strong> would say so. This theoryhas a l<strong>on</strong>g history--at least 100 years (Terry and Pellens 1928)--and isaccepted as axiomatic by many people. What, then, is the evidencefor it?Review of the literature discloses two arguments to support the psychogenic,or character defect, theory: the sociopathic behavior andattitude of addicts and the inability of addicts to c<strong>on</strong>trol their drugusingimpulse. Of these arguments, the first is the most telling.Even a sympathetic observer must c<strong>on</strong>cede that addicts are self-centeredand indifferent to the needs of others. To the family and the communitythe addict is irresp<strong>on</strong>sible, a thief, and a liar. These traits,which are quite c<strong>on</strong>sistently associated with addicti<strong>on</strong>, have beeninterpreted as showing a specific psychopathology. What is lacking inthis argument is proof that the sociopathic traits preceded addicti<strong>on</strong>.It is important to distinguish the causes from the c<strong>on</strong>sequences ofaddicti<strong>on</strong> . The decisive proof of a psychogenic theory would be adem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> that potential addicts could be identified by psychiatricexaminati<strong>on</strong> before drug usage had distorted behavior and metabolicfuncti<strong>on</strong>s. However, a careful search of the literature has failed todisclose any study in which a characteristic psychopathology or “addictivepers<strong>on</strong>ality” has been recognized in a number of individuals priorto addicti<strong>on</strong>. Retrospective studies, in which a record of delinquencybefore addicti<strong>on</strong> is taken as evidence of sociopathic tendencies, fail toprovide the comparative data needed for diagnosis of deviant pers<strong>on</strong>ality.Most of the street addicts in large cities come from the slums wherefamily structure is broken and drugs are available. Both juveniledelinquency and drug use are comm<strong>on</strong>. Some delinquents becomeaddicted to narcotic drugs under these c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s, whereas others d<strong>on</strong>ot. There is no known way to identify the future addicts am<strong>on</strong>g thedelinquents. No study has shown a c<strong>on</strong>sistent difference in behavioror pattern of delinquency of adolescents who later become addicts andthose who do not.Theft is the means by which most street addicts obtain m<strong>on</strong>ey to buyheroin and, therefore, is nearly an inevitable c<strong>on</strong>sequence of addicti<strong>on</strong>.For the majority this is the <strong>on</strong>ly way that they can support an expensiveheroin habit. The crime statistics show both the force of drughunger and its specificity; almost all of the crimes committed by addictsrelate to the procurement of drugs. The rapid disappearance of theftand antisocial behavior in patients <strong>on</strong> the methad<strong>on</strong>e maintenanceprogram str<strong>on</strong>gly supports the hypothesis that the crimes that theyhad previously committed as addicts were a c<strong>on</strong>sequence of drug hunger,not the expressi<strong>on</strong> of some more basic psychopathology. The so-calledsociopathic pers<strong>on</strong>ality was no l<strong>on</strong>ger evident in our patients.The sec<strong>on</strong>d argument, that of deficient self-c<strong>on</strong>trol, is more complicatedbecause it involves the pers<strong>on</strong>al experience of the critic as well as thatof the patient. Moralists generally assume that opiates are dangerouslypleasant drugs that can be resisted <strong>on</strong>ly by strength of character.The pharmacology is somewhat more complicated than this. For mostnormal pers<strong>on</strong>s morphine and heroin are not enjoyable drugs--at least258


not in the initial exposures. Given to a postoperative patient theseanalgesics provide a welcome relief of pain, but addicti<strong>on</strong> from suchmedical use is uncomm<strong>on</strong>. When given to an average pain-free subject,morphine produces nausea and sedati<strong>on</strong>, but rarely euphoria. What,then, is the temptati<strong>on</strong> to become an addict? So far as can be judgedfrom the histories of addicts, many of them found the first trials of anarcotic in some sense pleasurable or tranquilizing, even though thedrug also caused nausea and vomiting. Perhaps their reacti<strong>on</strong> to thedrug was abnormal, even <strong>on</strong> the first exposure. However this maybe, with repeated use and development of tolerance to side effects, theeuphoric acti<strong>on</strong> evolved and the subjects became established addicts.<strong>Drug</strong>-seeking behavior, like theft, is observed after addicti<strong>on</strong> isestablished and the narcotic drug has become euphorigenic. Thequesti<strong>on</strong> as to whether this abnormality in reacti<strong>on</strong> stems from a basicweakness of character or is a c<strong>on</strong>sequence of drug usage is beststudied when drug hunger is relieved. Patients <strong>on</strong> the methad<strong>on</strong>emaintenance program, blockaded against the euphorigenic acti<strong>on</strong> ofheroin, turn their energies to schoolwork and jobs. It would be easyfor them to become passive, to live indefinitely <strong>on</strong> public support andclaim that they had d<strong>on</strong>e enough in winning the fight against heroin.Why they do not yield to this temptati<strong>on</strong> is unclear, but in generalthey do not. Their struggles to become self-supporting members ofthe community should impress the critics who had c<strong>on</strong>sidered themself-indulgent when drug-hungry addicts. When drug hunger isblocked without producti<strong>on</strong> of narcotic effects, the drug-seeking behaviorends.So far as can be judged from retrospective data, narcotic drugs havebeen quite freely available in some areas of New York City, and experimentati<strong>on</strong>by adolescents is comm<strong>on</strong>. The psychological and metabolictheories diverge somewhat in interpreting this fact; the first postulatespreexisting emoti<strong>on</strong>al problems and a need to seek drugs for escapefrom reality, whereas the alternative is that trial of drugs, like smokingthe first cigarette, may be a result of a normal adolescent curiosityand not of psychopathology (Wikler and Rasor 1953). As to the mostimportant point--the reas<strong>on</strong>s for c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> of drug use in somecases and not in others--there is no definitive informati<strong>on</strong>, eitherpsychological or metabolic. This is obviously a crucial gap in knowledge.Systematic study of young adolescents in areas with high addicti<strong>on</strong>rates is needed to define the process of becoming addicted and to openthe way for preventi<strong>on</strong>.The other extreme--the cured addict--involves a c<strong>on</strong>troversy as to thegoal of therapy. Those of us who are primarily c<strong>on</strong>cerned with thesocial productivity of our patients define success in terms of behavior--the ability of the patients to live as normal citizens in the community--whereas other groups seek total abstinence, even if it means c<strong>on</strong>finementof the subjects to an instituti<strong>on</strong>. This c<strong>on</strong>fusi<strong>on</strong> of goals has barredeffective comparis<strong>on</strong> of treatment results.Actually, the questi<strong>on</strong>s to be answered are straightforward and ofgreat practical importance. Do the abstinent patients in the psychologicalprograms have a residual metabolic defect that requires c<strong>on</strong>tinuedgroup pressure and instituti<strong>on</strong>alizati<strong>on</strong> to enforce the abstinence?C<strong>on</strong>versely, do the patients who are blockaded with methad<strong>on</strong>e exhibitany residual psychopathology? No evidence is available to answer thefirst questi<strong>on</strong>. As to the latter point, we can state that the evidenceso far is negative. The attitudes, moods, and intellectual and social259


performance of patients are under c<strong>on</strong>tinuous observati<strong>on</strong> by a team ofpsychiatrists, internists, nurses, counselors, social workers, andpsychologists. No c<strong>on</strong>sistent psychopathology has been noted by theseobservers or by the social agencies to which we have referred patientsfor vocati<strong>on</strong>al placement. The good records of employment and schoolwork further document the patients’ capacity to win acceptance asnormal citizens in the community.The real revoluti<strong>on</strong> of the methad<strong>on</strong>e era was its emphasis <strong>on</strong> rehabilitati<strong>on</strong>rather than <strong>on</strong> detoxificati<strong>on</strong>. This reversed the traditi<strong>on</strong>alapproach to addicti<strong>on</strong>, which had been based <strong>on</strong> the assumpti<strong>on</strong> thatabstinence must come first. According to the old theory, rehabilitati<strong>on</strong>is impossible while a pers<strong>on</strong> is taking drugs of any kind, includingmethad<strong>on</strong>e. The success of methad<strong>on</strong>e programs in rehabilitatingaddicts who had already failed in abstinence programs decisively refutedthis old theory. Indeed, nowhere in the history of treatment has aprogram with the abstinence approach achieved even a fracti<strong>on</strong> of theretenti<strong>on</strong> rate and social rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> now seen in the average methad<strong>on</strong>eclinic. This statement includes all of the abstinence-orientedprograms of governmental instituti<strong>on</strong>s, therapeutic communities, andreligious groups for which any data are available (Brecher 1972;Glasscote 1972).We believe that it is a serious mistake for programs to put a highervalue <strong>on</strong> abstinence than <strong>on</strong> the patient’s ability to functi<strong>on</strong> as anormal member of society. After the patient has arrived at a stableway of life with a job, a home, a positi<strong>on</strong> of respect in his community,and a sense of worth, it may, or may not, be best to disc<strong>on</strong>tinuemethad<strong>on</strong>e, but at least he can c<strong>on</strong>sider this opti<strong>on</strong> without pressure.The pharmacologic symptoms of withdrawal will be the same whether ornot the addict is socially rehabilitated, but with a job and family thereis much more to lose if relapse occurs, and therefore the motivati<strong>on</strong> toresist a return to heroin will be str<strong>on</strong>g. The time spent in maintenancetreatment does not make detoxificati<strong>on</strong> more difficult. It has provedvery easy to withdraw methad<strong>on</strong>e from patients who have been maintainedfor <strong>on</strong>e to eight years when the reducti<strong>on</strong> in dose has been gradual andthe patient free from anxiety.As with heroin, the real problems begin after withdrawal. The sec<strong>on</strong>daryabstinence syndrome, first described by Himmelsbach, Martin,Wikler, and colleagues at the United States Public Health Hospital,Lexingt<strong>on</strong>, Kentucky, in patients detoxified from morphine and heroin,reflects the persistence of metabolic and aut<strong>on</strong>omic disturbances in thepostnarcotic withdrawal period (Himmelsbach 1942; Martin et al. 1963;Martin and Jasinski 1969): These persistent abnormalities in metabolismare clearly pharmacologic since they occur also in experimental animalsaddicted to narcotics and then detoxified. Followup studies of abstinentex-addicts have emphasized the frequency of alcoholism and functi<strong>on</strong>aldeteriorati<strong>on</strong> (Brecher 1972).An unfortunate c<strong>on</strong>sequence of the early enthusiasm for methad<strong>on</strong>etreatment is today’s general disenchantment with chemotherapy foraddicts. What was not anticipated at the <strong>on</strong>set was the nearly universalreacti<strong>on</strong> against the c<strong>on</strong>cept of substituting <strong>on</strong>e drug for another,even when the sec<strong>on</strong>d drug enabled the addict to functi<strong>on</strong> normally.Statistics showing improved health and social rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> of thepatients receiving methad<strong>on</strong>e failed to meet this fundamental objecti<strong>on</strong>.The analogous l<strong>on</strong>g-term use of other medicati<strong>on</strong>s such as insulin anddigitalis in medical practice has not been c<strong>on</strong>sidered relevant.260


Perhaps the limitati<strong>on</strong>s of medical treatment for complex medical-socialproblems were not sufficiently stressed. No medicine can rehabilitatepers<strong>on</strong>s. Methad<strong>on</strong>e maintenance makes possible a first step towardsocial rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> by stabilizing the pharmacological c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> ofaddicts who have been living as criminals <strong>on</strong> the fringe of society.But to succeed in bringing disadvantaged addicts to a productive wayof life, a treatment program must enable its patients to feel pride andhope and to accept resp<strong>on</strong>sibility. This is often not achieved inpresent-day treatment programs. Without mutual respect, an adversaryrelati<strong>on</strong>ship develops between patients and staff, reinforced by arbitraryrules and the indifference of pers<strong>on</strong>s in authority. Patients held inc<strong>on</strong>tempt by the staff c<strong>on</strong>tinue to act like addicts, and the overcrowdedfacility becomes a public nuisance. Understandably, methad<strong>on</strong>e maintenanceprograms today have little appeal to the communities or to themajority of heroin addicts <strong>on</strong> the street.Methad<strong>on</strong>e maintenance, as part of a supportive program, facilitatessocial rehabilitati<strong>on</strong>, but methad<strong>on</strong>e treatment clearly does not preventopiate abuse after it is disc<strong>on</strong>tinued, nor does social rehabilitati<strong>on</strong>guarantee freedom from relapse.For the previously intractable heroin addict with a pretreatment historyof several years of addicti<strong>on</strong> and social problems, the most c<strong>on</strong>servativecourse, in our opini<strong>on</strong>, is to emphasize social rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> and encouragec<strong>on</strong>tinued maintenance. <strong>On</strong> the other hand, for patients withshorter histories of heroin use, especially the young <strong>on</strong>es, a trial ofwithdrawal with a systematic followup is indicated when physician andpatient feel ready for the test, and when they understand the potentialproblems after detoxificati<strong>on</strong>. The first step of withdrawing methad<strong>on</strong>eis relatively easy and can be achieved with a variety of schedules,n<strong>on</strong>e of which have been shown to have any specific effect <strong>on</strong> thel<strong>on</strong>g-range outcome. The real issue is how well the patient does inthe years after terminati<strong>on</strong> of maintenance.261


A Chr<strong>on</strong>obiologicalC<strong>on</strong>trol TheoryMark Hochhauser, Ph.D.CHRONOBIOLOGICAL VARIABLESThe effects of a given drug are a functi<strong>on</strong> of a number of variables;some of these variables, such as dosage level, have been c<strong>on</strong>sidered asrepresenting a specific chemical effect, unique to the amount of thedrug ingested by the individual. Other variables, such as psychologicalset, are c<strong>on</strong>sidered to be n<strong>on</strong>specific, and may be viewed as an individualizedbehavioral process, insofar as each drug user will have his orher own idiosyncratic psychological resp<strong>on</strong>se to a given drug.Chr<strong>on</strong>obiology (Halberg et al. 1977) offers a possible synthesis ofthese chemical and behavioral variables. Briefly stated, chr<strong>on</strong>obiology(or biological rhythms) c<strong>on</strong>cerns the temporal aspects of biology;numerous experiments have shown that both animal and human behaviorvary as a functi<strong>on</strong> of such rhythms (Luce 1971; v<strong>on</strong> Mayersbach1967) and that drug effects may be particularly sensitive to changes insuch chr<strong>on</strong>obiological rhythms.A number of chr<strong>on</strong>obiological rhythms have been identified: circadian(about 24 hours), diurnal/nocturnal (variati<strong>on</strong>s in light and darkperiods), ultradian (less than 24 hours), m<strong>on</strong>thly, or even yearly.Unfortunately, the role of such rhythms in human behavior has oftenbeen grossly misrepresented (e.g., McC<strong>on</strong>nell 1978).An understanding of chr<strong>on</strong>obiological rhythms and how they affect(and are affected by) behavior is essential to a more complete understandingof subject-drug interacti<strong>on</strong>s. Unfortunately, very little isknown about the field of developmental chr<strong>on</strong>obiology (Petren andSollberger 1967), although it has been documented that drugs willexert differential effects, depending up<strong>on</strong> the level of physiologicaland psychological maturity achieved by the subject (Young 1967;Vessel 1968; Vernadakis and Weiner 1974; C<strong>on</strong>roy and Mills 1970;Yaffee et al. 1968).262


CHRONOBIOLOGY AND DRUGSThere has been some empirical and theoretical work d<strong>on</strong>e <strong>on</strong> the relati<strong>on</strong>shipbetween chr<strong>on</strong>obiology and drug effects (Nair 1974; Reinberg1973; Reinberg and Halberg 1971); however, such findings have notbeen extrapolated to problems of drug addicti<strong>on</strong>. The following is abrief summary of the relati<strong>on</strong>ship between drugs and chr<strong>on</strong>obiologicalvariables.AMPHETAMINESRats have dem<strong>on</strong>strated circadian variati<strong>on</strong> in their susceptibility tod-amphetamine sulfate (Scheving 1969); furthermore, diurnal variati<strong>on</strong>s(i.e., differences in resp<strong>on</strong>sivity between periods of light and dark)have also been found for methamphetamine and p-chloromethamphetamine(Evans et al. 1973).BARBITURATESRats have also dem<strong>on</strong>strated l<strong>on</strong>g-term variati<strong>on</strong> (i.e., seas<strong>on</strong>al effects)in their resp<strong>on</strong>sivity to barbiturates (Beuthin and Bosquet 1970), aswell as daily variati<strong>on</strong>s (Davis 1962). Such temporal effects have beenattributed to changes in the rate of barbiturate metabolism by enzymesin the liver (Radzialowski and Bosquet 1968). Further, daily variati<strong>on</strong>shave been observed with phenobarbital (Pauly and Scheving 1964), anddifferent doses of pentobarbital have had different effects as a functi<strong>on</strong>of circadian rhythms (Nels<strong>on</strong> and Halberg 1973). Moreover, there areapparent chr<strong>on</strong>obiological differences even within the barbituratecategory, as some barbiturates (e.g., phenobarbital) are l<strong>on</strong>g lasting,while others (e.g., hexobarbital) act for a shorter period of time(Muller 1974). Finally, it has been noted that the durati<strong>on</strong> ofbarbiturate-induced sleep in rats was a functi<strong>on</strong> of the circadian phaseof administrati<strong>on</strong>; the same barbiturate, administered in the same dosebut at different times, produced variable levels of sleep. Thesefindings suggested that the neurotransmitters that c<strong>on</strong>trol sleep maydisplay rhythmic levels of activity (Friedman 1974).ALCOHOLAlcohol studies <strong>on</strong> humans have found that ethanol is metabolizedfaster in the evening than in the afterno<strong>on</strong>, at least am<strong>on</strong>g somealcoholics (J<strong>on</strong>es and Paredes 1974). However, <strong>on</strong> cognitive tasks,J<strong>on</strong>es (1974) has found that alcohol impaired cognitive performancemore in the afterno<strong>on</strong> than in the evening, suggesting a faster metabolicrate for alcohol in the afterno<strong>on</strong>. <strong>Studies</strong> with mice have also dem<strong>on</strong>strateddramatic variati<strong>on</strong>s in alcohol susceptibility over a 24-hourperiod; dependinq up<strong>on</strong> the time of administrati<strong>on</strong>, the mortality ratecould be increased fivefold (Haus and Halberg 1959). More recently,Zeiner and Paredes (1978) have obtained racial differences in thecircadian variati<strong>on</strong> of ethanol metabolism; they found that a higherpeak blood alcohol c<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong> was reached in the morning than inthe afterno<strong>on</strong> am<strong>on</strong>g white male subjects, while for a male NativeAmerican group the peak blood alcohol c<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong> was lowest in themorning and highest at night.263


LIBRIUMTemporal variati<strong>on</strong>s have been found in the survival rate of rats tolethal doses of Librium (Marte and Halberg 1961). the amount requiredfor a lethal dose depending up<strong>on</strong> the time of administrati<strong>on</strong>.OPIATESSeveral studies have found that a rat’s resp<strong>on</strong>sivity to morphine ispartly a functi<strong>on</strong> of chr<strong>on</strong>obiological rhythms. For example, Morrisand Lutsch (1969) observed diurnal rhythms in resp<strong>on</strong>se to morphineanalgesia; later, they discovered that the effects of morphine could bemanipulated by changes in the lighting period (Lutsch and Morris1971, 1972). More recently, Bornschein (1975) observed that theeffective dose of morphine varied with the time of administrati<strong>on</strong>;morphine was most toxic at the end of the animal’s active phase, andleast toxic at the end of the animal’s rest phase. Similarly, Bornscheinet al. (1977) noted changes in the animal’s central nervous systemresp<strong>on</strong>siveness to morphine; they detected a threefold difference in theefficacy of morphine as a functi<strong>on</strong> of time of day (i.e., morphine was2.7 times more effective at 0300 hours than at 1500 hours). Unfortunately,there is very little research bearing <strong>on</strong> the relati<strong>on</strong>ship betweenchr<strong>on</strong>obiological rhythms and human opiate use (e.g., Ghodse et al.1977).OPIATE ANTAGONISTSIt has been reported (Fredericks<strong>on</strong> et al. 1977) that the administrati<strong>on</strong>of nalox<strong>on</strong>e, a narcotic antag<strong>on</strong>ist, will produce variable results inrats, depending up<strong>on</strong> the phase of circadian rhythm at administrati<strong>on</strong>.Much of the previously cited research relating drug use to chr<strong>on</strong>obiologicalfactors has emphasized the administrati<strong>on</strong> of a drug during aspecific time within the <strong>on</strong>going rhythmic period. C<strong>on</strong>sequently, theexperimental focus has been <strong>on</strong> how rhythmic activities affect theresp<strong>on</strong>sivity to a given drug. A complementary way of viewing therelati<strong>on</strong>ship between drug events and chr<strong>on</strong>obiological events is toc<strong>on</strong>sider how the drug itself may affect the level of rhythmic functi<strong>on</strong>ingin the subject.CHRONOBIOLOGY AND SELF-MEDICATIONIf <strong>on</strong>e views drug abuse as a possible form of self-medicati<strong>on</strong>, then itis c<strong>on</strong>ceivable that some drug use represents an attempt <strong>on</strong> the part ofthe user to induce artificially certain rhythmic patterns where n<strong>on</strong>ehave been before, or perhaps to reestablish such patterns when theyhave been lost. For example, Orr (1976) has suggested that amphetamineuse may represent an attempt by the drug user to get back to aregulated sleep-wakefulness schedule. Can the “uppers” and “downers”taken by many drug users be compared to the “ups” and “downs” ofchr<strong>on</strong>obiological rhythm periods? An additi<strong>on</strong>al possibility would be forthe drug to establish a “limit cycle,” in which the motivati<strong>on</strong> for druguse would not simply be the acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of a particular rhythm, but anattempt to avoid going too high or too low within the rhythm; as such,the drug would serve as a regulating device.264


Should this hypothesis prove relevant, future research, rather thanstudying <strong>on</strong>ly retrospective patterns of drug use (what drug wastaken, how often it had been used in the past) should focus up<strong>on</strong>when a given drug (or drugs) is used (Sinnet and Morris 1977),in as much as the timing of administrati<strong>on</strong> of a particular drug may beas significant as many of the other variables.In heroin addicti<strong>on</strong>, for example, there is the increase in pleasureobtained after the injecti<strong>on</strong>, the gradual reducti<strong>on</strong> of pleasure afterseveral hours, the <strong>on</strong>set of unpleasant withdrawal symptoms, theinjecti<strong>on</strong> of another dose of heroin, etc. Viewed in l<strong>on</strong>g-term chr<strong>on</strong>obiologicalpatterns, it seems possible that the heroin user might betaking heroin in an attempt to maintain some degree of rhythmicity inhis or her physiological and psychological functi<strong>on</strong>ing.As a final point, deaths from a heroin overdose might be due in partto when the heroin is taken; if injected at a time of maximal susceptibilitywithin the chr<strong>on</strong>obiological rhythm, the effect might be quitedifferent (i.e., death) than if it were taken during a time of minimalsusceptibility (i.e., survival).Unfortunately, most chr<strong>on</strong>obiological drug studies are bound to arelatively simplistic “time of day”; a more complex analysis arises fromthe possibility of “free running” rhythms that are not synchr<strong>on</strong>izedwith the envir<strong>on</strong>mental cycles. In such cases, the subject will drift inand out of phase with the chr<strong>on</strong>obiological clock, experiencing periodic“jet lag” discomfort. Perhaps narcotic addicts have such discomfortsand use heroin in an attempt to synchr<strong>on</strong>ize their internal rhythms tothe envir<strong>on</strong>ment.CHRONOBIOLOGY AND CONTROLAs chr<strong>on</strong>obiological rhythms are related to drug effects, so are theyimplicated in a number of different psychiatric problems. Recently,behavioral rhythms have been observed in several schizophrenics(Reynolds et al. 1978). and circadian rhythm disorders have beeninvestigated in manic-depressive patients (Kripke et al. 1978). <strong>On</strong>eimportant implicati<strong>on</strong> of this research is the possibility that suchpsychiatric problems may have a biological basis related to rhythmicactivity within the brain. Indeed, it has been found (Philipp andMarneros 1978) that some patients with endogenous depressi<strong>on</strong> aretreated more effectively with a single large dose of an antidepressantthan with three smaller doses throughout the day. Such findingssuggest that there may be circadian fluctuati<strong>on</strong>s within the neurotransmittersystem, thus making the depressi<strong>on</strong> more (or less) susceptibleto chemical treatment. Obviously, there are not <strong>on</strong>ly variati<strong>on</strong>sin chr<strong>on</strong>obiological rhythms, but in c<strong>on</strong>sciousness and psychologicalfactors as well (Brought<strong>on</strong> 1975).<strong>On</strong>e hypothesis c<strong>on</strong>cerning the motivati<strong>on</strong> for drug use (and abuse) isthat drugs may be c<strong>on</strong>sumed in an effort to self-medicate (e.g.,Mellinger 1978). This analysis is particularly attractive in light of theresearch <strong>on</strong> chr<strong>on</strong>obiological rhythms, since it suggests that (1) if anindividual cannot predict or c<strong>on</strong>trol his or her chr<strong>on</strong>obiological rhythms(e.g., manic depressi<strong>on</strong>) or (2) if the amplitude of the manic-depressivebehavior exceeds normal limits, the pers<strong>on</strong> may resort to licit and/orillicit drugs in an attempt to establish some c<strong>on</strong>trol over these fluctuating265


moods. Thus, heroin use may be viewed as a way of coping withpsychological problems (Khantzian et al. 1974) or, more specifically,with particularly stressful situati<strong>on</strong>s as assessed by life change units(Duncan 1977).This use of drugs to c<strong>on</strong>trol possible aberrant chr<strong>on</strong>obiological rhythmsis an important c<strong>on</strong>cept, especially as related to the c<strong>on</strong>cept of learnedhelplessness (Seligman 1975). A c<strong>on</strong>siderable amount of research, bothwith animals and with humans, has suggested that exposure to unpredictableand unc<strong>on</strong>trollable events may interfere with the individual’sability subsequently to master a learning task, even if such futuretasks are c<strong>on</strong>trollable. That is, the individual becomes psychologically“helpless.”<strong>On</strong>e intriguing aspect of this research has been the theoretical linkagebetween helplessness and depressi<strong>on</strong>. It was assumed initially thathelplessness might serve as a theoretical model of depressi<strong>on</strong>. Additi<strong>on</strong>alresearch, however (Huesmann 1978), has questi<strong>on</strong>ed the earlyc<strong>on</strong>cept of learned helplessness as a model of depressi<strong>on</strong>, and Seligmanand his associates (Abrams<strong>on</strong> et al. 1978) have recently reformulatedthe theory of learned helplessness to account for a wider range ofcognitive processes (e.g., attributi<strong>on</strong>). These modificati<strong>on</strong>s notwithstanding.the learned helplessness hypothesis is based primarily <strong>on</strong>learned experiences; if the evidence regarding chr<strong>on</strong>obiological rhythmsin depressi<strong>on</strong> is correct, however, then another phenomen<strong>on</strong> whichmight c<strong>on</strong>tribute to percepti<strong>on</strong>s of helplessness would be the unpredictableand unc<strong>on</strong>trollable chr<strong>on</strong>obiological rhythms that produce depressi<strong>on</strong>.As such, drugs may be used as agents of c<strong>on</strong>trol (Hochhauser 1978a)which permit the individual user to exert some degree of internalc<strong>on</strong>trol over his or her percepti<strong>on</strong>s of helplessness.Learned helplessness appears to play a role in alcohol and drug use(e.g., Sadava et al. 1978); moreover, the relati<strong>on</strong>ship between locusof c<strong>on</strong>trol (Rotter 1966) and alcohol and/or drug use is <strong>on</strong>e which hasgenerated much research. Locus of c<strong>on</strong>trol (whether <strong>on</strong>e believes<strong>on</strong>e’s behavior to be internally or externally c<strong>on</strong>trolled) has beenmeasured in a wide variety of drug-using populati<strong>on</strong>s (e.g., Plumb etal. 1975; Hall 1978): opiate addicts (Berzins and Ross 1973; Henikand Domino 1974; Obitz et al. 1974), alcoholics (Goss and Morosko1970; Gozali and Sloan 1971; Oziel et al. 1972; Oziel and Obitz 1975;Obitz and Swans<strong>on</strong> 1976; Hinrichsen 1976; Weissbach et al. 1976;Rohsenow and O’Leary 1978a,b), and polydrug users (Segal 1974).Such studies have often reported c<strong>on</strong>flicting results. <strong>On</strong>e reas<strong>on</strong> forsuch discrepancies might be that the initial locus-of-c<strong>on</strong>trol measurefocused primarily <strong>on</strong> behavioral indices; it may be that a locus-ofc<strong>on</strong>trolc<strong>on</strong>cept which takes into account other factors, such as health(e.g., Strickland 1978), may be more appropriate for alcohol and drugproblems.DISTINCTIONS BETWEEN DRUG USE,ABUSE, DEPENDENCY, AND ADDICTIONAssuming that drugs may be used as agents of c<strong>on</strong>trol, it is arguedthat--266


1.2.3.4.<strong>Drug</strong> use may represent an initial attempt to achieve some degreeof internal c<strong>on</strong>trol over percepti<strong>on</strong>s of helplessness; moreover,drugs may be a relatively quick and effective means of obtainingsuch c<strong>on</strong>trol, especially when other c<strong>on</strong>trol measures are unavailable;If a drug is used for c<strong>on</strong>trol and is found effective, then its usewill probably escalate, as the individual may develop a relativelypredictable and c<strong>on</strong>trollable method of coping;Dependency may develop if there are no other effective copingmechanisms available;Depending up<strong>on</strong> the addictive liability of the drug, addicti<strong>on</strong> mayoccur with c<strong>on</strong>tinued use, as the physiological c<strong>on</strong>sequences of thedrug (e.g., withdrawal symptoms) may eventually establish c<strong>on</strong>trolover the user. At this point, addicts may seek treatment, sincethey are no l<strong>on</strong>ger using the drug for c<strong>on</strong>trol; rather, they arebeing c<strong>on</strong>trolled by the drug.SPECIAL PROBLEMSACCIDENTAL DEATH/SUICIDEResearch <strong>on</strong> chr<strong>on</strong>obiological rhythms suggests that there may beperiods of minimal and maximal sensitivity to the lethal dose of a drug;c<strong>on</strong>sequently, problems such as heroin-overdose deaths or barbiturateoverdosedeaths may be related to when (in the rhythmic cycle) agiven drug is taken.PSYCHOPATHOLOGYIt is difficult to determine if psychopathological behaviors (e.g.,schizophrenia, manic-depressive behavior, etc.) lead to drug use(perhaps in an attempt to self-medicate such problems), or whetherc<strong>on</strong>tinued drug use (perhaps through changes in chr<strong>on</strong>obiologicalrhythms associated with psychopathology) may cause subsequent psychopathology.Relati<strong>on</strong>ships between chr<strong>on</strong>obiology, psychopathology, anddrug abuse require additi<strong>on</strong>al clarificati<strong>on</strong>.ADOLESCENTSSignificant psychological and physiological changes occur during adolescence,and the effect of drugs up<strong>on</strong> such developmental changes islargely unknown (Hochhauser 1978b). <strong>Studies</strong> of adolescent drugabuse suggest, however, that depressi<strong>on</strong> is often a characteristicvariable associated with drug abuse (Braucht et al. 1973) and that theinability to cope with stressful experiences may play a significant rolein the development of drug dependence (Duncan 1977). The interrelati<strong>on</strong>shipbetween changing chr<strong>on</strong>obiological rhythms, percepti<strong>on</strong>s ofinternal c<strong>on</strong>trol, and drug abuse must be more clearly defined.267


THE ELDERLYDuring the period of old age, there are often significant envir<strong>on</strong>mentalchanges (e.g., retirement, loss of a spouse, relocati<strong>on</strong>) which maymake the individual more helpless and possibly more susceptible todrugs as a way of coping. Moreover, important physiological changesare also taking place (e.g., reduced metabolism, changes in sleeppatterns, horm<strong>on</strong>e reducti<strong>on</strong>) which may substantially affect chr<strong>on</strong>obiologicalrhythms, thus making the elderly pers<strong>on</strong> more susceptible todrug effects.268


A BioanthropologicalOverview of Addicti<strong>on</strong>Doris F. J<strong>on</strong>as, Ph.D.A. David J<strong>on</strong>as, M.D.Sometimes a collaborati<strong>on</strong> between individuals occupied in separate biologicalfields and the applicati<strong>on</strong> of understandings from <strong>on</strong>e field tothe other leads to felicitous insights and new perspectives. Our ownexperience has encompassed studies of the evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary bases of humanbehavior <strong>on</strong> the <strong>on</strong>e hand and two decades of clinical experience withaddicts of various kinds (and their families) <strong>on</strong> the other.Departing from the c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al view that addicti<strong>on</strong> arises solely fromthe life history of an individual or out of an obscure chemical imbalance,we have come to a formulati<strong>on</strong> of the problem, rather, as <strong>on</strong>e of theeffects of group mechanisms up<strong>on</strong> the individual. The dynamics residingwithin the entity we call a society affect all its members. There arethose who can adapt themselves to group requirements and others whoin some or many ways cannot. This applies to all social groups of allcreatures, whether animal or human.Very frequently manifestati<strong>on</strong>s that appear to us to be peculiarlyhuman, when compared with the patterns of life of other animals, cometo be thought of as due to our cultural endowment or to our specificcivilizati<strong>on</strong> and as phenomena that therefore define a separati<strong>on</strong> betweenour species and all others. Language, love, politics, and the care ofthe sick are am<strong>on</strong>g many human propensities and predilecti<strong>on</strong>s thatcome into this category. Yet everything human has its origin in ananimal past, and such a view tends to prevent certain aspects ofhuman behavior from being seen in a c<strong>on</strong>text of overall natural patterns,hindering full understanding of their significance.The problem of addicti<strong>on</strong> is certainly a human <strong>on</strong>e, and it has not beenthought of in terms of comparative behavior. The reas<strong>on</strong> is simple.Reprinted with permissi<strong>on</strong> from Perspectives in Biology and Medicine,Spring: 345-354. 1977. Copyright © 1977 by The University of Chicago.All rights reserved.269


Addicti<strong>on</strong> does not occur in a natural state. Laboratory animals maybe induced artificially to become addicted to most of the substances <strong>on</strong>which a human being may become physiologically or psychologicallydependent, but this does not happen in feral c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. Nor, <strong>on</strong> theother hand, is the presence of “mind” in humans an explanati<strong>on</strong> forthe different behavior, since animals with no advanced neocorticaldevelopment can become addicted in laboratory c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s.Bey<strong>on</strong>d the failure to view addicti<strong>on</strong> in terms of overall natural processes--orperhaps a part of that failure--is the tendency we have hadto ask questi<strong>on</strong>s about the “whys” of addicti<strong>on</strong> in terms <strong>on</strong>ly of anaddicted individual’s life. We ask what pers<strong>on</strong>al problems led him toturn to drugs or alcohol for relief. Even if we take a step furtherand examine the social background of the addict, seeking a cause forhis problems <strong>on</strong> a wider basis, this larger dimensi<strong>on</strong> is c<strong>on</strong>sideredrelevant <strong>on</strong>ly in terms of its effect <strong>on</strong> the individual; and so theanswers we find, like the questi<strong>on</strong>s we pose, remain individual oriented.Since the study of the individual is the domain of the psychiatrist, theproblems of addicti<strong>on</strong> have come to be accepted as within his province.A further questi<strong>on</strong> that must arise, of course, is how it can happenthat addicti<strong>on</strong> can arise biologically. This questi<strong>on</strong> has been asked bysome, and answers to it have been sought in the physiology of thenervous system. But this step again focuses <strong>on</strong> the individual, evenwhen investigati<strong>on</strong>s are pursued into his genetic background; and so,while the questi<strong>on</strong> is right, the approach to answering it is limiting,since it leads no further than the previous <strong>on</strong>es--to the individual.Yet it is indeed in neurophysiology that we may begin to find clues tothe larger pattern. The nervous system is more than a recipient ofstimuli and regulator of an organism’s behavior. It is a repository ofreflex resp<strong>on</strong>ses that c<strong>on</strong>nect the individual to his phylogenetic pastand is also a regulator of interacti<strong>on</strong>s between the individual and thepresent society of which he is a part. What we call social pressuresare c<strong>on</strong>veyed to an individual, and he reacts to them, not <strong>on</strong>ly throughhis understanding but also through direct neural resp<strong>on</strong>ses, so that inthis sense the nervous system is the mediator between an individualand a society in a way analogous to the role of the horm<strong>on</strong>es in mediatingbetween the behavior of cells and the needs of the whole organism.In binding individuals to the needs of their societies, their nervoussystems serve to integrate group well-being. To see this clearly, it ishelpful to look at some group mechanisms in the breeding groups ofother species, and we may then see how these throw light <strong>on</strong> otherwisepuzzling human behavior.The pi<strong>on</strong>eer experiments of R.N. Chapman (1928) showed that, in anenclosed envir<strong>on</strong>ment in which the nutrient medium was a layer of flourtwo centimeters deep, a steady ceiling populati<strong>on</strong> of the flour beetle(Tribolium c<strong>on</strong>fusum) would ultimately be obtained. An experimentallyrepeatable, almost c<strong>on</strong>stant density of individuals per gram of flourwas finally arrived at, whether the culture was started with <strong>on</strong>e pairof adults or many pairs or whether the volume of flour was small orlarge. Of many subsequent workers, D.S. MacLagan (1962) performedparallel experiments with Tribolium and Sitophilus, the grain weevil.He found that there was a drop in the number of eggs laid per femaleassociated with crowding, and he c<strong>on</strong>cluded that natural populati<strong>on</strong>s aswell as experimental <strong>on</strong>es “automatically check their own increase byvirtue of this density effect, and that the organism itself imposes theultimate limit to its own abundance when all other factors (biotic and270


physical) have failed” (p. 452). Both Tribolium c<strong>on</strong>fusum and itsclose relative T. castaneum, when adult, have glands in the thoraxand abdomen that produce an irritant gas that P. Alexander andD. H.R. Bart<strong>on</strong> (1943) identified as ethylquin<strong>on</strong>e. The glands arestimulated to liberate this gas by disturbance and crowding. In crystal<strong>line</strong>form, it is lethal to first-instar larvae; as a gas it inducesdevelopmental abnormalities in late larvae and pupae, and it probablyhas a depressing effect <strong>on</strong> the well-being of the adults (Roth andHowland 1958).The now classic experiments of C.M. Breder and C.W. Coates (1932)showed that, in tanks c<strong>on</strong>taining an equal volume of water, whether asingle gravid female or a number of guppies (Labistes reticulatus)were placed in them, it took <strong>on</strong>ly about 20 weeks for the same c<strong>on</strong>stantpopulati<strong>on</strong> of nine or ten fish to be reached in each. Surplus individualswere cannibalized.Tadpoles overcrowded in their tanks excrete into the water metabolitesthat have the effect of stunting growth until the smaller individualsdie off and the populati<strong>on</strong> is adjusted to an uncrowded c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>(Richard 1958).Socially induced mortality occurs also in birds, and am<strong>on</strong>g them socialstatus becomes a factor. The most subordinate members of a populati<strong>on</strong>may be inhibited from breeding at all. Those a little higher in rankmay achieve a nest and mate and perhaps even eggs, but when envir<strong>on</strong>mentalc<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s impose a necessity for a reducti<strong>on</strong> in the number ofyoung birds being reared, the stress falls more tellingly <strong>on</strong> them than<strong>on</strong> better-established and higher-ranking members of the community.A wide variety of species-specific mechanisms are brought into play,from reduced egg producti<strong>on</strong> or the destructi<strong>on</strong> of eggs to the killingof young, but for our present purpose it is sufficient to note that thereducti<strong>on</strong> of the threshold of resistance to parasitic infestati<strong>on</strong> is <strong>on</strong>eof the many manifestati<strong>on</strong>s of crowding stress that have a homeostaticeffect. D. Lack (1954, chapter 54), in an extensive review of mortalityattributable to disease in birds, noted that, when they are in goodc<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, such birds as the red grouse (Lagopus lagopus) can carry ac<strong>on</strong>siderable burden of internal parasites without injury but that, ifthe quality of their staple food plant is affected by harsh weather orby unusually extensive damage by the heather beetle, then the birds’threshold of resistance is lowered so that the lower ranking appear todie of parasitic disease.The element of status in the survival of birds in crowded c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>swas noted in an extreme manifestati<strong>on</strong> by A.A. Allen (1934). Heobserved in a captive group of ruffed grouse (B<strong>on</strong>asa umbellus L.)what he called an intimidati<strong>on</strong> display. “A bird that has been completelysubjugated . . . is subject to attack from every other bird inthe enclosure. He has developed an inferiorism and usually, unlessremoved, he remains in a corner until he dies, not from mechanicalinjury nor from starvati<strong>on</strong>, but from some sort of nervous shock, anddeath is likely to occur within 24 hours.” V.C. Wynne-Edwards (1962)has commented that the functi<strong>on</strong> of hierarchy is to identify surplusindividuals whenever envir<strong>on</strong>mental necessities require a reducti<strong>on</strong> ofpopulati<strong>on</strong>.Wild mammals resp<strong>on</strong>d no less than other creatures to populati<strong>on</strong> density.In North Wales Brambell and his associates made the discovery in therabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus) that an average of 64 percent of embryos271


c<strong>on</strong>ceived perish before birth, usually by the twelfth day of pregnancy.The arrested embryos are not aborted, but their tissue is broken downand resorbed by the uterus, leaving nothing but an impermanent scar(Thomps<strong>on</strong> and Worden 1956, pp. 112-113). The percentage of embryosresorbed is resp<strong>on</strong>sive to envir<strong>on</strong>mental c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s.These are but arbitrary examples (that could be multiplied almostendlessly) taken from insect, fish, amphibian, avian, and mammaliansocieties, to give some idea of the types of social mechanisms to whichwe are referring. V.C. Wynne-Edwards states that there can remainno doubt that populati<strong>on</strong>s are effectively self-limiting, “and the inferencemust be very str<strong>on</strong>g that selecti<strong>on</strong> has perfected the adaptati<strong>on</strong>sso that populati<strong>on</strong> densities always tend to balance themselves at theoptimum level” (1962, p. 498). In his encyclopedic work, he hasshown that there is probably no species that does not have somebuilt-in method of populati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trol that effectively regulates thedensity of its breeding groups or societies. Indeed it is clear thatthis must be so, since any populati<strong>on</strong> that failed to effect this regulati<strong>on</strong>would very so<strong>on</strong> strip its habitat of the resources necessary forits sustenance and thus promote its own extincti<strong>on</strong>.For each species, the range of “pers<strong>on</strong>al space” required by individualsvaries, but the work of such researchers as J.B. Calhoun (1962, 1963)has made us aware of the gross distorti<strong>on</strong>s of normal behavior thatoccur when this space requirement is infringed. The more enlightenedcurators of zoos have recently come to recognize the modifying effects<strong>on</strong> the natural behavior of animals of cages that c<strong>on</strong>fine their livingspace too closely, and we ourselves have become aware of our ownneed for “pers<strong>on</strong>al space” and of human resp<strong>on</strong>ses ranging from mildirritati<strong>on</strong> all the way to violent aggressi<strong>on</strong> that may occur when it isinvaded.The potential for these resp<strong>on</strong>ses is carried genetically in all species,and H. Selye (1950) has shown that social stress can have depressingand injurious effects <strong>on</strong> the animal body just as severe as those producedby disease, hunger, or fatigue. But the mediating agencybetween the envir<strong>on</strong>ment and the individual is the nervous system, inthat the nervous system not <strong>on</strong>ly brings awareness of populati<strong>on</strong>pressures to the individual but also sets in moti<strong>on</strong> the adaptiveresp<strong>on</strong>ses, whether physiological or behavioral.We may now ask ourselves, Where does the human being fit into thispattern? It is evident that no natural design as universal as theresp<strong>on</strong>se against crowding in the interlocking network of mechanismsthat exist in the interest of species viability and survival can possiblybe without significance or c<strong>on</strong>sequence in our own. And, indeed, intoquite recent times many human practices, including infanticide, ger<strong>on</strong>tocide,social requirements governing marriage, taboos governing childspacing, and so <strong>on</strong>, have in effect achieved populati<strong>on</strong> regulati<strong>on</strong> bycultural means as efficient as the biological mechanisms of other species(Carr-Saunders 1926). Many observers have noted that tribal groupsrelatively out of c<strong>on</strong>tact with modern life had maintained stable populati<strong>on</strong>sover l<strong>on</strong>g ages. If this is the case, we must c<strong>on</strong>cede that it ishardly possible for modern societies of man to be entirely free not <strong>on</strong>lyof vestiges of earlier mechanisms of populati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trol but also of somethat operate in our own societies and ‘in our own times.It is indeed probable that such mechanisms in fact exist but that theyappear in the guise of cultural practices or individual characteriological272


idiosyncrasies that themselves become the subject of attenti<strong>on</strong> and soobscure awareness of the larger functi<strong>on</strong> they serve. This theninterferes with our realizati<strong>on</strong> that powerful biological mechanismsoperate <strong>on</strong> a group level in our culturally and technologically modifiedsocieties, no less than in all societies in the rest of nature. Doubtlessthe individuals living in a tribal society would see the observance oftheir taboos affecting, say, child spacing as acts of c<strong>on</strong>formance withtheir customary cultural practices and would not relate it to similarbehavior in many animal groups. Would it be possible for us, who areat a distance and not involved with them, to see those practices interms of their overall biological effect? May we then not ask ourselveswhether some of the social manifestati<strong>on</strong>s we see in our own communitiesand c<strong>on</strong>sider to be culturally c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed may not also fit into largerbiological patterns?We believe that this is so and that addicti<strong>on</strong> comes into this category.From the point of view of the individual c<strong>on</strong>cerned and of those whoattempt to relieve him of his habit, addicti<strong>on</strong> is an unmitigated ill.The initial gratificati<strong>on</strong>s derived from the drug, alcohol, or smokefrequently fade as an organism becomes habituated and relativelyimmune to their effects, and the addict is then driven to larger andmore frequent intake in order to capture the initial pleasurable feeling.In this process a crescendo of ills besets him. His health dec<strong>line</strong>s,and his social adjustment is increasingly disrupted, until eventually hesecedes from his community altogether and lives, so to speak, <strong>on</strong> itsfringes, either as a member of a deviant group or as a solitary outcast.His fertility is frequently impaired and his genetic endowment thusoften eliminated from the gene pool of his populati<strong>on</strong>.The pers<strong>on</strong>al involvement of medical workers in general and of psychiatristsin particular in attempts to help these individual reverse theirdownward drift is a factor in the failure to see the overall pattern.Given our current attitudes about what c<strong>on</strong>stitutes sickness and health,focused as they are <strong>on</strong> the well-being of individuals, perhaps it takesa quantum jump in our thinking to recognize the undoubted fact that awhole breeding group or society is an evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary unit in its ownright and must also maintain good health if it is to survive. <strong>On</strong> thehealth of its breeding groups the viability of an entire species depends.Here we must note that the general understanding of the principle ofnatural selecti<strong>on</strong> encompasses a misc<strong>on</strong>cepti<strong>on</strong>: it is that the genes offit individuals survive, while those of less fit individuals are weededout over time. The fact is a little different, but that small differenceputs a completely other complexi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the matter. It is that theprinciple of natural selecti<strong>on</strong> does not operate at the level of theindividual but at the level of the breeding populati<strong>on</strong> as a whole.At this level it is easy to see that, if every individual within a breedingpopulati<strong>on</strong> were exquisitely adapted to its current envir<strong>on</strong>ment, thenany external change in that envir<strong>on</strong>ment would wipe out the totalpopulati<strong>on</strong>. It is therefore an adaptive character of populati<strong>on</strong>s thatthey carry in their total gene pool several variati<strong>on</strong>s of individualtraits, including some that appear currently maladaptive at any giventime.The best-known example that illustrates this fact is the populati<strong>on</strong> ofmoths whose white wings are peppered with grey-to-black speckles.In an earlier period of pristine atmosphere, the whiter moths273


predominated numerically in the populati<strong>on</strong>. As industrial polluti<strong>on</strong>darkened the trunks and branches of the trees <strong>on</strong> which they settled,however, the whiter moths became c<strong>on</strong>spicuous to their predators andwere picked off in proporti<strong>on</strong>ately larger numbers, until at the presenttime the populati<strong>on</strong>s of this species are predominantly of the darkercolored variety (Kettlewell 1973). Should human antipolluti<strong>on</strong> endeavorsnow prevail, <strong>on</strong> the other hand, it is likely that the lightly speckledmoths will again come into their own. Thus it will be seen clearlythat, for this moth populati<strong>on</strong> as a whole, it is adaptive for it tocarry, if recessively, in all generati<strong>on</strong>s a potential for alternativecolorati<strong>on</strong> that at any particular time is ill adapted--indeed, dangerous--for the individuals that carry it. Without this potential for alternativecolorati<strong>on</strong>, the destiny of all the moths would be at the mercy of thevagaries of their habitat, and their species life would be very short.Variati<strong>on</strong> of color in the moths, of course, is an anatomical character.In our own species from its earliest emergence, however, behavior hasbeen as determinative of survival as morphology and a spectrum ofbehavioral traits therefore as important to the viability of our speciesas a whole as morphological variety. It would seem reas<strong>on</strong>able toassume that a range of sensitivity to external stimuli and group pressureswould have its place in this c<strong>on</strong>text and that those who take andbecome addicted to narcotics or stimulants in our present societies aream<strong>on</strong>g the more sensitive to such stimuli and pressures. In other erasor in other circumstances, such hypersensitivity might well have beenand may well yet become a species-saving characteristic. Yet in theuniversal ability of all groups of all species to reproduce a largernumber of offspring than they can sustain and the tendency of allgroups to do so lies the necessity that mechanisms must exist to ensurethat at all times all populati<strong>on</strong>s are, so to speak, thinned out. Inevitablyit is those individuals who are currently of the wr<strong>on</strong>g color, tootall or too short, too slow or too fast, or too little or too acutelysensitive who are the <strong>on</strong>es who become sacrifices to the need of thegroup to adjust its density. Those individuals then, depending <strong>on</strong>their species, fall prey to predators; to reduced resistance to parasites;to eliminati<strong>on</strong> by others before birth, in the perinatal period, or later;or to self-eliminati<strong>on</strong> as a result of social pressures.Within the c<strong>on</strong>text of the c<strong>on</strong>cept of the well-being of the larger entity,the whole society--its necessity to produce more recruits than it needsso that it may survive in case of calamity and therefore its equalnecessity to eliminate its surplus--we discover an overall design intowhich several c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s that have proved puzzling to investigatorsmay well fit. For example, no more than the potential addict does theschizophrenic show any organic impairment that could classify hisc<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> as a disease state; and, like the addict, the schizophrenic isalso hypersensitive to stimuli and to social signs--above all, to crowding.It would appear probable that both the addict and the schizophrenicare heirs to genetically carried behavioral resp<strong>on</strong>ses that were supremelyadaptive in earlier phases of mankind’s phylogeny when human groupswere smalI, when social stimuli were infinitely fewer, and when acreature’s awareness had to be c<strong>on</strong>stantly alert and finely attuned tosensing danger from the envir<strong>on</strong>ment in order to survive (J<strong>on</strong>as andJ<strong>on</strong>as 1975).A nervous system so exquisitely adapted to perceiving the minutestchanges in envir<strong>on</strong>mental signals clearly becomes overwhelmed andproduces dysphoria when its carrier must exist am<strong>on</strong>g the exp<strong>on</strong>entiallyincreased social stimuli of a modern envir<strong>on</strong>ment. Those individuals274


whose nervous systems are less sensitive and who would surely be atperil in, say, a forest habitat today are better adapted to our morecrowded living c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. The more sensitive can <strong>on</strong>ly attempt toease their discomfort by blunting their percepti<strong>on</strong>s with alcohol ordepressive drugs or, alternatively, by using c<strong>on</strong>sciousness-alteringdrugs to transport their senses from the dysphoric world in whichthey live to private worlds of their own.In the c<strong>on</strong>duct of group therapy am<strong>on</strong>g addicts in c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong> with theU.S. Army’s detoxificati<strong>on</strong> and rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> program, we have observedin practice that the members of these groups c<strong>on</strong>sistently show difficultyin relating to each other. They are plainly uncomfortable being togetherin a group, facing each other, and experiencing the social stimuliimplicit in any close human gathering. Even with those who attempt todissipate their discomfort in drunkenness, it is apparent that theirc<strong>on</strong>viviality or boisterousness does not lead them toward closer interacti<strong>on</strong>swith others but is, rather, a device that shields them from it.The alcoholics’ own belief that they drink to relax their sense oftensi<strong>on</strong> is misleading. What they are doing is blunting their percepti<strong>on</strong>sso that they no l<strong>on</strong>ger resp<strong>on</strong>d to those signals from others or fromwithin themselves that cause them feelings of embarrassment, inadequacy,or shame when they are sober. In doing so they eventually effect ageneral leveling of their mood, and then, paradoxically, the absence ofaffect itself produces an unhed<strong>on</strong>istic and dysphoric state.Today addicts of whatever kind form a sizable segment of the broadspectrum of our populati<strong>on</strong>. As such they provide an available pool ofindividuals that is readily amenable to a reducti<strong>on</strong> of populati<strong>on</strong> densityby reas<strong>on</strong> of their potential for reproductive failure. That is to saythat, although the ability to reproduce may not be impaired in theindividual case of any particular addict, nevertheless the addicti<strong>on</strong> ofitself renders successful mating less probable than for the n<strong>on</strong>addictivepers<strong>on</strong>. Our clinical experience has been that, even where successfulmating does occur am<strong>on</strong>g addicts, the problems which cause and are aresult of their habit tend to make them less able to rear their childrenin a socially satisfactory manner. The children of addicted parentsencounter more problems in social adjustment than most of their c<strong>on</strong>temporaries,making subsequent successful mating difficult for them intheir turn. Thus the potential for eventual reproductive impairmentexists am<strong>on</strong>g addicts, even if perhaps extending over several generati<strong>on</strong>s.And in this vital social functi<strong>on</strong> they may have replaced thatpool of children who in each generati<strong>on</strong> in earlier times were eliminatedby childhood diseases but who are now saved by medical interventi<strong>on</strong>.(The semipermanent state of warfare which is characteristic of ourspecies has not been an element in stabilizing populati<strong>on</strong>s because,until our own time, it has not reduced the female populati<strong>on</strong>. But inearlier times poor hygiene, productive as it was of plague diseasesam<strong>on</strong>g adults as well as adding to the toll of child mortality, was alsoa factor in the automatic spacing of populati<strong>on</strong>s whenever they becametoo dense.) The large increase in stress diseases in modern times andof stress resp<strong>on</strong>ses including anomie, accident pr<strong>on</strong>eness, a possibleincrease in homosexuality, addicti<strong>on</strong>, and so <strong>on</strong> are today probablyalso aspects of the operati<strong>on</strong> of this group mechanism.This bioanthropological overview of the adaptive significance of selfeliminatorybehaviors places these phenomena within the framework of ac<strong>on</strong>text wider than that to which we are accustomed in our professi<strong>on</strong>alc<strong>on</strong>cern for the well-being of the individual. In the process, it forcesup<strong>on</strong> us the necessity of c<strong>on</strong>templating ethicomoral issues--of making a275


decisi<strong>on</strong> as to whether our primary duty is to an individual or to oursociety. To offer an analogy, it is as though a surge<strong>on</strong> were obligedto decide whether to rescue an organ to the possible detriment of thewhole organism. It is a quandary that is currently becoming apparentto widening circles of resp<strong>on</strong>sible scientists. Our comparatively recentawareness of the limited nature of our biosphere and of the closelyinterlocked necessities both of inanimate material and of all forms oflife that sustain and are sustained by it enforces a reevaluati<strong>on</strong> ofmany, perhaps most, of our existing values. This awareness hasproduced a growing number of people committed to such c<strong>on</strong>cerns asenvir<strong>on</strong>mental quality, the preservati<strong>on</strong> of endangered species, thec<strong>on</strong>trol of populati<strong>on</strong>, the wider effects of pesticides, birth c<strong>on</strong>trol,and so <strong>on</strong>. 1In the present-day liberal political and philosophical climate, the interestof the individual reigns supreme. The idea that a society mightsacrifice certain of its individuals for the greater good of the whole isanathema, and we cannot dissociate ourselves from the reality of theprevailing morality. We are of our times, and it is our deepest desireto improve the lot of each and every human being. This does notpreclude the possibility that at some future time other moralities maysupervene.Addicti<strong>on</strong> and similarly dysfuncti<strong>on</strong>al social behavior, then, c<strong>on</strong>stitutepathways al<strong>on</strong>g which certain individuals move toward an exit from thegene pools of their populati<strong>on</strong>s, and the attempt to halt their departureand to encourage them to reverse their course fosters a biologicalparadox .We have seen that the phylogenetic preservati<strong>on</strong> of variety within apopulati<strong>on</strong>, whether of anatomy or behavior, does not <strong>on</strong>ly permit agroup to survive changes in its envir<strong>on</strong>ment. It also provides a groupwith a certain proporti<strong>on</strong> of individuals that it may safely discardwhenever its density exceeds an optimum. Thus we recognize thatthose who become addictive do not have within themselves the behavioralrepertoire that will enable them to move successfully into the mainstreamof the life of their group. They are, so to speak, biologically designedto fulfill a different role.1 We might note that, while it is reas<strong>on</strong>able to assume that c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>sinvolving hypersensitivity to the envir<strong>on</strong>ment may in the past havehad and may yet have adaptive elements, there are genetically determinedphysiological variati<strong>on</strong>s (such as juvenile diabetes mellitus, am<strong>on</strong>gnumerous other genetic abnormalities) that would seem to be n<strong>on</strong>adaptivein any circumstances. Such variati<strong>on</strong>s as these would be eliminatedfrom a populati<strong>on</strong> in a natural state simply by the death before reachingreproductive age of the individuals carrying them. Given the orientati<strong>on</strong>of our Western societies, however, we search for remedies that willallow so-afflicted individuals to live out their lives and perhaps to procreate.The heritable element of their disability may be masked throughseveral subsequent generati<strong>on</strong>s, depending up<strong>on</strong> several factors, includingthe genetic endowment of those with whom succeeding generati<strong>on</strong>smate. It is therefore usually extremely difficult, if not impossible, todetermine precisely the stage at which’maladaptive traits carried geneticallyare finally eliminated from the gene pool of a human populati<strong>on</strong>,although, by their nature, this must eventually occur if the society isto c<strong>on</strong>tinue in existence.276


In our individual-oriented, liberal society, such a c<strong>on</strong>cept, however, isunacceptable. Humanitarian principles impel c<strong>on</strong>cerned professi<strong>on</strong>als todevote all available resources to the task of rehabilitati<strong>on</strong>. As humanitarianswe ourselves (the authors) are also involved in such an endeavor.As biologists, however, we have to see that a remedy for addicti<strong>on</strong>does not lie in the realm of treatment for the individual but, rather,in a broader understanding of the ecological needs of the society as awhole. Unless we see to it that steps are taken to prevent overpopulati<strong>on</strong>,if not addicti<strong>on</strong> then other social mechanisms will emerge that willhave the effect of eliminating individuals, and a new set of problemswill then have to be faced.277


Emerging C<strong>on</strong>ceptsC<strong>on</strong>cerning <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>William R. Martin, M.D.PHARMACOLOGIC REDUNDANCYWhen I first arrived at the Addicti<strong>on</strong> Research Center in the fall of1957, I knew little about problems of drug abuse and of the pharmacologyof abused drugs. I came to the Addicti<strong>on</strong> Research Center withsome knowledge of neuropharmacology and neurotransmitters, and ahigh level of interest in drug receptor interacti<strong>on</strong>s. I also had aninterest in the limbic cortex and its interacti<strong>on</strong>s with the aut<strong>on</strong>omicnervous system and the EEG. I was particularly interested at thattime in the role of both descending and ascending catecholaminergicand cho<strong>line</strong>rgic paths in EEG activati<strong>on</strong> and vasomotor resp<strong>on</strong>ses, andin characterizing alterati<strong>on</strong>s of physiologic resp<strong>on</strong>ses with both stimulusresp<strong>on</strong>se and dose resp<strong>on</strong>se parameters.Results that I obtained with these studies of atropine led me to c<strong>on</strong>ceiveof the principle of pharmacologic redundancy as a mechanism of bothtolerance and dependence (Martin and Eades 1960). Subsequently, Igeneralized my thoughts c<strong>on</strong>cerning redundant processes in the centralnervous system (Martin 1970) and entertained the possibility thatpresynaptic elements might c<strong>on</strong>tain more than <strong>on</strong>e transmitter and thatthe postsynaptic neur<strong>on</strong> might have more than <strong>on</strong>e type of receptor.Further, our data <strong>on</strong> the effects of atropine <strong>on</strong> EEG activati<strong>on</strong> andvasomotor resp<strong>on</strong>ses suggested that parallel pathways c<strong>on</strong>tained avariety of synaptic mechanisms and that when <strong>on</strong>e synaptic processwas impaired, another parallel process using different synaptic mechanismscould assume the functi<strong>on</strong> of the impaired synaptic system.Subsequently data were obtained for the cotransmitters by others.HOMEOSTASISHimmelsbach’s (1943) c<strong>on</strong>cept of compensatory homeostatic mechanismsas an explanati<strong>on</strong> for both tolerance and dependence was the basis ofseveral experiments, and we were able to show that indeed homeostaticmechanisms played a role in both acute and chr<strong>on</strong>ic tolerance and278


physical dependence. The accumulati<strong>on</strong> of carb<strong>on</strong> dioxide was shownto be <strong>on</strong>e of the mechanisms involved in the acute diminuti<strong>on</strong> of morphine’sdepressant effects <strong>on</strong> respirati<strong>on</strong> in the acute decerebrate cat(Martin and Eisenman 1962), and panting induced by morphine in thedog was due to an alterati<strong>on</strong> of the temperature set point. Further,dissipati<strong>on</strong> of body heat was resp<strong>on</strong>sible for acute tolerance to morphineinducedpanting (Martin 1968). We further dem<strong>on</strong>strated in patientswho were physically dependent <strong>on</strong> morphine that the partial pressureof carb<strong>on</strong> dioxide minute-volume stimulus resp<strong>on</strong>se curve was shifted tothe left indicating that the respiratory set point had been sensitized toCO 2 as a c<strong>on</strong>sequence of chr<strong>on</strong>ic morphine administrati<strong>on</strong>. This, to myknowledge, was the first experimental evidence that a homeostatic setpoint could be altered as the c<strong>on</strong>sequence of chr<strong>on</strong>ic administrati<strong>on</strong> ofnarcotics (Martin et al. 1968).MULTIPLE OPIOID RECEPTORSIn 1967, I initiated studies reevaluating the abuse potentiality of themixed ag<strong>on</strong>ists/antag<strong>on</strong>ists, cyclazocine and nalorphine (Martin et al.1965; Martin and Gorodetzky 1965), which were to have far-reachingimpacts. We also initiated studies with a new antag<strong>on</strong>ist with questi<strong>on</strong>ableanalgesic activity, nalox<strong>on</strong>e (Jasinski et al. 1967). As a c<strong>on</strong>sequenceof the study of nalox<strong>on</strong>e, which proved to be an antag<strong>on</strong>istwithout ag<strong>on</strong>istic activity, and with the results that we obtained withcyclazocine and nalorphine, we made several speculati<strong>on</strong>s (Martin1967):1. We felt that for the first time, unequivocal evidence had beenobtained that morphine-like drugs were acting as ag<strong>on</strong>ists.2. The acti<strong>on</strong> of mixed ag<strong>on</strong>ist/antag<strong>on</strong>ists such as cyclazocine andnalorphine could not be explained <strong>on</strong> the basis of their interactingwith a single (morphine) receptor and we postulated that therewas another receptor (nalorphine).3. We felt that some of the ag<strong>on</strong>ists/antag<strong>on</strong>ists were acting as partialag<strong>on</strong>ists.4. The possibility of a naturally occurring ag<strong>on</strong>ist was entertained.We reas<strong>on</strong>ed,In attempting to explain the c<strong>on</strong>trastimulatory properties ofthe opioid antag<strong>on</strong>ist, <strong>on</strong>e is forced to rec<strong>on</strong>sider the natureof the ag<strong>on</strong>istic acti<strong>on</strong>s of narcotic analgesics. <strong>On</strong>e canassume, for argument sake, that opioids mimic a naturally<strong>on</strong>going process. If this hypothesis is true, then it wouldnot be unreas<strong>on</strong>able to assume that those antag<strong>on</strong>ists withlow intrinsic activity would antag<strong>on</strong>ize not <strong>on</strong>ly morphineinducedactivity, but the naturally <strong>on</strong>going activity that issimilar in nature to the effects of morphine, with the resultthat an antimorphine effect would become manifest.(Martin 1967, p. 508)For a time, I became involved in other pharmacologic problems, principallythe issue of whether tryptamine was a neurotransmitter, anddid not return to the issue of multiple opioid receptors again until Paul279


Gilbert joined my laboratory as a graduate student. In the interim,however, Dr. Jasinski and I reinvestigated the abuse potentiality ofpentazocine (Jasinski et al. 1970). In these studies we found thatalthough pentazocine appeared to produce morphine-like subjective,effects in small to moderate doses, it would not suppress the morphineabstinence syndrome. This observati<strong>on</strong> disturbed me and raised seriousquesti<strong>on</strong>s c<strong>on</strong>cerning the two-receptor theory of opioid acti<strong>on</strong>. WhenGilbert initiated his thesis work (Gilbert and Martin 1976). we decidedto reinvestigate the pharmacology of N-allylnormetazocine (SKF 10047),a benzmorphan derivative that had been studied by Keats and Telford(1964) and found to have a high degree of psychotomimetic and dysphoricactivity. We had also known that high doses of nalox<strong>on</strong>e wererequired to antag<strong>on</strong>ize the effects of cyclazocine, in both the dog(McClane and Martin 1967) and in humans (Jasinski et al. 1968). Wealso studied ketocyclazocine and ethylketocyclazocine, which <strong>on</strong> thebasis of work <strong>on</strong> the guinea pig ileum appeared to be str<strong>on</strong>g ag<strong>on</strong>istsdevoid of antag<strong>on</strong>ist activity for which nalox<strong>on</strong>e was a relatively impotentantag<strong>on</strong>ist (Kosterlitz et al. 1973). From the results of thesestudies, it became quite apparent that we were mistaken in thinkingthat there were <strong>on</strong>ly two opioid receptors, and it was thus necessaryto postulate a third receptor. We renamed the receptors µ (for morphine),κ (for ketocyclazocine), and σ (for SKF 10047) and felt thatthese three receptors were respectively resp<strong>on</strong>sible for the euphorigenic,sedative, and dysphoric acti<strong>on</strong>s of the mixed ag<strong>on</strong>ists/antag<strong>on</strong>ists. Wealso had obtained c<strong>on</strong>vincing evidence that buprenorphine was a partialag<strong>on</strong>ist of morphine in the dog (Martin et al. 1976). Thus, it becameapparent that the term ag<strong>on</strong>ist/antag<strong>on</strong>ist had two meanings: (1) apartial ag<strong>on</strong>ist and (2) ag<strong>on</strong>istic acti<strong>on</strong> at <strong>on</strong>e receptor and antag<strong>on</strong>istor partial ag<strong>on</strong>istic acti<strong>on</strong> at another receptor. These were the firstreceptors that were identified and differentiated <strong>on</strong> the basis of clinicaland neuropharmacologic studies.NARCOTIC ANTAGONIST IN THE TREATMENTOF HEROIN DEPENDENCEIn our studies of cyclazocine and nalorphine (Martin et al. 1965;Martin and Gorodetzky 1965) we observed that tolerance developed tothe subjective effects produced by cyclazocine and nalorphine and thatfollowing withdrawal of cyclazocine-dependent subjects, the abstinencesyndrome had a l<strong>on</strong>g latency of <strong>on</strong>set. Based <strong>on</strong> these observati<strong>on</strong>sand theoretical c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s, we speculated that tolerance developedto cyclazocine’s ag<strong>on</strong>istic acti<strong>on</strong>s but not its antag<strong>on</strong>istic effect. Wesubsequently c<strong>on</strong>firmed these speculati<strong>on</strong>s. I was privileged in beingat the Addicti<strong>on</strong> Research Center at a time when Dr. Abraham Wiklerwas evolving his ideas of c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed abstinence and c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed drugseekingbehavior. We c<strong>on</strong>sidered the possibility that if patients weremade tolerant to the ag<strong>on</strong>istic effects of cyclazocine, its prevailingantag<strong>on</strong>istic effects might allow the extincti<strong>on</strong> of these two types ofc<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing. The effects of heroin would be abolished, and thuscould not be reinforcing by virtue of its producing feelings of wellbeingor inducing physical dependence. In any event, we did stabilizepatients <strong>on</strong> high doses of cyclazocine and found that it not <strong>on</strong>ly blockedthe effects of large doses of morphine and heroin but also preventedsubjects from becoming physically dependent when morphine was administeredchr<strong>on</strong>ically in high doses (Martin et al. 1966). Cyclazocine wasgiven a clinical trial by Dr. Alfred Freedman of New York Medical280


College and Dr. Jerome Jaffe, then of Albert Einstein College of Medicine.Both initiated studies of the utility of antag<strong>on</strong>ist therapy in heroinaddicts. Cyclazocine was disappointing in that it was not well acceptedby addicts. In this regard, it should be menti<strong>on</strong>ed that the administrati<strong>on</strong>,clinical investigators, and scientists of Sterling Winthrop weresupportive of these investigati<strong>on</strong>s. It was felt that perhaps the dysphoriceffects of cyclazocine might have been resp<strong>on</strong>sible for the lackof acceptance of it by addicts. <strong>On</strong> the basis of our studies withnalox<strong>on</strong>e and cyclazocine, we speculated that naltrex<strong>on</strong>e, which ischemically related to both nalox<strong>on</strong>e and cyclazocine, might be a pureantag<strong>on</strong>ist with a l<strong>on</strong>g durati<strong>on</strong> of acti<strong>on</strong>. Indeed, the basic studies ofBlumberg et al. (1967) indicated that it was a pure antag<strong>on</strong>ist. Throughthe cooperati<strong>on</strong> of Drs. Harold Blumberg, Ralph Jacobs<strong>on</strong>, and IrwinPachter, all then of Endo Pharmaceutical, we were able to initiatestudies with naltrex<strong>on</strong>e in humans and found indeed that it was a pureantag<strong>on</strong>ist and that it had a sufficiently l<strong>on</strong>g durati<strong>on</strong> of acti<strong>on</strong> toproduce a high degree of antag<strong>on</strong>ism of morphine when administered<strong>on</strong>ce a day orally.Indeed, naltrex<strong>on</strong>e has turned out to be a pure antag<strong>on</strong>ist which has al<strong>on</strong>g durati<strong>on</strong> of acti<strong>on</strong>. It should be introduced into clinical medicineas the drug of choice for the treatment of narcotic overdose. Inadditi<strong>on</strong>, it is a most satisfactory drug for antag<strong>on</strong>ist therapy ofheroin dependence.PROTRACTED ABSTINENCEIn the early 1960s, at a time when Dr. Wikler was well into his studiesof c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed abstinence in the rat and at a time when Dr. Eisenman,Mrs. Sloan, and I were trying to dissect out the role of catecholaminesin the morphine abstinence syndrome, it became apparent to us thatmany of the dimensi<strong>on</strong>s of tolerance and physical dependence <strong>on</strong> morphinein the rat were not well established, such as the rate of <strong>on</strong>setand particularly the durati<strong>on</strong> of the abstinence syndrome (Martin et al.1963). We thus initiated a study of morphine dependence in the ratand, to our surprise, found that the abstinence syndrome had twophases, an early and a late <strong>on</strong>e, that were quite different. AlthoughI maintained an interest in this problem, I did not return to it forseveral years. In 1967, we initiated a l<strong>on</strong>g-term reinvestigati<strong>on</strong> ofmorphine dependence in humans (Martin and Jasinski 1969) and foundthat indeed humans also exhibited both an early and a protractedabstinence syndrome. However, the signs of protracted abstinencesyndrome were small in magnitude and, although dem<strong>on</strong>strable in anexperimental setting using a paired comparis<strong>on</strong>, could not be identifiedor diagnosed <strong>on</strong> the basis of physiologic abnormalities in a clinicalsetting. With the introducti<strong>on</strong> of methad<strong>on</strong>e maintenance, it was decidedto reinvestigate both the short- and the l<strong>on</strong>g-term effects of methad<strong>on</strong>emaintenance under carefully c<strong>on</strong>trolled experimental c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. Previousstudies of protracted abstinence were extended by making three additi<strong>on</strong>almeasures: (1) the psychometric changes that occur during acycle of addicti<strong>on</strong>, (2) the effects of a cycle of addicti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> EEG andsleep, and (3) the effects of addicti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> horm<strong>on</strong>al functi<strong>on</strong>. By farthe most exciting results that were obtained were with regard to thepsychologic changes. It was found that during chr<strong>on</strong>ic methad<strong>on</strong>eadministrati<strong>on</strong> negative feeling states prevailed and that these wereexacerbated and persisted through both early abstinence and protractedabstinence (Martin et al. 1973). We then initiated study of protracted281


abstinence in the dog and extended our observati<strong>on</strong>s by determiningthe resp<strong>on</strong>sivity to nociceptive stimuli during a cycle of morphinedependence (Martin et al. 1974). In these studies, it was found thatthe dog also exhibited a protracted abstinence syndrome and thatduring protracted abstinence resp<strong>on</strong>sivity to str<strong>on</strong>g nociceptive stimuliwas enhanced.THE PSYCHOPATHOLOGY OF THENARCOTIC ADDICTThe results we obtained in humans and in the dog during protractedabstinence suggested that protracted abstinence was associated with anexacerbati<strong>on</strong> of feelings of hypophoria and that these feelings of hypophoriamight be associated with an increased need state.These c<strong>on</strong>cepts of an affective state that was present in addicts becameclarified. In my clinical experience with addicts who had participatedin studies <strong>on</strong> the ward of the Addicti<strong>on</strong> Research Center, I recognizeda number of diatheses, the most prominent of which were feelings ofpoor self-image and unpopularity. Several investigators had observedthat drug abusers had had elevati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> the depressi<strong>on</strong> and the psychopathicdeviate scales of the MMPI, yet <strong>on</strong> the basis of experiences <strong>on</strong>the ward of the Addicti<strong>on</strong> Research Center few patients showed anysigns or symptoms comm<strong>on</strong>ly associated with depressi<strong>on</strong>. Table 1c<strong>on</strong>trasts the feelings of euphoria, hypophoria, and depressi<strong>on</strong>. Ascan be seen, hypophoria is in many areas the polar opposite of euphoria,being associated with feelings of unpopularity, being unappreciated,ineptness, and inefficiency, whereas patients under the influence ofeuphoria-producing drugs such as morphine-like narcotic analgesics,amphetamine-like agents, LSD-like hallucinogens, and barbiturates feelpopular, liked, appreciated, competent, and efficient. Howeverpatients who have feelings of hypophoria can readily be differentiatedfrom depressed patients in that they feel hopeful, worthy, can experiencejoy, can laugh, and feel guiltless. It became apparent that moreinformati<strong>on</strong> was needed to establish the c<strong>on</strong>cept of hypophoria as aunique and pathologic affective state and to begin speculati<strong>on</strong>s aboutTABLE 1.–Characteristics of euphoric, hypophoric,and depressive feelingsEuphoriaPopularLikedAppreciatedCompetentEfficientHopefulWorthyCan experience joyCan laughHypophoriaUnpopularUnappreciatedIneptInefficientHopefulWorthyCan experience joyCan laughGuiltless282HopelessUnworthyDepressi<strong>on</strong>Cannot experience joyCannot laughGuilty


the nature of this hypophoria. Our studies <strong>on</strong> protracted abstinencehad already indicated that l<strong>on</strong>g-term exposure to opiates gave rise topersisting and enhanced hypophoric feelings. We felt there was someevidence that suggested that these feelings might be related in someway to exaggerated need states which in turn were related to increasedegocentricity. Hypophoria, exaggerated need states, and egocentricityincrease the probability that individuals will have antisocial feelingsand exhibit impulsivity. With this theoretical basis, a maturati<strong>on</strong> scalewas c<strong>on</strong>structed that had items that were related to egocentricity,characterized by selfishness, inability to love, and callousness; impulsivebehavior, characterized by thoughtlessness and uninhibited behavior;a need scale, related to sexual desires, hunger, body health,pain, and general wanting; a hypophoria scale, related to a negativepercepti<strong>on</strong> of life, of poor self-image, feelings of being disrespected,disapproved of, and unappreciated, as well as feelings of ineffeciencyand ineptness, withdrawal from competiti<strong>on</strong>, worry, and anger; andfinally an antisocial scale c<strong>on</strong>sisting of items relating to antisocialfeelings, feelings of n<strong>on</strong>c<strong>on</strong>formity, poor judgment, and lack of socialc<strong>on</strong>cern (Martin et al. 1977).To study further the possibility that addicts and alcoholics might haveexaggerated need states, we compared a group of alcoholics and addictpris<strong>on</strong>ers with a group of n<strong>on</strong>sociopathic c<strong>on</strong>trol subjects. The maturati<strong>on</strong>scale and MMPI was administered to these subjects and a detailedhistory of antisocial behavior was obtained. In additi<strong>on</strong>, plasma levelsof follicle-stimulating and luteinizing horm<strong>on</strong>es and testoster<strong>on</strong>e weremeasured. It was found that the alcoholics, pris<strong>on</strong>ers, and addictshad significantly elevated levels of luteinizing horm<strong>on</strong>es and testoster<strong>on</strong>eas well as significant elevati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> the impulsivity, egocentricity,need, hypophoria, sociopathy, and maturati<strong>on</strong> scales. These findingswere supportive of the c<strong>on</strong>cept of exaggerated need states and of anaffective disorder being of importance in drug abusers and alcoholicsand that pers<strong>on</strong>s with a character disorder which manifested itself inan antisocial pers<strong>on</strong>ality could have a biologic pathology.NEUROTRANSMITTERS AND SUBJECTIVE STATEPart of the Addicti<strong>on</strong> Research Center’s effort was to develop predictorsof the abuse potential of psychoactive drugs. Drs. Harris Hill andCharles Haertzen developed a 550-item questi<strong>on</strong>naire that was especiallyuseful in identifying and characterizing the subjective effects of drugs.Much of this work has been summarized by Haertzen (1966). Am<strong>on</strong>gthe scales that were developed by Haertzen, the MBG scale (morphinebenzedrine group scale) proved to be the most useful measure of theeuphorigenic acti<strong>on</strong>s of drugs. Many items <strong>on</strong> this scale related tofeelings of well-being, popularity, and efficiency, and in this regardwere the polar opposites of the hypophoric subjective state. Amphetamine(Martin et al. 1971), narcotic analgesics (Jasinski et al. 1971),and pentobarbital (McClane and Martin 1976) caused dose-relatedelevati<strong>on</strong>s of MBG scale scores. This informati<strong>on</strong> was interpreted asindicating that morphine, amphetamine, and pentobarbital may be drugsthat were used by patients as an antidote for their hypophoric feelingsand to produce feelings of well-being.Other drugs that will produce feelings of well-being include the LSDlikehallucinogens. A large number of investigators have dem<strong>on</strong>stratedthat many of the acti<strong>on</strong>s of the amphetamine-like drugs are attributable283


to their ability to release dopamine. The narcotic analgesics arethought to mimic the enkephalins and endorphins. The LSD-likehallucinogens act both as serot<strong>on</strong>inergic and tryptaminergic ag<strong>on</strong>ists.Benzodiazepines, which also produce feelings of well-being, arethought to interact with a brain receptor; however, a natural ag<strong>on</strong>isthas not been identified. Thus there is reas<strong>on</strong> to believe that there isa neurochemistry and neurophysiology of euphoria and that a varietyof neurotransmitters, including catecholamines; the endorphins; theenkephalins; and the indoleamines, serot<strong>on</strong>in and tryptamine, may allplay a role in maintaining mood. Further deficiencies of these neurotransmittersmay give rise to feelings of hypophoria.CONCLUSIONSIt is now apparent that the brain has a variety of receptors andseveral neurotransmitters that are involved in feelings of well-being.Further many addicts and alcoholics have an affective disorder, hypophoria,that appears to be the polar opposite of feelings of well-beingproduced by drugs of abuse. The pathophysiology of hypophoria isnot known. A deficiency of neurotransmitters that are involved infeelings of well-being is a reas<strong>on</strong>able hypothesis that should be testable.It is known that the protracted abstinence syndrome, associated withmorphine physical dependence, is characterized by an exacerbati<strong>on</strong> offeelings of hypophoria. Genetic and heredity factors may also be ofimportance. Further, hypophoria may have a reactive comp<strong>on</strong>ent,possibly related to exaggerated needs and drives particularly duringadolescence and young adulthood, a time when social coping skills arenot fully developed.Thus work <strong>on</strong> problems of addicti<strong>on</strong> over some 20 years has led tosome interesting speculati<strong>on</strong>s about the psychopathology and pathophysiologyof drug abuse and to some innovati<strong>on</strong>s in the area of treatment.It was at first blush disappointing that the narcotic antag<strong>on</strong>ists hadsuch a poor patient acceptance. In retrospect this should have beenanticipated, for the narcotic antag<strong>on</strong>ists do not in any way relieve thehypophoric feelings of patients. This in no way detracts from thevalidity of the c<strong>on</strong>cepts of Wikler c<strong>on</strong>cerning the role of c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing inrelapse, for hypophoria and c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed abstinence and drug-seekingbehavior are probably coexisting pathologies. If treatment is to beoptimized,in all probability both will have to be dealt with. It is myc<strong>on</strong>victi<strong>on</strong> at this time that extincti<strong>on</strong> of c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed abstinence anddrug-seeking behavior using antag<strong>on</strong>ist therapy will be better acceptedby patients whose hypophoria has been decreased. <strong>On</strong>e of the fundamentalquesti<strong>on</strong>s is how we can develop antihypophoric drugs whichwill not induce tolerance andlor dependence and not exacerbate existinghypophoria. Perhaps in this regard we have attended too much to theearly abstinence syndrome and not enough to the pathophysiology ofthe protracted abstinence syndrome.There seems little questi<strong>on</strong> now that a variety of neurotransmitters andreceptors are involved in affective disorders. It thus should bepossible to identify ag<strong>on</strong>ists which when administered under appropriatecircumstances should be able to relieve feelings of hypophoria and thusrectify this pathologic situati<strong>on</strong>.This may represent a radical departure from current strategies in drugdevelopment for it is aimed at developing drugs that will be highly284


einforcing to patients suffering from hypophoria but which will neitherexacerbate their disease nor be toxic.285


Somatosensory Affecti<strong>on</strong>alDeprivati<strong>on</strong> (SAD) Theoryof <strong>Drug</strong> and Alcohol UseJames W. Prescott, Ph.D.The somatosensory affecti<strong>on</strong>al deprivati<strong>on</strong> (SAD) theory of drug andalcohol use is a developmental psychobiological theory that is proposedto account for the comm<strong>on</strong> ground of the many and diverse theories ofsubstance abuse. The first basic propositi<strong>on</strong> of this theory is that theneurobiology of our behavior is not <strong>on</strong>ly inseparable from, but is infact largely shaped by, culture. The shaping process of culture up<strong>on</strong>the developing brain (the organ of behavior) is accomplished throughour various sensory modalities and through the sensory processes ofdeprivati<strong>on</strong> and stimulati<strong>on</strong>.With few excepti<strong>on</strong>s, the developing mammalian brain, particularly theprimate brain, is highly immature at birth and is dependent up<strong>on</strong>sensory stimulati<strong>on</strong> for its normal growth, development, and functi<strong>on</strong>aland structural organizati<strong>on</strong>. The richness or paucity of dendriticstructures of the neur<strong>on</strong>e (brain cell), for example, is largely influencedby the sensory processes of stimulati<strong>on</strong> and deprivati<strong>on</strong> duringthe formative periods of brain development. The complexities andpossibilities of neur<strong>on</strong>al communicati<strong>on</strong> (and thus behavior) are dependentup<strong>on</strong> the complexity of dendritic structures of brain cells (Greenough1975; Greenough and Juraska 1979; Rosenzweig 1979; Floeter andGreenough 1979; Riesen 1975; Globus et al. 1973; Coss and Globus1979; Coleman and Riesen 1968; Horn et al. 1979; Spinelli and Jensen1979; Blakemore and Cooper 1970; Hirsch and Spinelli 1970; and Hubeland Wiesel 1970). Dendritic structures are analogous to teleph<strong>on</strong>ecables that interc<strong>on</strong>nect various teleph<strong>on</strong>e centers (brain cells) with<strong>on</strong>e another. These dendritic structures of brain cells form the structuralbasis of interneur<strong>on</strong>al communicati<strong>on</strong>. Another major element inthe story of interneur<strong>on</strong>al communicati<strong>on</strong> is neurochemical transmittersubstances which are present at synaptic juncti<strong>on</strong>s between dendritesand which make possible the transfer of “informati<strong>on</strong>” from <strong>on</strong>e braincell to another. These events are accompanied by electrophysiologicalactivity, which is another manifestati<strong>on</strong> of interneur<strong>on</strong>al communicati<strong>on</strong>.The point of this synoptic overview of interneur<strong>on</strong>al communicati<strong>on</strong> isto emphasize that the morphological (structural) and the neurochemicaland electrophysiological (functi<strong>on</strong>al) processes of interneur<strong>on</strong>al286


communicati<strong>on</strong> are all str<strong>on</strong>gly influenced by the sensory processes ofstimulati<strong>on</strong> and deprivati<strong>on</strong>. Thus, the effects of the social, physical,and cultural envir<strong>on</strong>ment are ultimately transformed into perceptualexperiences through the encoding and decoding of sensory processes.Further, whether certain perceptual experiences can ever be realizedwill be dependent up<strong>on</strong> the quality and quantity of our sensory experiences,as structured by our social, physical, and cultural envir<strong>on</strong>mentduring the formative periods of brain development (Prescott 1967,1971a,b, 1972a,b, 1973, 1975, 1976a,b, 1977, 1978, 1979b).The sec<strong>on</strong>d basic propositi<strong>on</strong> of SAD theory is that certain sensorymodalities and processes are more important than others in accountingfor emoti<strong>on</strong>al/social disturbances and substance abuse. Specifically, itis the emoti<strong>on</strong>al senses of somesthesis (touch), vestibulati<strong>on</strong> (movement),and olfacti<strong>on</strong> (smell), that are the primary mediators of our emoti<strong>on</strong>al/affective behaviors. Substance abuse that alters primarily our emoti<strong>on</strong>al/affectivestate must be understood within the c<strong>on</strong>text of ouremoti<strong>on</strong>al senses. It is the deprivati<strong>on</strong> of our emoti<strong>on</strong>al senses and notour cognitive (visual-auditory) senses during the formative periods ofbrain development that can account for and predict our emoti<strong>on</strong>al/affectivesocial behaviors, which include not <strong>on</strong>ly substance abuse butabusive social behaviors in general. Thus, the questi<strong>on</strong> of destructiveand exploitive behaviors toward ourselves and others becomes a questi<strong>on</strong>of whether affecti<strong>on</strong>al b<strong>on</strong>ds are formed or not formed during theformative periods of brain development. Within an evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary c<strong>on</strong>text,it should be noted that olfacti<strong>on</strong> assumes a greater role in lower mammals,and vestibular functi<strong>on</strong>s assume a greater role in higher mammalianforms, specifically the primate, in the formati<strong>on</strong> of affecti<strong>on</strong>al b<strong>on</strong>ds(Prescott 1976a, 1977). Similarly, substance abuse that alters primarilyour cognitive state (e.g., hallucinogens) must be understood withinthe c<strong>on</strong>text of our cognitive (visual/auditory) senses. It should benoted that movement (vestibulati<strong>on</strong>) is often involved in altered cognitivestates and it has been proposed that the vestibular-cerebellarneuraxis may be a master integrating/regulating system of sensoryemoti<strong>on</strong>aland motor processes. Thus, the vestibular-cerebellar systemmay serve as a “bridge” between our “emoti<strong>on</strong>al” and “cognitive”senses (Prescott 1976a, 1977; Erway 1975).In previous studies, the SAD theory has been successful in predictingphysical violence (high and low) in 100 percent of 49 primitive culturesdistributed throughout the world. This was made possible by evaluatingthe degree of physical affecti<strong>on</strong> (touching, holding, carrying) of theinfant by its mother or caretakers and by the degree of physicalaffecti<strong>on</strong> that was permitted to be expressed through the acceptance orrejecti<strong>on</strong> of premarital sexuality (Prescott 1975, 1977, 1979b).The issue of violence, i.e., the failure of nurturance and the failureto form affecti<strong>on</strong>al b<strong>on</strong>ds, is str<strong>on</strong>gly related to the issue of substanceabuse in several aspects. First, in a very general sense, the bodyneeds and “searches” for a state of harm<strong>on</strong>y, c<strong>on</strong>tentment, and inhigher life forms (homo sapiens), an altered and transcendent state ofc<strong>on</strong>scious “being.” A necessary c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> for the attainment of this“state of being” is the experiencing of physical (somatosensory) pleasurethat is essential for the formati<strong>on</strong> of affecti<strong>on</strong>al b<strong>on</strong>ds. When somatosensorypleasure and affecti<strong>on</strong>al b<strong>on</strong>ds are denied, then compensatorybehaviors to reduce tensi<strong>on</strong>, discomfort, and “anomie” become imperative.The comm<strong>on</strong> compensatory behaviors are physical violence (towardothers and <strong>on</strong>eself), alcoholism and drug abuse, and perseverativestimulus-seeking behaviors that attempt to provide the sensory287


stimulati<strong>on</strong> that was deprived early in life. The stereotypical rockingbehaviors of isolati<strong>on</strong>-reared Harlow m<strong>on</strong>keys and of instituti<strong>on</strong>alizedchildren is a case in point. The “quieting” effect of stimulant drugsup<strong>on</strong> some hyperactive children is another illustrati<strong>on</strong> of a “need forneural activati<strong>on</strong>” that is met by pharmacological stimulati<strong>on</strong> ratherthan by sensory stimulati<strong>on</strong>. The chr<strong>on</strong>ic stimulus-seeking behaviors,particularly of a sexual and violent nature, in the American culture(evidenced, for example, by massage parlors, pornography, violentfilms, rape) are also illustrative of this basic principle of stimulusseekingbehaviors c<strong>on</strong>sequent to early somatosensory deprivati<strong>on</strong>(Prescott 1972a, 1973, 1975, 1976a,b). Additi<strong>on</strong>al studies that relateearly sensory experiences to later behaviors, particularly aberrantsensory behaviors, can be usefully c<strong>on</strong>sulted (Ainsworth 1972; Cairns1966, 1972; Bowlby 1969; Harlow 1971; Harlow et al. 1963; Dokecki1973; Lichstein and Sackett 1971; Lynch 1970; Mas<strong>on</strong> 1968, 1971; Mas<strong>on</strong>and Kenney 1974; Mas<strong>on</strong> and Berks<strong>on</strong> 1975; Fuller 1967; Freedman1968; Friedman et al. 1968; Melzack and Burns 1965; Melzack andThomps<strong>on</strong> 1956; Melzack and Scott 1957; Mitchell 1968, 1970, 1975;Mitchell and Clark 1966; Sackett 1970; Riesen 1960, 1961a,b, 1965;Schaffer and Emers<strong>on</strong> 1964a,b; Spitz 1945, 1965; Suomi and Harlow1972; Zubek 1969).The self-mutilati<strong>on</strong> and pain agnosia of children characterized bypsychosocial dwarfism c<strong>on</strong>sequent to somatosensory affecti<strong>on</strong>al deprivati<strong>on</strong>and child abuse reported by M<strong>on</strong>ey et al. (1972), is a classicverificati<strong>on</strong> at the human level of the same behaviors (self-mutilati<strong>on</strong>and pain agnosia) found in animals reared under c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s of somatosensoryaffecti<strong>on</strong>al deprivati<strong>on</strong> (social isolati<strong>on</strong>) (Lichstein and Sackett1971; Melzack and Burns 1965; Melzack and Scott 1957; and Mitchell1968, 1970, 1975). The pain aqnosia of children subjected to physicalrestraint and immobilizatibn reported by Friedman et al. (1968) isanother dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> of these relati<strong>on</strong>ships at the human level.Another important dimensi<strong>on</strong> to these early experiences and behaviorsis the neurochemical and neuroendocrine mediators of pain hypersensitivityand pain hyposensitivity (pain agnosia) c<strong>on</strong>sequent tosomatosensory deprivati<strong>on</strong>. Harvey and Yunger (1973) have shownthat decreases in brain serot<strong>on</strong>in (-5-HT) result in an increased sensitivityto pain, and Coleman (1971) has shown that isolati<strong>on</strong>-rearedm<strong>on</strong>keys who are characterized by both tactile hypersensitivity andhyposensitivity (Lichstein and Sackett 1971) have significantly decreasedlevels of platelet serot<strong>on</strong>in.A number of investigators have also shown that there is significantreducti<strong>on</strong> in growth horm<strong>on</strong>e (GH) and adrenocorticotropin (ACTH) inpsychosocial dwarfism (reversible hyposomatotropism) (Patt<strong>on</strong> andGardner 1975; Powell et al. 1967a,b; Wolff and M<strong>on</strong>ey 1973; M<strong>on</strong>ey andWolff 1974; Brown 1976). Significant to these findings is the reportthat endoqenous opioids are involved in the regulati<strong>on</strong> of serum growthhorm<strong>on</strong>e (GH) and prolactin (PRL). Specifically, nalox<strong>on</strong>e depressesbasal serum c<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong> of GH and PRL. Related to the above arethe well-known phenomena that stress elicits an increase of endogenousopioids in the brain; and of ACTH and ß-endorphin in the systemiccirculati<strong>on</strong>; and that serot<strong>on</strong>in increases prolactin, growth horm<strong>on</strong>e,and adrenocorticotropin (Meites et al. 1979).These observati<strong>on</strong>s are made to suggest that psychosocial dwarfism maywell be characterized by abnormal endorphin mechanisms which may beresp<strong>on</strong>sible for the observed abnormalities of GH and ACTH in288


psychosocial dwarfism. Thus, these speculati<strong>on</strong>s suggest that endorphinmechanisms may assume a much greater role and significance in somatosensoryaffecti<strong>on</strong>al deprivati<strong>on</strong> phenomena than has heretofore beenrealized.The findings of Behling (1979) highlight the relati<strong>on</strong>ship betweenalcohol abuse, child abuse, and failure of nurturance, showing that in69 percent of 51 instances of child abuse, at least <strong>on</strong>e parent had ahistory of alcohol abuse.In the c<strong>on</strong>text of the SAD theory, it is not surprising to find thecompensatory behaviors of violence in the primitive culture study citedabove or the finding of Barry (1976) that the single greatest predictorof drunkenness in 13 primitive cultures was the large amount of cryingduring infancy (r=0.77) Drunkenness was also significantly correlated.with low general indulgence during infancy (r =0.40; N=26) and lowdurati<strong>on</strong> of bodily c<strong>on</strong>tact with caretaker during later stages of infancy(r=0.42; N=23). Significant relati<strong>on</strong>ships between deprivati<strong>on</strong> of parentalphysical affecti<strong>on</strong> and use of drugs and alcohol have been reported forcollege students (Prescott 1975), for pris<strong>on</strong>ers (Prescott and Wallace1978), for instituti<strong>on</strong>alized alcoholics, and for participants in a drugtreatment program (Prescott and Wallace 1976). Significant relati<strong>on</strong>shipsbetween high drug and alcohol usage with attitudes rejecting premaritaland extramarital sex have also been reported for college students(Prescott 1975).An interpretive statement of these relati<strong>on</strong>ships with respect to somatosensorypleasure seeking, isolati<strong>on</strong> rearing (somatosensory affecti<strong>on</strong>aldeprivati<strong>on</strong>), altered neur<strong>on</strong>al communicati<strong>on</strong>, and altered states of“c<strong>on</strong>sciousness” appears necessary. Briefly, the SAD theory postulatesthat somatosensory deprivati<strong>on</strong> from isolati<strong>on</strong> rearing leads to impairedbrain neur<strong>on</strong>al systems that mediate pleasure which now lack theneur<strong>on</strong>al structural bases to interact with and influence higher brain(cognitive) centers (neocortex). This impairment prevents an integrati<strong>on</strong>of somatosensory pleasure with higher brain centers and precludesthe normal development of altered states of c<strong>on</strong>sciousness or states of“transcendent being.” (See Teilhard de Chardin’s 1933 essay “TheEvoluti<strong>on</strong> of Chastity” <strong>on</strong> the role of pleasure in achieving states of“transcendent being.“) C<strong>on</strong>sequently, most of the somatosensorypleasure-stimulus-seeking behaviors of c<strong>on</strong>temporary Western civilizati<strong>on</strong>(not just America) appear to be “n<strong>on</strong>integrative” in nature, i.e.,primarily “reflexive.” This means the “pleasure experience” is amomentary and transitory phenomen<strong>on</strong> that produces a temporaryreducti<strong>on</strong> of physiological tensi<strong>on</strong> and discomfort but does not representa true positive state of “integrative pleasure” that is essential forexperiencing an “altered state of c<strong>on</strong>sciousness.” Thus, anomie remains,a high need for another “pleasure fix” remains, and the complex ofperseverative behaviors remains. <strong>Drug</strong>s and alcohol “bypass” thesomatosensory process and provide a direct route to higher braincenters that alter “states of c<strong>on</strong>sciousness” which simulate states of“transcendent being.” It should be noted that somatosensory affecti<strong>on</strong>aldeprivati<strong>on</strong> from social isolati<strong>on</strong> results in an aversi<strong>on</strong> to touch andthus c<strong>on</strong>stitutes a barrier to the “touch therapy” that is essential forrehabilitati<strong>on</strong>, namely, the establishment of emoti<strong>on</strong>al/affective-socialrelati<strong>on</strong>ships.Within the c<strong>on</strong>text of SAD theory, three basic groups of substanceabusers are proposed to exist and need to be evaluated and treateddifferently. These are (a) pleasure seekers (marijuana, heroin, etc.),289


(b) pleasure avoiders (alcohol, depressants, tranquilizers), and (c)“altered states of c<strong>on</strong>sciousness” seekers (hallucinogens).A factor-analytic study involving items of drug and alcohol usageproduced orthog<strong>on</strong>al (independent) factors for alcohol and marijuanausage (Prescott and Wallace 1976). Unfortunately, time and space d<strong>on</strong>ot permit review of these data or an elaborati<strong>on</strong> of SAD theory ofdrug typologies and their implicati<strong>on</strong>s for research and therapy. It issuggested, however, that a sensory process orientati<strong>on</strong> would behighly heuristic. Special attenti<strong>on</strong> should be given to evaluatingvestibular-cerebellar processes in alcoholics, somesthetic-cerebellarprocesses in pleasure-seeking drug users, and visual/auditory neocorticalprocesses in hallucinogen users. It should be recognized thatthese suggesti<strong>on</strong>s are highly speculative and have many limitati<strong>on</strong>s,but they may, nevertheless, have some merit in attempting to identifyspecific neurobiological brain processes with specific choices of substanceuse and abuse.Evidence that social isolati<strong>on</strong> rearing alters neurochemistry of brainfuncti<strong>on</strong> has been partially reviewed elsewhere (Prescott 1971a, 1976a;Lal et al. 1972; Essman 1971, 1974, 1979; Essman and Casper 1978;Welch and Welch 1969: Valzelli 1967; De Feudis and Marks 1973;Rosenzweig 1979; Rosenzweig et al. 1968). Certain studies, however,deserve special commentary, and recent developments with respect tothe endorphins are especially relevant to somatosensory affecti<strong>on</strong>aldeprivati<strong>on</strong> theory and data, as is the basic alterati<strong>on</strong> of the CNS’sresp<strong>on</strong>se to drugs that is induced by SAD of isolati<strong>on</strong> rearing.In this specific social-neurobiological c<strong>on</strong>text, Lal et al. (1972) havedem<strong>on</strong>strated that social isolati<strong>on</strong> rearing of mice (somatosensory affecti<strong>on</strong>aldeprivati<strong>on</strong>) significantly altered the pharmacological effects ofhexobarbital, pentobarbital, chloral hydrate, barbital, and chlorpromazine.Specifically, social isolati<strong>on</strong> enhances stimulant drug effects andreduces CNS depressant effects.B<strong>on</strong>net et al. (1976) reported that mice reared in social isolati<strong>on</strong> (somatosensoryaffecti<strong>on</strong>al deprivati<strong>on</strong>) for 20 weeks showed a significantreducti<strong>on</strong> in narcotic ag<strong>on</strong>ist and antag<strong>on</strong>ist binding. No differencescould be found in stereospecific binding between the rearing groupswith 15 weeks of differential rearing, but were found at 17 and 21weeks. These authors also reported a significant reducti<strong>on</strong> of thenumber of opiate binding sites in the brains of isolati<strong>on</strong>-reared micecompared to aggregati<strong>on</strong>-reared mice. This loss of opiate receptorsites in isolati<strong>on</strong>-reared mice may be analogous to the loss of dendritesc<strong>on</strong>sequent to social isolati<strong>on</strong> rearing.Panksepp et al. (1978) and Herman and Panksepp (1978) reported asignificant decrease in distress vocalizati<strong>on</strong>s of puppies which werebriefly separated from their mothers (15 minutes) with an injecti<strong>on</strong> of0.125 mg/kg of oxymorph<strong>on</strong>e, and they found that nalox<strong>on</strong>e increasedgroup vocalizati<strong>on</strong> of two- to five-day-old white Leghorn chicks brieflyseparated from their mother. These authors discuss the parallelsbetween the biological nature of narcotic addicti<strong>on</strong> and the formati<strong>on</strong> ofsocial b<strong>on</strong>ds, and their theoretical positi<strong>on</strong> is similar to SAD theoryand my belief that the brain endorphin systems may be <strong>on</strong>e of the mostimportant neurobiological systems mediating the development of affecti<strong>on</strong>alb<strong>on</strong>ds, including sexual affecti<strong>on</strong>al b<strong>on</strong>ds.290


The role of endorphins in sexual behavior has been studied by Gesa etal. (1979), and they have reported the following findings from theirrat study:a) DALA (D-Ala 2 -Met-enkephalinamide) given intracerebroventricularlyat a dose of six micrograms completely inhibited copulatory behaviorand the ability to ejaculate in sexually active rats. Nalox<strong>on</strong>e (fourmg/kg) given intraperit<strong>on</strong>eally completely reversed this effect.b) Nalox<strong>on</strong>e does not enhance sexual behavior in sexually active rats.c) Nalox<strong>on</strong>e (four mg/kg) given intramuscularly significantly enhancesmounting, intromissi<strong>on</strong>, and ejaculati<strong>on</strong> in sexually inactive rats.These authors suggest that endorphins may mediate sexual disordersand that opioid antag<strong>on</strong>ists “might become potentially useful therapeuticagents for sexual disturbances in man” (p. 204). A similar statementmight be made for the treatment of alcoholics whose somatosensorypleasure system is dysfuncti<strong>on</strong>al and often inoperable. Whether pleasureinducingdrugs, such as marijuana and the opioids, may prove to be auseful first step in a program of somatosensory rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> foralcoholics and other somatosensory impaired individuals remains to bedem<strong>on</strong>strated. Different therapeutic strategies appear indicated,however, for differing classes of substance abusers.Veith et al. (1978) have also reported the effects of endorphin compoundsup<strong>on</strong> emoti<strong>on</strong>al and sexual behaviors in rats. They examinedthe c<strong>on</strong>sequences of a single intraperit<strong>on</strong>eal injecti<strong>on</strong> of 100 mg ofα-endorphin (ß-LPH 61-76), γ-endorphin (ß-LPH 61-77). and ß-endorphin(ß-LPH 61-91), and a [D-Ala 2 ] analog of Met-enkephalin up<strong>on</strong> severalmeasures of open field behavior compared to sa<strong>line</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trols.In brief, these authors found that ß-endorphin enhanced groomingbehavior; γ-endorphin and its analog [D-Ala 2 ] increased emoti<strong>on</strong>alresp<strong>on</strong>ses (ran to the wall faster and greater defecati<strong>on</strong>); and α-endorphin [D-Ala 2 ] increased sexual arousal (penile erecti<strong>on</strong> andseminal discharge). The selective behavior effects of these variouspeptides were emphasized, and it was suggested that each peptide maybe coded to act up<strong>on</strong> receptor rates in a differential manner to mediatethe differing behavioral effects.From this writer’s perspective it is sufficient to emphasize the social,emoti<strong>on</strong>al, and pleasure (sexual) behaviors that are induced by endorphincompounds. In this c<strong>on</strong>text, it is heuristic to note the findingsof Houck et al. (1980) who reported two ß-endorphinlike materials inhuman placenta from three patients undergoing natural childbirth.These authors speculate up<strong>on</strong> the possible role of placental endorphins“as a natural antidote to the pain and stress of parturiti<strong>on</strong>.” Thiswriter cannot help but speculate further that the positive emoti<strong>on</strong>alstate toward pregnancy of women electing natural childbirth may bereflected in a “positive intrauterine state” that is characterized by thepresence of placental endorphin. This raises additi<strong>on</strong>al questi<strong>on</strong>swhether “stressful” pregnancies or “unwanted” pregnancies are characterizedby a significant decrease or lack of placental endorphins.Finally, does the presence or absence of placental endorphins reflect,in any way, the integrity of fetal endorphin mechanisms or the futuredevelopmental integrity of ne<strong>on</strong>atal/infant/child endorphin mechanisms?Does obstetric medicati<strong>on</strong> have any adverse effect <strong>on</strong> fetal endorphin291


mechanisms? Do such events have any l<strong>on</strong>g-term developmental implicati<strong>on</strong>sfor how pain and pleasure are experienced, the quality of developmentof emoti<strong>on</strong>al-social relati<strong>on</strong>ships, and whether and what coping/compensatory behaviors may be adopted as a c<strong>on</strong>sequence of dysfuncti<strong>on</strong>alpsychobiological affecti<strong>on</strong>al mechanisms?These studies are cited because of the increasing evidence that haslinked affecti<strong>on</strong>al variables and early social isolati<strong>on</strong> to (a) violence,drug and alcohol abuse, and sexual dysfuncti<strong>on</strong>ing; (b) altered neurochemistry,electrophysiology, and dendritic structures (neur<strong>on</strong>al communicati<strong>on</strong>)in somatosensory and motor cortex and cerebellar cortex;(c) altered narcotic ag<strong>on</strong>ist and antag<strong>on</strong>ist binding; and (d) alteredCNS resp<strong>on</strong>se to stimulant and depressant drugs. The role of sexualfuncti<strong>on</strong>ing and sexual pleasure in the developmental c<strong>on</strong>tinuum ofaffecti<strong>on</strong>al b<strong>on</strong>ding and its relati<strong>on</strong>ship to endorphins, drug and alcoholuse, and violence, particularly alcohol-induced violence, brings ac<strong>on</strong>vergence of theories and experimental evidence that were heretoforec<strong>on</strong>sidered disparate entities and phenomena. The report of Pradelleset al. (1979) that visual deprivati<strong>on</strong> decreases Met-enkephalin in variousamygdaloid and striatal structures provides further support for linkingsensory deprivati<strong>on</strong> phenomena to enkephalin neurotransmitter orneuroregulatory processes.The findings of Gesa et al. (1979) and of Panksepp (1978), however,appear c<strong>on</strong>tradictory and inc<strong>on</strong>sistent with this proposed c<strong>on</strong>vergence.In the former study, stimulati<strong>on</strong> of opiate receptors induced pleasuredeficitbehaviors (failure to copulate and ejaculate), whereas in thelatter study, stimulati<strong>on</strong> of opiate receptors induced pleasureenhancementbehaviors (decrease in distress vocalizati<strong>on</strong>s). Similarly,in the Gesa study nalox<strong>on</strong>e enhanced pleasure behaviors (increasedcopulati<strong>on</strong> and ejaculati<strong>on</strong>), whereas nalox<strong>on</strong>e decreased pleasurebehaviors (enhanced distress vocalizati<strong>on</strong>) in the Panksepp study.These apparent fundamental c<strong>on</strong>tradicti<strong>on</strong>s are, it is proposed, resolvablewithin SAD theory and Cann<strong>on</strong>’s Law of Denervati<strong>on</strong> Supersensitivity(Cann<strong>on</strong> 1939; Cann<strong>on</strong> and Rosenbleuth 1949; Collier 1968; Sharpless1975), which is an integral and essential neurophysiological mechanismof SAD theory (Prescott 1971a, 1972b).Briefly, fundamental distincti<strong>on</strong>s must be made between CNSs that arecharacterized by or not characterized by denervati<strong>on</strong> supersensitivity,which is induced by deafferentati<strong>on</strong>, i.e., a loss of afferent input.Sexual inactivity, like social isolati<strong>on</strong> rearing, involves somatosensorydeprivati<strong>on</strong> that c<strong>on</strong>stitutes a special case of functi<strong>on</strong>al deafferentati<strong>on</strong>.As reported by Struble and Riesen (1978), primate isolati<strong>on</strong> rearingresults in loss of dendrites in somatosensory cortex. The loss ofopiate receptor sites, reduced narcotic ag<strong>on</strong>ist and antag<strong>on</strong>ist binding,enhancement of stimulant drug effects, and inhibiti<strong>on</strong> of depressantdrug effects are also all c<strong>on</strong>sequent to social isolati<strong>on</strong> and thus share,in my view, a comm<strong>on</strong> explanatory mechanism, namely, Cann<strong>on</strong>’s Law ofDenervati<strong>on</strong> Supersensitivity. It is within this c<strong>on</strong>text that it isrelevant to emphasize that opioid substances act <strong>on</strong> their receptors todepress the activity of cells bearing these receptors and, c<strong>on</strong>sequently,are classed as inhibitory neurotransmitters (Fredericks<strong>on</strong> and Norris1976). The enhancement of these inhibitory neurotransmitters throughthe mechanism of denervati<strong>on</strong> supersensitivity might account for theinhibiti<strong>on</strong> of copulatory and ejaculatory behavior as reported by Gesaet al. (1979). Similarly, the absence of denervati<strong>on</strong> supersensitivityin Panksepp’s experimental subjects could account for his endorphinstress-reducing (pleasure-enhancing?) effects.292


The findings of Gispen et al. (1976) that low doses of ß-endorphin(0.01-0.3 micrograms) induced excessive grooming behavior in rats,and of Meyers<strong>on</strong> and Terenius (1977) that “higher” doses of ß-endorphin(<strong>on</strong>e and three micrograms) significantly reduced mounting and copulatorybehavior in Wistar rats exposed to estrous females support the“bidirecti<strong>on</strong>ality” (prosocial versus asocial behaviors) of endorphinmechanisms. Naltrex<strong>on</strong>e given subcutaneously 30 minutes before thepeptide blocked the effect of <strong>on</strong>e microgram ß-endorphin, thus c<strong>on</strong>firmingthat impaired sexual functi<strong>on</strong>ing was mediated via opiate receptors. Itshould be noted that <strong>on</strong>e microgram ß-endorphin did not interfere withsexual exploratory behavior that included active pursuit and investigati<strong>on</strong>of the anogenital area of the female.These reports of bidirecti<strong>on</strong>ality of endorphin activity as a functi<strong>on</strong> ofdosage level, the endorphin antag<strong>on</strong>istic effects, and the nalox<strong>on</strong>e ag<strong>on</strong>isticeffects c<strong>on</strong>cerning sexual behaviors are not unrelated to thenalox<strong>on</strong>e ag<strong>on</strong>istic effects c<strong>on</strong>cerning pain percepti<strong>on</strong>.Levine et al. (1978), in a study of human clinical pain (tooth extracti<strong>on</strong>),found that nalox<strong>on</strong>e produces analgesia at low doses (0.4 and 2mg) and hyperalgesia at high doses (7.5-10 mg) for a placebo-resp<strong>on</strong>dengroup. Interestingly, nalox<strong>on</strong>e had little effect <strong>on</strong> placebo n<strong>on</strong>resp<strong>on</strong>ders.Questi<strong>on</strong>s must be raised whether placebo resp<strong>on</strong>ders and thoseexperimental preparati<strong>on</strong>s that manifest nalox<strong>on</strong>e ag<strong>on</strong>istic effects(bidirecti<strong>on</strong>ality) could be characterized by SAD or other forms ofinduced denervati<strong>on</strong> supersensitivity. These questi<strong>on</strong>s are relevant tothe findings of Buchsbaum et al. (1977), who divided their subjectsinto pain-sensitive and pain-insensitive groups as determined by theirratings of an electric shock. They found that <strong>on</strong>ly the pain-sensitivesubjects reported a nalox<strong>on</strong>e (2 mg) analgesic effect and that paininsensitivesubjects showed nalox<strong>on</strong>e hyperalgesia.Although the studies of Levine et al. (1978) and Buchsbaum et al.(1977) are not directly comparable since Levine employed multiple dosesof nalox<strong>on</strong>e and Buchsbaum employed a single nalox<strong>on</strong>e dose, it is ofinterest to c<strong>on</strong>trast the two nalox<strong>on</strong>e hyperalgesia groups with respectto the issue of placebo resp<strong>on</strong>ding. Levine et al. reported a nalox<strong>on</strong>ebidirecti<strong>on</strong>al effect for placebo resp<strong>on</strong>ders, whereas Buchsbaum’spain-insensitive bidirecti<strong>on</strong>al resp<strong>on</strong>ders (nalox<strong>on</strong>e hyperalgesia) werecharacterized as placebo “n<strong>on</strong>resp<strong>on</strong>ders” since their placebo resp<strong>on</strong>sewas less than half that of the pain-sensitive group. These “inc<strong>on</strong>sistencies”require further experimental study.These observati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong>ly complicate an already very complicated set ofissues and phenomena of endorphin-related behaviors. However, thebidirecti<strong>on</strong>ality phenomena of nalox<strong>on</strong>e and the nalox<strong>on</strong>e ag<strong>on</strong>ist effectsand endorphin antag<strong>on</strong>ist effects involving not <strong>on</strong>ly pain phenomenabut also sexual-social and motor behaviors (Gesa et al. 1979; Meyers<strong>on</strong>and Terenius 1977; Gispen et al. 1976; Bloom et al. 1976; Jacquet andMarks 1976) suggest an extremely complex role of modulati<strong>on</strong>, regulati<strong>on</strong>,and integrati<strong>on</strong> of sensory, social, emoti<strong>on</strong>al, and motor behaviors bythe endorphin system.A theory of cerebellar regulati<strong>on</strong> and integrati<strong>on</strong> of sensory, social,emoti<strong>on</strong>al, and motor behaviors within the c<strong>on</strong>text of SAD theory hasbeen previously elaborated (Prescott 1971 a, 1976a, 1978). Heath andhis coworkers (Heath 1972, 1975a,b, 1976, 1977; Heath et al. 1978,1979) have established a wealth of data describing cerebellar-limbic293


elati<strong>on</strong>ships, which were postulated by SAD theory. They have furtherdramatized how cerebellar stimulati<strong>on</strong> can modulate extreme states ofemoti<strong>on</strong>al expressi<strong>on</strong> (positive and negative) in human subjects. Accordingto SAD theory, the cerebellum is not itself the site of these behaviors,but it exerts a regulatory influence <strong>on</strong> limbic, reticular, andfr<strong>on</strong>tal cortical structures to modulate these behaviors. Cerebellar modulati<strong>on</strong>of limbic-endorphin activity would be a natural extensi<strong>on</strong> of SADtheory and could be tested in both animal and human studies. It wouldbe expected, for example, that endorphin/nalox<strong>on</strong>e behaviors would bealtered with chr<strong>on</strong>ic cerebellar electrical stimulati<strong>on</strong> that resulted in profoundchanges in emoti<strong>on</strong>al behavior, as described by Heath et al. Inparticular, since Heath (1972, 1975a,b) has documented abnormal electricalspike discharges in the limbic and cerebellar structures of isolati<strong>on</strong>rearedprimates, and Saltzberg and colleagues (Saltzberg et al. 1971;Saltzberg and Lustick 1975; Saltzberg 1976) have developed signal analysismethods to detect these deep brain spike discharges from scalp EEGrecordings, it is now possible to undertake studies that could link aknown history of somatosensory affecti<strong>on</strong>al deprivati<strong>on</strong> to abnormal deepbrain spike activity and to specific patterns of endorphin/nalox<strong>on</strong>einducedbehaviors associated with dysfuncti<strong>on</strong>al behaviors, e.g., alcoholinducedviolence and impaired sexual functi<strong>on</strong>ing. Effective therapiesshould be reflected in eliminati<strong>on</strong> of spike discharges, altered endorphin/nalox<strong>on</strong>e behaviors, development of affecti<strong>on</strong>al emoti<strong>on</strong>al behaviors, andeliminati<strong>on</strong> of drug and alcohol dependence.The role of the cerebellum in somatosensory affecti<strong>on</strong>al deprivati<strong>on</strong> hasbeen given support by Berman et al. (1974); and Floeter and Greenough(1979), who reported significant increases in spiney branchlets ofPurkinje cells in the para flocculus and the nodulus of the cerebellumin m<strong>on</strong>keys reared in col<strong>on</strong>y c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s compared to isolate-reared andsocially experienced animals (envir<strong>on</strong>mental variati<strong>on</strong> of SAD). Thefinding of opiate receptors in the cerebellum should be noted in thisrespect (Meunier and Zajah 1979). Although denervati<strong>on</strong> supersensitivitymechanisms inherent in somatosensory affecti<strong>on</strong>al deprivati<strong>on</strong> areoffered as a major explanatory process in accounting for the variety ofdiverse and often apparently inc<strong>on</strong>sistent and c<strong>on</strong>tradictory findingsfrom the endorphin/nalox<strong>on</strong>e behavioral literature, it is recognized thatother factors, e.g., ne<strong>on</strong>atal anoxia, can induce denervati<strong>on</strong> supersensitivity(Berman and Berman 1975; Burch et al. 1975) and that the“family” of endorphins and their antag<strong>on</strong>ists are additi<strong>on</strong>al factors thatcan c<strong>on</strong>tribute to the complexity of findings reported in the literatureand their interpretati<strong>on</strong>.The major theoretical orientati<strong>on</strong> of this paper is to emphasize that anystudy of endorphin/nalox<strong>on</strong>e behaviors or drug/alcohol behaviors musttake into account the developmental history of the organism to determinewhether the CNS of that organism is characterized by denervati<strong>on</strong> supersensitivity,whether induced by somatosensory affecti<strong>on</strong>al deprivati<strong>on</strong> orother etiological developmental factors.The phenomen<strong>on</strong> of “hyperendorphinism” of affective disorders(Buchsbaum et al., in press), which may well be an expressi<strong>on</strong> of“neurotransmitter density” due to denervati<strong>on</strong> supersensitivity, is anexample of a c<strong>on</strong>struct that might be benefited by a developmentalperspective. (Neurotransmitter density in neurochemistry is analogousto current density in electrophysiology and expresses the relati<strong>on</strong>shipof the amount of released neurotransmitter substance available to thenumber of available receptors.)294


Since isolati<strong>on</strong> rearing results in a reducti<strong>on</strong> of the number of opioidreceptors, a state of “hyperendorphinism” may not reflect a change inabsolute volume of released endorphin but rather a change in thenumber of opioid receptors (endorphin density). The c<strong>on</strong>verse couldalso occur (increased volume of endorphin with receptor number remainingc<strong>on</strong>stant) for different etiological reas<strong>on</strong>s. This is menti<strong>on</strong>ed forthe purpose of suggesting that “hyperendorphinism” may not be aunitary phenomen<strong>on</strong> since different mechanisms and etiologies couldmediate this effect.It would be a serious omissi<strong>on</strong> not to menti<strong>on</strong> the classic theoreticalsystem developed by Petrie (1976), which has unusual relevance to theissues of substance abuse and to somatosensory affecti<strong>on</strong>al deprivati<strong>on</strong>theory. Briefly, Petrie has proposed a theoretical system that postulatesCNS processes of reducti<strong>on</strong> and augmentati<strong>on</strong> of the sensoryenvir<strong>on</strong>ment to describe an individual’s “reactance” to pain and sensorydeprivati<strong>on</strong>. The “CNS augmenters” are characterized by an intolerancefor pain and a tolerance for sensory deprivati<strong>on</strong>. This pattern ofreactance occurs because the CNS of these individuals acts to augmentor enhance the impact of a sensory event up<strong>on</strong> the CNS. C<strong>on</strong>versely,the “CNS reducers” are characterized by a tolerance for pain and anintolerance for sensory deprivati<strong>on</strong>. This pattern of reactance occursbecause the CNS of these individuals acts to reduce or inhibit theimpact of a given sensory event up<strong>on</strong> the CNS. Thus, the “CNSreducers” are characterized by a chr<strong>on</strong>ic state of insufficient afferentstimulati<strong>on</strong> (stress of sensory insufficiency or sensory deprivati<strong>on</strong>) andengage in behaviors that are designed to maximize afferent stimulati<strong>on</strong>of the CNS. C<strong>on</strong>sequently, these “CNS reducers” are those whoengage in a variety of stimulus-seeking behaviors, e.g., when punishedwith solitary c<strong>on</strong>finement, delinquents who are CNS reducers willfrequently engage in self-mutilative behaviors, such as cutting themselveswith razors or burning themselves with cigarettes (note selfmutilati<strong>on</strong>of isolati<strong>on</strong>-reared animals).Petrie (1976) described the resp<strong>on</strong>se of reducers, moderates, andaugmenters to alcohol and found that augmenters were most affected bydramatically changing from an augmenting reactance mode to a reducingreactance mode. Similar but less str<strong>on</strong>g reducing effects were obtainedwith reducers. Comparable results were obtained with other drugs,such as aspirin and chlorpromazine. Thus, augmenters as a groupwere shifted away from pain intolerance to pain tolerance. Buchsbaum(1978) has provided a review of a number of neurophysiological studiesfrom his laboratory and others <strong>on</strong> reducers and augmenters. Withoutreviewing all of his findings, suffice it to point out that he reportedthat reducti<strong>on</strong> of the amplitude of sensory-evoked potentials to increasedstimulus intensity was associated with pain tolerance and analgesia,and that augmentati<strong>on</strong> was linked to substance abuse. The studies ofBuchsbaum and Ludwig (in press) and v<strong>on</strong> Knorring and Oreland(1978) are also relevant to these issues.It has been previously suggested that somatosensory affecti<strong>on</strong>al deprivati<strong>on</strong>of isolati<strong>on</strong> rearing is a major c<strong>on</strong>tributing factor in the developmentalneuropsychobiological substrate of Petrie’s typology of reducersand augmenters (Prescott 1967). Chr<strong>on</strong>ic or perseverative stimulusseekingbehaviors and impaired pain percepti<strong>on</strong>, for example, arepredominant characteristics of somatosensory affecti<strong>on</strong>al deprivati<strong>on</strong>(denervati<strong>on</strong> supersensitivity) and the “CNS reducer.” There are,however, significant differences in the communality of the two theoreticalsystems in which SAD is characterized by “paradoxical” behaviors,295


e.g., simultaneous supersensitivity to touch and impaired pain percepti<strong>on</strong>that are not accounted for by Petrie’s typology. Zuckerman’s(1979) theory of sensati<strong>on</strong> seeking is also intrinsically related to thetheories of Petrie (1976) and Prescott (1967, 1971a,b, 1972a,b, 1973,1975, 1976a,b, 1977).This writer has attempted to link these basic developmental neurobiologicalprocesses of SAD to cross-cultural characteristics of childrearingpractices; to social and religious mores and customs thatregulate sexual behaviors; and to pers<strong>on</strong>ality characteristics of authoritarianism,exploitati<strong>on</strong>, and narcissism in c<strong>on</strong>trast to egalitarianism,nurturance, and altruism. Further, it is postulated that these c<strong>on</strong>trastsin pers<strong>on</strong>ality characteristics, c<strong>on</strong>sidered at the microsocial level,c<strong>on</strong>stitute the bases for the political structure of a culture, namely,egalitarian-democratic societies versus authoritarian-fascist societies(Prescott 1975, 1976, 1977). It is of some significance that Petrie(1976) draws exactly the same parallels from her theory to the characteristicsof both pers<strong>on</strong>ality and culture with her typologies of “compassi<strong>on</strong>”(augmenter) versus “callousness” (reducer) (pp. xii-xiv).In c<strong>on</strong>cluding this theoretical essay it hardly needs to be emphasizedthat the social-emoti<strong>on</strong>al dysfuncti<strong>on</strong>ing of the individual in society, inwhatever form it may be expressed, is not <strong>on</strong>ly an intrinsic aspect ofneurobiological functi<strong>on</strong>ing of the individual but also of the socialpsychologicalforces of culture that shape the individuality of neurobiologicalfuncti<strong>on</strong>ing through the formative developmental processes ofsensory stimulati<strong>on</strong> and deprivati<strong>on</strong>, and through a culture of chemicaland physical envir<strong>on</strong>ments that influence fetal, ne<strong>on</strong>atal, and postnataldevelopment. Maternal habits of chemical ingesti<strong>on</strong>, e.g., alcohol,drugs, food/spice preferences, or exposure to certain chemical envir<strong>on</strong>mentsduring gestati<strong>on</strong>, may well “imprint” up<strong>on</strong> the developing fetuscertain “sensitivities” and “predispositi<strong>on</strong>s” for use or avoidance ofthose chemical agents during postnatal life with all the implicati<strong>on</strong>s thatthis has for behavior.It necessarily follows that preventive and therapeutic programs cannotbe restricted to molecular biological strategies that are directed at theindividual organism. The rec<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong> of the individual requires alsothe rec<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong> of society and culture.The elements of societal and cultural rec<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong> involve not <strong>on</strong>lyshaping a safe, beneficent physical envir<strong>on</strong>ment but also a nurturant,caring, and affecti<strong>on</strong>ate envir<strong>on</strong>ment of human relati<strong>on</strong>ships. Thelatter touches deeply up<strong>on</strong> philosophical and religious ideologies thatregulate the morality of pain and pleasure in human relati<strong>on</strong>ships andthe role of women in society.The matri<strong>line</strong>al/patri<strong>line</strong>al structure of human cultures and their relati<strong>on</strong>shipto nurturance in human relati<strong>on</strong>ships, as well as the c<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong>of the supernatural in human cultures, are a logical extensi<strong>on</strong> of SADtheory. However, it is bey<strong>on</strong>d the scope of this essay to develop thesetopics and relate them to what has been reviewed herein.296


A Theory of Alcohol and<strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>A Genetic ApproachMarc A. Schuckit, M.D.A GENERAL OVERVIEWGenetically influenced biological factors explain <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e part of thevariance in the development of alcoholism and drug abuse. Even forthose pers<strong>on</strong>s genetically predisposed, the final clinical picture involvesa combinati<strong>on</strong> of genetic factors (leading both toward and away fromsubstance abuse) and envir<strong>on</strong>mental events (with similar positive andnegative aspects).Before proceeding with the theory <strong>on</strong> the importance of genetics insubstance abuse, it is necessary to present briefly some of the datasupporting the c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> that genetics plays any role at all. Thepicture is not irrefutable, as it is difficult to carry out human studieswhile c<strong>on</strong>trolling enough factors to make definite c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>s. Themost important aspect of this research is the manner in which thedifferent methods carried out in different settings generate suchc<strong>on</strong>sistent data (Robins 1978).DATA SUPPORTING GENETICS INALCOHOLISM AND OTHER DRUG ABUSEThe most impressive amount of informati<strong>on</strong> is available <strong>on</strong> alcohol, withmuch less data <strong>on</strong> other substances. Thus, the two topics will bediscussed separately.The first indicati<strong>on</strong> of a possible genetic influence comes from thestudies of families of alcoholics, where it has been repeatedly shownthat the chances of a child developing alcoholism as an adult increasewith the number of alcoholic relatives, the severity of the alcoholproblems in those relatives, and the degree of genetic closeness to theill relative (Schuckit et al. 1972; Goodwin 1976). The hypothesis isfurther strengthened by genetic marker studies dem<strong>on</strong>strating a possiblelink between the number of factors known to be genetically influenced297


(e.g., blood type) and alcoholism within certain populati<strong>on</strong>s or families,although these results are difficult to replicate. Data from animalstudies are c<strong>on</strong>sistent with the theory of the importance of genetics inthat they show that it is possible to breed strains of animals withrelatively higher and lower tendencies toward drinking alcohol, afactor which may shed light <strong>on</strong> the <strong>on</strong>set of drinking but not necessarily<strong>on</strong> alcoholism itself.The most persuasive alcoholism-related genetic informati<strong>on</strong> in humanscomes from twin studies and adopti<strong>on</strong> investigati<strong>on</strong>s. In twin researchthe level of similarity for alcoholism (i.e., c<strong>on</strong>cordance) in fraternaltwins, who share <strong>on</strong>ly 50 percent of their genes, is compared to thelevel of c<strong>on</strong>cordance in identical twins, who share 100 percent of theirgenes. These studies have shown a level of heritability for drinkingand drinking problems (Partanen et al. 1966). as well as a higherc<strong>on</strong>cordance rate for alcoholism in identical twins (around 60 percent)than in fraternal twins (around 30 percent) (Kaij 1960). The adopti<strong>on</strong>studies, comparing the outcome for alcoholism in children of alcoholicsseparated from their parents near birth to that of a suitable c<strong>on</strong>trolpopulati<strong>on</strong>, have used diverse methodologies ranging from half-siblingsto actual adopti<strong>on</strong> records in three different countries and yet haveshown similar results (Schuckit et al. 1972; Goodwin 1976; Bohman1977). The children of alcoholics dem<strong>on</strong>strate elevated risks for alcoholismeven if separated from their parents near birth and raisedwithout knowledge of their biological parents, while the children ofn<strong>on</strong>alcoholics do not have elevated risks for alcoholism even if rearedby alcoholic adoptive parents (Schuckit et al. 1972; Goodwin et al. 1974).The data supporting the importance of genetic factors for abuse ofdrugs other than alcohol are much less complete. There are somelimited family data showing a correlati<strong>on</strong> between drug use in groupsof young men and drug use and problem patterns in their parents(Tennant 1976; Smart and Fejer 1972; Annis 1974). There is alsoinformati<strong>on</strong> dem<strong>on</strong>strating the possibility of breeding high and lowmorphine-preferring strains of rats and mice (Nichols and Hsiao 1967;Erikss<strong>on</strong> and Kiianmaa 1971). Unfortunately, there are no wellc<strong>on</strong>trolledstudies of twins or investigati<strong>on</strong>s utilizing the separati<strong>on</strong>model for studying drug abuse.A number of investigati<strong>on</strong>s have looked for possible ties betweengenetic factors which might underlie drug abuse and those which mightbe resp<strong>on</strong>sible for alcoholism. The results are tentative, dem<strong>on</strong>strating,for instance, that alcohol and drug problems may run in the samefamilies (Tennant 1976), but such studies rarely define what is meantby alcoholism or drug abuse and almost never c<strong>on</strong>trol for relateddiagnoses such as the antisocial pers<strong>on</strong>ality (Schuckit 1973). Thislatter diagnosis might be resp<strong>on</strong>sible for the dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> of sec<strong>on</strong>daryalcohol and drug problems within the same family group. Animalstudies do dem<strong>on</strong>strate some degree of crossover between alcohol- andmorphine-seeking behavior in strains of animals (Erikss<strong>on</strong> and Kiianmaa1971; Sinclair et al. 1973; Nichols 1972). In another approach, anumber of theorists have attempted to establish a tie between alcoholand drug abuse and a vulnerability to stress factors such as overcrowding,but the data are inc<strong>on</strong>clusive (Bihari 1976; Westermeyer 1971;J<strong>on</strong>as and J<strong>on</strong>as 1977). Finally, we must c<strong>on</strong>sider the possibility thatabuse of <strong>on</strong>e drug (e.g., alcohol) might induce biochemical changessimilar to those noted with other drugs (e.g., opiates) (Davis andWalsh 1970; Doust 1974). However, in the absence of more c<strong>on</strong>clusivedata, I feel that while alcoholism may be a genetically influenced disorder298


involving a number of genes, and while it is possible that <strong>on</strong>e orseveral of those genes might also influence abuse of other drugs,there are not sufficient data to indicate that the same c<strong>on</strong>stellati<strong>on</strong> ofgenetic and/or biological factors underlie the abuse of alcohol andother substances.In summary, there is good evidence from divergent methodologies indifferent countries which points to the probable importance of geneticsas a c<strong>on</strong>tributory factor in alcoholism. Similar data <strong>on</strong> other drugs ofabuse are not available, and the existing informati<strong>on</strong> is preliminary andopen to interpretati<strong>on</strong> either as being c<strong>on</strong>sistent with an envir<strong>on</strong>mentalor social model, or as indicating an influence of genetics.POSSIBLE GENETIC MECHANISMSFor a genetic model to have credence, the populati<strong>on</strong> being studiedmust be carefully defined so that <strong>on</strong>e does not c<strong>on</strong>fuse transient alcoholrelateddifficulties, which may be seen in the majority of people age 18to 25 (Cahalan 1970), with persistent alcohol- and drug-related difficulties--i.e.,alcoholism or drug abuse (Schuckit 1973). It is essentialthat those pers<strong>on</strong>s with major preexisting psychiatric disorders inwhich alcohol or drug abuse might be symptomatic (sec<strong>on</strong>dary alcoholicsor drug abusers) be excluded from the generalizati<strong>on</strong>s about thegenetics of alcoholism or drug abuse, as they may be carrying geneticloading for other problems. It is also important to note at this juncturethat even with carefully defined alcoholism or drug abuse there willprobably be intense and unusual envir<strong>on</strong>mental situati<strong>on</strong>s which cancopy the clinical picture. In this case a good example might be thedrug use and abuse patterns noted in soldiers sent to Vietnam, whomight otherwise never have used drugs and who, according to somefine followup studies, cease their drug misuse <strong>on</strong>ce they return totheir home communities (Robins et al. 1975).With these caveats in mind, in the genetic hypothesis an individualwould enter life with a certain level of a genetically influenced biologicalpredispositi<strong>on</strong> toward alcoholism or drug abuse. It is probable thatmultiple genes are involved or that other factors affect the strength ofthe acti<strong>on</strong>s of a particular gene (i.e., incomplete penetrance of thegene). If the disorder is polygenic (i.e., involving more than <strong>on</strong>egene) there is probably a combinati<strong>on</strong> of genes which might predisposean individual to alcoholism (e.g., the possibility of getting a differentlevel of intoxicati<strong>on</strong> when drinking or an unusual effect of alcohol <strong>on</strong>anxiety, etc.) and some which might help to protect a pers<strong>on</strong> fromdem<strong>on</strong>strating alcoholism (e.g., becoming very ill at even low alcoholdoses). The pers<strong>on</strong>, then, could go through a variety of life eventsand stresses, some of which would predispose him or her to alcoholism(e.g., working in a heavy-drinking envir<strong>on</strong>ment, such as the armedservices) and others which would protect the pers<strong>on</strong> from dem<strong>on</strong>stratingthe predispositi<strong>on</strong> (e.g., being a woman in a society with heavyproscripti<strong>on</strong>s against drinking for women). The final alcoholic picturewould depend up<strong>on</strong> the balance between the positive and negativegenetic effects interacting with the positive and negative envir<strong>on</strong>mentalfactors.In this model, a genetic predispositi<strong>on</strong> toward alcoholism might havenothing to do with why people begin drinking in a heavy-drinkingsociety such as ours. Genetic factors might make a modest c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong>to the development of relatively minor and evanescent alcohol-related299


problems, such as those seen in late adolescence and early adulthood.The greatest impact might be <strong>on</strong> factors which determine why someindividuals c<strong>on</strong>tinue to increase their alcohol intake during their thirddecade while others gradually but significantly decrease their drinkingand, thus, decrease the risk of associated problems.It is probable that no <strong>on</strong>e genetic factor explains the entire predispositi<strong>on</strong>toward alcoholism. There may be a variety of things involved,including those which might affect the metabolism of alcohol, differencesin reacti<strong>on</strong>s to acute doses of alcohol, differential resp<strong>on</strong>ses to moresubacute exposure to the drug, differential vulnerabilities to adversec<strong>on</strong>sequences from c<strong>on</strong>tinued use, different pers<strong>on</strong>ality types, etc.(Omenn 1975). At the present time, there are some preliminary datato support the theory that the offspring of alcoholics metabolize alcoholdifferently, showing higher levels of the toxic substance acetaldehyde.At the same time they show a decreased sensitivity of the nervoussystem to the acute effects of alcohol (perhaps equivalent to innatetolerance) (Schuckit and Rayses 1979; Schuckit 1979c).The degree of “genetic loading” could combine with the intensity ofenvir<strong>on</strong>mental events to determine the characteristics of the drug-relatedproblems as well. For example, the level of genetic factors coulddetermine which alcoholics begin to have problems in the third decade(heavier genetic load) and which do not dem<strong>on</strong>strate difficulties untilreaching the mid-fifties. Biological factors might also be involved insp<strong>on</strong>taneous remissi<strong>on</strong> from alcoholism through alterati<strong>on</strong>s in either thereacti<strong>on</strong> to or metabolism of alcohol which may parallel aging and whichmight negate the original biological factors resp<strong>on</strong>sible for the predispositi<strong>on</strong>.Envir<strong>on</strong>mental events could have a large impact in determiningage of <strong>on</strong>set and may help to explain some of the “sp<strong>on</strong>taneous remissi<strong>on</strong>”seen with all drugs of abuse, as the decisi<strong>on</strong> to c<strong>on</strong>tinue misuse of thesubstance may represent a cost/benefit ratio, with the chances ofc<strong>on</strong>tinued abuse decreasing with increasing costs of life problems.In summary, I feel that alcoholism is probably a multifactorial, polygenicallyinfluenced disorder. The relative balance between the degreeof genetic loading toward alcoholism and the detrimental as well asprotective envir<strong>on</strong>mental influences could determine the age of <strong>on</strong>set ofalcohol abuse and the characteristic course for primary alcoholics.Comparable data are not available <strong>on</strong> drug misuse, but for heuristicpurposes, I would picture a similar situati<strong>on</strong>. At the present time,data are not str<strong>on</strong>g enough to support a theory wherein the samegenetic mechanisms would be resp<strong>on</strong>sible for a general propensitytoward all types of drugs, and thus I favor a theory in which thebiological mechanisms for alcoholism are different from those for othersubstances of abuse.SPECIAL PROBLEMSA short methodological note is needed. To optimize the chances ofdiscovering any relevant genetic factor, it is important that the groupof alcoholics studied be as homogeneous as possible. This requiresusing a definiti<strong>on</strong> stated in relatively objective terms (so that similarstudies can be d<strong>on</strong>e in different settings) which has been applied toother populati<strong>on</strong>s which were followed up over time and shown to run arelatively homogeneous course (Schuckit 1973; Haglund and Schuckit1977; Woodruff et al. 1974). While there is a great deal of crossover300


in the populati<strong>on</strong>s out<strong>line</strong>d by definiti<strong>on</strong>s utilizing physical dependence,psychological dependence (which is quite difficult to define), a quantityfrequencyapproach to alcoholism, and the life-problem definiti<strong>on</strong>, mostof the studies <strong>on</strong> the genetics of alcoholism have utilized the life-problemdefiniti<strong>on</strong>. Simply stated, pers<strong>on</strong>s would be c<strong>on</strong>sidered alcoholics whodem<strong>on</strong>strate any <strong>on</strong>e of a number of end-stage life problems related toalcohol (e.g., a marital separati<strong>on</strong> or divorce because of alcohol orphysical evidence that alcohol has harmed health or a job loss or layoffrelated to alcohol or multiple arrests related to drinking) (Schuckit1979b).Of course, alcohol or other drug problems can be primary or candevelop in the midst of another (possibly genetically influenced) psychiatricdisorder (i.e., sec<strong>on</strong>dary). It would not make much sense,however, to include in studies of the genetics of alcoholism people whofulfill the research criteria for schizophrenia and who then go <strong>on</strong> todevelop alcohol or other drug problems. It would be equally selfdefeatingto include in such studies those individuals with unipolaraffective disorder, manic depressive disease, or the antisocial pers<strong>on</strong>ality(Schuckit 1973; Woodruff et al. 1974; Schuckit 1979b). While sec<strong>on</strong>daryalcoholics (i.e., individuals dem<strong>on</strong>strating alcoholism <strong>on</strong>ly after the<strong>on</strong>set of another major psychiatric problem) might be genetically predisposedtoward both alcoholism and the primary disease, this would bevery difficult to pick up by our present methods.SPECIAL GROUPSThe pattern of use and abuse of a substance within any populati<strong>on</strong>subgroup is, of course, the result of a combinati<strong>on</strong> of social, psychological,and biological factors. In this secti<strong>on</strong>, I will discuss a number ofpossible genetically influenced biological factors and envir<strong>on</strong>mentalevents. These will be applied to a variety of subgroups includingNative Americans, other ethnic groups including the Irish and Jews,the elderly, women, and youth, and substance-related difficulties inhealth-care deliverers such as physicians.Native American groups have excepti<strong>on</strong>ally high rates of alcoholism.This might result in part from high levels of any of the proposedgenetically influenced biological mechanisms, although data to date <strong>on</strong>differences between Native Americans and Caucasians in the metabolismof alcohol have been inc<strong>on</strong>clusive (Benni<strong>on</strong> and Li 1976). Becausemembers of this group tend to marry other members, any geneticallyinfluenced factor raising the propensity toward alcoholism would belikely to be perpetuated. No matter what the level of biological predispositi<strong>on</strong>,the high rate of alcoholism is probably also a resp<strong>on</strong>se to theheavy-drinking lifestyle <strong>on</strong> the reservati<strong>on</strong>, the extreme level of socialstress that comes from the disintegrati<strong>on</strong> of the Native American culture,historical differences in the meanings of alcohol use and intoxicati<strong>on</strong>between Native American cultures and Caucasian groups, etc. Thefinal prevalence of alcoholism in this group probably reflects anincreased level of genetic predispositi<strong>on</strong> within Native Americans andan envir<strong>on</strong>ment which maximizes the chance that any such predispositi<strong>on</strong>would become manifest.The purported high rates of alcoholism in Ireland and am<strong>on</strong>g Americansof Irish descent (persisting even when <strong>on</strong>e c<strong>on</strong>trols for the level ofavailable income after bare necessities are met) compared with the low301


ate of alcoholism in Jews in both the United States and Israel is alsoof interest (Haalund and Schuckit 1977). As is true for NativeAmericans, individuals in these groups tend to marry other peoplewithin the same subgroup, thus perpetuating any genetic propensitythat exists, no matter which of the hypothesized mechanisms might beinvolved. At the same time, envir<strong>on</strong>mental factors might alter theexpressi<strong>on</strong> of any biological propensities. Thus any genetic predispositi<strong>on</strong>toward alcoholism existing in Jews might be dampened by theheavy proscripti<strong>on</strong>s against intoxicati<strong>on</strong> and the emphasis <strong>on</strong> learninghow to drink in moderati<strong>on</strong> seen within closely knit Jewish families.Even low to modest rates of biological predispositi<strong>on</strong> in the Irish mightbe clinically expressed through such factors as the tendency towardlate marriage, the ethic of needing to “learn to drink like a man,” thesocial life centering <strong>on</strong> the pub, etc. (Schuckit and Haglund 1977).Three other subgroups present interesting questi<strong>on</strong>s regarding agenetic hypothesis in alcoholism. The actively drinking elderly alcoholicis likely to have begun alcohol abuse in his or her forties or fifties,after many years of “normal” drinking (Schuckit 1977). This is probablythe result of a combinati<strong>on</strong> of a lowered level of genetic propensityand earlier life experiences of drinking in a relatively structuredenvir<strong>on</strong>ment. The problem may be more likely to become manifest whenprotective factors disappear as <strong>on</strong>e’s children grow up and leave thehome, romance leaves the marriage, <strong>on</strong>e recognizes the probability ofno further advancements at work, approaching retirement, etc.The lowered risk for alcoholism in women (Haglund and Schuckit 1977)might reflect some modest differences in metabolism of alcohol or acutereacti<strong>on</strong>s to alcohol at various phases of the menstrual cycle (Greenblattand Schuckit 1976). The alcoholism rate is also c<strong>on</strong>sistent with astr<strong>on</strong>g differential effect of envir<strong>on</strong>ment <strong>on</strong> men and women, perhapsreflecting the (historically) heavier proscripti<strong>on</strong>s against heavy alcoholintake for women (Cl<strong>on</strong>inger et al. 1978).The purported increase in alcohol problems in youths is menti<strong>on</strong>ed here<strong>on</strong>ly in passing, as a reliable definiti<strong>on</strong> for primary alcoholism inadolescents has not yet been developed. Most young people dem<strong>on</strong>stratingalcohol-related difficulties have been shown either to have a primaryantisocial pers<strong>on</strong>ality or to dem<strong>on</strong>strate polydrug misuse and rarely fiteven tentative criteria for primary alcoholism (Greenblatt and Schuckit1976).While not many data are available, similar generalizati<strong>on</strong>s can probablybe made for other drugs of abuse. <strong>On</strong>e notable example is the reportedhigh rate of substance abuse in physicians and nurses when comparedto other individuals of the same socioec<strong>on</strong>omic class (J<strong>on</strong>es 1977). Inthis instance, the increased level of problems might not reflect aheightened genetic loading but rather an increased chance that anybiological propensity will be expressed. This would reflect the readyavailability of drugs and the l<strong>on</strong>g hours and life stresses inherent inthe health-care professi<strong>on</strong>s. However, it is possible that there is somec<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong> between the type of individual likely to go into the healthcareprofessi<strong>on</strong>s and an altered acute reacti<strong>on</strong> to drugs, metabolism ofdrugs, or pers<strong>on</strong>ality traits predisposing <strong>on</strong>e toward drug misuse.302


Opiate Receptors and TheirImplicati<strong>on</strong>s for <strong>Drug</strong> Addicti<strong>on</strong>Eric J. Sim<strong>on</strong>, Ph.D.The implicati<strong>on</strong> of the opiate receptor in human disease has not beenfully established (Sim<strong>on</strong> and Hiller 1978; Terenius 1978). but it promisesto hold exciting implicati<strong>on</strong>s for the future. The hypothesis held bysome investigators for several decades and discussed here is thatnarcotic analgesics bind to highly specific sites or receptors in thecentral nervous system to produce their many well-known resp<strong>on</strong>ses.Why the human and animal brain should c<strong>on</strong>tain sites that can bindwith high specificity and affinity substances derived from plants wasan important questi<strong>on</strong> that led to the discovery of the endogenousopioid peptides. The evidence for receptor sites is compelling, andc<strong>on</strong>sists primarily of the remarkable stereospecific acti<strong>on</strong> displayed bythe narcotic analgesic drugs. This stereospecific characteristic refersto the structural specificity opiate molecules exhibit in interacting withparticular substances in the central nervous system.THE DISCOVERY OF OPIATE RECEPTORSThe search for opiate receptors began in the 1950s and bore fruit inthe early 1970s. It was easy to show binding of opiates to cell c<strong>on</strong>stituents(Sim<strong>on</strong> and van Praag 1966) but to distinguish specific from n<strong>on</strong>specificbinding proved difficult.It was the measurement of stereospecific binding that led to success.lngoglia and Dole (1970) were the first to apply stereospecificity to thesearch for receptors. Goldstein et al. (1971) devised a method formeasuring stereospecific binding in mouse brain tissue. In 1973, thelaboratories of Sim<strong>on</strong> (Sim<strong>on</strong> et al. 1973). Snyder (Pert and Snyder1973). and Terenius (1973), using modificati<strong>on</strong>s of the Goldstein procedure,independently and simultaneously reported the observati<strong>on</strong> inanimal brains of stereospecific binding of opiates. Since that timestereospecific binding studies have been d<strong>on</strong>e in many laboratories andmuch evidence has been accumulated suggesting that these stereospecificsites are indeed receptors which are resp<strong>on</strong>sible for many of thepharmacological acti<strong>on</strong>s of the opiates. They have been found in303


humans (Hiller et al. 1973) and in all vertebrates so far studied, butthey have not been found in invertebrates (Pert et al. 1974).PROPERTIES AND DISTRIBUTIONOF OPIATE RECEPTORSThe properties of the opiate binding sites have been studied extensivelyand their distributi<strong>on</strong> in the brain and spinal cord has been mapped inc<strong>on</strong>siderable detail by dissecti<strong>on</strong> and in vitro binding measurements(Hiller et al. 1973; Kuhar et al. 1973) as well as by autoradiography(Pert et al. 1975; Atweh and Kuhar 1977a,b,c).The results of extensive mapping studies can be summarized here <strong>on</strong>lybriefly. The highest levels of opiate receptors are found in areas ofthe limbic system and in the regi<strong>on</strong>s that have been implicated in thepathways involved in pain percepti<strong>on</strong>. It has been suggested that thelimbic system receptors may be involved in opiate-induced euphoria (ordysphoria) and in the affective aspects of pain percepti<strong>on</strong>.Recently, there has been c<strong>on</strong>siderable interest in the questi<strong>on</strong> of whethermultiple types of opiate receptors exist. Using classical pharmacologicalapproaches Martin and collaborators (Martin et al. 1976; Gilbert andMartin 1976) have suggested the existence of three types of receptors,named µ (for morphine), κ (for ketocyclazocine), and σ for SKF 10,047.Results from Kosterlitz' laboratory also provide evidence for the heterogeneityof opiate receptors (Lord et al. 1977). Thus, the receptorspresent in the guinea pig ileum seem to have properties distinct fromthose in the mouse vas deferens. These authors have also reportedevidence which suggests that the brain may possess at least two familiesof receptors differing in their affinity for enkephalins and for exogenousopiates.DISCOVERY OF ENDOGENOUS OPIOID PEPTIDESThe evidence that the brains of all vertebrates investigated from thehag fish to man c<strong>on</strong>tain opiate receptors led investigators to raise thequesti<strong>on</strong> why such receptors exist in the central nervous system andhave survived e<strong>on</strong>s of evoluti<strong>on</strong>. A physiological role for opiate receptorsthat c<strong>on</strong>ferred a selective advantage <strong>on</strong> the organisms seemedprobable. N<strong>on</strong>e of the known neurotransmitters or neurohorm<strong>on</strong>es wasfound to exhibit high affinity for opiate receptors, which encouraged anumber of laboratories to search for new opiatelike substances inextracts of animal brain. This search was successful first in thelaboratories of Hughes and Kosterlitz (Hughes 1975) and of Tereniusand Wahlstrom (1974). Goldstein and his collaborators (Teschemacheret al. 1975), at about the same time, reported opioid activity in extractsof pituitary glands.These studies culminated in the identificati<strong>on</strong> of the opioid substancesin extracts of pig brain by Hughes et al. (1975). They reported thatthe activity resided in two pentapeptides which they named methi<strong>on</strong>ine(Met) and leucine (Leu) enkephalin. This was c<strong>on</strong>firmed by Pasternaket al. (1975) who found the same peptides in extracts of bovine brain.The report of Hughes et al. al<strong>on</strong>g with that of the Goldstein group of304


the existence of opioid activity in the pituitary gland led Guillemin toexamine the extracts of pig hypothalami and pituitary glands. Twopolypeptides with opioid activity were found and sequenced (Ling etal. 1976). The proliferati<strong>on</strong> of endogenous peptides with opioid activitycaused the author of this paper to suggest the generic term “endorphin”(for endogenous morphinelike substance), which has been widelyaccepted. The C-terminal fragment was renamed ß-endorphin by Li(1964). while LPH 61-76 and 61-77 were named α-and γ-endorphin,respectively, by Guillemin (Rossier et al. 1977). In this paper I useendorphin as the generic term for endogenous opioid peptides of whichthe enkephalins are a subgroup. 1Endorphins look (structurally) and behave like opiates, binding to thesame brain receptors. All the endorphins, including the enkephalins,exhibit opiatelike activity when injected intraventricularly. Thisactivity includes analgesia, respiratory depressi<strong>on</strong>, and a variety ofbehavioral changes including the producti<strong>on</strong> of a rigid catat<strong>on</strong>ia. Thepharmacological effects of the enkephalins are very fleeting. Thel<strong>on</strong>ger chain endorphins are more stable and produce l<strong>on</strong>g-lived effects.Thus, analgesia due to ß-endorphin (the most potent of all the endorphinsso far found) can last three to four hours. 2 All of the resp<strong>on</strong>sesto endorphins are readily reversed by opiate antag<strong>on</strong>ists, such asnalox<strong>on</strong>e.<strong>Studies</strong> <strong>on</strong> the distributi<strong>on</strong> of ß-endorphin in the laboratories of Guillemin(Rossier et al. 1977) and Wats<strong>on</strong> (Wats<strong>on</strong> et al. 1977) have providedc<strong>on</strong>vincing evidence for a distributi<strong>on</strong> that is very different from thatof the enkephalins. This has led to the suggesti<strong>on</strong> that the centralnervous system has separate enkepha<strong>line</strong>rgic and endorphinergic neur<strong>on</strong>alsystems. ß-endorphin is present in the pituitary, where there islittle or no enkephalin, as well as in certain regi<strong>on</strong>s of the brain.Brain ß-endorphin seems to originate in a single set of neur<strong>on</strong>s locatedin the hypothalamus, with ax<strong>on</strong>s projecting throughout the brain stem.PAIN AND ITS MODULATIONSince it was work <strong>on</strong> the opiate analgesics that led to the discovery ofthe endorphins and their receptors, it was natural to postulate thatthey might be involved in pain modulati<strong>on</strong>. The fact that all centralnervous system regi<strong>on</strong>s implicated in the c<strong>on</strong>ducti<strong>on</strong> of pain impulseshave high levels of opiate receptors supports this hypothesis. Thesefindings do not prove that endogenous opioids are involved in the painpathway, but are sufficiently suggestive to encourage further testingof this hypothesis.Attempts were made to dem<strong>on</strong>strate the role of the natural opioidsystem in pain percepti<strong>on</strong> by the use of the opiate antag<strong>on</strong>ist nalox<strong>on</strong>e.It was postulated that, if receptor occupancy by endorphins was1 A c<strong>on</strong>sensus, however, has not been reached. A number of prominentinvestigators preferred to call <strong>on</strong>ly the l<strong>on</strong>ger peptides endorphins,and the shorter <strong>on</strong>es (viz., 5 amino acid residue) enkephalins, whileusing the term “opioid peptide” in the generic sense.2 A newly described pituitary peptide, dynorphin (Goldstein et al. 1979),is claimed to be even more potent than ß-endorphin.305


involved in pain modulati<strong>on</strong>, the administrati<strong>on</strong> of an opiate antag<strong>on</strong>istshould lower the threshold or exacerbate perceived pain. Such aneffect has been surprisingly difficult to dem<strong>on</strong>strate c<strong>on</strong>clusively, butat least partial evidence has been developed by several researchers,especially in the case of n<strong>on</strong>drug-induced analgesia such as that resultingfrom. electrical stimulati<strong>on</strong>, acupuncture, and placebo effect (Jacobet al. 1974; Fredericks<strong>on</strong> et al. 1977: El-Sobky et al. 1976: Grevertand Goldstein 1978; Akil et al. 1976; Hosobuchi et al. 1977; Pomeranzand Chiu 1976; Mayer et al. 1977; Peets and Pomeranz 1978; Levine etal. 1978; Goldstein and Hilgard 1975).Their results, though indirect, are supportive of the idea that theendorphin system may be involved in an endogenous pain modulati<strong>on</strong>system. Such a system is likely to be of great survival value to theorganism since it will permit it to experience pain as an importantwarning of tissue damage without the suffering of unbearable, disablingpain, except in pathological states. The importance of pain to theindividual is best dem<strong>on</strong>strated by a disease called c<strong>on</strong>genital insensitivityto pain. Individuals with this c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> are unable to feel painfrom either visceral or superficial tissue damage. This is a seriouspathology which results in a significantly shortened life expectancy.A number of laboratories including our own are currently studyingsuch patients to determine whether an abnormality in the opiate receptorendorphinsystem may play a role in this inborn error. Preliminaryreports have appeared that nalox<strong>on</strong>e causes pain-associated reflexes andelectrical discharges in such patients.NARCOTIC ADDICTIONAnother expected acti<strong>on</strong> of the endogenous opioid system is its participati<strong>on</strong>in the development of narcotic addicti<strong>on</strong>. The evidence for thisturns out to be more difficult to obtain than that for pain modulati<strong>on</strong>.All opioid peptides will produce tolerance and physical dependencewhen injected repeatedly. This does not prove that tolerance/dependencedevelops to endogenously produced and released endorphins northat these peptides and their receptors are involved in the formati<strong>on</strong>of tolerance and dependence to narcotics.A report by Simantov and Snyder (1976), for example, that enkephalinlevels are elevated in brains of tolerant rats was recently refuted byexperiments from the same laboratory (Childers et al. 1977). Theearlier work which had been d<strong>on</strong>e using a radioreceptor assay was notsupported when the much more specific radioimmunoassay was used.Recently, however, there was a report (Su et al. 1978) that the intravenousadministrati<strong>on</strong> of four milligrams of human ß-endorphin tohuman addicts led to dramatic improvement in severe abstinence syndromes.There was no euphoria and little adverse effect. In a doubleblindstudy it was found that subjects were able to distinguish morphineand ß-endorphin. After endorphin treatment they felt thirsty, dizzy,sleepy, warm, and had “a strange feeling throughout the body.” Allthese symptoms disappeared in 20 minutes, but the beneficial effects ofendorphin <strong>on</strong> the withdrawal syndrome lasted for several days. Thel<strong>on</strong>g-lasting suppressi<strong>on</strong> of especially the most severe symptoms ofabstinence (vomiting, diarrhea, tremor, and restlessness) by a singledose of ß-endorphin suggested to the authors the possibility that this306


endogenous peptide may indeed have a role in the mechanism of tolerance/dependencedevelopment to opiates.Thus, a role of the opiate receptor-endorphin system, while expectedand fervently hoped for, has not yet been established. The evidencecited is sufficiently suggestive to warrant further research in thisarea.For completeness, I should like to menti<strong>on</strong> two recent developments ofc<strong>on</strong>siderable interest for which the relati<strong>on</strong>ship to the opiate receptoris still unknown.Walter et al. (1978) reported that it was possible to suppress theabstinence syndrome when rats were withdrawn from chr<strong>on</strong>ic morphineby administrati<strong>on</strong> of the dipeptide Z-Pro-D-Leu. There was no effect<strong>on</strong> the analgesic resp<strong>on</strong>se to morphine. The mechanism of this phenomen<strong>on</strong>is not understood.Based <strong>on</strong> the abundant literature which seems to implicate catecholaminesin the acti<strong>on</strong>s of opiates, Gold et al. (1978) treated human heroinaddicts with cl<strong>on</strong>idine. In a double-blind, placebo-c<strong>on</strong>trolled study,cl<strong>on</strong>idine eliminated objective signs and subjective symptoms of opiatewithdrawal for four to six hours in all addicts. In an open pilotstudy, the same patients did well while taking cl<strong>on</strong>idine for <strong>on</strong>e week.All of the patients had been addicted to opiates for six to ten yearsand had been <strong>on</strong> methad<strong>on</strong>e for six to 60 m<strong>on</strong>ths at the time of thestudy.CONCLUSIONThe discovery of opiate receptors and their supposed endogenousligands, the endorphins, has kindled the excitement and imaginati<strong>on</strong> ofmany scientists and, through ample coverage in the news media, of thegeneral public as well. Hopes have been raised that these findingsmay c<strong>on</strong>tribute to the soluti<strong>on</strong> of a number of human pathologies rangingfrom intractable pain to mental disease.There is not yet clear-cut evidence for the involvement of the opiatereceptor in any human disease, but the evidence is sufficiently suggestiveto encourage much further research in many competent laboratoriesand hospitals.There is an interesting difference between this area of research andthose involving receptors for other horm<strong>on</strong>es and neurotransmitters.In the other cases the endogenous ligand was discovered and knownfor some time before a receptor was postulated, searched for, andidentified. The opiate field began with the identificati<strong>on</strong> of a drugreceptor. The proof that such a receptor existed led to the searchfor endogenous ligands for the receptors and to the identificati<strong>on</strong> of anumber of peptides with opioid activity. This approach is now beingapplied to other drug receptors, where it is felt unlikely that theirexistence anticipated the relatively recent development of the drug. Acase in point is the discovery of specific binding sites for the tranquilizerbenzodiazepine. Many laboratories are presently engaged in asearch for the endogenous ligand for this receptor. Does the bodyproduce its own tranquilizing substance? Is it <strong>on</strong>e of the substanceswe are already familiar with or is it a substance yet to be identified?307


This approach might c<strong>on</strong>ceivably be generalizable to other receptorsfor exogenous substances, such as drugs, viruses, and toxins. Inthose cases in which a selective advantage to the organism is notevident, a search for an endogenous ligand and a physiological role forthe receptor might prove worthwhile.It should be remembered that the opiate receptor field is <strong>on</strong>ly sevenyears old and fundamental informati<strong>on</strong> regarding the physiological roleof the endorphins and of the receptor is still missing. A real understandingof the role of this receptor-ligand system in human diseasemay have to await the elucidati<strong>on</strong> of its functi<strong>on</strong>s in normal animals andhumans.308


PART 2THEORYCOMPONENTS


CONTENTS– Part 2310


CONTENTS—Part 2—C<strong>on</strong>tinued311


Initiati<strong>on</strong>PERSONALITY-DEFICIENCY THEORY (p. 4)Ausubel<strong>Drug</strong> abuse is generally initiated as a result of an individual’s socialinvolvement with drug-using age-mates: The adolescent who is motivati<strong>on</strong>allyimmature, in additi<strong>on</strong> to comm<strong>on</strong>ly having ready access todrugs and living in a sociocultural milieu attitudinally tolerant of druguse, in c<strong>on</strong>trast to his or her n<strong>on</strong>addicted, motivati<strong>on</strong>ally maturefellows, experiences the tremendous adjustive value of the drug <strong>on</strong>ceovercoming its initial unpleasant c<strong>on</strong>sequences or side effects, such asnausea or vomiting. After about 10 to 14 days of multiple daily usageshe or he becomes physiologically addicted and develops abstinence orwithdrawal symptoms 6 to 12 hours after involuntary disc<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> ofthe drug.ADDICTION-TO-PLEASURE THEORY (p. 246)BejerotInitiati<strong>on</strong> into the use of addicting drugs may occur al<strong>on</strong>g at least fourmain routes (Bejerot 1975), which also have some byways.The Therapeutic RouteOpiate dependence has been a dreaded complicati<strong>on</strong> of medical treatmentfor centuries. From the time that physicians learned to handle opiates,however, opiate dependence of a therapeutic type has become rare(apart from cancer cases and patients in terminal treatment).Nowadays there are many pers<strong>on</strong>s in industrial countries who havebecome str<strong>on</strong>gly dependent up<strong>on</strong> sedatives and hypnotics during medicaltreatment. This group has a large number of characteristics whichhave been well defined by Brill (1968) and Allgulander (1978), am<strong>on</strong>gothers. The patients usually feel ashamed of and guilty about theirdrug dependence and try to hide it from even their nearest relatives.The tendency to spread this form of addicti<strong>on</strong> to others is thereforevery smalI, almost n<strong>on</strong>existent. The frequency of these therapeutic313


accidents seems to be intimately related to the density of physicians insociety.The Professi<strong>on</strong>al RouteMedical staff, and particularly physicians, run a c<strong>on</strong>siderable risk ofaddicti<strong>on</strong>. Pescor (1942) has estimated the risk in different countriesto be between 20 and 100 times that of the normal populati<strong>on</strong>. Pharmacistsand veterinary surge<strong>on</strong>s are said to have a far lower rate thanphysicians and nurses, indicating that intimate familiarity with theeffects of the drugs <strong>on</strong> humans, coupled with the ready availability ofthe drugs, seems to be necessary.The Epidemic RouteIn epidemics of drug abuse, the intoxicant is not socially accepted.Initiati<strong>on</strong> occurs almost without excepti<strong>on</strong>, from established abusers t<strong>on</strong>ovices, in a densely branched network (Bejerot 1965; Alarc<strong>on</strong> 1969).The spread occurs in intimate relati<strong>on</strong>ships between friends, sexualpartners, etc. (Brown et al. 1976). and it is str<strong>on</strong>gly c<strong>on</strong>nected to thefirst year of abuse, “the h<strong>on</strong>eymo<strong>on</strong> of drug addicti<strong>on</strong>.”The Cultural RouteIn the cultural or endemic addicti<strong>on</strong>s, the intoxicant is socially accepted(alcohol in the Christian part of the world, cannabis in some Muslimareas , coca am<strong>on</strong>g some South American Indian tribes, etc.).The frequency of addicti<strong>on</strong> of a cultural type varies greatly in differentsocieties. Lewin (1924) states that the whole adult populati<strong>on</strong> in thetribes descended from the Incas are cocainists. The other extreme isfound in Jewish cultural circles, in which for thousands of years nocases of alcoholism were known, in spite of the fact that Jews, inc<strong>on</strong>trast to Muslims, are allowed to drink alcohol, and although theJews are am<strong>on</strong>g the most persecuted people in the history of theworld. <strong>On</strong>ly as a result of secularizati<strong>on</strong> during the last severalgenerati<strong>on</strong>s have cases of alcoholism begun to appear in this populati<strong>on</strong>.DISRUPTIVE ENVIRONMENT THEORY (p. 76)CheinA pers<strong>on</strong> can take his first shot of a drug at almost any age, and fora wide range of reas<strong>on</strong>s, but in our studies of juvenile males we foundthat the majority did not begin their experimentati<strong>on</strong> with drugs untilthey were in their late teens, frequently not until they had stoppedattending school. However, 16 seemed to be the most comm<strong>on</strong> age.We found that juvenile users who become addicts showed evidence ofdeep pers<strong>on</strong>ality disturbances prior to the <strong>on</strong>set of drug use, and thatthe vast majority of them live in the most deprived slum areas of thecity. While not all juvenile addicts have been delinquent prior to theiraddicti<strong>on</strong>, they share with other kinds of delinquents a special orientati<strong>on</strong>to life, <strong>on</strong>e which c<strong>on</strong>sists of general pessimism, unhappiness, anda sense of futility <strong>on</strong> the <strong>on</strong>e hand, and mistrust, negativism, anddefiance <strong>on</strong> the other. These attitudes stem from a family life inwhich the parents are of low socioec<strong>on</strong>omic status and have little hope314


of a better future for either themselves or their children; in whichthere is a lack of love and support for the children and no clearstandards of behavior, with inc<strong>on</strong>sistent applicati<strong>on</strong> of rewards andpunishments, and in which there is usually no male to whom the boycan relate in a warm and sustained fashi<strong>on</strong>. Moreover, the parentsare usually distrustful of representatives of society such as teachersor social workers.The c<strong>on</strong>sequence of the c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s just out<strong>line</strong>d is that the boy growsup with no sense of identity, no belief in his own abilities, and nofaith in the future. When he is faced with the resp<strong>on</strong>sibilities ofapproaching adulthood he finds himself unable to cope and, surroundedas he is by others who use drugs, he begins to experiment with themhimself.INCOMPLETE MOURNING THEORY (p. 83)ColemanThe misuse of drugs is viewed as a structural or functi<strong>on</strong>al imbalancein the family; it is not a problem experienced by a single individual ina family (Steinglass 1976). Thus, the initiati<strong>on</strong> of heroin use cannotbe ascribed to a <strong>line</strong>ar, cause-and-effect model. Rather, heroin abuseis part of a cycle in which each family member’s behavior affects andis affected by another member’s behavior in reciprocal fashi<strong>on</strong>. AsHaley (1973, 1976) and Hoffman (1976) suggest, it is the sequence ofinteracti<strong>on</strong>s and behaviors which serves a homeostatic functi<strong>on</strong> for thefamily; the drug abuse is merely embedded in a host of other acti<strong>on</strong>s.The incomplete loss theory views drug addicti<strong>on</strong> as a means of copingwith a traumatic family experience. It is much like Bowen’s (1978)“emoti<strong>on</strong>al shock wave,” which he describes as a network of underground“after shocks” of serious life events that occur anywhere inthe extended family system in the m<strong>on</strong>ths or years following a seriousemoti<strong>on</strong>al family event. He feels that these usually occur after thedeath or threatened death of a significant family member but suggeststhat they could follow other types of losses. Bowen relates the reacti<strong>on</strong>to a denial of emoti<strong>on</strong>al dependence am<strong>on</strong>g family members and feelsthat it most often occurs in families with a significant degree of deniedemoti<strong>on</strong>al “fusi<strong>on</strong>.” He illustrates with a case example of a grandmother’sthreatened death from cancer surgery, followed by a two-yearperiod of a chain of catastrophes am<strong>on</strong>g her children and their families.Reacti<strong>on</strong>s included drinking, depressi<strong>on</strong>, automobile accidents, delinquency,and business failure.The initial experience with a drug is apt to be associated with age orstage of development. Although the family’s sequential interacti<strong>on</strong>s arehistorically unchanged, the first act of drug experimentati<strong>on</strong> generallyarises during adolescence (Stant<strong>on</strong> 1977a, 1979d). Like acne or otherage-related phenomena, the predisposing factors have l<strong>on</strong>g been present.<strong>Drug</strong> use is, again, an integral comp<strong>on</strong>ent of the family’s relati<strong>on</strong>shippatterns and feedback system and its initiati<strong>on</strong> cannot be ascribed to asingular or direct causal factor.315


LEARNED BEHAVIOR THEORY (p. 191)Frederick<strong>Drug</strong> use is initiated primarily as a functi<strong>on</strong> of the destructive comp<strong>on</strong>entsin the pers<strong>on</strong>ality (Pd) and the risk-taking aspects that predominatein the life of the individual at the time of the <strong>on</strong>set of substanceabuse or addicti<strong>on</strong> (Rd). While there is no drug abusive or addictivepers<strong>on</strong>ality, per se, it is not unlikely that those with weaker, dependentpers<strong>on</strong>ality traits may be more inc<strong>line</strong>d toward problems of drug usagethan other pers<strong>on</strong>s without such traits. Moreover, individuals withrebellious tendencies are also likely to express a greater affinitytoward drug use, particularly at certain points in their lives. Thereas<strong>on</strong> why drug use occurs at a particular point in an individual’s lifedepends up<strong>on</strong> cultural influences and drug availability. These comp<strong>on</strong>entsare particularly related to those risk factors involved that are ofa deleterious or destructive nature. Of course, some individuals movefrom alcohol abuse to drugs as a result of these same factors. Arbitrarily,the numerical values already cited may be employed here to illustratehow the counterproductive pers<strong>on</strong>ality factors and risk factors can beincreased and, thereby, can alter the ratio in the directi<strong>on</strong> of initiati<strong>on</strong>of drug abuse/addicti<strong>on</strong>. The basic formula, described earlier, states:When the destructive factors (Pd) and (Rd) become affected, theexisting equal balance of 50-percent probability changes as follows:The likelihood of drug abuse occurring has now increased markedly,since a value of 1.0 represents the point at which it will unequivocallydevelop. The next reinforcement of (Pd) or (Rd) or a diminuti<strong>on</strong> inthe strength of <strong>on</strong>e of the c<strong>on</strong>structive factors will readily bring aboutdrug addicti<strong>on</strong> or abuse.COGNITIVE CONTROL THEORY (p. 8)GoldThe CAP c<strong>on</strong>trol theory does not specifically address the issue ofinitiati<strong>on</strong> of drug use. In today’s society almost every<strong>on</strong>e is exposedto and experiments with some drugs, including alcohol. The drug ofpreference is likely to be a functi<strong>on</strong> of availability, frequency of usein the individual’s subculture, and affordability. <strong>Drug</strong> experimentati<strong>on</strong>is not seen as a sign of psychopathology or pers<strong>on</strong>ality weakness.316


BAD-HABIT THEORY (p. 12)GoodwinAvailability, peer pressure, rebelliousness, family attitudes, andpossibly even psychiatric symptoms such as anxiety and depressi<strong>on</strong>may c<strong>on</strong>tribute to initiati<strong>on</strong> of drug use. Based <strong>on</strong> other studies,,antisocial behavior in adolescence is an important predictor in initiati<strong>on</strong>.My theory would indicate that the genetically predisposed pers<strong>on</strong> wouldmore rapidly be initiated into alcohol abuse (and, by inference, otherdrug abuse) and that the switch from use to abuse would occur veryrapidly. We have some data <strong>on</strong> this. So-called “familial alcoholics” areyounger than n<strong>on</strong>familial alcoholics when they start having troublesfrom alcohol.MULTIPLE MODELS THEORY (p. 18)GorsuchGorsuch has derived three interactive models for the initiati<strong>on</strong> of illicitdrug use: the n<strong>on</strong>socialized drug users model, the prodrug socializati<strong>on</strong>model, and the iatrogenic model. The first model describes the propensityfor drug use in the n<strong>on</strong>socialized pers<strong>on</strong>, who, without internalizednorms against drug use, will be more susceptible to it. The prodrugsocializati<strong>on</strong> model is c<strong>on</strong>cerned with those people in whose societydrug use is sancti<strong>on</strong>ed. This applies to societies in which drugs arepart of religious or other cultural rituals and to groups whose membersuse drugs for licit purposes. The iatrogenic model pertains to individualswho have been introduced to a drug in a medical setting. Thesepeople may seek the drug’s beneficial effects again when they nol<strong>on</strong>ger have the original medical need.It is apparent in all these models that availability of illicit drugs is aprimary prerequisite to initial use. The n<strong>on</strong>socialized individual generallyseems to have little real drive to seek out drugs and would beparticularly unlikely to do so if drugs were difficult to obtain. However,the iatrogenic and prodrug subculture users are more likely toseek out a drug regardless of its availability, the former perceiving areal and str<strong>on</strong>g need for it and the latter with numerous models fordoing so.In spite of the fact that the usual sources of illicit drugs are throughpeers, peer interventi<strong>on</strong> has high potential as a preventi<strong>on</strong> measure.If norms of the peer group are antidrug, then the n<strong>on</strong>socialized individualshave little chance to partake of the drugs and will avoid initialillicit drug experiences. However, this approach is more problematicwhere there is a prodrug subculture, for attempts to suppress thatsubculture could be expected to solidify the group “against the comm<strong>on</strong>enemy.” But methods which encourage development of antidrug valueswithout suppressing the peer group, such as those used by Carney(1972) and the YMCA (Corsuch, in press), are effective. For thisreas<strong>on</strong>, parenting agents play a crucial role in preventing initial druguse. If they socialize the individual into the traditi<strong>on</strong>al, anti-illicitdrugculture, then the individual is much less likely to have an initial317


drug experience regardless of availability. From a l<strong>on</strong>g-term perspective,this is probably the most effective interventi<strong>on</strong> technique.However, it most likely involves a greater depth of understanding ofparenting techniques and of teaching such techniques than is currentlyavailable. <strong>On</strong>e aspect which could be stressed to parents is implicitprodrug socializati<strong>on</strong> through parental use of drugs. Those parentscan be encouraged to discriminate between the drugs of particularimportance to their subculture and the illicit use of drugs.Socializing agents other than parents can also be important. Attitudestoward drug abuse can be readily changed both in school (Carney1972) and in other settings. The evidence <strong>on</strong> religious membership(Linden and Currie 1977) suggests that this is a powerful force. inadditi<strong>on</strong>, values clarificati<strong>on</strong> programs in YMCA settings have also beenfound to alter attitudes toward drug abuse (Gorsuch, in press).EXISTENTIAL THEORY (p. 24)GreavesInitiati<strong>on</strong> of drug use is not seen as a significant issue by Greavesinsofar as numerous hypotheses, individually and collectively, seem toadequately explain initiati<strong>on</strong>. These include, but are not limited to,peer pressure, pursuit of novelty, antisocial experimentati<strong>on</strong>, perceivedstatus, curiosity, escape, and sexual stimulati<strong>on</strong>.ADAPTATIONAL THEORY (p. 195)HendinFrom an adaptive standpoint, initiati<strong>on</strong> of drug use, that is, determinati<strong>on</strong>of the circumstances surrounding the individual’s first use ofdrugs, has been overly emphasized, particularly with marijuana andalcohol, which are widely accepted am<strong>on</strong>g and available to teenagers.Much of the emphasis <strong>on</strong> initiati<strong>on</strong> derives from the implicati<strong>on</strong> that <strong>on</strong>ehas begun a process, and in so doing, has heightened the danger ofexcess, so that the way to deal with the problem is to stop it before itstarts. This is akin to believing that loss of virginity leads to promiscuity.The resp<strong>on</strong>se to initiating experiences is a more critical andinformative variable. For a small percentage that resp<strong>on</strong>se is s<strong>on</strong>egative that it leads to rejecti<strong>on</strong> of further drug use.Since drug abuse usually grows out of adaptive difficulties, <strong>on</strong>e wouldexpect that the earlier in life the individual finds it necessary to usedrugs, the greater the impairment is likely to be. And, in general,the younger the age at which an individual begins drug abuse, themore likely it is that he or she is a disturbed, vulnerable pers<strong>on</strong>.The preadolescent (9 to 12 years old) drug abusers seen in the urbanghetto are the most tragic illustrati<strong>on</strong>. Initiati<strong>on</strong> in early adolescenceusually reflects difficulties in the changing relati<strong>on</strong>ship to the familythat adolescence brings. Even though these difficulties often stemfrom early childhood experiences, the individual who can deal with lifethrough adolescence without large amounts of drugs has a better318


chance of not being destroyed by drug abuse even if he or she becomesinvolved later.SOCIAL DEVIANCE THEORY (p. 90)HillThere appear to be several powerful interacting factors which determinethe vulnerability of the social deviant to initial addicti<strong>on</strong>. The first,which has been discussed at some length by others, is that suchbehavioral equipment is found most frequently in the underprivilegedand slum areas in which opiates and other drug supplies have “high”availability (Chein and Rosenfeld 1957; Cohen 1955; Clausen 1957) andin which both narcotic addicti<strong>on</strong> and alcoholism are comm<strong>on</strong>. Theenvir<strong>on</strong>mental c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s which produce the deviant in these areas alsoprovide more ready access to opiates than in the larger society, andwith regard to both opiates and alcohol, provide a greater degree ofexposure to models of excessive use. But, to a more limited degree,this would appear to hold also for the social deviant in all societalstrata. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, lack of social c<strong>on</strong>trols (shared resp<strong>on</strong>ses) appears todetermine the degree of acceptability, to the deviant, of experimentati<strong>on</strong>with drugs as well as with other forms of unusual behavior (Chein andRosenfeld 1957). Although a certain degree of privati<strong>on</strong> and socialisolati<strong>on</strong> in the “fringe” areas are c<strong>on</strong>tributing factors to social devianceas well as to addicti<strong>on</strong>, they appear to be neither necessary nor sufficientcausal antecedents of such behavior.The following appear to be the chief factors which produce the specialvulnerability of social deviants to addicti<strong>on</strong>. They are deficient indaily pursuits which are reinforced by and bring satisfacti<strong>on</strong> to thelarger society; they are not deterred from unusual behavior by counteranxiety,which in the “mature” adult can be partially identified asinhibiti<strong>on</strong>s; because of these deficiencies they are especially susceptibleto short-term satisfacti<strong>on</strong>s, and if drugs are available they can themselvesrapidly manipulate their pers<strong>on</strong>al state.<strong>On</strong>e of the most difficult problems in the etiology of the addicti<strong>on</strong>s,and <strong>on</strong>e which apparently has a direct c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong> with specific effectsof drugs, is c<strong>on</strong>cerned with the use of a particular agent when othersare equally available. Alcohol and opiates, although having someeffects in comm<strong>on</strong>, perhaps even some comm<strong>on</strong> effects <strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>flict andanxiety, frequently produce diametrically opposite acti<strong>on</strong>s. It thusseems apparent that alcohol and opiates differentially but specificallyalter the probability of occurrence of particular classes of resp<strong>on</strong>ses.Briefly, in this c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>, it is assumed for the general case that thebehavioral equipment of the individual is composed of specific resp<strong>on</strong>sesor resp<strong>on</strong>se patterns which have certain probabilities of occurrence(strength) in any given situati<strong>on</strong>. Since different resp<strong>on</strong>ses of theindividual differ in strength, they form a resp<strong>on</strong>se hierarchy for agiven situati<strong>on</strong> ranging from the resp<strong>on</strong>se which is most likely to thatwhich is least likely to occur (Hull 1934; Miller and Dollard 1941). ASan organizing principle in research <strong>on</strong> psychopharmacology, and for itsapplicability to the addicti<strong>on</strong>s, it is hypothesized that drugs rearrangethe individual’s resp<strong>on</strong>se hierarchy in ways which are specific for aparticular drug and for a given situati<strong>on</strong>.319


BIOLOGICAL RHYTHM THEORY (p. 262)HochhauserThe chr<strong>on</strong>obiological c<strong>on</strong>trol theory suggests that an individual whoperceives himself or herself in a helpless situati<strong>on</strong>, in terms either ofbehavioral or internal events, may resort to drug use in an effort toachieve some degree of perceived c<strong>on</strong>trol over these experiences,especially when other n<strong>on</strong>drug alternatives are not available or havebeen found ineffective. In summarizing what is known about theseearly drug experiences, Gorsuch and Butler (1976a) suggest thatinitial drug use may occur (1) to resp<strong>on</strong>d to a state of physical pain;(2) to deal with mental anguish; (3) to provide relief from boredomthrough sensati<strong>on</strong> seeking. Future research must focus <strong>on</strong> the sourcesof the physical/mental pain and how a particular strategy is selected inorder to cope with such pain.INTERACTIVE FRAMEWORK (p. 95)Huba/Wingard/BentierIn our current c<strong>on</strong>cepti<strong>on</strong>, we believe that initiati<strong>on</strong> of drug use,particularly when it occurs during adolescence, is almost entirelyderived from self-perceived behavioral pressure resulting from theintimate support system. This support system plays a role in movingthe individual to drug use through peer values, models, and reinforcers,and <strong>on</strong>e of inadequacy in reinforcing alternative, healthy behaviorsand goals that would inhibit susceptibility to drug use. The pers<strong>on</strong>alitysystem plays a much smaller role, with such dimensi<strong>on</strong>s as extroversi<strong>on</strong>,leadership or aut<strong>on</strong>omy strivings, and rebelliousness needs seekingfulfillment in drug-taking behavior. This manifestati<strong>on</strong> is particularlytrue when the majority culture defines drug taking as illegal anddangerous, in which case we posit that a negative psychological cyclemay be instigated with initiati<strong>on</strong> into use. The “backlash” effect iscaptured in the current model by the reciprocal arrows from perceivedbehavioral pressure to pers<strong>on</strong>ality (figure 1, p. 96). The “backlash”may also be exacerbated when the individual’s felt pressure not to usedrugs is communicated to members of the intimate culture who arguec<strong>on</strong>vincingly that drug taking is desirable. It should also be notedthat financial resources may preclude the initiati<strong>on</strong> of certain forms ofdrug taking, although this is unlikely within current youth cultures.We do not think that organismic status plays any major role during theinitiati<strong>on</strong> stage since the individual has had no direct experience withthe mood-altering properties of the drugs. To the extent that individualsattribute their initiati<strong>on</strong> of use to pharmacological properties,we may infer that they have been educated in drug effects by eitherthe intimate culture or sources in the sociocultural influence system.320


DRUG SUBCULTURES THEORY (p. 110)Johns<strong>on</strong>Initiati<strong>on</strong> to drug use was studied carefully in the 1970s (Johnst<strong>on</strong> etal. 1978; Jessor and Jessor 1977; Jessor 1979; Kandel 1975, 1976,1978b). All studies show that initiati<strong>on</strong> to marijuana is critical to theinitiati<strong>on</strong> to other drugs (except alcohol). Three major factors havebeen identified in the initiati<strong>on</strong> of marijuana: (1) prior use of alcohol,(2) predisposing factors (sex, family cohesi<strong>on</strong>, political c<strong>on</strong>servatism/leftism, ethnicity, religiosity, etc.), and (3) friends’ use of marijuana.Borrowing from reference-group (Sherif and Sherif 1964), differentialassociati<strong>on</strong>(Sutherland 1939), and social-learning (Akers 1977) theory,drug-subculture theory hypothesizes that the predisposing factorsindicate the influence of the parent culture up<strong>on</strong> youths; parent culturevalues may also influence the choice of friends and patterns of friendshipchoice. The activities of friendship cliques are also str<strong>on</strong>gly influencedby the peer culture. Many peer groups, following peer culture valuesand c<strong>on</strong>duct norms, expect group members to engage in various formsof unc<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al behavior of which cigarette and alcohol use areusually begun earliest. In additi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>on</strong>e or more peer group membersmay, through c<strong>on</strong>tact with other friends, by following examples givenin the mass media, or by learning via other informal communicati<strong>on</strong>(Fine and Kleinman 1979), also orient themselves toward the cannabissubculture and begin use. As the proporti<strong>on</strong> of the peer group orother friends (or other reference group) using marijuana increases,the probability that any individual member will begin using marijuanaincreases steadily (Kandel 1978b). N<strong>on</strong>users may be directly pressuredby friends (“Are you afraid to try pot? It’s harmless and gives agreat high”) or indirectly pressured because of the belief that mostof their friends are using marijuana (even though they may not be) orthe feeling that use is expected by their friends.The actual initiati<strong>on</strong> to marijuana use almost always occurs am<strong>on</strong>g relativelyclose friends, from whom the n<strong>on</strong>user learns the smoking techniquesand how to define the sensati<strong>on</strong>s of intoxicati<strong>on</strong> as a pleasurableand valuable experience (Becker 1963; Orcutt 1978; Akers et al.1979). Thus, cannabis subcultural values and c<strong>on</strong>duct norms aremediated through the peer group. The precise order of events leadingto marijuana use probably varies from case to case, and the causalorder, if it exists, has yet to be untangled.Initiati<strong>on</strong> to the n<strong>on</strong>medical use of drugs other than marijuana generallyoccurs after marijuana initiati<strong>on</strong> and subsequent use. Initiati<strong>on</strong> to thecannabis subculture (and to the alcohol misuse subculture) teaches thecritical value comm<strong>on</strong> to all drug subcultures--the desire to get “high”via the c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong> of substances. After this value is learned, euphoricexperiences c<strong>on</strong>tinue to reinforce it, and, as a result, other substancesare frequently and easily redefined as potential sources of enjoyment.The neophyte user may also be expected to initiate the use of <strong>on</strong>e ormore other substances, to which he or she may be introduced byfriends or other associates, thus becoming involved in the multiple-drugusesubculture (Single et al. 1974).Initiati<strong>on</strong> to the heroin-injecti<strong>on</strong> subculture is str<strong>on</strong>gly influenced byhaving heroin-using friends that may have been gained via extensiveinvolvement in selling marijuana and other drugs (Johns<strong>on</strong> 1973) andafter relatively extensive use of cannabis and other drugs (O’D<strong>on</strong>nelland Clayt<strong>on</strong> 1979).321


DEVELOPMENTAL STAGES THEORY (p. 120)KandelThe findings that different social psychological factors predict adolescentinitiati<strong>on</strong> into different stages of drug use provide evidence for theexistence of stages. We have combined the noti<strong>on</strong> that adolescent druguse involves sequential stages with a l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal research design inwhich the populati<strong>on</strong> at risk for initiati<strong>on</strong> into each of the stages couldbe clearly identified. This has allowed us to assess the relative importanceof various factors in predicting initial transiti<strong>on</strong>s into varioustypes of drug behaviors. The three sequential stages of adolescentdrug use are hard liquor, marijuana, and other illicit drugs. Each offour clusters of predictor variables--parental influences, peer influences,adolescent involvement in various behaviors, and adolescent beliefs andvalues--and single predictors within each cluster assume differentialimportance for each stage of drug behavior. Prior involvements in avariety of activities, such as minor delinquency and use of cigarettes,beer, and wine, are most important for predicting hard liquor use.Adolescents’ beliefs and values favorable to the use of marijuana andassociati<strong>on</strong> with marijuana-using peers are the str<strong>on</strong>gest predictors ofinitiati<strong>on</strong> into marijuana. Poor relati<strong>on</strong>s with parents, feelings ofdepressi<strong>on</strong>, and exposure to drug-using peers are most important forpredicting initiati<strong>on</strong> into illicit drugs other than marijuana.Thus, at the earliest levels of involvement, adolescents who haveengaged in a number of minor delinquent or deviant activities, whoenjoy high levels of sociability with their peers, and who are exposedto peers and parents who drink start to drink themselves. The relati<strong>on</strong>shipwith parental use of hard liquor suggests that these youths learndrinking patterns from their parents. The use of marijuana is precededby acceptance of a cluster of beliefs and values that are favorable tomarijuana use and in oppositi<strong>on</strong> to many standards upheld by adults,by involvement in a peer envir<strong>on</strong>ment in which marijuana is used, andby participati<strong>on</strong> in the same minor forms of deviant behavior thatprecede the use of hard liquor. By comparis<strong>on</strong>, use of illicit drugsother than marijuana is preceded by poor relati<strong>on</strong>ships with parents,by exposure to parents and to peers who themselves use a variety oflegal, medical, and illegal drugs, by psychological distress, and by aseries of pers<strong>on</strong>al characteristics somewhat more deviant than thosethat characterize the novice marijuana or hard liquor user.SELF-DEROGATION THEORY (p. 128)Kaplan<strong>Drug</strong> use/abuse patterns are am<strong>on</strong>g alternative deviant patterns adoptedin resp<strong>on</strong>se to intense self-rejecting attitudes resulting from a historyof being unable to forestall or assuage the self-devaluing implicati<strong>on</strong>sof experiences in normative membership groups (family, school, peers,etc.).By virtue of the (actual) associati<strong>on</strong> between past membership groupexperiences and the development of intensely distressful negativeself-attitudes, the pers<strong>on</strong> loses motivati<strong>on</strong> to c<strong>on</strong>form to and becomes322


motivated to deviate from membership group patterns. Simultaneously,the unfulfilled self-esteem motive prompts the subject to seek alternative(that is, deviant) resp<strong>on</strong>se patterns which offer hope of reducing theexperience of negative (and increasing the experience of positive)self-attitudes.Which of several deviant patterns is adopted will be a functi<strong>on</strong> of thepers<strong>on</strong>’s history of experiences influencing the visibility and subjectiveevaluati<strong>on</strong> of the self-enhancing/self-devaluing potential of the pattern(s)in questi<strong>on</strong>.Given the predispositi<strong>on</strong> to adopt some form of deviance, an illicit druguse pattern (rather than patterns of theft, interpers<strong>on</strong>al violence,suicide, etc.) would be adopted insofar as (perhaps due to the availabilityof the drug) the behavior was apparent in the envir<strong>on</strong>ment, thepers<strong>on</strong> did not anticipate adverse c<strong>on</strong>sequences (e.g., loss of c<strong>on</strong>trol,incarcerati<strong>on</strong>), and did anticipate self-enhancing outcomes (e.g.,acceptance by a positive reference group, anesthetizati<strong>on</strong> of selfrejectingfeelings).EGO/SELF THEORY (p. 29)KhantzianMy work with drug dependency has focused <strong>on</strong> individuals in whom theinitiati<strong>on</strong> of drug use progressed to drug dependency. Therefore, myunderstanding of the initiati<strong>on</strong> and subsequent drug use patterns hasbeen necessarily influenced by my experience which involves moreextreme cases. Nevertheless, taken from the psychoanalytic perspective,the meaning, causes, and c<strong>on</strong>sequences of drug use can be understoodbest by c<strong>on</strong>sidering how the pers<strong>on</strong>ality organizati<strong>on</strong> (particularly egopsychological and self structures) of an individual interacts withenvir<strong>on</strong>mental influences and drug effects. Such an approach canaccount for and explain both more benign, self-limited degrees of druginvolvement, and the more malignant patterns of misuse and dependency.I will focus <strong>on</strong> the latter instances where initiati<strong>on</strong> has led to moreextreme patterns of involvement and dependency.The nature of the ego and self disturbances of certain individualsleaves them more pr<strong>on</strong>e to begin drug use. The nature of these egoand self disturbances is related to failures or deficiencies in drive/affect defense, self-esteem, and self-care. Having failed to developadequate internal mechanisms for coping with internal drives andemoti<strong>on</strong>s, the addicti<strong>on</strong>-pr<strong>on</strong>e individual is c<strong>on</strong>stantly involved with arange of behaviors and activities, including drug use, in the externalworld to serve the needs for a sense of well-being, security, andpleasure. Shaky or rigid defenses and low self-esteem cause him orher to turn more exclusively to the external envir<strong>on</strong>ment for thesatisfacti<strong>on</strong> of such needs and wants. Wurmser (1974) has referred tothis predispositi<strong>on</strong> as an “addictive search” and has expanded eloquently<strong>on</strong> how such predispositi<strong>on</strong>s are part of the necessary and sufficientcauses that lead to addicti<strong>on</strong>. It is the c<strong>on</strong>stant search and hungerfor satisfacti<strong>on</strong>s from <strong>on</strong>e’s envir<strong>on</strong>ment interacting with the moreincidental and adventitious influences such as exposure to drugs,availability, and peer-group pressures that determine the initiati<strong>on</strong> ofand experimentati<strong>on</strong> with drug use.323


The tendency toward initiati<strong>on</strong> and use of drugs of dependence isfurther compounded by an impairment in a specific ego functi<strong>on</strong> called“self-care.” Whereas most people would be apprehensive or fearful ofthe dangers of using such drugs, or might be equally apprehensiveabout the appeal of such drugs, we have been impressed repeatedlythat such worries and fears were never c<strong>on</strong>sidered by drug addictsand that the eventualities of the drugs’ seducti<strong>on</strong> or dangers werenever (or insufficiently) anticipated. Such problems are related toself-care (ego) functi<strong>on</strong>s that are impaired, deficient, or absent in somany of the addicts we see. The problems with self-care and regulati<strong>on</strong>are apparent in their past histories (predating their addicti<strong>on</strong>) by ahigh incidence of preventable medical and dental problems, accidents,fights, violent behavior, and delinquent behavioral problems. Theirimpaired self-care functi<strong>on</strong>s are also evident in relati<strong>on</strong> to their drug/alcohol problems, where despite obvious deteriorati<strong>on</strong> and imminentdanger as a result of their substance use, there is little evidence offear, anxiety, or realistic assessment about their substance involvement.<strong>On</strong>e might correctly argue that in this latter instance, the lack ofself-care is sec<strong>on</strong>dary to regressi<strong>on</strong> as a result of prol<strong>on</strong>ged substanceuse. Although this is probably quite true, we have been impressedwith the presence and persistence of these described tendencies insuch individuals both prior to becoming addicted and after becomingdetoxified and stabilized (Khantzian 1978).GENERAL ADDICTION THEORY (p. 34)LindesmithSince the theory is c<strong>on</strong>cerned with the development of the characteristiccraving of the addict, it does not purport to explain initialuse. The first experience may occur in a wide variety of ways, undermany different kinds of circumstances, and from a c<strong>on</strong>siderable rangeof motives. It may result from a doctor’s prescripti<strong>on</strong> and have nothingto do with the motivati<strong>on</strong>s of the recipient, who may not even be awareof the nature of the medicati<strong>on</strong>. Most c<strong>on</strong>temporary American addictsacquired their initial experience with heroin through associati<strong>on</strong> withaddicts who obtained the drug from the illicit market. The situati<strong>on</strong>during the 19th century was quite different; initial use then ordinarilyoccurred in c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong> with medical practice or self-medicati<strong>on</strong> withpatent medicines and opiate products that were widely available indrug stores. The situati<strong>on</strong>s that lead to the first use of an opiate-typedrug vary widely in different parts of the world and tend to changewith the passing of time.It seems probable, c<strong>on</strong>sidering that opiates c<strong>on</strong>stituted the primetherapeutic agent of medicine for close to 2,000 years and that morphineis still perhaps the most valuable analgesic available to doctors, that ac<strong>on</strong>siderable percentage of the adult populati<strong>on</strong> has experienced atleast <strong>on</strong>e dose of an opiate. Initial use, therefore, poses not <strong>on</strong>etheoretical problem but a number of quite different problems. Sincemost pers<strong>on</strong>s who have had the initial experience do not go <strong>on</strong> tobecome addicted, the significance of initial use is that it may be thoughtof as the beginning of a process which may result in addicti<strong>on</strong>, withsome kinds of initial use more likely than others to have this effect.324


HYPERACTIVE ADOLESCENTS THEORY (p. 132)L<strong>on</strong>eyInitiati<strong>on</strong> is experimentati<strong>on</strong> with or initial recreati<strong>on</strong>al use of thosesubstances (cigarettes, alcohol, marijuana) early in the sequence thatultimately leads to abuse of opiates (Kandel 1975). It is precisely withinitiati<strong>on</strong> that our theory is so far c<strong>on</strong>cerned. It states that initiati<strong>on</strong>is (1) produced by an interacti<strong>on</strong> between childhood aggressi<strong>on</strong> and itsfamilial and social antecedents and (2) facilitated by factors that promoteindividual susceptibility and substance availability. Am<strong>on</strong>g the additi<strong>on</strong>aldeterminants of availability is peer acceptance. which is postulated tobe low am<strong>on</strong>g exclusively hyperactive youngsters. Low self-esteem,which is linked to aggressi<strong>on</strong> in our data, may also increase susceptibility.Am<strong>on</strong>g youngsters treated with CNS-stimulant medicati<strong>on</strong>, an additi<strong>on</strong>aldeterminant of susceptibility to substance use is whether the treatmentwas successful in reducing symptoms (Kramer and L<strong>on</strong>ey 1978). <strong>Drug</strong>treatment per se probably does not effect a major increase in subsequentsusceptibility, since most children have quite negative reacti<strong>on</strong>s toingesting the medicati<strong>on</strong> and, despite their positive evaluati<strong>on</strong> of itsgeneral effects <strong>on</strong> their behavior, they are glad to disc<strong>on</strong>tinue takingit. However, few drug-treated children speak in terms of externalc<strong>on</strong>trol or adult dominati<strong>on</strong> (the “chemical straightjacket” decried bythe critics of drug treatment is apparently more an adult c<strong>on</strong>cept),and at followup, more medicated youngsters felt that treatment hadbeen a good idea rather than a bad <strong>on</strong>e. At the same time, many ofour treated youngsters, like their parents and physicians, feared thedevelopment of addicti<strong>on</strong> to the medicati<strong>on</strong>, and their most vivid associati<strong>on</strong>sto taking stimulants were of unpleasant side effects (e.g.,stomach cramps), nuisance, and social embarrassment. Perhaps suchassociati<strong>on</strong>s generalize to all drugs--perhaps <strong>on</strong>ly to all orally ingesteddrugs--or perhaps to all prescribed medicati<strong>on</strong>s. Or perhaps unpleasantassociati<strong>on</strong>s remain specific to CNS stimulants.COMBINATION-OF-EFFECTS THEORY (p. 137)McAuliffe/Gord<strong>on</strong>First use of opiates varies according to which of two types of addict isbeing c<strong>on</strong>sidered. Am<strong>on</strong>g street addicts, use is initiated c<strong>on</strong>tagiouslyby other users, typically for n<strong>on</strong>medical or recreati<strong>on</strong>al reas<strong>on</strong>s (i.e.,pleasure seeking, curiosity, socializing, or going al<strong>on</strong>g with the crowd).Am<strong>on</strong>g iatrogenic and medical-professi<strong>on</strong>al addicts (e.g., physicians),use begins through c<strong>on</strong>tact with the drug rather than with users--eitheras the result of treatment by medical pers<strong>on</strong>nel or as the result ofself-treatment by medical pers<strong>on</strong>nel.Pers<strong>on</strong>s introduced by c<strong>on</strong>tagi<strong>on</strong> have usually been involved in the useof other drugs for euphoria, and have c<strong>on</strong>siderable interest in tryingheroin, even though few actively seek out an opportunity. Youngerneophytes more often cite acceptance am<strong>on</strong>g and pressure from peersas reas<strong>on</strong>s for trying opiates, but older teenagers have usually heardthat heroin produces the ultimate high and want to try it for this325


eas<strong>on</strong> (Hendler and Stephens 1977, pp. 30-31; Brown et al. 1971).They may also have heard of some unpleasant effects, such as vomitinG,but have learned that these are temporary. Despite the apparentcasualness of the first try (Chein et al. 1964; Hughes and Crawford1972; Hendler and Stephens 1977, p. 31), it would be a mistake toregard it merely as a chance occurrence. Most street addicts-to-beare already heavy polydrug users when first exposed to heroin (e.g.,Hughes and Crawford 1972; Sheppard et al. 1972, p. 112) and willingto try almost anything. In this sense, they have already developedsome addicti<strong>on</strong> (resp<strong>on</strong>se strength) prior to using an opiate. Theintent of use in peer groups is evident in a survey by O’D<strong>on</strong>nell et al.(1976, p. 67), who found that 75 percent of novices cited “to gethigh, or st<strong>on</strong>ed” as their reas<strong>on</strong> for using heroin.In c<strong>on</strong>trast, pers<strong>on</strong>s first exposed through treatment or self-treatment(by medical pers<strong>on</strong>nel) rarely menti<strong>on</strong> euphoria as a reas<strong>on</strong> for usingopiates. Although medical patients treated briefly with opiates foracute problems seldom acquire a str<strong>on</strong>g addicti<strong>on</strong>, patients with chr<strong>on</strong>icdisorders run a higher risk. When such patients do develop an attachmentto an opiate that is independent of the drug’s analgesic properties,unlike street addicts they rarely become interested in euphoric effectssince they had no prior orientati<strong>on</strong> toward those effects and sincetheir c<strong>on</strong>text of use does not promote hed<strong>on</strong>istic pursuits. Str<strong>on</strong>glyaddicted medical pers<strong>on</strong>nel usually begin taking opiates not for recreati<strong>on</strong>,but for pain, fatigue, or treatment of hangover (J<strong>on</strong>es andThomps<strong>on</strong> 1958; Little 1971; Pescor 1942; Poplar 1969; Winick 1961a).COPING THEORY (p. 38)Milkman/FroschThe predilecti<strong>on</strong> toward use of a specific pharmacologic agent is determinedby the unique psychophysical and/or sociocultural events in anindividual’s life. Heroin users may have c<strong>on</strong>stituti<strong>on</strong>ally based lowstimulus thresholds and phase-specific disturbances in ego developmentas early as the first year of life. Amphetamine users show ego impairmentwhich may be related to problems in the sec<strong>on</strong>d or third year.Actual initiati<strong>on</strong> may be related to the development of a seducti<strong>on</strong>-pr<strong>on</strong>epers<strong>on</strong>ality (Blachly 1970) with seducti<strong>on</strong> thresholds lowered duringcritical, high-risk periods, e.g., parental separati<strong>on</strong>, negative peerinfluence during adolescence, etc.Initiati<strong>on</strong> of a particular psychoactive substance is related to bothavailability and peer influence. Initiati<strong>on</strong> is not viewed as a singularlysufficient or potent factor in the process of becoming harmfully involvedin the use of drugs. Rather, initiati<strong>on</strong> must be coupled with psychophysicaland/or sociocultural determinants, predisposing an individualtoward c<strong>on</strong>tinued involvement.326


ACHIEVEMENT-ANXIETY THEORY (p. 212)MisraPers<strong>on</strong>s often get so tired, exhausted, and fed up with the process oftrying to achieve the so-called “good life” that they go to the otherextreme, namely, to a life that is so individualized, pers<strong>on</strong>al, andunique that there is no worry about comparing their achievements withthose of “the J<strong>on</strong>eses.” It is in this c<strong>on</strong>text that drug addicti<strong>on</strong>surfaces as an attractive relief. The argument runs somewhat likethis: “When I take drugs, my feeling is my own; I couldn't care lesshow it compares with yours or theirs; it IS my feeling; I own it. Thequesti<strong>on</strong> of its being better than yours is moot, for it is my feeling,you can’t own it; I’ve achieved my own identity,” and so <strong>on</strong>.ADDICTIVE EXPERIENCES THEORY (p. 142)PeeleA pers<strong>on</strong> can begin to use or try a drug for any of the whole rangeof human motivati<strong>on</strong>s; indeed, the desire to alter c<strong>on</strong>sciousness throughdrug use seems to be nearly universal. The reas<strong>on</strong>s for intial use candetermine whether or not the user will ultimately become addicted. Inapproximately descending order of the likelihood of a motivati<strong>on</strong> leadingto addictive use are the following reas<strong>on</strong>s for starting to take a drug:a sense of adventure; a need for stimulati<strong>on</strong>; a desire to emulateothers in the peer group; and pers<strong>on</strong>al needs, such as to avoid pain,to escape from reality, to gain a predictable gratificati<strong>on</strong> in the absenceof other life rewards, to compensate for a sense of pers<strong>on</strong>al inadequacy.It is these latter ego and life deficiencies which most readily embarkan individual <strong>on</strong> the addicti<strong>on</strong> cycle, although no initial reas<strong>on</strong> fortaking a drug is entirely free of these comp<strong>on</strong>ents.SOCIAL NEUROBIOLOGICAL THEORY (p. 286)PrescottFactors that are resp<strong>on</strong>sible for the initiati<strong>on</strong> of drug and alcohol useare many and varied. From the perspective of somatosensory affecti<strong>on</strong>aldeprivati<strong>on</strong> (SAD) theory there is first the establishment of a neuropsychobiologicalpredispositi<strong>on</strong> or need for drugs and alcohol. Any factorthat c<strong>on</strong>tributes to a reducti<strong>on</strong> of afferent activity in the somesthetic(touch) and vestibular (movement) sensory modalities (partial functi<strong>on</strong>aldeafferentati<strong>on</strong>) from the fetal period of development and throughoutthe formative periods of postnatal life can be c<strong>on</strong>sidered as c<strong>on</strong>tributingto potential substance abuse. Fetal c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing to maternal substanceusage during gestati<strong>on</strong> may be a variable of some significance in thisc<strong>on</strong>text (stimulus-seeking behavior at the neurophysiological level).Early separati<strong>on</strong> of newborns from their mothers--a comm<strong>on</strong> hospitalpractice--and c<strong>on</strong>tinuing “instituti<strong>on</strong>alizati<strong>on</strong>” of infants and children(infant nurseries and child day-care centers that are characterized bySAD) are c<strong>on</strong>sidered to be c<strong>on</strong>tributing factors. Failure to breast327


feed, short-term breast feeding (less than two years) that reflects lownurturance or avoidance of intimacy, and breast feeding that is “mechanical”(reflecting duty and resp<strong>on</strong>sibility) and not “joyous” are additi<strong>on</strong>alfactors for c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>. Permitting infants and children to cry forprol<strong>on</strong>ged periods without providing immediate nurturance and permittingthem to cry themselves to sleep are additi<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>tributing factors, asis the intenti<strong>on</strong>al inflicti<strong>on</strong> of pain up<strong>on</strong> infants and children. Thefailure of fathers to be physically affecti<strong>on</strong>ate with their infants andchildren (s<strong>on</strong>s and daughters) is c<strong>on</strong>sidered to be a variable of majorsignificance for future substance abuse. The failure to provide c<strong>on</strong>tinuousvestibular stimulati<strong>on</strong> by not carrying the infant throughout theday results in impaired neurointegrative vestibular-somesthetic andother sensory processes that may result in a need for artificial psychochemicalstimulati<strong>on</strong> later in life or other forms of compensatory stimulusseekingbehaviors.Finally, the failure of children to develop close friendships am<strong>on</strong>g theirpeers and the failure of adolescents to develop not <strong>on</strong>ly close friendshipsbut intimate caring and affecti<strong>on</strong>ate sexual relati<strong>on</strong>ships am<strong>on</strong>g theirpeers are also c<strong>on</strong>sidered to be significant factors in establishing aneuropsychobiological foundati<strong>on</strong> for substance abuse and other aberrantsocial behaviors (Reich 1973).NATURAL HISTORY PERSPECTIVE (p. 215)RobinsThe introducti<strong>on</strong> to drugs is almost exclusively through friends.<strong>Studies</strong> agree that almost all users had friends who were using beforetheir own use began. The typical first drug used was a gift from apeer, not a purchase or a prescripti<strong>on</strong>. This picture is in markedc<strong>on</strong>trast to the older pattern, in which the physician was often thesource of the initial drug exposure. It also differs from the earlyGovernment antidrug propaganda, which invented the evil drug “pusher”in the schoolyard giving away free samples to create a market for hisdevilish products. There has been no need for “pushers” in recentyears. At least in the United States, the illicit drug market hasdefinitely been a seller’s market.Since World War II, young drug users have tended to be urban, male,minority-group members, particularly black and Spanish-American.The period of risk for the <strong>on</strong>set of illegal drug use begins in theteens and ends in the mid-twenties. The behavior of drug abusersprior to the <strong>on</strong>set of drugs resembles that of mild delinquents, as isdiscussed more fully in part I.GENETIC THEORY (p. 297)SchuckitAlcohol is a legal, readily available, and potent substance which isc<strong>on</strong>sumed by almost 90 percent of all teenagers by the end of highschool and which, <strong>on</strong> any <strong>on</strong>e day, is taken by 70 percent or more of328


the general populati<strong>on</strong> (Haglund and Schuckit 1977). The initiati<strong>on</strong> ofuse of this substance, therefore, may be a resp<strong>on</strong>se to factors whichare quite different from those influencing temporary problems or l<strong>on</strong>gtermmisuse (i.e., alcoholism).If “use” is defined as voluntary intake <strong>on</strong> multiple occasi<strong>on</strong>s in any<strong>on</strong>e year. then it is likely that genetic factors play <strong>on</strong>ly a minor role.It is possible to hypothesize an inheritance of a certain level of anxietyor of other pers<strong>on</strong>ality characteristics likely to influence the degree ofrisk taking <strong>on</strong>e is willing to experience and which may affect thedecisi<strong>on</strong> to begin to use drugs.The major factors having an impact <strong>on</strong> the initial use of a ubiquitousdrug like alcohol, however, are more likely to be envir<strong>on</strong>mental.Anecdotically, the initiati<strong>on</strong> of alcohol use probably follows experiencewith caffeine and tobacco and usually precedes experimentati<strong>on</strong> withother classes of drugs, such as marijuana and stimulants (Kandel andFaust 1975). While alcohol intake probably begins in the early teensand becomes more routinized by the end of high school, the chancesfor initiati<strong>on</strong> of use increase with a history of parental substance use,the degree of life instability (such as school or police problems), andthe level of sensitivity to peer pressure. Certain envir<strong>on</strong>mental circumstances,such as entering an excepti<strong>on</strong>ally heavy-drinking envir<strong>on</strong>mentat a time of heightened stress (e.g., living in an isolated armed forcesduty stati<strong>on</strong>) may also c<strong>on</strong>tribute greatly to the initiati<strong>on</strong> of drinkingin an individual otherwise not so inc<strong>line</strong>d. C<strong>on</strong>sidering how thisreadily available and legal drug has become equated with a passagefrom adolescence into adulthood, it is not surprising that the vastmajority of Americans at some time in their lives c<strong>on</strong>sider themselvesdrinkers.It is likely that the same types of factors are involved in the initiati<strong>on</strong>of use of many other drugs. Whether or not <strong>on</strong>e tries the more availablesubstances like marijuana, hallucinogens, or brain-depressing orbrain-stimulating drugs probably rests more with social than withbiological factors. This would depend up<strong>on</strong> the type of peer pressureplaced <strong>on</strong> the adolescent, the availability of drugs, parental models ofdrug use, and passing through levels of experience with the “lesspotent” drugs, as have been described by other authors (Kandel andFaust 1975). Here again, the ready availability of mind-altering drugsin a highly stressful setting may be important to the <strong>on</strong>set of drug useeven in those individuals who might otherwise never have tried thesubstances, as exemplified by the high rate of use in Vietnam and thesubsequent abstenti<strong>on</strong> in individuals returned to their home envir<strong>on</strong>ment(Robins et al. 1975). For initiati<strong>on</strong> into the “harder” or less availabledrugs such as heroin, genetically influenced factors such as pers<strong>on</strong>alitytype (e.g., the antisocial pers<strong>on</strong>ality) may play a more important role.In the theoretical framework presented in this secti<strong>on</strong>, the reas<strong>on</strong>s forinitiating use may be quite different from those factors leading torepeated intake and persistent abuse.329


AVAILABILITY AND PRONENESS THEORY (p. 46)SmartAccording to the availability-pr<strong>on</strong>eness theory, drug use can start<strong>on</strong>ly when the values for both of the factors are above zero for anindividual. Users will start using a drug because they meet it in theireveryday lives, for example, when their friends, associates, oldersiblings, or parents use drugs. <strong>Drug</strong>s may be readily accessible inthe school or workplace if there is no str<strong>on</strong>g countervailing tendencynot to use them, such as a religious or ethically based proscripti<strong>on</strong>.Some pr<strong>on</strong>eness is also necessary. In order to begin drug use ofmany types (e.g., cannabis, tobacco, hallucinogens), the pr<strong>on</strong>enessmay c<strong>on</strong>sist <strong>on</strong>ly of an attitude of curiosity or a desire to experiment.Most users of drugs (including the opiates) initially intend to takethem <strong>on</strong>ly a few times and then to stop. Pr<strong>on</strong>eness may be related tounusual stress, anxiety, or boredom, much as occurred am<strong>on</strong>g soldiersin Vietnam, many of whom experimented with opiates when they maynot have d<strong>on</strong>e so at home in the United States (Robins et al. 1974b).The more dangerous the drug, the greater the pr<strong>on</strong>eness required inorder to take the first dose, given equal availability of each drug.Since drugs such as tobacco and cannabis are known to have a lowtoxicity and addictive liability, users should require less “pr<strong>on</strong>eness”to try them than the opiates or exotic hallucinogens.PERCEIVED EFFECTS THEORY (p. 50)SmithInitiati<strong>on</strong> of substance use depends <strong>on</strong> availability; <strong>on</strong> behavior andattitudes regarding drug use of role models and “significant others”;<strong>on</strong> attitudes, beliefs, and expectati<strong>on</strong>s regarding the immediate andl<strong>on</strong>ger term advantages and disadvantages of use; and <strong>on</strong> pers<strong>on</strong>alitycharacteristics that facilitate or inhibit use.Although illicit drugs can be purchased at most schools, drugs are notequally available to all students. Availability depends <strong>on</strong> who theadolescent or preadolescent knows and how he or she is perceived bypotential suppliers. If friendship groups include users, availability isgreater, and the likelihood of initiati<strong>on</strong> is increased; so is the likelihoodof very early use.Attitudes and behavior regarding substance use <strong>on</strong> the part of friendsand role models (e.g., older siblings, parents, salient members ofreference groups) influence the probability of initiati<strong>on</strong>. If use ispracticed by (or is acceptable to) such “significant others,” initiati<strong>on</strong>is more likely; it is also more likely to occur at an early age.Although most initiates believe that the benefits of occasi<strong>on</strong>al useoutweigh its risks, any particular initiate will have varied and mixedattitudes, beliefs, and expectati<strong>on</strong>s regarding the potential advantagesand disadvantages of substance use. This complex mix of attitudes,beliefs, and expectati<strong>on</strong>s generates a net effect representing an overallpredispositi<strong>on</strong> that can range from extremely positive to extremely330


negative. The more positive the net effect, the higher the probabilityof initiati<strong>on</strong>, and the earlier it is likely to occur.L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal evidence now available indicates that certain pers<strong>on</strong>alitycharacteristics are highly predictive of subsequent substance use.Details regarding these relati<strong>on</strong>ships are presented in part 1 in a morecomprehensive manner.LIFE-THEME THEORY (p. 59)Spotts/Sh<strong>on</strong>tzOur data indicate that initiati<strong>on</strong> into the drug culture is more a matterof social exposure and c<strong>on</strong>tact than of intense pers<strong>on</strong>al need. That is,users do not at the outset specifically seek out drugs to solve pers<strong>on</strong>alproblems. Rather, they are in a social situati<strong>on</strong> where drug use iscomm<strong>on</strong>, and a friend offers a sample of a new substance <strong>on</strong> a trialbasis. Rarely are drug dealers or pushers directly involved at thisstage. However, <strong>on</strong>ce inducted into the drug culture, the user so<strong>on</strong>discovers that the various substances produce predictably differentego states and hence may be used to provide “soluti<strong>on</strong>s” (albeit counterfeit)to problems in pers<strong>on</strong>al adjustment. At this point, the userbegins a search for those substances or palliatives which are mostc<strong>on</strong>gruent with his unique needs and c<strong>on</strong>cerns.Usually, the drug of eventual choice is not the first substance thepers<strong>on</strong> tries. Most of the men we studied had experimented with awide variety of drugs before making a commitment to a specific substanceor a class of drugs.As might be expected, alcohol and marijuana are usually the firstdrugs taken with any degree of regularity. However, there is noevidence that these are maliciously employed by dealers to seducepeople into taking more serious substances.FAMILY THEORY (p. 147)Stant<strong>on</strong>Most initial drug use appears to be a peer-group phenomen<strong>on</strong> of adolescence.It is tied to the normal, albeit troublesome process of growingup, experimenting with new behaviors, becoming self-assertive, developingclose (usually heterosexual) relati<strong>on</strong>ships with people outside thefamily, and leaving home. This stage is nearly always accompanied bya certain amount of rebelli<strong>on</strong> and self-asserti<strong>on</strong>, and the use of drugsas a means for such expressi<strong>on</strong> is certainly abetted if parents indulgein compulsive drug use or heavy drinking themselves. Obviously,drugs are now more a part of the process than they were, but if wehad no drugs, other things would probably take their place. Programsaimed simply at keeping all young people from trying a substanceseveral times may be overly ambitious, even if nobly intended. Blum(1972) has c<strong>on</strong>cluded that drug educati<strong>on</strong> has rarely helped youngpeople’s decisi<strong>on</strong>making about use, and, further still, he states that331


actual failure experiences may be what are needed in order for youthto reorient toward less dysfuncti<strong>on</strong>al alternatives. The problem maybe more <strong>on</strong>e of parental fears than of actual dangers. This is not todeny harmful drug effects so much as to questi<strong>on</strong> how effectively wecan prevent young people from doing a few “stupid” things, whetherdrug related or not (Stant<strong>on</strong> 1979b). <strong>On</strong>e might legitimately ask,then, how realistic it is for adults to mobilize and direct energy toeradicate <strong>on</strong>e symptom of a process that will probably always exist.In other cases, drug use can initiate in resp<strong>on</strong>se to other types ofstress, such as (a) with the “empty nest” syndrome, (b) with familiesfacing an ec<strong>on</strong>omic or other sort of crisis, (c) with family deaths orlosses, or (d) when parents immigrate from other countries or othersecti<strong>on</strong>s of the same country. As with adolescence, these are stageswithin the family developmental life cycle, and they require new copingand readjustment to the alterati<strong>on</strong>s of the family structure whichaccompany them (Minuchin 1974; Stant<strong>on</strong> 1979a,b,c, 1980).From a broader perspective, much of the drug use (and misuse) vis-avisthe family stems from changes in the fabric of the larger society.Br<strong>on</strong>fenbrenner (1974) lists a number of societal trends (fragmentati<strong>on</strong>of the extended family, use of televisi<strong>on</strong> as a substitute for childsupervisi<strong>on</strong>, etc.) which have led to alienati<strong>on</strong> and isolati<strong>on</strong> of youngpeople from others older and younger than themselves; the informalpeer group has filled in the vacuum. In additi<strong>on</strong>, belief in (and mediacoverage of) the efficacy of drug c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong>, with a c<strong>on</strong>comitantincrease in overall adult drug usage, have served to provide a propersetting for greater drug use and misuse by citizens both old andyoung. In this sense, drugs are a symptom and a result of societaltrends and of the relati<strong>on</strong>ships am<strong>on</strong>g people within the society (Stant<strong>on</strong>1979b).SELF-ESTEEM THEORY (p. 157)SteffenhagenThe preservati<strong>on</strong> of the “self” is the most important variable underlyinghuman behavior. <strong>Drug</strong> use is a compensatory mechanism, an excusefor life’s failures, which can insulate <strong>on</strong>e from social resp<strong>on</strong>sibility.Low self-esteem can provide the impetus for initiati<strong>on</strong> for <strong>on</strong>e lookingfor immediate gratificati<strong>on</strong>, but low self-esteem, by itself, is not sufficientto account for initiati<strong>on</strong> into drug use. For that we have to lookto the social milieu which provides the basis for such initiati<strong>on</strong>. Thepeer group provides the greatest pressure and opportunity for theinitiati<strong>on</strong> into drugs, although we have to look to a wider community tosee what drugs are provided, and how: <strong>On</strong>e cannot use a drug whichdoes not exist or for which the zeitgeist is not right. For example,marijuana has been known since the col<strong>on</strong>ial period in the UnitedStates but did not become popular until the late 1960s.332


CYCLICAL PROCESS THEORY (p. 164)van DijkIn principle, drugs are taken for their desired pharmacological effector acti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> mood states, although there is a wide variability for anyspecific drug effect across individual users. Pers<strong>on</strong>al factors encompass(a) need for relief from feelings of intense discomfort or tensi<strong>on</strong>;(b) absence of possibilities to master, sublimate, or canalize suchfeelings, and (c) occasi<strong>on</strong>al influence of such factors as age (e.g.,there is increased risk during adolescence), or the potentially debilitatingeffects of physical and psychiatric illness. The social meaning ofa drug (and of drug taking) is viewed as critically important in themotivati<strong>on</strong> to use the drug, but also as an important influence <strong>on</strong> theindividual’s perceived effect of the drug. Social meanings and valuesof a drug and drug taking entail such factors as its cultural or subculturalacceptance, ritualizati<strong>on</strong>, social and legal norms and sancti<strong>on</strong>s,the symbolic significance of the drug (i.e., a symbol for masculinity,potency, or perhaps n<strong>on</strong>violence and n<strong>on</strong>authoritarianism), and as asignifier of in-group or out-group membership.CONDITIONING THEORY (p. 174)WiklerPsychoanalytical theories of addicti<strong>on</strong> virtually ignored the specificpharmacological acti<strong>on</strong>s of the drug of addicti<strong>on</strong> but stressed theimportance of alleged intrapsychic “impulses” and “archaic l<strong>on</strong>gings.”Thus, Rado (1933) stated, “. . . not the toxic agent, but the impulseto use it, makes an addict out of a given individual.” Fenichel (1945)wrote, “. . . origin and nature of addicti<strong>on</strong> are not determined by thechemical effect of the drug but by the psychological structure of thepatient.” Be this as it may, the author is not aware of any data <strong>on</strong>the results of psychoanalytical therapy in the treatment of addicts;indeed, apart from the prohibitive cost of such therapy, it would seemthat in view of the prevalence of psychopathy (sociopathy) and thinkingdisorder am<strong>on</strong>g detoxified opioid addicts (Hill et al. 1960; M<strong>on</strong>roe etal. 1971), psychoanalytical therapy would be futile. Furthermore, thefact that rats and m<strong>on</strong>keys, equipped with intravenous cannulas forself-injecti<strong>on</strong>, will readily take and maintain themselves <strong>on</strong> morphine,amphetamines, cocaine, and pentobarbital (Schuster and Thomps<strong>on</strong>1969) casts some doubt <strong>on</strong> the necessity of such psychoanalyticalvariables for the genesis of addicti<strong>on</strong>.In the cases of young pers<strong>on</strong>s with prevailing moods of hypophoria andanxiety and with str<strong>on</strong>g needs to bel<strong>on</strong>g to some identifiable group,self-administrati<strong>on</strong> of heroin is often practiced in resp<strong>on</strong>se to thepressure of a heroin-using peer group in a social envir<strong>on</strong>ment in whichsuch a peer group exists.333


ROLE THEORY (p. 225)WinickOur three-pr<strong>on</strong>ged theory suggests that the incidence of drug dependencewill be high in those groups in which there is--1. Access to dependence-producing substances;2. Disengagement from proscripti<strong>on</strong>s against their use; and3. Role strain and/or role deprivati<strong>on</strong>.A role is a set of expectati<strong>on</strong>s and behaviors associated with a specificpositi<strong>on</strong> in a social system. A role strain is a felt difficulty in meetingthe obligati<strong>on</strong>s of a role. By role deprivati<strong>on</strong>, we mean the reacti<strong>on</strong> tothe terminati<strong>on</strong> of a significant role relati<strong>on</strong>ship.A role approach can help to minimize fruitless debates over whether<strong>on</strong>e specific factor is more important than another in the genesis ofdrug dependence, because role is a sufficiently dynamic c<strong>on</strong>cept tosubsume a number of other dimensi<strong>on</strong>s.Instead of having to say that people become drug dependent in orderto meet their pers<strong>on</strong>ality needs, we are suggesting that it is possibleto locate the structural sources of role strain and deprivati<strong>on</strong> withinthe social system. We hypothesize that all points of taking <strong>on</strong> newroles or all points of being tested for adequacy in a role are likely tobe related to role strain and thus to a greater incidence of drugdependence in a group. We also hypothesize that incompatible demandswithin <strong>on</strong>e role, such as between two roles in the same role set, arelikely to lead to a greater incidence of drug dependence.<strong>On</strong>e clear applicati<strong>on</strong> of the theory is to pers<strong>on</strong>s whose drug of choiceis heroin. Heroin users are likely to be pers<strong>on</strong>s whose substance useis overdetermined and who have a multiplicity of problems and difficulties,whereas users of other substances are more likely to take themfor specific problems (Blum and Blum 1969). Heroin users are thereforepers<strong>on</strong>s who are especially likely to experience role difficulties.DEFENSE-STRUCTURE THEORY (p. 71)WurmserPsychodynamically, initiati<strong>on</strong>, repetiti<strong>on</strong>, and resumpti<strong>on</strong> of compulsivedrug use follow a similar, fairly typical pattern that can be summarizedin the following circular schema. It starts out (1) with the narcissisticcrisis, leading (2) to overwhelming affects, to an affect regressi<strong>on</strong>, aradicalizati<strong>on</strong> of these feelings. (3) As direct affect defenses, theclosely related phenomena of splitting (ego splits) and fragmentati<strong>on</strong>are deployed. The defense in form mainly of denial, but also ofrepressi<strong>on</strong> and other “mechanisms,” is carried out partly by psychologicalmeans al<strong>on</strong>e, partly and sec<strong>on</strong>darily by pharmacological proppingup (pharmacogenic defense). (4) The latter requires an additi<strong>on</strong>alform of defense, the element most specific for this syndrome am<strong>on</strong>g334


this c<strong>on</strong>stellati<strong>on</strong> of seven, the defense by externalizati<strong>on</strong>, the importanceof reasserting magical (narcissistic) power by external acti<strong>on</strong>,including magical “things.” (5) This reasserti<strong>on</strong> of power by externalizati<strong>on</strong>requires the use of archaic forms of aggressi<strong>on</strong>, of outwardlyattacking and self-destructive forms of sadomasochism. (6) In mostcases this is <strong>on</strong>ly possible by a sudden splitting of the superego anddefenses against superego functi<strong>on</strong>s. (7) The final point is the enormouspleasure and gratificati<strong>on</strong> which this complex of compromisesoluti<strong>on</strong>s of various instinctual drives with various defenses bringsabout. Most importantly, the acute narcissistic c<strong>on</strong>flict appears resolved,for the moment, but, as Rado (1933) described, the patient is caughtin a vicious circle: “The elati<strong>on</strong> had augmented the ego [now wewould say the self] to gigantic dimensi<strong>on</strong>s and had almost eliminatedthe reality; now just the reverse state appears, sharpened by thec<strong>on</strong>trast. The ego is shrunken, and reality appears exaggerated in itsdimensi<strong>on</strong>s.” The patient is not merely back at the start, but <strong>on</strong> astill lower level of self-esteem.335


C<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong>PERSONALITY-DEFICIENCY THEORY (p. 4)AusubelTo the psychological motivati<strong>on</strong> for drug abuse, i.e., the desire for itsadjustive euphoric effects <strong>on</strong> the part of the inadequate pers<strong>on</strong>ality, isadded the need to c<strong>on</strong>tinue chr<strong>on</strong>ic use in order to avoid unpleasantabstinence symptoms. The latter syndrome, however, is a relativelyminor factor in comparis<strong>on</strong> to the addicts’ desire for the “high,” asthey themselves readily admit; the threat of abstinence symptoms <strong>on</strong>lyadds an element of uncertainty and urgency to the desire. In fact,addicts often delay administrati<strong>on</strong> of the “fix” because such delaysignificantly enhances the high.The relatively minor role of withdrawal symptoms in perpetuating thec<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> of all further drug use <strong>on</strong>ce addicti<strong>on</strong> occurs is supportedby the facts that addicts use up to 30 times the daily dosage neededto suppress withdrawal symptoms; that eventually, in most cases,addicts “shoot” the drug “main<strong>line</strong>” to enhance the euphoria (runningthe risk of septicemia, thrombophlebitis, syphilis, malaria, and hepatitis),when simple hypodermic use would effectively suppress abstinencesymptoms; and that many medically addicted normal pers<strong>on</strong>alities, whobecome physiologically dependent in the course of treatment for majorsurgery, accidents, massive burns, etc., easily overcome their physiologicaldependence, in as much as narcotics have no psychopharmacologicaladjustive value for them. In my view, it is difficult to believe thataddicts would accept social ostracism and the hazards of supportingtheir habits simply to avoid an <strong>on</strong>ly moderately severe 10-day illnessunless opiates had adjustive psychopharmacological value for theirparticular pers<strong>on</strong>ality structures.Claims regarding intracellular “tissue hunger” for heroin followingchr<strong>on</strong>ic use (Dole and Nyswander 1965, 1967) and the so-called idiosyncraticdevelopment of atypically severe withdrawal symptoms that leadto chr<strong>on</strong>ic addicti<strong>on</strong> (Lindesmith 1947) appear to me to be purelyspeculative. The so-called “blockade” value of methad<strong>on</strong>e maintenancein preventing heroin highs (Dole and Nyswander 1965, 1967) is notc<strong>on</strong>vincing because no acquired tolerance for any drug is absolute innature and, in any case, is relative to the doses of both the methad<strong>on</strong>eand the heroin used. Many MMTP (methad<strong>on</strong>e maintenance treatmentprogram) patients admittedly achieve chr<strong>on</strong>ic subliminal highs <strong>on</strong> their336


stabilized methad<strong>on</strong>e dose, or even more blatant highs by “doublingup,” by disc<strong>on</strong>tinuing methad<strong>on</strong>e use prior to shooting heroin, or byusing massive doses of heroin.ADDICTION-TO-PLEASURE THEORY (p. 246)BejerotIt is biologically normal to c<strong>on</strong>tinue a pleasure stimulati<strong>on</strong> when <strong>on</strong>cebegun. To interrupt it sp<strong>on</strong>taneously is associated with culturalattitudes (sin, guilt, and shame), fear of complicati<strong>on</strong>s, or str<strong>on</strong>gpleasurable stimuli from other sources.DISRUPTIVE ENVIRONMENT THEORY (p. 76)CheinA positive reacti<strong>on</strong> to heroin does not always occur with the first shot.But the inadequacies that drove a pers<strong>on</strong> to trying the first time willencourage him to try again, hoping to capture the increased c<strong>on</strong>fidence,the sense of serenity and relaxati<strong>on</strong> he observes in regular users.After a time, he finds that heroin offers pleasurable relief in situati<strong>on</strong>sof strain. If the young pers<strong>on</strong>’s daily life c<strong>on</strong>tains strain and frustrati<strong>on</strong>,the relief brought by the drug comes to be welcome at any time.Simultaneously, the drug makes it easy to deny and to avoid facingthe deep-seated problems that led to his experimenting with drugsoriginally.INCOMPLETE MOURNING THEORY (p. 83)ColemanThe c<strong>on</strong>ceptual foundati<strong>on</strong>s of the incomplete loss theory provide therati<strong>on</strong>ale for c<strong>on</strong>tinuing heroin abuse. The circular, homeostatic modelas elaborated by Stant<strong>on</strong> (1977b) and Stant<strong>on</strong> and Coleman (1979)explains the means by which drug use is reinforced and maintained.This model is based <strong>on</strong> a complex set of feedback mechanisms whichinvolve, as a minimum, a triadic family subsystem, most likely mother,father, and drug abuser. In c<strong>on</strong>tradistincti<strong>on</strong> to the <strong>line</strong>ar or causalchain of family events, the circular model suggests that the incompletemourning of a deceased member (or other loss experience) keeps thefamily in a c<strong>on</strong>tinuous grieving process. Because they have not masteredthe loss, the drug abuser becomes the revenant of the deceasedand is encouraged to stay close to the family. When he or she attemptsto leave home, a family crisis ensues and he or she will be “calledback.” As Coleman and Stant<strong>on</strong> (1978) and Stant<strong>on</strong> et al. (1978)suggest, these families would rather have the addict dead than lost tooutsiders. The “moving in and moving out” of the addict serves afamily maintenance functi<strong>on</strong> and preserves the homeostasis. It is partof the cycle of interlocking behaviors and, if the addict should die,337


another member will most likely start to use drugs, insuring the family’senmeshment in an endless cycle of mourning, loss, and mourning.Bowen (1978) describes a similar cyclical phenomen<strong>on</strong> am<strong>on</strong>g alcoholicfamilies. He suggests that the symptom of excessive drinking occurswhen family anxiety is high. The emergence of the drinking stimulateseven higher anxiety am<strong>on</strong>g those who are dependent <strong>on</strong> the drinker.The higher the anxiety, the more other family members react by anxiouslydoing more of what they are already doing. Thus the processof drinking to relieve anxiety and the increased family anxiety inresp<strong>on</strong>se to drinking can either lead to a functi<strong>on</strong>al collapse or theprocess becomes a chr<strong>on</strong>ic pattern.LEARNED BEHAVIOR THEORY (p. 191)FrederickThe use of drugs is c<strong>on</strong>tinued largely because of the increase in thehabit factor (H) in the equati<strong>on</strong> described in part 1. The increase inthe strength of the drug habit is a direct functi<strong>on</strong> of the number ofreinforcements. As the tensi<strong>on</strong> and anxiety are reduced, the strengthof the habitual act grows. No other comp<strong>on</strong>ent is necessary to effecta satisfactory explanati<strong>on</strong> of the c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> of drug usage. Habitsare the singularly most clearly dem<strong>on</strong>strable factor in the learningsequence of drug-related behaviors. As the figures chosen to illustratethis phenomen<strong>on</strong> indicate, <strong>on</strong>e can hypothetically dem<strong>on</strong>strate how thec<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> of drug usage maintains itself by doubling the habit valuein the numerator. Thus, drug usage can easily c<strong>on</strong>tinue for a greatlength of time, since the probability of drug usage has now reachedthe value of 1.0. The decay, extincti<strong>on</strong>, and growth of every salientfactor inevitably will c<strong>on</strong>tribute to the strength of each and, thereby,become manifest in the relati<strong>on</strong>ship between c<strong>on</strong>structive and destructivefactors to patterns of drug usage. The important thing to remember,however, is the fact that a small increase in the value of a singlefactor can become both a necessary and a sufficient c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> for thedevelopment and c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> of drug usage. This is particularly sowhen a habit has already begun to gain strength in the complex butdelicate equati<strong>on</strong> of abusive/addictive behavior. In substituting thevalues previously shown, when the small value of (Hd) is doubled instrength, the formula becomes unequivocally abusive/addictive.COGNlTlVE CONTROL THEORY (p. 8)GoldC<strong>on</strong>tinued use of drugs depends up<strong>on</strong> users’ obtaining the desiredcognitive-affective-pharmocogenic effects. If drug taking helps pers<strong>on</strong>sfeel good about themselves, decreases their anxiety levels, and mostimportantly, makes them believe they are in c<strong>on</strong>trol of their lives,drug taking is likely to c<strong>on</strong>tinue. Usage is predicted to c<strong>on</strong>tinue and338


increase unless the individual has alternative ways of feeling goodabout himself or herself. Thus, the individual most likely to movefrom experimentati<strong>on</strong> to c<strong>on</strong>tinued usage is having difficulty copingwith anxiety and, most critically, believes that c<strong>on</strong>tinued effort orstruggling will not be successful.BAD-HABIT THEORY (p. 12)GoodwinThe drive behind c<strong>on</strong>tinued heavy, destructive use of a substanceresults from the “addictive cycle,” in which the individual is c<strong>on</strong>stantlyseeking to relieve aversive effects from the substance rather than toreproduce initial positive reinforcing effects. In fact, c<strong>on</strong>tinued usemay be motivated by a need to do both: feel good and stop fromfeeling bad. The essential point is that c<strong>on</strong>tinued abuse of a drugproducing harmful effects suggests “addicti<strong>on</strong>,” and <strong>on</strong>e theory ofaddicti<strong>on</strong> (mine am<strong>on</strong>g others) is that the pers<strong>on</strong> uses the drug moreto relieve bad feelings from the drug than to achieve good. In otherwords, during the period of drug use and for a time afterwards, theabuser is experiencing a series of minihangovers and what drives theuse to destructive levels is the repeated attempt to relieve subclinicalwithdrawal symptoms.MULTIPLE MODELS THEORY (p. 18)GorsuchThe research literature has not distinguished carefully between initialand c<strong>on</strong>tinuing stages of drug involvement, but some studies (e.g.,Jessor and Jessor 1978) suggest that the causative factors in initialuse are still at work in c<strong>on</strong>tinuing drug involvement. The n<strong>on</strong>socializedindividual will c<strong>on</strong>tinue to use drugs based up<strong>on</strong> availability and motivatingfactors such as sensati<strong>on</strong> seeking. The prodrug socialized pers<strong>on</strong>will c<strong>on</strong>tinue use as an expressi<strong>on</strong> of habitual involvement in thatculture and from a c<strong>on</strong>formative motive. The iatrogenic drug userc<strong>on</strong>tinues to seek drug benefits <strong>on</strong> occasi<strong>on</strong>s of mental or physicalanguish.But for c<strong>on</strong>tinued drug use there is <strong>on</strong>e other feature which is uniqueand has potentially powerful effects: the initial drug experienceitself. Unfortunately research in this area is difficult since mostdescripti<strong>on</strong>s of the initial drug experience are reported l<strong>on</strong>g after thatexperience has occurred and are influenced str<strong>on</strong>gly by later percepti<strong>on</strong>s.General retrospective studies give expected c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>s: Those whoc<strong>on</strong>tinue their experience report positive initial experiences. Thosewho stop after the first initial experience feel that they might havec<strong>on</strong>tinued use except for the bad experiences.The existence of a drug-using peer group appears important to thec<strong>on</strong>tinuing use of illicit drugs. First, psychological research suggeststhat interpretati<strong>on</strong> of the drug experience is influenced by the settingand group norms. If the initial experience is with prodrug peers, the339


peers would encourage positive interpretati<strong>on</strong>s of initial experiencesand provide support to reduce the negative aspects which might occur,thus encouraging c<strong>on</strong>tinued drug experiences. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, c<strong>on</strong>tinuingillicit drug users tend to replace their previous friends with newfriends who are also drug users. This has not <strong>on</strong>ly the advantage ofcamaraderie but also of providing ready access to drugs. The drugpeer group may become somewhat str<strong>on</strong>ger than other peer groups fortwo reas<strong>on</strong>s. First, there is societal pressure against illicit drug use.This means that the individual must rely up<strong>on</strong> a close network ofassociates who are also drug users in order to guarantee availability ofthe drug, thus encouraging a distinctive subculture. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, althoughthe research is not c<strong>on</strong>clusive <strong>on</strong> this point, it may be that those inthe n<strong>on</strong>drug culture reject the drug users, who are then left <strong>on</strong>ly withother drug users as potential friends. (Note that this occurrence willcause a shift from the iatrogenic or n<strong>on</strong>socialized model to the prodrugsocializati<strong>on</strong> model.)latrogenic drug users seem to be least likely to become involved in adrug peer group. Their need is the obvious <strong>on</strong>e of satisfying a particularinternal motivati<strong>on</strong> which has little relati<strong>on</strong>ship to other people.Indeed the primary motivati<strong>on</strong> is <strong>on</strong>e of return to normalcy, not thedevelopment of a new lifestyle. Availability through peers is not acritical factor in this model, as people in this group generally havemedical or quasi-medical sources.EXISTENTIAL THEORY (p. 24)GreavesThis theory makes no unique c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> to the understanding ofc<strong>on</strong>tinued use. Such use may be indicative of excessive dependence<strong>on</strong> “passive euphoria,” may be situati<strong>on</strong>al in character, or may berelated to peer-group pressure or other social psychological effects.In any event, except for the illegal status of most drug use, guiltreacti<strong>on</strong>s, and anxiety reacti<strong>on</strong>s, drug use, as such, is felt to be oflittle clinical significance.ADAPTATIONAL THEORY (p. 195)HendinC<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> of drug use <strong>on</strong> an occasi<strong>on</strong>al basis may occur if the drugrelieves tensi<strong>on</strong>, increases sociability, or just makes the individual feelbetter. C<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> a regular basis without abuse suggests thatthe drug suits the individual’s adaptive needs. Although such c<strong>on</strong>trolleduse may not present a problem, most drug abuse usually beginsthis way.It is important to define the adaptive functi<strong>on</strong>s a particular drug ordrugs serve. Is the drug used to deal with the rage and frustrati<strong>on</strong>sof relati<strong>on</strong>ships within the family? (Zinner and Shapiro 1974) Is itused, as marijuana often is, to ease the pressure of academic life?(Hendin 1973a) Is it used, as amphetamines often are, to push young340


women toward achievement that runs counter to their inner feelings?(Hendin 1974b) Is it used, as heroin often is, to create a barrier tointimacy? (Hendin 1974a) Is it used to achieve a defensive fragmentati<strong>on</strong>,as psychedelics often are? (Hendin 1973b, 1974c)Adolescence is a period in which youngsters experiment with manyforms of behavior that they then reject as not suitable for themselves.It is from this perspective that the occasi<strong>on</strong>al heavy use of drugs fora brief period of time must be evaluated. During a <strong>on</strong>e- to two-m<strong>on</strong>thperiod of experimentati<strong>on</strong> with heavy use, such youngsters would seemto be drug abusers; over a l<strong>on</strong>ger period it becomes clear they arenot.BIOLOGICAL RHYTHM THEORY (p. 262)HochhauserWhether or not a given drug, or combinati<strong>on</strong> of drugs, c<strong>on</strong>tinues to beused will be a functi<strong>on</strong> of the efficacy of the drug(s) in meeting thephysiological and psychological needs of the user. If the drug(s)permits some degree of c<strong>on</strong>trol over envir<strong>on</strong>mental experiences orinternal percepti<strong>on</strong>s, it may c<strong>on</strong>tinue to be used. If the drug is foundeffective in affecting either the regularity or the amplitude of thechr<strong>on</strong>obiological rhythm, its use may c<strong>on</strong>tinue.INTERACTIVE FRAMEWORK (p. 95)Huba/Wingard/Bentler<strong>Drug</strong> taking is maintained primarily by its reinforcing effects, broadlyc<strong>on</strong>ceived. These effects may be in the form of alleviati<strong>on</strong> of pressureto perform undesirable behaviors, affect enhancement, a change inorganismic status, or desirable c<strong>on</strong>sequences <strong>on</strong> the pers<strong>on</strong>ality, cogniti<strong>on</strong>,percepti<strong>on</strong>, or c<strong>on</strong>sciousness systems. Thus, psychopharmacologicalreacti<strong>on</strong> to the drug is but <strong>on</strong>e type of reinforcer. Systems whichare directly affected by the ingesti<strong>on</strong> of drugs may themselves sec<strong>on</strong>darilyinfluence other systems. For instance, changes in psychologicalstatus or of perceived behavioral pressure may cause an individual toredefine members of the intimate culture, alter family relati<strong>on</strong>ships, orchange friends. To the extent that such direct and indirect changesare ultimately desirable to the individual, in either the short or thel<strong>on</strong>g term, drug taking will be maintained.We would like to differentiate between early and later stages of maintenance,particularly for those drugs which foster either physical orpsychological dependence. During the early stages, drug effects areprobably evaluated by the individual as desirable because they changethe systems in a way that is psychophysiologically desirable. That isto say, the ingesti<strong>on</strong> of the drug serves to enhance some positivepsychological functi<strong>on</strong> for the individual. During the later stages ofmaintenance, or dependence, it is likely that the effects for the individualare primarily those of warding off the unpleasant organismic effects341


associated with cessati<strong>on</strong> of the drug; these effects may operate directly<strong>on</strong> behavior without psychological mediati<strong>on</strong>.DRUG SUBCULTURES THEORY (p. 110)Johns<strong>on</strong>After initiati<strong>on</strong> to marijuana use, the cannabis subculture’s maintenancec<strong>on</strong>duct norms begin to apply. The new user is expected to usemarijuana when offered; to seek out marijuana; to become as frequenta user as others in the group; and to learn the appropriate argot,rituals, and symbols of subculture participati<strong>on</strong>. The routine andc<strong>on</strong>tinued c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong> of marijuana becomes defined as normal; whatwas <strong>on</strong>ce a risky and innovative behavior is now an expected behaviorfor all peer group members. As a pers<strong>on</strong> becomes increasingly involved,he or she will develop a self-identity as a marijuana user, which maybecome an important identity or role (Rubingt<strong>on</strong> and Weinberg 1973;Kandel 1975). In additi<strong>on</strong>, other users and n<strong>on</strong>users may informallylabel the pers<strong>on</strong> as a marijuana user. Thus, in a process that Lemert(1972) calls sec<strong>on</strong>dary deviance, the user may attain a social andself-identity as a user.As marijuana use becomes increasingly regular, three major c<strong>on</strong>ductnorms of this drug subculture become operative. The user is expectedto buy some marijuana and/or provide marijuana to others in the peergroup (reciprocity c<strong>on</strong>duct norms). While buying cannabis, the userwill frequently be greeted as a friend by the seller and receive offersof an introducti<strong>on</strong> to other drugs or may gain new friends who useother drugs. In additi<strong>on</strong>, the regular user is increasingly expected toprovide and to make small purchases to give or sell to friends; thisreflects involvement in the cannabis subculture’s sharing c<strong>on</strong>ductnorms and low-level distributi<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>duct norms. Of course, theselow-level cannabis transacti<strong>on</strong>s violate criminal law (the potential penaltiesare serious), but as with regular use, such transfers quicklybecome defined as normal by subcultural standards.Abiding by the maintenance, reciprocity, and distributi<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>ductnorms of the cannabis subculture greatly increases the probability ofadopting as a reference group (Sherif and Sherif 1964) and gainingfriends am<strong>on</strong>g those who use other drugs. The process of initiati<strong>on</strong> toother drugs appears to be similar to that for cannabis, with the pers<strong>on</strong>’sfrequency of cannabis use and the number of friends using otherdrugs being the immediate precursors to initiati<strong>on</strong> to a specific substance(Johns<strong>on</strong> 1973; Kandel 1978b). The multiple-drug-use subculture hassomewhat different maintenance c<strong>on</strong>duct norms than the cannabis subculture.Participants are expected to use a variety of substances, althoughcertain drugs may be emphasized within a particular peer group (Waldorfet al. 1977; Feldman et al. 1979). The weekly or more regular use of<strong>on</strong>e n<strong>on</strong>cannabis substance, however, is relatively uncomm<strong>on</strong>, althoughtwo or more n<strong>on</strong>cannabis drugs may be used during the week (Divisi<strong>on</strong>of Substance <strong>Abuse</strong> Services 1978). Frequently, reciprocity anddistributi<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>duct norms of the multiple-drug-use subculture arecritical to the specific drugs used. That is, if <strong>on</strong>e member of a peergroup has a supply of barbiturates, these will be shared and used byother members. If peer group members who wish to use LSD cannot342


find a dealer or supplier, they may buy and use another drug thatwill be offered, such as PCP or stimulants. Thus, the actual drug(s)used by peer groups or individuals is closely related to patterns ofdrug supply and availability within the community.Multiple substance use c<strong>on</strong>tinues for an individual mainly as a functi<strong>on</strong>of peer group activity. To the extent that the peer group seeks andobtains drugs as a source of recreati<strong>on</strong> and a desired activity, themore regular the use episodes and the more different substanceseventually used. While the individual learns the rituals, argot, andstreet pharmacology associated with various n<strong>on</strong>cannabis drugs, thedevelopment of a social identity or a self-identity as a n<strong>on</strong>cannabisdrug user does not appear to be as str<strong>on</strong>gly held as the identity of“pothead” or “addict.” Pers<strong>on</strong>s who develop a str<strong>on</strong>g self-identity orwho acquire a social identity as a n<strong>on</strong>cannabis drug user generallyspecialize in or heavily use a particular drug--which they frequentlysell. But for every weekly user of a specific n<strong>on</strong>cannabis, n<strong>on</strong>heroindrug, there are probably ten or more pers<strong>on</strong>s who abide by the multipledrug-usesubculture c<strong>on</strong>duct norms of using several different substancesduring a given time period and who use drugs in relatively low dosagesin a c<strong>on</strong>trolled manner (Waldorf et al. 1977; Zinberg 1979; Divisi<strong>on</strong> ofSubstance <strong>Abuse</strong> Services 1978).The c<strong>on</strong>duct norms of the heroin-injecti<strong>on</strong> subculture expect the individualto seek heroin c<strong>on</strong>stantly, to inject it at least daily, and tospend most resources to obtain heroin. While many heroin injectorshave some days of n<strong>on</strong>use (Johns<strong>on</strong> et al. 1979), the individual tendsto remain routinely involved in the heroin-injecting subculture’s rolestructure (as a user, buyer, or seller), participating in subcultureargot and rituals, committing minor and major crimes to finance heroinpurchases, and evading law enforcement. The individual quicklydevelops a self-identity as an addict, which is reinforced by thenecessity for interacting with other heroin injectors and dealers toobtain the drug, and by social labeling and rejecti<strong>on</strong> by n<strong>on</strong>heroin-usingfamily, friends, and neighborhood acquaintances.DEVELOPMENTAL STAGES THEORY (p. 120)KandelAt this time in history in the United States, adolescents’ involvementin drugs appears to follow certain paths. Beer and wine are the firstsubstances used by youth. Tobacco and hard liquor are used next.The use of marijuana rarely takes place without prior use of liquor ortobacco, or both. Similarly, the use of illicit drugs other than marijuanararely takes place in the absence of prior experimentati<strong>on</strong> withmarijuana.The documentati<strong>on</strong> that different factors are important for differentdrugs provides additi<strong>on</strong>al support for the claim that drug involvementproceeds through discrete stages. The noti<strong>on</strong> of “stage” itself allowsa more fruitful specificati<strong>on</strong> of the role and structure of differentcausal factors at different stages of involvement.For example, as regards interpers<strong>on</strong>al influences, we find at differentstages not <strong>on</strong>ly differences in source of influence but also differences343


in the aspects of interpers<strong>on</strong>al influences that are important. In theearly stage of drug use, parental behavior seems to be critical inleading the youth to experiment with hard liquor. In later phases ofinitiati<strong>on</strong>, the quality of the parent-child relati<strong>on</strong>ship becomes important,with closeness to parents shielding adolescents from involvement in themost serious forms of drug use. Similarly, there is evidence that ageneralized peer influence, which is important in predicting initiati<strong>on</strong>to legal drugs and marijuana, is partially supplanted by the influenceof a single best friend in leading to the initiati<strong>on</strong> of other illicit drugs.Findings of this kind point to the importance of examining profiles ofinterpers<strong>on</strong>al influences over a series of behaviors, values, and attitudesin order to better understand their dynamic nature. Thus, if <strong>on</strong>eaccepts the noti<strong>on</strong> that progressively more serious involvement indrugs underlies the stages we have out<strong>line</strong>d, the data suggest that themore serious the behavior, the greater the relative importance of thespecific role model provided by <strong>on</strong>e friend in c<strong>on</strong>trast to the samebehavior of the whole group.Similar specificati<strong>on</strong> occurs with respect to the role of participati<strong>on</strong> indeviant behaviors. Participati<strong>on</strong> in various deviant behaviors is mostrelevant in starting to use alcohol, least for illicit drugs. The lessserious the drug, the more its use or n<strong>on</strong>use may depend <strong>on</strong> situati<strong>on</strong>alfactors. By c<strong>on</strong>trast, initiati<strong>on</strong> into illicit drugs other than marijuanaappears to be a c<strong>on</strong>scious resp<strong>on</strong>se to intrapsychic pressures of somesort or other.Many theories of drug dependence offer some c<strong>on</strong>cept of individualpathology as a primary explanati<strong>on</strong>, while others stress social factors.Each of these c<strong>on</strong>cepts may apply to different stages of the process ofinvolvement in drug behavior, social factors playing a more importantrole in the early stages; psychological factors, in the later <strong>on</strong>es.The identificati<strong>on</strong> of cumulative stages in drug behavior has importantc<strong>on</strong>ceptual and methodological implicati<strong>on</strong>s for identifying the factorsthat relate to drug use, either as causes or as c<strong>on</strong>sequences. In al<strong>on</strong>gitudinal analytical framework, there should be decompositi<strong>on</strong> of thepanel sample into appropriate subsamples of individuals at a particularstage who are at risk for initiati<strong>on</strong> into the next stage. Since eachstage represents a cumulative pattern of use and c<strong>on</strong>tains fewer adolescentsthan the preceding stage in the sequence, comparis<strong>on</strong>s of usersand n<strong>on</strong>users must be made am<strong>on</strong>g members of the restrictive group,which has already used the drugs at the preceding stage. Otherwise,the attributes identified as apparent characteristics of a particularclass of drug users may actually reflect characteristics important forinvolvement in drugs at the preceding stage(s). The definiti<strong>on</strong> ofstages allows <strong>on</strong>e to define a populati<strong>on</strong> at risk and to isolate systematically,within that populati<strong>on</strong>, those individuals who succumb to thisrisk within a specific time interval.The noti<strong>on</strong> of “stage” itself is somewhat ambiguous. Am<strong>on</strong>g developmentalpsychologists, c<strong>on</strong>troversy exists about whether the noti<strong>on</strong> ofstages implies that development must necessarily occur in a hierarchicaland fixed order, as Piaget, for example, proposes. However, thenoti<strong>on</strong> of invariance must be subjected to empirical test. This isespecially important for drug behavior. Indeed, as regards the noti<strong>on</strong>of stages in drug use, two reservati<strong>on</strong>s must be kept in mind. Todate, the stages have been identified in populati<strong>on</strong>s of American adolescents.The specific sequences are probably culturally and historicallydetermined. Crosscultural studies are required in order to determine344


the extent to which the order that has been observed is in fact aninvariant <strong>on</strong>e. These studies would indicate whether or not involvementin illicit drugs is always preceded by use of legal drugs, as appearsto be the case in the United States, or whether, in certain cultures,involvement in cigarettes, alcohol, and marijuana proceeds al<strong>on</strong>g paralleland n<strong>on</strong>overlapping paths. Furthermore, while the data show a veryclear-cut sequence in the use of various drugs, they do not provethat the use of a particular drug infallibly leads to the use of otherdrugs higher up in the sequence. Many youths stop at a particularstage without progressing any further. Nor can the findings beinterpreted to show that there is something inherent in the pharmacologicalproperties of the drugs themselves that leads inexorably from<strong>on</strong>e to another.SELF-DEROGATION THEORY (p. 128)KaplanFollowing adopti<strong>on</strong> of the drug use/abuse pattern, to the extent thatthe pers<strong>on</strong> in fact experiences self-enhancing c<strong>on</strong>sequences, is able todefend against any intervening adverse c<strong>on</strong>sequences of the behavior(anticipated or unanticipated), and does not perceive alternativeresp<strong>on</strong>ses with greater self-enhancing potential, the pattern is likelyto be c<strong>on</strong>firmed. The deviant resp<strong>on</strong>se has self-enhancing c<strong>on</strong>sequencesif it facilitates intrapsychic or interpers<strong>on</strong>al avoidance of self-devaluingexperiences associated with the predeviance membership group, servesto attack (symbolically or otherwise) the perceived basis of the pers<strong>on</strong>’sself-rejecting attitudes (that is, representati<strong>on</strong>s of the normative groupstructure), and/or offers substitute patterns with self-enhancingpotential for behavior patterns associated with the genesis of selfrejectingattitudes.EGO/SELF THEORY (p. 29)KhantzianNot surprisingly, the influences operating to cause the initiati<strong>on</strong> ofdrug use are intimately linked to the causes that predispose to thec<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> of drug use, namely, impairments in self-care and thetendency to seek and search for external soluti<strong>on</strong>s, including druguse, to what are internal problems-coping with emoti<strong>on</strong>s and needsatisfacti<strong>on</strong>.The likelihood of c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> in the addicti<strong>on</strong>-pr<strong>on</strong>e individual is alsoenhanced because of a very important discovery, namely, that certaindrugs have a specific appeal based <strong>on</strong> a c<strong>on</strong>stellati<strong>on</strong> of emoti<strong>on</strong>alproblems and pers<strong>on</strong>ality organizati<strong>on</strong> with which such a pers<strong>on</strong> struggles.I have referred to this process as <strong>on</strong>e of “self-selecti<strong>on</strong>,” inwhich a pers<strong>on</strong> discovers that the short-term effect of a certain drugresults in improved functi<strong>on</strong>ing or sense of well-being by augmentingshaky or impaired defenses, or by producing a release of feelings fromrigid and c<strong>on</strong>straining defenses.345


The stimulants, amphetamine and cocaine, have appeal because of theirenergizing properties. They overcome fatique and depleti<strong>on</strong> statesassociated with depressi<strong>on</strong>. The problem with many drug-dependentindividuals is that they are unable to identify and verbalize theirfeelings, and their depressi<strong>on</strong> is <strong>on</strong>ly vaguely or dimly perceived(Krystal and Raskin 1970). Thus, they particularly welcome a drugthat helps to override such vaguely perceived dysphoria. The stimulantsimprove self-esteem, asserti<strong>on</strong>, and frustrati<strong>on</strong> tolerance (Wieder andKaplan 1969) and eliminate feelings of boredom and emptiness by engenderingfeelings of invincibility and grandiosity as the drugs relievedepressi<strong>on</strong> (Wurmser 1974).Sedative-hypnotics and alcohol help to overcome neurotic inhibiti<strong>on</strong>sand anxieties, but their main appeal resides in their acti<strong>on</strong> of overcomingrigid defenses that stand in oppositi<strong>on</strong> to primitive narcissisticl<strong>on</strong>gings. Krystal and Raskin (1970) have stressed how such individualshave adopted rigid defenses against affecti<strong>on</strong>ate and aggressive feelingstoward the self and others because of enormous difficulties with ambivalence.The short-acting hypnotics and alcohol are enjoyed and usedbecause they allow the brief (and therefore tolerable) experience andexpressi<strong>on</strong> of these feelings.My own specific c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> to the noti<strong>on</strong> of self-selecti<strong>on</strong> has centeredaround the anti-aggressi<strong>on</strong> acti<strong>on</strong> of opiates. I attempted in my earlyreports to explore systematically how problems with aggressi<strong>on</strong> predisposeand play a central part in a pers<strong>on</strong>’s becoming addicted to opiates.In this work I emphasized the disorganizing influences of rage andaggressi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the ego and how the anti-aggressi<strong>on</strong> and muting acti<strong>on</strong>of opiates helped the pers<strong>on</strong> to cope by counteracting and relievingthe dysphoric states associated with such rage and aggressi<strong>on</strong> (Khantzian1972, 1974).GENERAL ADDICTION THEORY (p. 34)LindesmithIf use c<strong>on</strong>tinues after the initial experience, and if the use is suchthat the effects of each dose do not overlap those of the precedingand following <strong>on</strong>es, the characteristic craving does not appear as l<strong>on</strong>gas this episodic use lasts. I am acquainted with a pers<strong>on</strong> who hasused heroin in this manner for around 40 years without becomingaddicted. This outcome is implied by the theory since physical dependenceand withdrawal distress are absent when use is irregular in thismanner.During such a period of use, users tend to become c<strong>on</strong>fident of theirability to c<strong>on</strong>trol usage and comm<strong>on</strong>ly develop a firm belief that theycannot become addicts. Their attitudes toward addicts tend to benegative, like those of most n<strong>on</strong>addicts. They often say, when queried<strong>on</strong> this matter, that they are unable to understand why an addictwould make the enormous sacrifices and take the risks that are necessaryto obtain a drug which, from their own direct pers<strong>on</strong>al experience,is not all that w<strong>on</strong>derful or sensati<strong>on</strong>al. Ordinary citizens who haveexperienced the effects of morphine in medical practice usually expressthis same attitude of n<strong>on</strong>comprehensi<strong>on</strong>. From experiences with thedrug, this type of user naturally learns about the usefulness of opiates346


in relieving pains and discomforts of various sorts. This, coupledwith a feeling of invulnerability to addicti<strong>on</strong>, can readily lead to carelessnessin the spacing of shots and trigger the regular daily usagethat creates physical dependence.All of the above is implied in the theory, since it attributes the cravingto effects of opiates experienced after the initial effects have beenreversed by physical dependence. Irregular users experience <strong>on</strong>ly theinitial effects of the drug; they have never had the dramatic andcrucial experience of knowingly using a shot to relieve and banishwithdrawal suffering.HYPERACTIVE ADOLESCENTS THEORY (p. 132)L<strong>on</strong>eyLittle is known about the determinants of c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong>, as distinguishedfrom those of initiati<strong>on</strong>, although it is clear that they may be different(Robins 1975b). The antecedents of initial drug choice have beenhard to determine, and the reas<strong>on</strong>s for drug preference are even moredifficult to elucidate. Many believe that stimulant drug treatmentincreases the probability of drug abuse by changing the child’s attitudestoward himself or herself and toward legal and illegal substances, butthe value of soliciting the attitudes and reacti<strong>on</strong>s of hyperkineticchildren to their c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> or to its treatment has <strong>on</strong>ly recently beenbrought to our attenti<strong>on</strong> (Whalen and Henker 1976). Hechtman et al.(in press) found that more classmate c<strong>on</strong>trols reported using hallucinogensthan did hyperactive youngsters, and it would be easy to believethat previously hyperactive adolescents might experiment impulsivelybut then disc<strong>on</strong>tinue using those substances that proved disorganizing.<strong>On</strong>e might postulate that hyperkinetic children would be especiallylikely to c<strong>on</strong>tinue using stimulants because of their “paradoxically”calming and therapeutic effect. Research <strong>on</strong> the resp<strong>on</strong>ses of normalchildren to CNS stimulants (Rapoport et al. 1978) suggests that theresp<strong>on</strong>ses of hyperkinetic children are not paradoxical at all. However,the alerting and organizing effects of stimulants might be similar forboth hyperkinetic and normal children, but especially reinforcing tochildren with residual attenti<strong>on</strong>al deficits. Schuckit et al. (1978) notethat 12 percent of hyperactive/antisocial drug abusers have abusedstimulants, as compared with six percent of n<strong>on</strong>hyperactive drugabusers. As Schuckit et al. also note, their findings are neitherdramatic nor c<strong>on</strong>sistent, and this particular <strong>on</strong>e is not statisticallysignificant. They also make a point similar to our own: that thehyperkinetic diagnosis is applied to a heterogeneous group of youngsters,many of whom are aggressive as well. To date, there are no findingslinking hyperactivity, as such, with increased stimulant abuse.COMBINATION-OF-EFFECTS THEORY (p. 137)McAuliffe/Gord<strong>on</strong>Researchers know far more about the recreati<strong>on</strong>al pattern, typified instreet addicts, than about the medical-professi<strong>on</strong>al or iatrogenic pattern347


of addicti<strong>on</strong>. The following account is addressed mainly, therefore, toexplaining c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> of use within the euphoria-seeking pattern. Itis expected that the other pattern would differ in important ways inview of the different kinds of pers<strong>on</strong>s involved in and differing goalsof the two patterns.Addicti<strong>on</strong> to opiates begins to grow from the first reinforced doses,which are often the very first. Pooled data from various studies showthat 65 percent of 717 addicts experienced euphoria to some degree <strong>on</strong>their first dose (Chein et al. 1964; Hendler and Stephens 1977; McAuliffe1975a; Waldorf 1973; Willis 1969). Although nausea and vomiting oftenaccompany the first dose, these reacti<strong>on</strong>s may be mixed with euphoria,or found not unpleasant by addicts-to-be, who learn from more experiencedusers that they are temporary (McAuliffe 1975a). Althoughc<strong>on</strong>tinued unpleasant reacti<strong>on</strong>s cause some novices to give up use, foraddicts the unpleasant effects usually disappear so<strong>on</strong>. After <strong>on</strong>ly afew doses, virtually all street addicts experience euphoric effects: 90percent by the fifth dose in Waldorf’s (1973) study of 422 addicts andpractically 100 percent by the sec<strong>on</strong>d dose in Hendler and Stephens’(1977) study of 30 addicts.As with other reinforcers, the strength of the drug-taking resp<strong>on</strong>seshould increase most from the first reinforcement. Str<strong>on</strong>g addicti<strong>on</strong>does not develop from <strong>on</strong>e dose, however, no matter how rewarding.More pers<strong>on</strong>s have used heroin, c<strong>on</strong>sequently, than have becomestr<strong>on</strong>gly addicted (O’D<strong>on</strong>nell et al. 1976, pp. 13, 126). Lack of availabilityof heroin may therefore terminate use short of str<strong>on</strong>g addicti<strong>on</strong>,by allowing extincti<strong>on</strong> to occur. (See Schasre 1966, table Ill.)Extremely early heroin use is apparently maintained largely by peergroup rewards derived from doing things with friends (e.g., Gord<strong>on</strong>1967, p. 58; Hendler and Stephens 1977, p. 38; Howard and Borges1970), but c<strong>on</strong>tinued drug taking becomes increasingly a functi<strong>on</strong> ofthe drive produced by the effects of the drug itself. By the sec<strong>on</strong>ddose the modal reas<strong>on</strong> for use am<strong>on</strong>g neophytes studied by Hendlerand Stephens (1977) had shifted from peer influence to enjoyment ofthe “high,” and am<strong>on</strong>g heroin novices studied by O’D<strong>on</strong>nell et al.(1976, p. 67) 75 percent gave “to get high, or st<strong>on</strong>ed” as their reas<strong>on</strong>for use, compared to <strong>on</strong>ly 18 percent giving “because it was expected. . . in the situati<strong>on</strong>.” A study by Powell (1973) indicates thatpredependence heroin use occurs in sprees that seem to increase inlength with durati<strong>on</strong> of use.Although peer group influences play a major role in the earliest stagesof use, interest in the drug for its own effects so<strong>on</strong> begins to alterthe compositi<strong>on</strong> of the peer group so that more time is spent withindividuals who share that interest, and those who do not share iteither drop their friendship or are dropped by the user (Hendler andStephens 1977, pp. 35-37). Such alterati<strong>on</strong>s in social patterns areoften well underway even before the more severe social disrupti<strong>on</strong>sbrought about by the appearance of physical dependence, with itsdemands for steady access to supply and larger sums of m<strong>on</strong>ey thatdraw the user more heavily than ever into close associati<strong>on</strong> with l<strong>on</strong>gtermaddicts (Hughes and Crawford 1972). During this “h<strong>on</strong>eymo<strong>on</strong>”period, methods of self-administrati<strong>on</strong> also shift toward those designedto yield more pleasure, from usually “snorting” to usually “mainlining”(Hendler and Stephens 1977, pp. 33-34). In many cases, occasi<strong>on</strong>aluse c<strong>on</strong>tinues for years before the psychological attachment to opiatesbecomes str<strong>on</strong>g enough that daily use results (McAuliffe and Gord<strong>on</strong>1974; Schasre 1966).348


Daily use of heroin for a sufficient period of time at last introduceswithdrawal sickness into the reinforcement picture. Physical dependenceadds a potentially powerful source of negative reinforcement andintroduces regularity to the addict’s drive state by serving as a pacemakerfor the lower bound frequency of use. <strong>Studies</strong> show that drugsthat do not cause physical dependence (e.g., amphetamines) producesporadic resp<strong>on</strong>ding (Bejerot 1972, p. 12; Carey and Mandel 1968;Schuster and Thomps<strong>on</strong> 1969, p. 489; Spealman 1979), whereas drugsthat produce physical dependence (such as opiates) keep animalsresp<strong>on</strong>ding <strong>on</strong> a regular basis (Pickens et al. 1967). In early heroinuse, the new pattern of avoiding withdrawal combines with the alreadyexisting recreati<strong>on</strong>al pattern of seeking the drug’s positive effects.In the l<strong>on</strong>g-term addict, euphoria, withdrawal sickness, and othermiscellaneous reinforcing effects combine in various proporti<strong>on</strong>s toyield a complex schedule of reinforcement that sustains c<strong>on</strong>tinued use(McAuliffe and Gord<strong>on</strong> 1974). The exact weighting of each effect inthe reinforcement schedule may vary from time to time within a givenindividual and from addict to addict. At the time of injecti<strong>on</strong>, streetaddicts who are sick from withdrawal and who have <strong>on</strong>ly maintenancedoses <strong>on</strong> hand are obviously satisfied to resp<strong>on</strong>d to just <strong>on</strong>e comp<strong>on</strong>entof their schedule (McAuliffe and Gord<strong>on</strong> 1974). With a larger supply,they typically resp<strong>on</strong>d to both comp<strong>on</strong>ents by reducing sickness andenjoying euphoric effects, too. Oftentimes, having d<strong>on</strong>e so, they willtake another dose so<strong>on</strong> afterward, to produce even more intense euphoria.Having already attended to withdrawal needs, this time the resp<strong>on</strong>se issolely to the euphoric comp<strong>on</strong>ent. The weighting of these comp<strong>on</strong>entsacross c<strong>on</strong>temporary addicts ranges from <strong>on</strong>e extreme, exemplified byrare addicts who almost never experience euphoria, to the other,exemplified by rare addicts we have interviewed who get high <strong>on</strong>virtually every injecti<strong>on</strong>. At any given time, most street addicts aredistributed in intermediate positi<strong>on</strong>s, where they avoid withdrawal andreceive intermittent positive rewards. Quite different combinati<strong>on</strong>s maybe typical of medical-professi<strong>on</strong>al addicts, iatrogenic addicts. soldiersaddicted in Vietnam (Gord<strong>on</strong> 1979). and so <strong>on</strong>. It is the history of reinforcementgained from using drugs in all of these ways that accounts foran individual’s overall drug-derived motivati<strong>on</strong> for opiate use.COPING THEORY (p. 38)Milkman/FroschThe ready availability of a wide range of psychoactive agents providesthe user with the freedom to select, with some degree of accuracy, aspecifically altered ego state with known physical and psychologicalproperties. Although initiati<strong>on</strong> of use of a particular substance maybe circumstantially determined, c<strong>on</strong>tinued use or rapid cessati<strong>on</strong> isrelated to the individual’s unique psychophysical reacti<strong>on</strong> to the drug.The motivati<strong>on</strong> toward c<strong>on</strong>tinued involvement is the integrated result ofc<strong>on</strong>stituti<strong>on</strong>al, social/envir<strong>on</strong>mental, and intrapsychic factors. Disturbancesin the normally expected mastery of phase-specific c<strong>on</strong>flicts inearly childhood are hypothesized to result in defective ego functi<strong>on</strong>ingin the substance-pr<strong>on</strong>e individual. The overly stressed characteristicdefense mechanisms of the defective ego are temporarily bolsteredthrough pharmacologic support. If a particular drug-induced ego state349


provides a mechanism for easing the discomfort of c<strong>on</strong>flict, an individualmay seek out that particular drug when that c<strong>on</strong>flict is reexperienced.The reinforcing quality of temporary stress reducti<strong>on</strong> leads to c<strong>on</strong>tinuedreliance and utilizati<strong>on</strong>. The drug of choice will be the pharmacologicagent which proves harm<strong>on</strong>ious with the user’s characteristic mode ofreducing anxiety. Furthermore, the selected drug appears to producean altered ego state which is reminiscent of and may recapture specificphases of early child development, e.g., heroin, first year; amphetamine,sec<strong>on</strong>d to third year.ADDICTIVE EXPERIENCES THEORY (p. 142)PeelePers<strong>on</strong>s use drugs, simply speaking, when they find such use to berewarding in terms of values, needs, and overall life structure.C<strong>on</strong>ceivably a drug can tulfill positive functi<strong>on</strong>s for an individual--suchas enabling him or her to work better or to relate to others. Even inthis case there is the danger that functi<strong>on</strong>ing in a positive sense willbecome dependent <strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tinued drug use. In all cases, use of thedrug will probably make it harder for the pers<strong>on</strong> to eliminate underlyingand unresolved problems.While the experience the drug produces for the pers<strong>on</strong> must providerewards for him or her in order to maintain drug use, this is not tosay that its objective impact <strong>on</strong> the user’s life will not be negative.Thus narcotic or barbiturate users find the removal of pain and theabsence of anxiety induced by the drug to be rewarding, even thoughthese effects make them less sensitive to and less effective in dealingwith their envir<strong>on</strong>ment. In fact, it is this very depleti<strong>on</strong> of capabilitieswhich best guarantees c<strong>on</strong>tinued use of the drug.C<strong>on</strong>sider the stimulant addict, such as the addicted coffee drinker,who uses caffeine to provide energy throughout the day. By maskingfatigue, inadequate nutriti<strong>on</strong>al input, lack of exercise, etc., and allthose deficiencies which force reliance <strong>on</strong> the caffeine, the drug makesthe pers<strong>on</strong> less aware of the need to change his or her habits so as tobe able to supply energy needs naturally. In this way, the caffeineperpetuates its own use.SOCIAL NEUROBIOLOGICAL THEORY (p. 286)PrescottThe c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> of substance usage is dependent, in part, up<strong>on</strong> thec<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> of somatosensory affecti<strong>on</strong>al deprivati<strong>on</strong> and the need tomaintain friendships and social positi<strong>on</strong>s where those friendships andsocial positi<strong>on</strong>s are c<strong>on</strong>tingent up<strong>on</strong> the use of drugs or alcohol.Support for the c<strong>on</strong>tinuing use of drugs is facilitated by the practicesof modern medicine and the advertising practices of the pharmaceuticalcorporati<strong>on</strong>s. Social learning processes which operate at all levels ofdevelopment (childhood to adulthood) capitalize up<strong>on</strong> the need for thebody to find relief from tensi<strong>on</strong> and pain created in large part by350


somatosensory affecti<strong>on</strong>al deprivati<strong>on</strong>. Societal and moral values thatare intrinsically opposed to somatosensory pleasure and sexual pleasure,in particular, provide support for the alternatives of drugs and alcohol.Societal oppositi<strong>on</strong> to massage parlors and prostituti<strong>on</strong> but open acceptanceand support of the alcohol industries is a case in point. Societalacceptance of addicting drugs that impair somatosensory pleasure,e.g., alcohol and methad<strong>on</strong>e, and oppositi<strong>on</strong> to drugs that facilitatepleasure, e.g., marijuana and heroin, is another case in point.Carstairs’ (1966) classic study should be c<strong>on</strong>sulted in this c<strong>on</strong>text as adramatic illustrati<strong>on</strong> of the reciprocal inhibitory relati<strong>on</strong>ships betweendrug use and behaviors that are culturally determined. Carstairsreported <strong>on</strong> the use of bhang (marijuana) and alcohol in the two highestcaste groups, Rajput and Brahmin, in a village in northern India.The Rajput, the warrior class, indulged in alcohol, which facilitatedthe expressi<strong>on</strong> of sexuality and violence. The Brahmin was the religiousclass and indulged in bhang, which facilitated religious experiencesand enhanced their spiritual life. The holy men avoided alcohol, whichthey c<strong>on</strong>sidered destructive to salvati<strong>on</strong>, and would not permit a Hinduwho had c<strong>on</strong>sumed alcohol to “enter <strong>on</strong>e of his temples (not even agoddess temple) without first having a purgatory bath and change ofclothes” (p. 105).The c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> of use or abuse and the choice of drug are culturallyinfluenced. A culture will support the use of certain drugs that arec<strong>on</strong>sistent with and supportive of its own mores and values and willoppose the use of those drugs that interfere with these mores andvalues. Thus, the U.S. culture, which is predominantly an extroverted,violent, and exploitive culture (sexually and ec<strong>on</strong>omically), supportsthe use of alcohol, which facilitates these behaviors. C<strong>on</strong>versely, theU.S. culture opposes the “pleasure” drugs (marijuana and heroin),which inhibit violence and exploitati<strong>on</strong> and facilitate introspective andc<strong>on</strong>templative behaviors. (This statement should not be c<strong>on</strong>strued assupporting drug use for recreati<strong>on</strong>al purposes.) The issue is notwhether a drug is addicting or n<strong>on</strong>addicting--alcohol is addicting(culturally supported) and marijuana is n<strong>on</strong>addicting (culturally opposed);heroin is addicting (culturally opposed) and methad<strong>on</strong>e is addicting(culturally supported). Both the fabric and the loom of culture mustbe understood if the choice of specific drugs and the c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> ofuse and abuse are to be understood.GENETIC THEORY (p. 297)Schuckit<strong>On</strong>ce some<strong>on</strong>e does try a drug, the decisi<strong>on</strong> to c<strong>on</strong>tinue using thesubstance probably involves a combinati<strong>on</strong> of social and biologicalfactors. While genetically mediated reacti<strong>on</strong>s to the drug may play alarger role here than in the initiati<strong>on</strong> of use, social factors still holdgreat influence.Genetically influenced biological factors may be important in the balanceof pleasant and unpleasant effects seen with almost all substances <strong>on</strong>their first try. C<strong>on</strong>stituti<strong>on</strong>al factors may determine the incidence andseverity of adverse problems, such as coughing, nausea, or vomiting,and may mediate the intensity of pleasant effects as well. Thus, theindividual’s pers<strong>on</strong>ality, usual level of anxiety, the mechanisms and351


ate of metabolism of the substance, and the nervous system’s sensitivityto the substance may all c<strong>on</strong>tribute to the final balance between thepositive and negative effects of the first ingesti<strong>on</strong> and in this wayc<strong>on</strong>tribute to the individual’s decisi<strong>on</strong> to try the substance again.It also seems apparent from individual histories of smoking or opiateuse, for example, that there are a variety of social and psychologicalfactors which interact with the biological reacti<strong>on</strong>s. These wouldinclude peer pressures, the desire to assume an adult role and theneed to copy parental models, and general societal values about thedrug (e.g., alcohol and marijuana) which may influence an individualto try the drug <strong>on</strong> repeated occasi<strong>on</strong>s despite the early adverse c<strong>on</strong>sequencesof taking the substance. With each repeated use, there maybe a tendency for the more positive aspects of drug effects to predominateas “tolerance” develops to the negative c<strong>on</strong>sequences of the drug.This may make repeated use more and more likely.AVAlLABlLlTY AND PRONENESS THEORY (p. 46)SmartMost users of illicit drugs do not c<strong>on</strong>tinue their use to the point ofaddicti<strong>on</strong>. A more comm<strong>on</strong> event is that the user tries the drug a fewtimes, has his or her curiosity satisfied or finds the drug unrewardingand disc<strong>on</strong>tinues its use. Those who c<strong>on</strong>tinue drug use to becomedaily or addicted users will display an unusually high level of pr<strong>on</strong>enessin terms of social or psychological needs. Pr<strong>on</strong>eness is likely to be amore important factor in c<strong>on</strong>tinuing use than is availability. Since thefirst use has already taken place, the user has overcome the majordifficulties in obtaining drugs. The user will know peers, siblings, orassociates who are users and hence have some reas<strong>on</strong>able access to thedrug. Those who experience especially great frustrati<strong>on</strong>s with ghettolife or who have major psychological problems will be more likely toc<strong>on</strong>tinue use. As use c<strong>on</strong>tinues, the user gains more access to drugs,and physical availability becomes less a problem than it is for newusers or n<strong>on</strong>users. However, for daily users of expensive drugs, alimit <strong>on</strong> their availability is set by ec<strong>on</strong>omic costs. Users must increasetheir income by either legitimate or, more likely, n<strong>on</strong>legitimate meansin order to maintain their access to drugs at high levels of usage.PERCEIVED EFFECTS THEORY (p. 50)SmithAny single act produces numerous and varied positive and negativec<strong>on</strong>sequences for the actor. Some will be recognized by the actor;some will not. Those that are recognized will be accorded differentialimportance. The aggregate of this mix of perceived c<strong>on</strong>sequencesdetermines the likelihood that the act will be repeated. Substance usewill c<strong>on</strong>tinue as l<strong>on</strong>g as the aggregate benefits are perceived as beinggreater, or more valued, than the aggregate costs. The cost-benefitrelati<strong>on</strong>ship depends <strong>on</strong> many variables, such as which substance is352


used, its strength, the frequency of its use, the immediacy and intensityof its perceived effects, the needs the substance is perceived assatisfying and frustrating, the intensity of those needs, their importanceand centrality in the user’s life; and the effects use has <strong>on</strong> the user’sc<strong>on</strong>cepts of Self and Ideal Self.The match of the perceived drug-induced changes and the perceivedneeds of the user is important in determining whether or not use willc<strong>on</strong>tinue. The individual who places high value <strong>on</strong> feeling str<strong>on</strong>g,alert, decisive, and masterful is apt to find amphetamine or cocainemuch more satisfying than a pers<strong>on</strong> who emphasizes peace, physicalrelaxati<strong>on</strong>, and the c<strong>on</strong>templati<strong>on</strong> of philosophical and metaphysicalissues. A pers<strong>on</strong> of the latter type would probably find drugs likemarijuana and LSD far more enjoyable. The better the match betweenthe perceived substance effects and the user’s needs, the more likelyuse is to c<strong>on</strong>tinue.The mood and cognitive changes caused by use of certain substancescan temporarily alter the user’s c<strong>on</strong>cepts of Self and Ideal Self. Ifuse reduces the discrepancy between the user’s percepti<strong>on</strong>s of Selfand Ideal Self, c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> of use is likely--even if those changes last<strong>on</strong>ly as l<strong>on</strong>g as the drug effect itself.Whatever the substance, its use is likely to c<strong>on</strong>tinue as l<strong>on</strong>g as theamount and pattern of use are perceived by the user as providing anet aggregate benefit, whether by physical or psychological gratificati<strong>on</strong>,reducti<strong>on</strong> of physical or psychological distress, alterati<strong>on</strong> of the user’spercepti<strong>on</strong> of Self or Ideal Self, perceived enhancement of performance,or some other mechanism.LIFE-THEME THEORY (p. 59)Spotts/Sh<strong>on</strong>tzAfter a period of experimentati<strong>on</strong> with many substances (usually in thecompany of friends) the pers<strong>on</strong> who becomes a heavy, chr<strong>on</strong>ic userchooses the single drug or class of drugs which most nearly producesthe ego state that is needed to patch over the problem <strong>on</strong> the ego/psycheaxis. Another factor determining final choice is regular access to thedesired substance. Some pers<strong>on</strong>s will settle <strong>on</strong> a drug that is sec<strong>on</strong>dbest, from a pers<strong>on</strong>al point of view (e.g., amphetamine), because theycannot obtain or afford the <strong>on</strong>e they really want (cocaine). <strong>On</strong>ce adrug that produces the desired ego state has been found, and <strong>on</strong>cesources of supply have been established, social support for c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong>of its use is no l<strong>on</strong>ger required. The pers<strong>on</strong>-drug relati<strong>on</strong>ship becomesself-sustaining.FAMILY THEORY (p. 147)Stant<strong>on</strong>When drug use, especially heavy use, is c<strong>on</strong>tinued for a prol<strong>on</strong>gedperiod, it is helpful to view it as indicative that the user and the353


user’s family have gotten stuck at some point in the family life cycle.They have hit a developmental milest<strong>on</strong>e and cannot get past it, slippinginto a repetitive behavioral pattern. In additi<strong>on</strong> to the turmoil ofadolescence, a variety of extrafamilial factors can threaten the familysystem and trigger a cycle of c<strong>on</strong>tinued use in <strong>on</strong>e or more members.These factors might include the father losing his job or facing retirement,a family member becoming seriously ill, the death of an importantmember, or a sibling marrying or leaving home. Social systems outsidethe family, including peers (Wikler 1973b), social agencies, and legalinstituti<strong>on</strong>s, can affect the drug user directly, and through the user,the family. However, without denying the importance of extrafamilialsystems, the family’s influence should be c<strong>on</strong>sidered the primary <strong>on</strong>ein most cases of c<strong>on</strong>tinued use, since the family accentuates or attenuatesthe impact of these external influences.<strong>Drug</strong> abusers are locked <strong>on</strong> the horns of a dilemma. <strong>On</strong> the <strong>on</strong>ehand, they are under great pressure to remain intensely involved inthe family to keep it intact, while <strong>on</strong> the other, sociocultural andpsychobiological forces dictate the establishment of intimate outsiderelati<strong>on</strong>ships. C<strong>on</strong>tinued heavy drug use is the unique paradoxicalsoluti<strong>on</strong> to the dilemma of maintaining or dissolving the triadic interacti<strong>on</strong>.<strong>On</strong> the systems level, the drug use cycle serves to give theappearance of dramatic movement within the family as the triad isdissolved, re-established, dissolved, and re-established again. Inadditi<strong>on</strong>, drug abusers become involved in a homeostatic pattern ofshuttling back and forth between peers and home. An interpers<strong>on</strong>alanalysis of the system reveals, however, that abusers form relati<strong>on</strong>shipswithin the drug culture which effectively reinforce their dependence <strong>on</strong>the family. Aagin. the outside relati<strong>on</strong>ships can be c<strong>on</strong>sidered as thearena for pseudo-independent and pseudo-competent behavior, whileparadoxically, the greater the involvement with the peer group, themore the abuser becomes helpless, i.e., addicted. This helplessnessis redefined by the family in a dependency-engendering way, i.e., asa “sickness,” and is therefore acceptable.SELF-ESTEEM THEORY (p. 157)SteffenhagenLow self-esteem provides the basis for c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> of drug use sincesuch use could be a coping mechanism for the protecti<strong>on</strong> of the “self.”Individuals with inferiority feelings marked by inadequate interpers<strong>on</strong>alrelati<strong>on</strong>s are prime targets because they use drugs as a way of relatingto each other; drugs are the b<strong>on</strong>d for camaraderie, the cultural itemaround which the group revolves. In this instance, the behaviordefeats the very purpose for which it was intended because theiralready fragile c<strong>on</strong>tact with reality will be further impaired by thedrug. <strong>Drug</strong> use could move quickly toward drug abuse, and theindividual could then say, “See, if it weren’t for the fact that I amphysically addicted to heroin, I would be able to get a job and make asuccess of myself.”354


CYCLICAL PROCESS THEORY (p. 164)van DijkIn explaining the c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> or maintenance of drug use, four typesof vicious circles or cycles in the addicti<strong>on</strong> process are de<strong>line</strong>ated:(a) pharmacological, (b) cerebra-ego-weakening, (c) social, and(d) psychic. Pharmacologically, the use of a drug creates metabolicchanges (tolerance, withdrawal syndrome) which, in turn, increase theindividual’s need for, and use of, the drug. Cerebro-ego-weakeningmeans that the use of a drug may interfere with or alter the individual’scerebral functi<strong>on</strong>s which regulate use. Ultimately, the ego is weakened,and, in turn, the resistance against the motivati<strong>on</strong> for drug use isdecreased; c<strong>on</strong>sequently, drug use escalates. The social vicious cycledepicts the use of drugs as leading to negative social c<strong>on</strong>sequences(reproaches of family, friends, employers). Slowly, the individualadopts the social role of being an addict and experiences some reinforcementas a result of identificati<strong>on</strong> with the drug-using subculture.Such identificati<strong>on</strong>, in turn, fosters c<strong>on</strong>tinued drug use. Finally, thepsychic cycle is characterized by increased feelings of guilt and shame,regressive and infantomimetic behaviors, and predominance of thepleasure principle. These feelings and effects ultimately increase theneed for more drugs (in the hope that the drug will decrease thesefeelings) and the cycle becomes complete. Given the force of thesevicious cycles, the prospects for cessati<strong>on</strong> of use are minimal, unlessthe cycles can be short-circuited, perhaps with methad<strong>on</strong>e as a drugsubstitute.CONDITIONING THEORY (p. 174)WiklerThe pharmacological effects of heroin (miosis, respiratory depressi<strong>on</strong>,analgesia, etc.) are c<strong>on</strong>ceived as reflex resp<strong>on</strong>ses to the receptoracti<strong>on</strong>s of the drug, but its “direct” reinforcing properties are ascribedto acceptance by the peer groups and reducti<strong>on</strong> of hypophoria andanxiety. With repetiti<strong>on</strong> of self-administrati<strong>on</strong> of heroin, tolerancedevelops rapidly to the direct pharmacological effects of the drug andphysical dependence begins (dem<strong>on</strong>strable by administrati<strong>on</strong> of narcoticantag<strong>on</strong>ists after <strong>on</strong>ly a few doses of morphine, heroin, or methad<strong>on</strong>e;see Wikler et al. 1953). The prevailing mood of the heroin user isnow predominantly dysphoric, and withholding of heroin now has as itsreflex c<strong>on</strong>sequence the appearance of signs of heroin abstinence (mydriasis,hyperpnea, hyperalgesia, etc.), which generate a new need,experienced as abstinence distress. Because of previous reinforcementof heroin self-administrati<strong>on</strong>, the heroin user engages in “hustling” foropioids--i.e., seeking “c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s,” earning or stealing m<strong>on</strong>ey, attemptingto outwit the law--which eventually becomes self-reinforcing,though initially at least, it is maintained by acquiring heroin forself-administrati<strong>on</strong>. In this stage, the “indirect” reinforcing propertiesof heroin are attributed to its efficacy in suppressing abstinencedistress. “<strong>On</strong> the street,” the heroin user who is both tolerant andphysically dependent frequently undergoes abstinence phenomenabefore he is able to obtain and self-administer the next dose. Givencertain more or less c<strong>on</strong>stant exteroceptive stimuli (street associates,355


neighborhood characteristics, “strung out” addicts or leaders, “dope”talk) that are temporally c<strong>on</strong>tiguous with such episodes, the cycle ofheroin abstinence and its terminati<strong>on</strong> can become classically c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>edto such stimuli, while heroin-seeking behavior is operantly c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed.DEFENSE-STRUCTURE THEORY (p. 71)WurmserThe same circle of specificity (depicted in figure 1) as was menti<strong>on</strong>edin regard to initiati<strong>on</strong> is actualized in c<strong>on</strong>tinued drug use. There isalso, as with all neurotic phenomena, a process of spreading andgeneralizing. For more and more “narcissistic crises,” anxiety situati<strong>on</strong>s,and dysphoric affects relief is sought in form of this selftreatment.It becomes a “cure” for all ills. Its pleasure is used as amore and more global defense against all the unhappiness derived fromthe primary pathology. It is part of the sec<strong>on</strong>dary defensive struggleknown in all nosologic entities in psychiatry (Freud 1926).356


Transiti<strong>on</strong>: Use to <strong>Abuse</strong>PERSONALITY-DEFICIENCY THEORY (p. 4)AusubelThe distincti<strong>on</strong> between narcotic use and abuse is analogous to thedistincti<strong>on</strong> between marijuana use and abuse, i.e., the differencebetween casual, sporadic, or recreati<strong>on</strong>al users, <strong>on</strong> the <strong>on</strong>e hand, andthose who are almost permanently “st<strong>on</strong>ed” or narcotized as a style oflife <strong>on</strong> the other. Narcotics, by virtue of their more potent euphoriceffects, obviously lend themselves more easily than does marijuana tochr<strong>on</strong>ic abuse.<str<strong>on</strong>g>Theories</str<strong>on</strong>g> hypothesizing that heroin use at a certain critical level leadsto a state of intracellular “tissue hunger” that is satisfied by c<strong>on</strong>tinuousadministrati<strong>on</strong> of a stabilized dose of methad<strong>on</strong>e are unable to explainadequately why many MMTP patients still seek euphoria from “doublingup” foregoing their methad<strong>on</strong>e before shooting heroin and overindulgingin alcohol, barbiturates, amitripty<strong>line</strong>, and the benzodiazepines. 1 Ifheroin addicti<strong>on</strong> were caused by “tissue hunger” to begin with, andthen relieved by stabilized doses of methad<strong>on</strong>e, why should <strong>on</strong>e seekthis surreptitious form of euphoria from heroin and other drugs thatjeopardizes <strong>on</strong>e’s status in MMTP programs? A more parsim<strong>on</strong>iousexplanati<strong>on</strong>, therefore, is that they relapse to drug use because of thevery same reas<strong>on</strong>s that cause their addicti<strong>on</strong> in the first place, i.e.,various forms of pers<strong>on</strong>ality predispositi<strong>on</strong>s, reassociati<strong>on</strong> with addictswhen they return from isolated treatment centers to their old neighborhoods,the accessibility of drugs in their envir<strong>on</strong>ment, communityattitudinal tolerance for the practice, and insufficient character reeducati<strong>on</strong>during “treatment” to withstand the blandishments of heroin-inducedeuphoria.1 Evaluati<strong>on</strong> studies of MMTPs (e.g., Gearing 1971), which treat urinesamples as if they were authentic and reliable research evaluati<strong>on</strong> materialare misleading. In most MMTPs, urine samples are not randomizedor supervised, and the more expensive tests for the benzodiazepinesare usually not performed. For other methodological deficiencies ofmany of the evaluati<strong>on</strong> studies that grossly overestimate the retenti<strong>on</strong>and success rates of methad<strong>on</strong>e maintenance treatment programs, seeLukoff (1974, 1975).357


<str<strong>on</strong>g>Theories</str<strong>on</strong>g> of addicti<strong>on</strong> that explain the transiti<strong>on</strong> between drug use andabuse (e.g., Becker 1953; Lindesmith 1947) <strong>on</strong> the grounds thataddicts become habituated to a substance when they perceive therelati<strong>on</strong>ship between c<strong>on</strong>tinued use and relief of distress beg thesignificant questi<strong>on</strong> of differential susceptibility.ADDICTION-TO-PLEASURE THEORY (p. 246)BejerotWhen the pleasure stimulati<strong>on</strong> becomes str<strong>on</strong>g enough (either through afew intensive positive experiences or from many less intensely appreciated),there occurs a learned c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing to the intoxicati<strong>on</strong> experience,probably when new and shorter nerve courses come into functi<strong>on</strong> andhigher centers are disc<strong>on</strong>nected. The process should accelerate ifother sources of pleasure are neglected or for other reas<strong>on</strong>s havebecome less interesting (sexuality for opiate abusers, etc.).DISRUPTIVE ENVIRONMENT THEORY (p. 76)CheinNot all of the youngsters who experiment with drugs, or even all ofthose who become habitual users, become addicts. Many of them, asthey get older, mature sufficiently to become interested in finding ajob or a steady girlfriend, and if they are successful they no l<strong>on</strong>gerneed drugs. Some find that drugs do little for them, and so theygive them up. Not all people react to opiates in the same way. Theaddicti<strong>on</strong>-pr<strong>on</strong>e youngster apparently reacts to the drug in an especiallyintense manner. The more severe his pers<strong>on</strong>ality disturbances are,the more likely he is to become addicted. The lack of a cohesive andsupportive family is probably the determining factor in the transiti<strong>on</strong>from use to addicti<strong>on</strong>.INCOMPLETE MOURNING THEORY (p. 83)ColemanThe shift from drug use to misuse (i.e., abuse) depends <strong>on</strong> the extentof dysfuncti<strong>on</strong> within the family. Recently Ols<strong>on</strong> et al. (1979) developeda circumplex model to identify 16 types of marital and family systems.The circumplex model is based <strong>on</strong> the c<strong>on</strong>cepts of family cohesi<strong>on</strong> andfamily adaptability and has been used for both diagnosis and treatment.The authors define family cohesi<strong>on</strong> as “the emoti<strong>on</strong>al b<strong>on</strong>ding membershave with <strong>on</strong>e another and the degree of individual aut<strong>on</strong>omy a pers<strong>on</strong>experiences in the family system.” A high extreme of cohesi<strong>on</strong> is“enmeshment,” which is an overidentificati<strong>on</strong> with the family, resultingin extreme b<strong>on</strong>ding and limited individual aut<strong>on</strong>omy. “Disengagement”is the low extreme and c<strong>on</strong>sists of low b<strong>on</strong>ding and high aut<strong>on</strong>omy fromthe family. Ols<strong>on</strong> et al. hypothesize that a balanced degree of family358


cohesi<strong>on</strong> is necessary for effective family functi<strong>on</strong>ing and individualdevelopment.The sec<strong>on</strong>d dimensi<strong>on</strong> of the circumplex model is adaptability, which isdefined as “the ability of a marital/family system to change its powerstructure, role relati<strong>on</strong>ships and relati<strong>on</strong>ship rules in resp<strong>on</strong>se tosituati<strong>on</strong>al and developmental stress.” Both morphogenesis (change)and morphostasis (stability) must necessarily be balanced in an adaptivesystem.The circumplex model describes 16 possible types of marital and familysystems with accompanying labels or descriptive terms relative to thelevel of adaptability and cohesi<strong>on</strong>. These terms are used to describethe underlying dynamics of the marital/family system. The four typesin the center of the circle represent balanced levels of adaptability andcohesi<strong>on</strong> and are c<strong>on</strong>sidered as most functi<strong>on</strong>al to individual and familydevelopment. The four types in the extreme area of the circle reflectvery high or low levels of adaptability and cohesi<strong>on</strong> and are viewed asmost dysfuncti<strong>on</strong>al to individual and family development. In an effortto avoid the unidimensi<strong>on</strong>ality of many classificati<strong>on</strong> systems, whichassume a <strong>line</strong>ar relati<strong>on</strong>ship from <strong>on</strong>e end of the c<strong>on</strong>tinuum to theother, the circumplex model is dynamic and permits movement in anydirecti<strong>on</strong>.Although <strong>on</strong>ly four drug-dependent families were diagnosed accordingto the circumplex model, they were all found to have extreme scores<strong>on</strong> both the adaptability and cohesi<strong>on</strong> dimensi<strong>on</strong>s. The systems werevery different despite the comm<strong>on</strong>ality of the presenting problems andthe similarity of the extreme scores. This suggests that extremescores might be characteristic of families in which drugs are abused,i.e., more dysfuncti<strong>on</strong>al, whereas families that are experiencing casualdrug use could be placed closer to the central regi<strong>on</strong> of the circle,i.e., more functi<strong>on</strong>al. Within the c<strong>on</strong>text of the incomplete loss theory,the degree of pathology in family interacti<strong>on</strong>s might account for druguse becoming an abusive or addictive problem. It is suggested thatmany subtle factors accompany the loss experience, thus accountingfor variati<strong>on</strong>s between families. Although they may have similar etiologicalcomp<strong>on</strong>ents, the intervening family acti<strong>on</strong>s and reacti<strong>on</strong>s couldclearly account for different resp<strong>on</strong>se patterns. Certainly Eisenstadt’s(1978) c<strong>on</strong>cept of creative bereavement is supportive of this premise.It is assumed that Eisenstadt’s eminent subjects were able to mastertheir loss because their family systems were closer to optimal withregard to their cohesiveness and adaptability, placing them in a morecentral part of the circumplex model.LEARNED BEHAVIOR THEORY (p. 191)FrederickIn the transiti<strong>on</strong> from use to abuse, the most likely factor involved is<strong>on</strong>e of risk taking (Rd). Within a given pers<strong>on</strong>ality, if motivati<strong>on</strong> andhabits remain relatively c<strong>on</strong>stant, then merely an alterati<strong>on</strong> in risktakingbehavior in a negative directi<strong>on</strong> will be enough to tip the scalestoward drug abuse. Since the drug habit has already been formed byrepeated use, it appears probable that seeking a more dramatic form oftensi<strong>on</strong> reducti<strong>on</strong> becomes necessary. Seeking a “new high” or some359


other form of escape from the anxieties of the day would account forthe increase in risk-taking behavior. The likelihood of drug-seekingactivities mounts when the envir<strong>on</strong>ment or cultural associati<strong>on</strong>s aresuch that it becomes easy for the individual to engage in drug abuse.This is particularly prevalent in the milieu of the so-called drug culture,yet this phenomen<strong>on</strong> can occur in far more banal settings of everydaylife. The fact that our modern society has become oriented toward theacceptance of drug ingesti<strong>on</strong> sets the stage for easy learning of drugabuse later. Children who mimic parents often request pleasant-tastingaspirin for headaches, having witnessed the taking of many tranquilizers,analgesics, and soporifics by their mothers, fathers, and other adults.Needless to say, emulating the behavior of older teenagers is a part ofpeer-group pressure, which young people find increasingly difficult toresist. Teenagers, of course, do not c<strong>on</strong>stitute the <strong>on</strong>ly high-riskgroup <strong>on</strong> the current scene. A menopausal woman, for example, canaccomplish the same thing by obtaining prescripti<strong>on</strong>s from a variety ofphysicians with whom she makes c<strong>on</strong>tact. Substituting the numericalvalues shown in the basic formula will illustrate the point.COGNITIVE CONTROL THEORY (p. 8)GoldThe move from drug use to abuse or addicti<strong>on</strong> is seen as an intensificati<strong>on</strong>of the processes involved in the individual’s move from experimentati<strong>on</strong>to regular usage. The individual who is abusing drugs is nowunable to cope with anxiety and c<strong>on</strong>flict without the drug. <strong>Drug</strong>shave become’the <strong>on</strong>ly way abusers can feel good about themselves,cope with anxiety, and feel in c<strong>on</strong>trol. The stage of addicti<strong>on</strong> isreached because of a vicious cycle established by c<strong>on</strong>tinued use. Asdrug users rely more and more <strong>on</strong> drugs for feeling good and inc<strong>on</strong>trol, they repeatedly c<strong>on</strong>firm their belief that they are powerless tocope <strong>on</strong> their own. Each failure to functi<strong>on</strong> without drugs strengthensthe belief that drug-free coping is impossible. The vicious cycle iscomplete when the drug abuser is c<strong>on</strong>vinced that these fears are true;the addict is powerless to cope with the envir<strong>on</strong>ment without drugs.MULTIPLE MODELS THEORY (p. 18)GorsuchWith c<strong>on</strong>tinued drug use at a fairly heavy level, <strong>on</strong>e or both of twoadditi<strong>on</strong>al processes may occur. First, if drugs such as heroin areused <strong>on</strong> a daily basis then physical addicti<strong>on</strong> can occur, with thecomplicating factor of withdrawal problems. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, there are thoseindividuals who have trivial withdrawal symptoms or who use a n<strong>on</strong>addictingdrug but who nevertheless have made the drug a focal point oftheir lives. These individuals are c<strong>on</strong>sidered psychologically dependent.360


In the areas of psychological dependence and physiological addicti<strong>on</strong>,there is little research because of difficulties inherent in examining thephenomena. Retrospective reports of the more important variables areopen to subjective distorti<strong>on</strong> and forgetting, so interviewing a groupof those who have been addicted or dependent sufficiently l<strong>on</strong>g to besure the c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> exists provides little useful informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> manymatters of importance. In comparing both psychological and physiologicaladdicts with others, <strong>on</strong>e suffers from the problem of not knowingwhat is a cause of the addicti<strong>on</strong> and what is a result of the addicti<strong>on</strong>.The best research design, the l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal study, suffers from thefact that few individuals in the populati<strong>on</strong>s most readily accessible forl<strong>on</strong>gitudinal studies become addicts. For example, it would take astudy involving thousands of college freshmen to obtain a sufficientsample for research purposes of college seniors who could be definedas addicts. Futhermore, although the phenomen<strong>on</strong> of addicti<strong>on</strong> isapparent am<strong>on</strong>g l<strong>on</strong>g-term users, definiti<strong>on</strong>s which separate the c<strong>on</strong>tinueddrug user from the addict are difficult to develop for research purposes.Physiological addicti<strong>on</strong> is associated with a major shift away frommodels describing an initial drug experience. Whether the individual’spath was through n<strong>on</strong>socializati<strong>on</strong>, prodrug socializati<strong>on</strong>, or iatrogenicuse is no l<strong>on</strong>ger relevant. The primary feature now is satisfacti<strong>on</strong> ofthe physiological need and preventi<strong>on</strong> of withdrawal symptoms.The limited research that has been d<strong>on</strong>e <strong>on</strong> psychological dependenceindicates that it may stem from the “rush” experience or from thesocial reinforcement found in prodrug subcultures, where a pers<strong>on</strong> maydevelop a distinctive role for relating to others based up<strong>on</strong> the drugorientati<strong>on</strong>. In additi<strong>on</strong>, the research <strong>on</strong> aversive c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing suggeststhat the individual who finds that taking an illicit drug preventsa negative experience (such as physical pain or anguish) from occurringmay develop a particularly str<strong>on</strong>g dependence which is extremelyresistant to change even after the logical possibility of the negativeexperience becomes slight.Psychological and physiological addicti<strong>on</strong> are not mutually exclusive.While psychological dependence may well occur without physical addicti<strong>on</strong>,they may also appear together and reinforce each other.EXISTENTIAL THEORY (p. 24)GreavesAccording to Greaves’ existential theory, some individuals are highlysusceptible to drug dependency. These are primarily individuals whoare dysphoric and who, by virtue of adverse patterns of pers<strong>on</strong>alitydevelopment, have not learned to generate euphoria or to access alteredstates of c<strong>on</strong>sciousness in more normal and less destructive ways.The more severe the pers<strong>on</strong>ality disturbance, the lower the thresholdof abuse. Thus, severely disturbed users of drugs will abuse drugsdespite str<strong>on</strong>g peer and social disapproval and despite major negativesancti<strong>on</strong>s. Less severely disturbed individuals may be led to abusedrugs with sufficient peer support, but are malleable in their usepatterns depending <strong>on</strong> their envir<strong>on</strong>ment.361


With normal individuals the drug abuse threshold is high and anypeer-stimulated or automedicati<strong>on</strong> abuse tends to be situati<strong>on</strong>al andtransient, as c<strong>on</strong>tinued abuse tends to interfere with normal adaptiveand functi<strong>on</strong>al processes. In other words, healthy people cannot bec<strong>on</strong>sistently persuaded by other individuals or events to behave inneed-frustrating ways.Thus, the drug abuse threshold can be defined as an interacti<strong>on</strong>between an individual’s pers<strong>on</strong>ality state (healthy versus unhealthy),social factors (support versus dissuasi<strong>on</strong>), and transient interveningevents (crises versus stable states).ADAPTATIONAL THEORY (p. 195)HendinThe shift to abuse usually is a sign that the pressures of c<strong>on</strong>flictsinstigating the use are so great that larger doses of the drug areneeded while the relief given by the drug is now being counteractedby the psychological, physiological, and social complicati<strong>on</strong>s that resultfrom its use. For example, young women who hate school but feel theneed to comply with parental wishes for achievement may do so withthe aid of massive doses of amphetamines. At some point, however,the amphetamine toxicity often causes them virtually to cease functi<strong>on</strong>ing,and in the worst cases causes a transient psychosis.<strong>Abuse</strong> usually indicates that the drug is not helping the user in evenmarginal attempts to deal with problems. At this point it can become away of aband<strong>on</strong>ing these efforts. A man may take a few drinks toease his anxiety with a woman; he gets drunk to avoid having to dealwith her or with his anxiety. A student may use marijuana to easethe competitive struggle of academic life; he or she may become a “pothead” when the struggle becomes overwhelming (Hendin 1973a, 1975).Something of a dividing <strong>line</strong> exists between drug abusers who will usedrugs orally and those who will also inject them intravenously. Youngpeople who are almost perpetually st<strong>on</strong>ed may be nevertheless shockedat the idea of using drugs intravenously. Youngsters willing to do soare usually less self-protective, more reckless, and more self-destructivethan those who will not. Frequently their attitude toward life is thatthey do not have much to lose. The initiati<strong>on</strong> into intravenous use istherefore a critical variable suggestive of serious adaptive failure(Hendin 1974a, 1975).Most adolescents fluctuate in the intensity of their drug abuse. Duringperiods of less use, they tend to gravitate to friends who are notdrug abusers. During periods of their greatest abuse of drugs, theirrelati<strong>on</strong>ships with other drug abusers become more significant to them.Thus peer relati<strong>on</strong>ships seem to support the youngster’s immediateadaptive needs rather than to cause them (Pittel et al. 1971).362


SOCIAL DEVIANCE THEORY (p. 90)HillSome of the c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s necessary for transiti<strong>on</strong> from occasi<strong>on</strong>al druguse to abuse have been menti<strong>on</strong>ed in part 1. Their “direct” reinforcingproperties are ascribed to acceptance by the peer group and reducti<strong>on</strong>of hypophoria, anxiety, and pains after tolerance and withdrawaloccur. Wikler’s principles (1953) of c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing will almost surely befound to be operative, and some factors at which we now <strong>on</strong>ly speculatewill emerge.<strong>On</strong>ly in the last decade, 1970-1980, has a serious sec<strong>on</strong>d look beentaken at the role psychopathic and sociopathic characteristics may playin opiate and alcohol addicti<strong>on</strong> and in criminality. The str<strong>on</strong>g evidencerecently reported by Martin et al. (1977) is <strong>on</strong>e example of furtherpsychological and physiological differences between opiate addicts,instituti<strong>on</strong>alized alcoholics, pris<strong>on</strong>ers, and the normal populati<strong>on</strong>.BIOLOGICAL RHYTHM THEORY (p. 282)HochhauserIf the drug(s) is effective in c<strong>on</strong>trolling the chr<strong>on</strong>obiological rhythmsor in generating percepti<strong>on</strong>s of psychological c<strong>on</strong>trol, the use mayshift from a pattern of use to abuse. Since the drug use itself mayinterfere with chr<strong>on</strong>obiological processes (e.g., sleep patterns), theindividual may develop a vicious cycle behavior of using drugs toc<strong>on</strong>trol rhythms, which are then disrupted by the drugs, which leadsto more drug use, and so <strong>on</strong>. For the user/abuser, <strong>on</strong>e perceivedpositive aspect of drug dependence may be an initial feeling of c<strong>on</strong>trol--regular drug use may provide a relatively high degree of predictabilityand c<strong>on</strong>trollability. The addict in the early stage of addicti<strong>on</strong> mayhave a high degree of internal c<strong>on</strong>trol, especially if narcotic use iseffectively reducing levels of physical and/or psychological pain.DRUG SUBCULTURES THEORY (p. 110)Johns<strong>on</strong>A theme of the theory developed by Johns<strong>on</strong> (1973) involves the importanceof drug selling within and between drug subcultures. (Also seeSingle and Kandel 1978.) The reciprocity c<strong>on</strong>duct norms shift todistributi<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>duct norms when individuals begin to provide or sellmore drugs than they receive or buy for their own use. The distributi<strong>on</strong>c<strong>on</strong>duct norms change even more dramatically when the individualexpects close friends to pay cash for the drugs they receive. Anindividual attains the role of “dealer” within the subculture when(a) sales are made to pers<strong>on</strong>s other than close friends, (b) sales arelarge enough to provide the pers<strong>on</strong> and/or close friends with “free”drugs, or (c) the net income from sales becomes a substantial porti<strong>on</strong>of total income.363


A major indicator of what social-c<strong>on</strong>trol authorities refer to as drugabuse (although drug-subculture theory avoids this term) is the frequencyand amount of drugs dealt. Escalati<strong>on</strong> from casual transfers ofmarijuana between friends to the purchase and subsequent sale of apound or more of marijuana and mid-level sales of other drugs shiftsthe user/seller into a fundamentally different role in drug subculture.The dealer’s role is central to the drug subculture in several respects(Langer 1977). Mid-level dealers assure the availability of drugs toless frequent users; without dealers, supplies of drugs would be cutoff to the average user (indirectly or directly). Dealers are respectedbecause they take the risk of committing a fel<strong>on</strong>y for which a stiffpris<strong>on</strong> sentence could be imposed. Users know that the dealer mustpick friends and buyers carefully to avoid arrest. Dealing is frequentlya means of supporting the c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong> of more drugs (both in quantityand frequency) than most n<strong>on</strong>dealers use. Many dealers also obtain amajority of income from such activity. Dealers generally are verylikely to exhibit the most extreme patterns of drug-related behaviors;they also symbolize or teach innovative behaviors to those peer groupsand individuals to whom they sell (Carey 1968; Preble and Casey 1969;Johns<strong>on</strong> 1973; Blum and Associates 1972a; Langer 1977; Waldorf et al.1977; Johns<strong>on</strong> and Preble 1978; Smith and Stephens 1976). Dealersfrequently use large quantities of drugs for relatively little or no cashexpenditure, and a high proporti<strong>on</strong> exhibit other n<strong>on</strong>c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>albehaviors (crime, poor performance in legitimate roles). Thus, dealersare very likely (Waldorf et al. 1977; Johns<strong>on</strong> 1973; Single and Kandel1978) to be the heaviest drug abusers. These same behaviors, however,are widely respected, envied, and important to drug-subculture participantsand to the c<strong>on</strong>tinued maintenance of subcultural values andc<strong>on</strong>duct norms (Waldorf et al. 1977; Feldman et al. 1979).EGO/SELF THEORY (p. 29)KhantzianThe addicti<strong>on</strong>-pr<strong>on</strong>e individuals’ ego and self disturbances predisposethem to dependence <strong>on</strong> drugs, given the general and specific appeal ofdrugs. Given this appeal, there is a natural tendency in such individualsto use heavier and heavier amounts, resulting in physiologicaldependence <strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>e’s drug or drugs of choice. However, I also believethere is a psychological basis to depend increasingly <strong>on</strong> drugs. Ihave c<strong>on</strong>cluded that heavy drug use and dependence predispose pers<strong>on</strong>sto progressi<strong>on</strong> in their drug-use patterns, with a tendency to precludethe development of more ordinary human soluti<strong>on</strong>s to life’s problems.In repeatedly resorting to a drug to obtain a desired effect, theindividual becomes less and less apt to come up<strong>on</strong> other resp<strong>on</strong>ses andsoluti<strong>on</strong>s in coping with internal life and the external world. It is inthis respect that an addicti<strong>on</strong> takes <strong>on</strong> a life of its own. C<strong>on</strong>sequentlythere is an ever-increasing tendency for regressi<strong>on</strong> and withdrawalwhich is further compounded by society’s inclinati<strong>on</strong> to c<strong>on</strong>sider suchbehavior as deviant and unacceptable. Regressed and withdrawnindividuals discover that in the absence of other adaptive mechanismsthe distressing aspects of their c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> can be relieved <strong>on</strong>ly byeither increasing the use of this preferred drug or switching to otherdrugs to overcome the painful and disabling side effects of the originaldrug of dependence (Khantzian 1975).364


GENERAL ADDICTION THEORY (p. 34)LindesmithSince the theory places the source of craving in the experience ofrelieving withdrawal distress, it is centered <strong>on</strong> this stage--the shiftfrom use to abuse (addicti<strong>on</strong>). In order for this effect to occur, it isnecessary that the user correctly identify and understand this distress.Prior to this point of no return, she or he may have been totallyunaware of the identity of the drug and of becoming physically dependent<strong>on</strong> it; indeed, the user could have been unc<strong>on</strong>scious during thisentire preliminary period. But if the user recovered c<strong>on</strong>sciousnessjust before the drug was withdrawn, she or he could still become anaddict if the whole situati<strong>on</strong> was explained and if allowed to use drugsto cure the withdrawal distress that was being experienced and understoodfor the first time. It should be noted that addicti<strong>on</strong> produced inmedical practice by the administrati<strong>on</strong> of morphine to a patient with achr<strong>on</strong>ic, painful disease, such as terminal cancer, ordinarily involvesno self-administrati<strong>on</strong> of the drug. This form of addicti<strong>on</strong> shouldprobably not be identified as “drug abuse.”As implied by the theory, users’ first experiences with withdrawal in afairly severe form are sometimes sufficient to start a cognitive revoluti<strong>on</strong>in their minds as they begin to restructure their c<strong>on</strong>cepti<strong>on</strong>s of thedrug habit, of drug addicts, and of themselves. As the cravinggrows and expands with c<strong>on</strong>tinued use, they first begin to fear andthen to admit that they are junkies just like the other junkies theyknow.HYPERACTIVE ADOLESCENTS THEORY (p. 132)L<strong>on</strong>eyDiagnosis and drug treatment of the hyperkinetic/minimal brain dysfuncti<strong>on</strong>syndrome were not widespread until the sixties (Clements andPeters 1962; American Psychiatric Associati<strong>on</strong> 1968; Laufer and Denhoff1957), and adolescent followup studies of treated hyperkinetic childrendid not begin to appear until the seventies (Laufer 1971; Mendels<strong>on</strong> etal. 1971; Weiss et al. 1971). The majority of adults who were diagnosedand treated for childhood hyperactivity are still in their early twenties;and l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal studies of the precursors of drug use are <strong>on</strong>ly recentlybeing undertaken, even with normal samples (Kandel 1978b). Thus,the attenti<strong>on</strong> of most investigators is still focused <strong>on</strong> attitudes andinitial experimentati<strong>on</strong>, rather than <strong>on</strong> clear-cut abuse, and <strong>on</strong> alcoholand marijuana rather than <strong>on</strong> opiates (Kandel 1975). Even am<strong>on</strong>gat-risk populati<strong>on</strong>s, abuse is relatively infrequent during early adolescence.Because stimulant drugs have been the medicati<strong>on</strong> of choice forhyperactive children, the major fear has been of subsequent stimulantabuse due to treatment-produced changes in the children’s attitudestowards drugs. Therefore, it has seemed wise to study at-risk samplesdrawn from young, rural populati<strong>on</strong>s, who are known to prefer marijuanaand stimulants. At the same time, the infrequency of opiate abuseam<strong>on</strong>g rural hyperactive individuals may ultimately preclude effectivestatistical inference at any age.365


C<strong>on</strong>siderable fear has also been expressed that hyperkinetic childrenwill become “hooked” <strong>on</strong> their medicati<strong>on</strong> and c<strong>on</strong>tinue it <strong>on</strong> their own.It has been assumed that such an addicti<strong>on</strong> would be accompanied bythe usual signs of dependency: euphoria, tolerance, withdrawal, etc.Such signs have seldom been reported. Our own subjects described apanoramic assortment of reacti<strong>on</strong>s to medicati<strong>on</strong>. Most were “calmed”but a few were rendered oblivious and immobile, while a few became“wound up” and high. These more dramatic effects may be doserelated. Few of our subjects seemed to like the calmness that themedicati<strong>on</strong> produced; instead, they seemed to realize and eventually tovalue the fact that medicati<strong>on</strong> kept them out of trouble. In effect, themedicati<strong>on</strong> kept their parents and teachers calm as well. Less thanfive percent of the boys described positive mood reacti<strong>on</strong>s, and virtuallyall of those also had marked aggressive symptoms. Goyer et al.(1979) have presented a case study of an adolescent boy with anapparently addictive reacti<strong>on</strong> to treatment with a CNS stimulant; thatboy was clearly aggressive and antisocial as well as hyperactive.COMBINATION-OF-EFFECTS THEORY (p. 137)McAuliffe/Gord<strong>on</strong>In comm<strong>on</strong> parlance, pers<strong>on</strong>s are said to be “addicted” when they havebecome physically dependent or at least seem unable to refrain fromusing a drug. We regard these events as merely signalling that asufficient history of reinforcement has probably been acquired to impela high rate of use. In the case of str<strong>on</strong>g physical dependence, theuser is c<strong>on</strong>fr<strong>on</strong>ted with the necessity of resp<strong>on</strong>ding at a minimal rate(which happens to be also a high rate) if immediate use for whateverreas<strong>on</strong> is to c<strong>on</strong>tinue at all and if a negative reinforcer is to be successfullyavoided. In our theory, there is no single point at which anindividual suddenly becomes “addicted.” Instead, the individual’saddicti<strong>on</strong> develops insidiously and varies c<strong>on</strong>tinuously, so that whatothers seemingly mean when they label some<strong>on</strong>e an “addict” is merely apers<strong>on</strong> with a str<strong>on</strong>g addicti<strong>on</strong> (i.e., a history of reinforced drugtaking sufficient to outweigh the more acceptable reinforcers of life,such as are associated with <strong>on</strong>e’s job, family, friends, sex life, andrespectability).Physical dependence <strong>on</strong> opiates is neither a necessary nor a sufficientc<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> for the development of addicti<strong>on</strong>. Physical dependencesimply sets the stage for experiencing withdrawal distress, reducti<strong>on</strong>of which c<strong>on</strong>stitutes <strong>on</strong>e of the drug’s powerful reinforcing effects.Other effects (principally euphoria, but including sec<strong>on</strong>dary socialgains, and relief of pain, anxiety, and fatigue) can themselves produceor c<strong>on</strong>tribute to addicti<strong>on</strong>. Most, if not all, street addicts are reinforcedin the early stages of heroin use by effects other than withdrawal,and their drug-taking resp<strong>on</strong>se at that stage must be str<strong>on</strong>g enoughso that it occurs every day for a few weeks in order for them todevelop physical dependence. Since c<strong>on</strong>temporary opiate abusers knowabout physical dependence and usually prefer to avoid it, their dailyuse prior to dependence must reflect the existence of an addicti<strong>on</strong> ofsome strength. We have interviewed heroin users who had never beendependent but who were either adamant about wanting to c<strong>on</strong>tinueheroin use despite the risks and severe social pressures or c<strong>on</strong>vincedthat they could not stop even though they wanted to. We and other366


esearchers (Lindesmith 1947; Robins 1974a) have also interviewedpers<strong>on</strong>s who have used opiates compulsively <strong>on</strong> a daily basis for manym<strong>on</strong>ths without ever interrupting l<strong>on</strong>g enough to experience withdrawalsickness.The distincti<strong>on</strong> between addicti<strong>on</strong> and physical dependence is alsoevident in detoxified addicts who are temporarily free of dependencebut who are still str<strong>on</strong>gly addicted, as witnessed by their expresseddesire for opiates and their dispositi<strong>on</strong> to relapse, and in those medicalpatients who become physiologically dependent without knowing it butwho remain indifferent because they have not developed a str<strong>on</strong>gpsychological attachment to opiates. (See Lindesmith 1947 for examples.)Our theory implies that singling out any particular point in a reinforcementhistory as the stage of “addicti<strong>on</strong>” is more or less arbitrary. Werecognize, however, that there are advantages associated with employingphysical dependence as a tacit operati<strong>on</strong>al criteri<strong>on</strong> of “addicti<strong>on</strong>.”Because the withdrawal syndrome (1) is a salient phenomen<strong>on</strong> thatusually implies a substantial history of prior reinforcement, (2) introducesa potent new reinforcer, and (3) sets a new lower bound <strong>on</strong> therate of c<strong>on</strong>tinued use, the point at which physical dependence appearsserves as a useful peg <strong>on</strong> which to hang a definiti<strong>on</strong> of “addict” thatsignals important changes in lifestyle. This highly visible point dividesopiate users into those with and without such major lifestyle changeswith great efficiency (i.e., low false-positive and false-negative rates).Indeed some addicts date their being “hooked” from the time theyrecognized major changes in their lifestyle, such as intense craving,getting fired from their job, or realizing that they preferred heroin tosex (Hendler and Stephens 1977, p. 41).C<strong>on</strong>venient though it may be, there are important disadvantages associatedwith equating addicti<strong>on</strong> with physical dependence as laymen do,or with making it a necessary but not sufficient c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> of addicti<strong>on</strong>in a theory of opiate use (Lindesmith 1947). By encouraging thenoti<strong>on</strong> that physical dependence is necessary in order for addicti<strong>on</strong> tobe present, <strong>on</strong>e also encourages the seriously misleading impressi<strong>on</strong>--according to our theory--that a user is relatively safe as l<strong>on</strong>g asphysical dependence is avoided. This c<strong>on</strong>cepti<strong>on</strong> opens neophytes tothe insidious features of <strong>on</strong>set underscored by the reinforcementperspective, according to which predependence use is more dangerousthan seems apparent because the actual <strong>on</strong>set accrues gradually witheach reinforcement.COPING THEORY (p. 38)Milkman/FroschIsolati<strong>on</strong> of the transiti<strong>on</strong> from use to abuse is evasive because druginvolvement is viewed in the larger c<strong>on</strong>text of addictive processes.The transiti<strong>on</strong> to abuse is interpreted as that period in which theindividual begins the “progressive or repetitious patterns of socioculturallyand/or psychophysically determined seductive behaviors, detrimentalto the individual, the society, or both” (Milkman 1979). According tothis c<strong>on</strong>ceptual model, the individual may embark <strong>on</strong> an abusive styleof living prior to, during, or after involvement with substances.367


To be sure, c<strong>on</strong>tinued use of psychoactive substances often culminatesin marked deteriorati<strong>on</strong> of systems vital to the individual’s adaptivecommunity functi<strong>on</strong>ing. In the case of heroin, for example, prol<strong>on</strong>geduse may coincide with decrements in adaptive functi<strong>on</strong>s of the ego.Psychological deteriorati<strong>on</strong> combined with the pressures of physiologicaldependency sets the groundwork for a vicious cycle. The heroin usermust rely increasingly <strong>on</strong> a relatively intact ego to procure drugs andattain satiati<strong>on</strong>. Ultimately she or he is driven to withdrawal fromheroin by the discrepancy between intrapsychic needs and externaldemands. Hospitalizati<strong>on</strong>, incarcerati<strong>on</strong>, or self-imposed abstinencesubserve the user’s need to resolve growing c<strong>on</strong>flicts with reality.As in the case of heroin, the alterati<strong>on</strong>s induced by amphetamine areinitially harm<strong>on</strong>ious with the user’s characteristic modes of adaptati<strong>on</strong>.C<strong>on</strong>tinued failure, however, to achieve overinflated self-expectati<strong>on</strong>sleads to growing c<strong>on</strong>flicts with reality. Increasingly large and frequentpharmacologic supports are called up<strong>on</strong> to bolster failing ego defensemechanisms. The recurrent disintegrati<strong>on</strong> of mental and physicalfuncti<strong>on</strong>ing is a dramatic manisfestati<strong>on</strong> of the amphetamine syndrome.ACHIEVEMENT-ANXIETY THEORY (p. 212)MisraInitially, drugs are used to seek relief from the pressures of achievement(Misra 1976). Using drugs is relaxing; they provide a quick “chemicalvacati<strong>on</strong>” from the stresses and strains of living (Laws<strong>on</strong> and Winstead1978). Over a period of time, however, the increase in physicaltolerance, <strong>on</strong> the <strong>on</strong>e hand, and the desire for c<strong>on</strong>trolling <strong>on</strong>e’s periodsof relaxati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>on</strong> the other, tend to reduce the distance between thework life and the leisure-time activities. Achieving and maintaining afeeling of freedom--of n<strong>on</strong>achievement or, perhaps, antiachievement--becomes a crucial goal in life. It is at this point that drug use becomesdrug abuse. The goal is no l<strong>on</strong>ger freedom from the pressures ofachievement. Rather, it is to have a feeling of n<strong>on</strong>achievement. It isthe work ethic reversed: a thrill in not achieving.<strong>Drug</strong> abuse is, in a sense, a silent protest against the achievingsociety. It protects us from a sense of failure: I may not be achievingwhat my neighbors and colleagues are, but I do attain a unique feelingof relaxed carelessness. Addicti<strong>on</strong> forms the nucleus of a subcultureof people who all have the same feeling of n<strong>on</strong>achievement, and friendshipsand groups evolve around this theme as efforts are made tocreate and maintain fellowship am<strong>on</strong>g the addicts.ADDICTIVE EXPERIENCES THEORY (p. 142)PeeleAddicti<strong>on</strong> occurs al<strong>on</strong>g a c<strong>on</strong>tinuum, so that it is impossible to designatean exact point at which a drug habit becomes an addicti<strong>on</strong>.Viewing addicti<strong>on</strong> as an extreme at <strong>on</strong>e end of this c<strong>on</strong>tinuum, we cansay drug abuse is any use which tends to move the individual in thisdirecti<strong>on</strong> al<strong>on</strong>g the c<strong>on</strong>tinuum.368


There are several criteria in terms of which it is meaningful to evaluatea drug involvement for its addictive potential. Some of these criteriaderive from initial motivati<strong>on</strong>s for using a drug and from the motivati<strong>on</strong>sfor c<strong>on</strong>tinuing use. If a drug is used in order to eradicate c<strong>on</strong>sciousnessof pain, problems, and anxieties, then its use will tend to beaddictive. Another aspect of this type of abuse is the inability ofusers to derive pleasure from drug use, since they are relying <strong>on</strong> thedrug primarily to avoid unpleasantness rather than for any positiveeffect. In this case, a criteri<strong>on</strong> for abuse and addicti<strong>on</strong> is that thedrug is relied <strong>on</strong> at regular times for the very predictability of itseffects. The most crucial criteri<strong>on</strong> for the addictiveness of an involvementis whether use of the drug destroys or harms other involvements.For when this is the case, abuse moves inexorably al<strong>on</strong>g the c<strong>on</strong>tinuumtoward addicti<strong>on</strong> as other reinforcers fall away, and the drug experiencebecomes the primary source of reward for the individual.The sign of addicti<strong>on</strong> is the absence of a degree of choice about druguse. The sense of suitability or appropriateness, where certain situati<strong>on</strong>sor people rule out use of the drug, is lost. Also lost is thecapability for making discriminati<strong>on</strong>s with regard to the experience thedrug produces. That is, addicts will not reject a brand of cigarette,a type of alcohol, or a narcotic of inferior purity, since they areinterested in <strong>on</strong>ly the grossest sensati<strong>on</strong>s of the drug experience.Finally, identity and c<strong>on</strong>tinued functi<strong>on</strong>ing have become so c<strong>on</strong>nectedto the effects of the drug that it is impossible for the addict to c<strong>on</strong>ceiveof life proceeding without the drug.SOCIAL NEUROBIOLOGICAL THEORY (p. 286)PrescottThe transiti<strong>on</strong> from use to abuse of psychochemical substances accordingto somatosensory affecti<strong>on</strong>al deprivati<strong>on</strong> (SAD) theory is dependentup<strong>on</strong> the following factors:1.2.3.4.5.Time of <strong>on</strong>set of SAD.Durati<strong>on</strong> of SAD.Severity of SAD.Nature, quality, durati<strong>on</strong>, and time period during formative periodsof development of intervening, restorative, and rehabilitativeexperiences of somatosensory affecti<strong>on</strong>al relati<strong>on</strong>ships. Absence ofsuch experiences is c<strong>on</strong>sidered to be particularly pathogenic forabusive behaviors.Nature, quality, durati<strong>on</strong>, and time period during formative periodsof development of other experiences or factors that result inimpaired somesthetic and vestibular functi<strong>on</strong>ing, which interefereswith the rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> of somatosensory affecti<strong>on</strong>al processes. Ingeneral, it is the chr<strong>on</strong>ic failure, for whatever reas<strong>on</strong>s, to experiencethe enrichment of somatosensory affecti<strong>on</strong>al experiences in thec<strong>on</strong>text of meaningful relati<strong>on</strong>ships that sets the c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> for thetransiti<strong>on</strong> from use to abuse. Individuals who do not or cannotmake the transiti<strong>on</strong> from states of “reflexive” pleasure to states of369


“integrative” pleasure are at risk for making the transiti<strong>on</strong> fromsubstance use to substance abuse (Prescott 1977).NATURAL HISTORY PERSPECTIVE (p. 215)RobinsTypical patterns of changes in dosage of illicit drugs over time havebeen difficult to study because the strength of street drugs varies sogreatly over time and from <strong>on</strong>e locati<strong>on</strong> to another that changes infrequency of administrati<strong>on</strong> cannot be readily interpreted as changes indosage. In additi<strong>on</strong>, fluctuating availability and cost greatly influenceuse patterns. It does appear, however, that frequency of use tendsto increase over time, suggesting the development of tolerance to mostillicit drugs. How much tolerance develops can be studied <strong>on</strong>ly inexperimental settings where amount of access to drugs of standardquality is known. Such experiments have been carried out in pris<strong>on</strong>swhere pris<strong>on</strong>ers were allowed free access to marijuana cigarettes ofstandard quality. They were found to use up to 17 or 18 a day.Thus there may be a maximum amount of cannabis that can be metabolizedin a day, just as there is for alcohol.It is known that illicit drugs vary greatly in their addictive potential.It was inferred from laboratory experiments showing the high addicti<strong>on</strong>liability of heroin that first use of heroin would progress rapidly toregular use and then to daily use. This assumpti<strong>on</strong> seemed to bec<strong>on</strong>firmed by observing the high rate of relapse to addicti<strong>on</strong> of treatedaddicts, about two-thirds of whom generally appear to be readdictedwithin six m<strong>on</strong>ths after treatment (Stephens and Cottrell 1972). Recentresearch, however, shows that heroin as used in the streets of theUnited States does not differ from other drugs in its liability to beingused regularly or <strong>on</strong> a daily basis. O’D<strong>on</strong>nell et al. (1976) comparedthe frequency of progressi<strong>on</strong> to regular use am<strong>on</strong>g men who had everused a particular drug. He defined regular use as at least twice am<strong>on</strong>th. Progressi<strong>on</strong> to regular use was most comm<strong>on</strong> for alcohol. Allbut nine percent of drinkers drank at least as frequently as twice am<strong>on</strong>th. Stimulants and heroin had similar rates--about half of theusers ever became regular users. Marijuana showed the least progressi<strong>on</strong>to regular use, with <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e-third of users doing so. Am<strong>on</strong>gusers, likelihood of daily use was similar for heroin and for alcohol;that is, about <strong>on</strong>e-third of those who ever used either drug began touse it <strong>on</strong> a daily basis. Marijuana was next most comm<strong>on</strong>ly used <strong>on</strong> adaily basis, with <strong>on</strong>e-quarter progressing to that level, while <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>ein ten stimulant users ever became daily users.Our study of Vietnam veterans found this same pattern for heroin usein the United States. While most narcotic users in Vietnam had progressedto regular use, and half became addicted, in the States heroinwas not distinctive from other drugs in the likelihood that men wouldprogress to regular or daily use of it. It may well be that the highaddicti<strong>on</strong> liability of heroin found in laboratory experiments and inVietnam does not apply to the very adulterated product typicallypurchased in the streets.What was distinctive about heroin am<strong>on</strong>g the returned veterans wasthat daily users were much more likely to perceive themselves asdependent <strong>on</strong> the drug than were daily users of barbiturates, amphetamines,or marijuana.370


A comm<strong>on</strong> belief that has turned out to be largely a myth is that <strong>on</strong>ceheroin use begins, it tends to c<strong>on</strong>tinue indefinitely. O’D<strong>on</strong>nell et al.(1976) found that of all men aged 20 to 30 who had ever used heroin,<strong>on</strong>ly 31 percent had taken any of the drug within the last year.Their rate of c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> with heroin was lower than the c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong>rate for any other drug. Those who had ever used stimulants, sedatives,or cocaine had used some of that drug in the last year in about<strong>on</strong>e-half of cases. Those who had ever used marijuana had used somein the last year in two-thirds of cases. Those who had used tobaccoor alcohol had almost all used some within the last year. Thus thereseems to be much more movement out of heroin use than there is outof use of other drugs. There is remarkably little movement out of theuse of tobacco, despite health warnings by the Government.Again, the same findings applied to the Vietnam veterans. Nearly halfof them used narcotics at least <strong>on</strong>ce while in Vietnam, and more than<strong>on</strong>e-fourth had used them at least weekly there for a m<strong>on</strong>th or more.N<strong>on</strong>etheless, at the time we studied them when they had been back inthe States three years, they were hardly more likely to be usingnarcotics than were n<strong>on</strong>veterans. Thus we found no special likelihoodfor the use of heroin to persist even am<strong>on</strong>g those who had used itregularly. In their sec<strong>on</strong>d and third postwar years, veterans were nomore often readdicted than were n<strong>on</strong>veterans. (<strong>On</strong>ly two percent ofeither group were addicted at any time during this period.) Thereaddicti<strong>on</strong> rate of Vietnam addicts was <strong>on</strong>ly 12 percent within thethree post-Vietnam years. Our results and those of O’D<strong>on</strong>nell showthat, given the heroin market of the 1970s in the United States, it ispossible to use heroin occasi<strong>on</strong>ally without becoming addicted. It isstill not known how l<strong>on</strong>g such occasi<strong>on</strong>al use can persist. The timeover which addicts have used heroin prior to becoming addicted variesenormously, according to Waldorf (1973). The addicts he studiedreported use anywhere from three weeks to six years prior to theirfirst experience of addicti<strong>on</strong>.GENETIC THEORY (p. 297)SchuckitThe greatest impact of genetics might hypothetically occur in thetransiti<strong>on</strong> between use and abuse. The best data <strong>on</strong> this subject areavailable for alcohol.In a heavy-drinking society such as ours, str<strong>on</strong>g social factors probablypredominate in determining whether an individual will begin drinkingand in the decisi<strong>on</strong> to take the substance two, three, or more times.The genetically influenced biological factors might have their greatestimpact in explaining why in the mid-twenties to thirties most individualsdecrease their drinking, while some maintain their high level of intakeand even increase their c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong>.In the genetic theoretical framework, each individual enters life with avariety of genetically influenced factors which interact to give a levelof biological predispositi<strong>on</strong> toward alcoholism. The best guess, based<strong>on</strong> family and twin studies, would be either that multiple genes areinvolved (i.e., a polygenic inheritance) or that <strong>on</strong>e major gene exerts371


its effect differently in different circumstances (i.e., incomplete penetrance).This genetic predispositi<strong>on</strong> would help to explain why someindividuals go <strong>on</strong> to alcoholism after a number of years of limiteddrinking while others cut down their intake over time.The factors could be any <strong>on</strong>e or a combinati<strong>on</strong> of such things as adifferential metabolism of alcohol, a biologically mediated differentialsensitivity to the acute affects of alcohol, differences in subacuteaffects (e.g., acute tolerance), a differential sensitivity to organ-systemdamage in the presence of chr<strong>on</strong>ic exposure to alcohol, different predisposingpers<strong>on</strong>alities, etc. In each of these areas, the geneticallyinfluenced biological factors could help either to protect some peoplefrom becoming alcoholic (e.g., having an adverse acute reacti<strong>on</strong> toalcohol, such as str<strong>on</strong>g facial flushing [Seto et al. 1978]) or to predisposethe pers<strong>on</strong> toward alcoholism (e.g., having an acute reacti<strong>on</strong> toalcohol which is less intense than that of other individuals, thusleading to intake of higher levels of ethanol to obtain the same pleasanteffects as n<strong>on</strong>predisposed individuals).The pers<strong>on</strong>s thus predisposed would enter their early drinking yearsand progress over time to more frequent drinking and heavier intakeper occasi<strong>on</strong>. During their early twenties, the differences between“prealcoholics” and individuals not so predisposed could be obscuredby the heavy intake of the average pers<strong>on</strong>. At the critical stage inthe mid-twenties to thirties, where the average drinker is cuttingdown, the alcoholic begins to become more apparent through c<strong>on</strong>tinuedhigh intake and resulting life difficulties. The heavier the geneticloading toward alcoholism and the less intense the envir<strong>on</strong>mental factorswhich might protect <strong>on</strong>e from developing alcohol abuse, the earlier the<strong>on</strong>set of alcoholism and the more pervasive the alcohol problems arelikely to be.This level of biological predispositi<strong>on</strong> must, of course, interact withthe social and psychological envir<strong>on</strong>ment. Thus, a pers<strong>on</strong> carryingthe relatively light biological predispositi<strong>on</strong> who is raised in a stablefamily where abstenti<strong>on</strong> or moderate drinking is emphasized and who<strong>on</strong>ly experiences periods of stress in the presence of a generallysupportive envir<strong>on</strong>ment may never dem<strong>on</strong>strate alcoholism. Anotherpers<strong>on</strong>, with the same level of biological predispositi<strong>on</strong>, however, whohas a very tumultuous late adolescence, or who lives in a locati<strong>on</strong>where alcohol is readily available, or who in the early thirties tomid-thirties goes through a serious life stress such as a divorce willbe much more likely to dem<strong>on</strong>strate alcoholism despite the level ofgenetic loading.In adequately evaluating the possible genetic causes of alcoholism, it isnecessary to recognize that not every<strong>on</strong>e who becomes an alcoholic willhave an obvious family history of the disorder. In some instances,alcoholism may appear to “skip” a generati<strong>on</strong> if, for example, the s<strong>on</strong>of an alcoholic chooses not to drink or places heavy restricti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong>alcohol intake to avoid his father’s problems (an example of envir<strong>on</strong>mentalfactors overriding a genetic propensity), while his s<strong>on</strong> (i.e., thegrands<strong>on</strong> of an alcoholic), having no warning about alcoholism, attemptsto drink like every<strong>on</strong>e else <strong>on</strong>ly to end up an alcoholic. In otherinstances, a family history of alcoholism could be hidden because thefather or mother had already recovered from alcoholism by the time thechild was old enough to observe what was going <strong>on</strong>. Finally, alcoholismmust begin somewhere in a family <strong>line</strong>, and the alcoholic patient mightbe the first pers<strong>on</strong> in a family with the necessary genetic combinati<strong>on</strong>372


to raise the biological propensity for alcoholism bey<strong>on</strong>d the necessarythreshold for expressi<strong>on</strong> in that particular envir<strong>on</strong>ment.Similar hypothetical mechanisms can be invoked for other substances ofabuse. Because the data to date are inc<strong>on</strong>sistent, I favor the hypothesisthat the biological factors involved in the propensity toward alcoholismare different from those predisposing toward analgesic or opiate abuse,Polydrug misuse (i.e., abuse of multiple substances other than alcoholor opiates) may be either a separate entity or just the prodromal phasefor individuals who are likely to go <strong>on</strong> to opiate misuse or alcoholism.Of course, an opiate abuser who cannot obtain heroin is likely tomisuse alcohol temporarily until the preferred drug is available (perhapsin an effort to treat some protracted abstinence symptoms) (Schuckit1979a; Green and Jaffe 1977). <strong>On</strong>e cannot rule out the possibility thatif both alcoholism and heroin abuse are polygenically influenced disorders,the two problems might have a number of influential genes in comm<strong>on</strong>.However, the dissimilarities in age of <strong>on</strong>set and natural history ofthese two types of problems lead me to feel that the clearest researchapproach and hypothetical c<strong>on</strong>cept would be to look for differentgenetic factors for the misuse of separate drugs.AVAILABILITY AND PRONENESS THEORY (p. 46)SmartThe theory generally predicts a gradual movement from use to abuseor addicti<strong>on</strong> when both pr<strong>on</strong>eness and availability allow it. “<strong>Abuse</strong>”or use with harmful physical or social c<strong>on</strong>sequences, is most likely forthe heaviest users or those with the greatest initial pr<strong>on</strong>eness andavailability. <strong>Abuse</strong> resulting in criminality should also occur whenphysical or ec<strong>on</strong>omic availability is low. These points should see aturning to acquisitive crime in order for the drug to be obtained. Astrue addicti<strong>on</strong> is developed, pr<strong>on</strong>eness will lose its original importance,and availability will determine usage. In general, pr<strong>on</strong>eness is mostimportant in the early experimental, heavy-use, and n<strong>on</strong>addictedphases.PERCEIVED EFFECTS THEORY (p. 50)SmithThe factors that account for c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> of substance use also c<strong>on</strong>tributeto the escalatory process. There are, however, importantadditi<strong>on</strong>al processes that promote the escalati<strong>on</strong>. During the relativelyearly phases of escalati<strong>on</strong>, c<strong>on</strong>sciously recognized dangers associatedwith substance use can facilitate rather than inhibit use if those dangersare experienced as more exhilarating than anxiety-provoking; if theself-initiated risks bring the user status and social approval; or if theuser pits any perceived dangers against his or her competence andself-c<strong>on</strong>trol, and then treats the matter as a c<strong>on</strong>test which he or sheis sure to win. As l<strong>on</strong>g as the user c<strong>on</strong>tinues to perceive the overallgain as greater than the overall cost, use will c<strong>on</strong>tinue; and the riskof escalati<strong>on</strong> to more dangerous levels of use becomes more likely.373


AS escalati<strong>on</strong> progresses, cognitive functi<strong>on</strong>s (percepti<strong>on</strong>, memory, andjudgment) tend to be altered in a manner that restricts and vitiatesthe feedback available to the user regarding the benefits and costs ofuse. This undermines the reality testing processes that might otherwisealert the user to his or her increasing vulnerability. During theearliest stages of c<strong>on</strong>tinuing use, the ratio of benefit to cost is seductivelyattractive. As escalati<strong>on</strong> proceeds, c<strong>on</strong>victi<strong>on</strong>s based <strong>on</strong> earlierobservati<strong>on</strong>s may cause new and c<strong>on</strong>tradictory evidence to be discounted,misinterpreted, or denied altogether.As escalati<strong>on</strong> advances, there is an increasingly frequent and powerfulneed to use the substance not for pleasure but simply to avoid thephysical and psychic ag<strong>on</strong>y of abstinence. The importance of thisfactor varies from substance to substance and seems to be totallyinapplicable for some. C<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong> of six cups of coffee in quicksuccessi<strong>on</strong> will produce a rapidly accelerating negative effect no matterhow l<strong>on</strong>g the user has abstained from drinking coffee, but c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong>of six ounces of whiskey durinq a severe hangover will produce arapidly accelerating positive effect. Rapid development of unregulated,compulsive use is a serious danger with anv substance that can beingested to alleviate withdrawal distress resulting from previous ingesti<strong>on</strong>.It is well known, for example, that the aversiveness of abstinenceeffects is powerfully important in driving the heroin addict to readminister.LIFE-THEME THEORY (p. 59)Spotts/Sh<strong>on</strong>tzSometimes, the early stages of use of a drug of choice are experiencedas extremely pleasant, even overwhelmingly so. For example, a chr<strong>on</strong>icuser of amphetamine reported that his first injecti<strong>on</strong> of the drugproduced a reacti<strong>on</strong> so ecstatic that he has been seeking to recaptureit ever since. Whether the commitment of the chr<strong>on</strong>ic abuser to hisdrug of choice develops rapidly or slowly, however, it eventuallybecomes so intense and deep that the need for it becomes numinousand the user’s attachment takes <strong>on</strong> an almost religious t<strong>on</strong>e.This “soluti<strong>on</strong>” seems all the more desirable to the user, for the chosensubstance seemingly produces something akin to the desired ego statewithout any of the pain and suffering that genuine growth or individuati<strong>on</strong>would require. In this situati<strong>on</strong>, the substance becomes an objectof devoti<strong>on</strong>, if not actual worship, a counterfeit symbol of the desiredself. When this depth of attachment has been reached, the pers<strong>on</strong> isengaged in the ultimate of drug abuse, for his dependence up<strong>on</strong> itblocks further growth and endangers pers<strong>on</strong>al integrity and even lifeitself in many instances.Of c<strong>on</strong>siderable interest is the fact that few of the men we studiedreported that physiological addicti<strong>on</strong> to narcotics, in and of itself, wasa significant factor in causing them to c<strong>on</strong>tinue to use these drugs.However, it must be noted that we studied Midwestern addicts who,for the most part, had access to heroin of <strong>on</strong>ly two to three percentpurity. Our participants were aware of the reality of addicti<strong>on</strong> andthe pains and dangers associated with withdrawal. Indeed, some menavoided narcotics and used other substances instead, precisely because374


they feared addicti<strong>on</strong> to heroin. Nevertheless, few of the men westudied who used narcotic substances reported that they wanted toquit but could not because withdrawal was too painful. In fact, severaltook pride in the fact that they had endured withdrawal from heroinand other opiates al<strong>on</strong>e, <strong>on</strong> their own initiative, more than <strong>on</strong>ce. Atthe same time, it must be admitted that the two heaviest abusers ofpharmaceutical narcotics (hydromorph<strong>on</strong>e) we studied never attemptedwithdrawal and perhaps never will. So at very high levels of usage,it cannot be said that addicti<strong>on</strong> per se is never a factor in c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong>.As a group, the men we studied reported greater fear aboutmanaging withdrawal from barbiturates than from narcotics.FAMILY THEORY (p. 147)Stant<strong>on</strong>C<strong>on</strong>cerning the important factors in the shift from drug use to abuse,Kandel et al. (1976) propose that there are three stages in adolescentdrug use, each with different c<strong>on</strong>comitants. The first is the use oflegal drugs, such as alcohol, and is mainly a social phenomen<strong>on</strong>. Thesec<strong>on</strong>d involves use of marijuana and is also primarily peer influenced.The third stage, frequent use of other illegal drugs, appears c<strong>on</strong>tingentmore <strong>on</strong> the quality of the parent-adolescent relati<strong>on</strong>ship than <strong>on</strong> otherfactors. Thus, it is c<strong>on</strong>cluded that more serious drug misuse ispredominantly a family phenomen<strong>on</strong>.Regarding the relati<strong>on</strong>ship between fear of separati<strong>on</strong> that drug abusers’families show and the shift from use to abuse, again, abusers in mostcases do not become problematic until adolescence. It is at this pointthat they should be expected to actively engage in heterosexual andother intense outside relati<strong>on</strong>ships. If they do, however, they becomeless available and less attached to the family. Since they seem to bebadly needed by the family, their threatened departure can causepanic. C<strong>on</strong>sequently, the pressure not to leave is so powerful thatthe family will endure (and even encourage) terrible indignities suchas lying, stealing, and public shame rather than take a firm positi<strong>on</strong>.Families also tend to protect addicted children from outside agencies,relatives, and other social systems. Rather than accept resp<strong>on</strong>sibilitythemselves, families usually blame external systems, such as peers orthe neighborhood, for the drug problem. When parents take effectiveacti<strong>on</strong>, such as evicting their addicted offspring, they often undotheir acti<strong>on</strong>s by encouraging their return. Families seem to be saying,“We will suffer almost anything, but please d<strong>on</strong>’t leave us.” Thus itbecomes nearly impossible for addicts to negotiate their way out of thefamily, and they slip into greater abuse as a means for resolving thebind within which they are caught. The transiti<strong>on</strong> to abuse, then,can be seen as an example of a family getting stuck at a developmentalpoint in its life cycle and not being able to get bey<strong>on</strong>d it (Stant<strong>on</strong> etal. 1978).Even as a young adult the drug user may be closely tied into thefamily, serving much the same functi<strong>on</strong> as during adolescence when theproblem (probably) had its <strong>on</strong>set. This model of compulsive drug usefits many of the data and helps to explain the repetitiveness of seriousmisuse and the c<strong>on</strong>tinuity both (a) across generati<strong>on</strong>s, and (b) throughoutmuch of a compulsive user’s own lifetime. While there is evidence375


for more frequent substance abuse am<strong>on</strong>g parents of drug abusers,relative to parents of n<strong>on</strong>abusers (Stant<strong>on</strong> 1979b), the view presentedhere accentuates the importance of the “identified” patient in thefamily versus his or her siblings. The limitati<strong>on</strong>s of a simple “modeling”theory of drug abuse are underscored, since a particular offspring isusually selected for this role; all children in a family are not treatedsimilarly. Even if they all have equal opportunity to observe thedrug-taking patterns of their parents, they generally do not all takedrugs with equal frequency. Modeling parents’ behavior is <strong>on</strong>ly apartial explanati<strong>on</strong> of drug taking by their children.SELF-ESTEEM THEORY (p. 157)SteffenhagenThe self-esteem theory adequately explains the transiti<strong>on</strong> from use toabuse for all dependency-producing drugs. The individual with lowself-esteem moves easily to drug abuse because it provides immediategratificati<strong>on</strong>. Individuals with low self-esteem must defend themselvesagainst insecurity and are excepti<strong>on</strong>ally sensitive to changes in thesocial milieu. Given a situati<strong>on</strong> of perceived social stress they arelikely to abuse drugs as a mechanism of freeing themselves from socialresp<strong>on</strong>sibility. A l<strong>on</strong>ging for power to allay all feelings of inferioritycould also be provided by the drug.While low self-esteem is the basic psychodynamic mechanism underlyingdrug abuse, it accounts for individuals with different pers<strong>on</strong>alityc<strong>on</strong>stellati<strong>on</strong>s (different neurotic symptoms) choosing different drugswhich might be related to the pers<strong>on</strong>ality of the abuser, e.g., thetriad of neurotic symptoms manifested by the heroin addict: anxiety,depressi<strong>on</strong>, and craving.ROLE THEORY (p. 225)WinickThere are three criteria for a high likelihood of drug dependence:(1) access to dependence-producing substances, (2) disengagementfrom proscripti<strong>on</strong>s against their use, and (3) role strain and/or roledeprivati<strong>on</strong>. If <strong>on</strong>ly two criteria are met, there is a lesser likelihoodof a user becoming dependent. The transiti<strong>on</strong> to dependence is morelikely to be crossed when all three criteria are met.DEFENSE-STRUCTURE THEORY (p. 71)WurmserIn a narrow sense, wherever the (emoti<strong>on</strong>ally) compulsive aspectsprevail, regardless of presence of physiologic dependence, use goesover into abuse. The need for drugs assumes drive-like qualities; it376


ecomes peremptory, driven from within, less and less dependent <strong>on</strong>circumstance, feeding <strong>on</strong> itself, gratificati<strong>on</strong> calling for its own rigid,stereotypical, irresistibly demanded repetiti<strong>on</strong> (Kubie 1954)--as ischaracteristic for all neurotic symptoms, and particularly for sexualperversi<strong>on</strong>s. The use itself c<strong>on</strong>tributes directly to some of the majorunderlying c<strong>on</strong>flicts. For example, increased shame and sense offailure and defeat exacerbate the preexisting narcissistic c<strong>on</strong>flict, andthus it increases in turn the need for new pharmacological denial ofthe shame and low self-esteem. The transiti<strong>on</strong> from occasi<strong>on</strong>al to suchcompulsive use is usually not sharply delimited.Broadly defined, all use of mind-altering drugs that interferes withsocial, emoti<strong>on</strong>al, intellectual, or somatic functi<strong>on</strong>ing can be c<strong>on</strong>sideredabuse--far short of any compulsive pattern. Such substance abuse isenormously frequent; to speak of “transiti<strong>on</strong>” would not be appropriate.Characteristic of both broadly and narrowly defined substance abuse isthe superimposed screen of denials and of rati<strong>on</strong>alizati<strong>on</strong>s: that it isfun, natural, part of the social ambiance, d<strong>on</strong>e for curiosity, “everybodyelse does it,” and so <strong>on</strong>.377


Cessati<strong>on</strong>PERSONALITY-DEFICIENCY THEORY (p. 4)AusubelBecause of the almost miraculously efficacious adjustive properties ofnarcotics for inadequate pers<strong>on</strong>alities, users are reluctant to seekcures voluntarily. Very few (at most 20 percent at any given time),are under treatment (DeL<strong>on</strong>g 1975). Our experience at the Lexingt<strong>on</strong>Hospital indicates that patients apply for voluntary treatment mostlywhen they are at the point of apprehensi<strong>on</strong> by the law, when theywant to reduce the dose that is euphoric, or when they lose their“c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s.” Few remain to complete the treatment and almost allrelapse almost immediately to drug use up<strong>on</strong> release from the hospital(Pescor 1943b; Vaillant 1966c). This situati<strong>on</strong> was somewhat less truefor pris<strong>on</strong>er patients (Ausubel 1948; Vaillant 1966c). Why then dosome chr<strong>on</strong>ic addicts volunteer for MMTPs? Apparently, they tire ofthe c<strong>on</strong>tinuous hassle of supporting their habits and “settle” for aguaranteed kind of subliminal euphoria (e.g., freedom from psychictensi<strong>on</strong>), as l<strong>on</strong>g as it is free and licit, plus whatever euphoria theycan derive from polydrug abuse.Finally, cessati<strong>on</strong> of use seems to be an outcome of delayed (retarded)rather than arrested pers<strong>on</strong>ality maturati<strong>on</strong>. Most addicts are “burnedout” by their mid-forties and then settle down to a c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al existence.Addicts over 50 years of age are a statistical rarity.ADDICTION-TO-PLEASURE THEORY (p. 246)BejerotDisc<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> of a drug stimulati<strong>on</strong> which has reached addictive formmay occur for many different reas<strong>on</strong>s.The social counterforce against the addictive behavior may be sostr<strong>on</strong>g that the individual can no l<strong>on</strong>ger or dares not c<strong>on</strong>tinue drugstimulati<strong>on</strong>. In this way Mohammed, in the seventh century, forcedthe whole Islamic world out of alcoholism. During a 16-year period(1923-1939), the estimated rate of addicti<strong>on</strong> in the United States was378


educed by 90 percent by the use of a restrictive and c<strong>on</strong>sistent drugpolicy (Harney and Cross 1961). Between 1951 and 1953, about 20milli<strong>on</strong> opium addicts in China were rehabilitated by means of str<strong>on</strong>gsocial pressure. When I visited Peking in 1978, I was told that about90 percent stopped <strong>on</strong> their own, without interference from society.Shortly afterward (1954-1958). Japan eliminated a widespread epidemicof drug abuse in a similar way. Of the 600,000 estimated intravenousamphetamine abusers, it was <strong>on</strong>ly necessary to take acti<strong>on</strong> againstabout 20 percent; the rest stopped as a result of social pressure (Brilland Hirose 1969).Fear of medical complicati<strong>on</strong>s is a comm<strong>on</strong> reas<strong>on</strong> for disc<strong>on</strong>tinuingaddictive intoxicant behavior. The addict may have been frightenedby a paranoid intoxicati<strong>on</strong> psychosis (cocaine, amphetamine), a deathfrom overdose am<strong>on</strong>g friends, a severe abstinence experience (deliriumtremens), the threat of liver cirrhosis, etc.Inability to go <strong>on</strong> any l<strong>on</strong>ger with a far-too-expensive and hazardouslifestyle, when many relati<strong>on</strong>ships have become strained and complicati<strong>on</strong>sof all kinds pile up, is usually called “maturing out of addicti<strong>on</strong>.”This is not a general phenomen<strong>on</strong> but is associated with epidemicaddicti<strong>on</strong> and seems to require a restrictive policy in society in regardto illicit drugs. The phenomen<strong>on</strong> is seldom seen in therapeutic orcultural addicti<strong>on</strong>. If a society wages a prol<strong>on</strong>ged and intensivecampaign against the use of drugs, results may sometimes be achieved(the reducti<strong>on</strong> in tobacco smoking am<strong>on</strong>g physicians and upper classpeople during the last ten years, the fall in abuse of alcohol to <strong>on</strong>eseventhin Sweden during the sec<strong>on</strong>d half of the nineteenth century,etc.).Reducti<strong>on</strong> in pleasure stimulati<strong>on</strong> and rising discomfort should lead toan interrupti<strong>on</strong>. This phenomen<strong>on</strong> is sometimes seen am<strong>on</strong>g elderlyalcoholics. The situati<strong>on</strong> is reminiscent of the failing interest insexual activity <strong>on</strong> declining potency. Possibly both phenomena are theresult of a neurophysiological dec<strong>line</strong> in the effect of pleasure stimulati<strong>on</strong>with rising age.The introducti<strong>on</strong> of another str<strong>on</strong>g pleasurable experience to compensatefor the loss of drug stimulati<strong>on</strong> should lead to the disc<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> ofaddictive behavior. Religious salvati<strong>on</strong> is a typical example. <strong>On</strong>lyexcepti<strong>on</strong>ally can other events fill the same functi<strong>on</strong>. This is notsurprising since the drug experience is often more pleasurable thansexual satisfacti<strong>on</strong>.Systematic treatment should be menti<strong>on</strong>ed, even if in practice it stillplays a very small part, since ineffective treatment techniques, based<strong>on</strong> inadequate analyses and models of the nature of dependence, areusually employed. A prol<strong>on</strong>ged and thorough rec<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing of valuesis <strong>on</strong>e possible method (e.g., Daytop model), as are simpler forms ofbehavior modificati<strong>on</strong>. Unc<strong>on</strong>scious rec<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing (for instance,disulfiram medicati<strong>on</strong> to alcoholics without their knowledge) is unethicaland unsuitable in practice, but it is theoretically possible. C<strong>on</strong>sciouslyaccepted aversi<strong>on</strong> therapy of various types usually has <strong>on</strong>ly temporaryeffects but may act as a support in a wider program.379


DISRUPTIVE ENVIRONMENT THEORY (p. 76)CheinIn areas where drug taking is widespread, a certain number of comparativelyhealthy and normal pers<strong>on</strong>s will, through inc<strong>on</strong>tinent use of thedrug, develop physical dependence. Such users might be expected tobe capable of breaking the dependence. Indeed, this happens in somecases. But while some users manage to free themselves of the habit,most do not. In our investigati<strong>on</strong> of heroin use, both in delinquentgangs and in other cases we studied, there was some evidence that aminority of habitual users manage to disc<strong>on</strong>tinue drug use (in ourgang sample, there were 14 such cases out of 94 present or formerheroin users) (Research Center for Human Relati<strong>on</strong>s 1954c). But manymore--about <strong>on</strong>e-half--make the effort and fail (Research Center forHuman Relati<strong>on</strong>s 1957a).Given the multiple motives of drug abuse, cessati<strong>on</strong> of drug use withouteffective outside help is impossible for the majority of addicts, andlittle help is available. Users who are arrested sometimes receive somemedical attenti<strong>on</strong>, usually limited to easing the pains of withdrawal. Inour sample of 94 users who were members of gangs, more than <strong>on</strong>e-halfwere arrested at <strong>on</strong>e time or another, but <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e in ten received anymedical attenti<strong>on</strong> related to their use of drugs (Research Center forHuman Relati<strong>on</strong>s 1954c).Nor are parents of much help. Most do nothing. Those who do try,usually take drastic, punitive acti<strong>on</strong>, ordering the boy out of thehouse, taking him to court, or beating him. Or they rem<strong>on</strong>strate,giving expressi<strong>on</strong> to their hurt, dismay, and unhappiness. In general,few parents seem aware that anything effective can be d<strong>on</strong>e to helptheir children help themselves (Research Center for Human Relati<strong>on</strong>s1954a).In spite of the lack of help, about <strong>on</strong>e-half of the boys in our samplemade more than <strong>on</strong>e effort to stop using drugs. This was especiallytrue of those users who had not previously been delinquent and whocame from relatively cohesive families (Research Center for HumanRelati<strong>on</strong>s 1954a).Sometimes the most genuine help comes from the user’s own friends.Group workers report that gang members sometimes try to dissuadeother members who are increasing their intake of heroin (ResearchCenter for Human Relati<strong>on</strong>s 1954c). The nature of the support theygive indicates that they sense the basic oral needs and the unc<strong>on</strong>trollableanxiety of the users: They treat them to food, wine, or marijuana,and they try to be with them all the time and watch over them to helpat times of stress. The other boys intuitively feel that the user’sneed for support and his intolerance of anxiety are crucial factors inthe process of giving up the habit.Users do not take easily to psychotherapy. The experience of therapistsworking with juvenile users points to several comm<strong>on</strong> difficultiesin treatment: resistance to insight into inner problems, difficulty inestablishing rapport with and trust in the therapists, and ease ofrelapse. Apparently, having discovered an effective palliative in theform of the drug, the user finds it extremely difficult to give it upwithout at the same time getting some compensatory palliative. Many,380


if not most, users who have been hospitalized for a period of three tosix m<strong>on</strong>ths relapse immediately up<strong>on</strong> release (Research Center forHuman Relati<strong>on</strong>s 1957b; Riverside Hospital 1954). Most users mustexperience repeated failure in order to realize that they have beenoverestimating their powers of self-c<strong>on</strong>trol, that the trouble is notsimply an external “m<strong>on</strong>key <strong>on</strong> your back,” but that they have inherentpers<strong>on</strong>ality problems that must be dealt with if they are to be cured.The motivati<strong>on</strong> to be cured must be str<strong>on</strong>g. Also, recurrent opportunitiesfor therapy must be so structured that each successive cyclecan begin at a more advanced level so that repeated failures do notlead to the c<strong>on</strong>victi<strong>on</strong> that the struggle is hopeless. It is thereforenot surprising that even after a number of such cycles, very fewex-users can be said to be cured of the habit.<strong>Drug</strong> users need sustained help over a l<strong>on</strong>g period of time. Therapistswho have had some experience with youthful users and are searchingfor more effective ways of cure and rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> differ am<strong>on</strong>g themselvesas to which of several patterns of treatment is likely to prove mostsuccessful. There is general c<strong>on</strong>currence, however, c<strong>on</strong>cerning theneed to provide supportive and protective services for the addict inthe community.The main kind of support needed for the addict or postaddict is, ofcourse, a sustained therapeutically oriented relati<strong>on</strong>ship. Successfulcures are, as a rule, with those youngsters who succeeded in establishinggenuine c<strong>on</strong>tact with a therapist in an instituti<strong>on</strong> and who, up<strong>on</strong>release, c<strong>on</strong>tinue to see the same pers<strong>on</strong> in an aftercare clinic. Itwould obviously be desirable for the therapist to be able to commandservices which would help to cushi<strong>on</strong> the addict or postaddict fromunduly frustrating or anxiety-producing situati<strong>on</strong>s. Vocati<strong>on</strong>al guidanceand placement is <strong>on</strong>e such service. A “transiti<strong>on</strong> home” for thosewhose family situati<strong>on</strong> is too damaging and impedes their efforts atbetter adjustment is also advisable (Riverside Hospital 1954). Planningof leisure time and social c<strong>on</strong>tacts with n<strong>on</strong>delinquent peers who arenot involved with drugs is also of prime importance: Addicts usuallyagree that rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> is hopeless if <strong>on</strong>e returns to the same community,the same crowd of “junkies.”INCOMPLETE MOURNING THEORY (p. 83)ColemanThe resoluti<strong>on</strong> of the heroin problem is increasingly being sought bytreating the family. A nati<strong>on</strong>al survey of drug abuse and familytreatment (Coleman 1976; Coleman and Davis 1978) reported that 93percent of the resp<strong>on</strong>dent clinics (N =2,012) were providing some formof treatment to families. Stant<strong>on</strong>’s (1979d) review of the literature <strong>on</strong>family treatment of drug problems indicates that this approach and itsvariati<strong>on</strong>s, e.g., multiple family therapy, marital therapy, etc., areboth “beneficial and effective.”The incomplete loss theory is indeed dependent <strong>on</strong> family therapy inorder for delayed bereavement to be mastered. Some of the clinicalinterventi<strong>on</strong>s for directly dealing with unresolved loss have previouslybeen described by Coleman and Stant<strong>on</strong> (1978).381


The extent to which heroin abuse is disc<strong>on</strong>tinued depends also <strong>on</strong> thedegree to which families are able to restructure their relati<strong>on</strong>shippatterns, their power and c<strong>on</strong>trol systems, their roles, and theirfeedback mechanisms. In terms of the circumplex model, those drugdependentfamilies that are able to shift and rebalance their cohesi<strong>on</strong>and adaptability, according to life’s stress and change, will undoubtedlybe less apt to have a relapse of heroin abuse. As a c<strong>on</strong>sequence ofsevering the c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong> with the loss and grief, families generallydevelop a renewed sense of meaning, both individually and together.LEARNED BEHAVIOR THEORY (p. 191)FrederickWithout some change in virtually every factor in the drug abuse/addictiveequati<strong>on</strong>, even from a logical point of view, it is difficult toc<strong>on</strong>ceive of the cessati<strong>on</strong> of such str<strong>on</strong>gly reinforced behavior, bothphysiologically and psychologically. <strong>On</strong>ce deeply engrained into thepsyche and body of the abuser/addict, major changes are necessary inorder to diminish the behavior appreciably, to say nothing of itscessati<strong>on</strong>. Because an alterati<strong>on</strong> in pers<strong>on</strong>ality is less likely to occur,that factor has been left unaffected in our illustrati<strong>on</strong>s as <strong>on</strong>e of thecrucial links in the chain of events required for drug cessati<strong>on</strong>. Inpoint of fact, psychotherapy al<strong>on</strong>e is often insufficient to bring lastingchanges in ridding the individual of serious drug-taking behavior. Anessential comp<strong>on</strong>ent in cessati<strong>on</strong> is the n<strong>on</strong>reinforcement of key ingredientsin order to bring about extincti<strong>on</strong> of the previously c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>edbehavior.The ceasing of drug abuse or addicti<strong>on</strong> primarily involves changes inthree factors: destructive motivati<strong>on</strong> (Md), c<strong>on</strong>structive habit formati<strong>on</strong>(Hc), and destructive risk factors (Rd). In such a case, there is adiminuti<strong>on</strong> in the motivati<strong>on</strong> to engage in drug-related behavior and anincrease in habits that c<strong>on</strong>structively counteract stress. Simultaneously,there is a decrease in the risk factor which no l<strong>on</strong>ger tempts theindividual to partake in drug use. By substituti<strong>on</strong> of the appropriatevalues, as the formula shows, the proporti<strong>on</strong> has now reached 0.09and is thereby approaching zero, where all drug usage terminates.COGNITIVE CONTROL THEORY (p. 8)GoldEffective treatment of the drug abuser requires a multimodal therapyapproach. A therapeutic strategy must be developed to help theabuser cope with anxiety, modify faulty cognitive beliefs, learn appropriateinterpers<strong>on</strong>al skills, and interfere with intrusive and unpleasantimagery. <strong>Drug</strong> abuse affects all aspects of the abuser’s thinking,emoti<strong>on</strong>s, and behavior, and any therapy that has a narrow focus islikely to fail. The overall strategy is, therefore, to eliminate old382


patterns and develop new <strong>on</strong>es that help the individual see himself orherself as competent and in c<strong>on</strong>trol. To this end, a variety of therapeuticstrategies must be employed. Systematic desensitizati<strong>on</strong> may beused to help the abuser cope with anxiety, cognitive restructuring ornew “self-talk” may be needed to combat the individual’s expectati<strong>on</strong> offailure or rejecti<strong>on</strong>, and training in the use of imagery and fantasymay help the individual see himself or herself in a more positive lightand provide a means to rehearse new interpers<strong>on</strong>al skills.BAD-HABIT THEORY (p. 12)GoodwinThe use of drugs described in part 1 produces massive reinforcementbased <strong>on</strong> the combinati<strong>on</strong> of genetic vulnerability and classical c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing.It produces a “bad habit” that is singularly difficult toextinguish. Cessati<strong>on</strong> of use occurs (if at all) when the overall l<strong>on</strong>gtermill effects from drug use greatly outweigh the short-term positiveeffects. The addict stops, in my experience, because of fear of losinghealth or life, of losing a spouse and family, of losing a valued job,and, finally, of losing the respect of peers. Permanent cessati<strong>on</strong>occurs when the addict fails to resp<strong>on</strong>d to the multitude of c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>edstimuli associated with drug use. Surrounded by temptati<strong>on</strong>--drinkingcues--the c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed resp<strong>on</strong>se of drinking can be extinguished <strong>on</strong>ly if<strong>on</strong>e fails c<strong>on</strong>sistently to resp<strong>on</strong>d to the cues. After a time, followingthe laws of Pavlovian c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing, the habit will cease, although thismay take a very l<strong>on</strong>g time.MULTIPLE MODELS THEORY (p. 18)GorsuchHow c<strong>on</strong>tinued illicit drug use can be prevented after the initial drugexperience or disrupted after it has begun is a functi<strong>on</strong> of the modelmost appropriate for the initial drug experience. Since individualsentered into drug experiences by different paths and since at least theearly stages of c<strong>on</strong>tinued drug use are an extensi<strong>on</strong> of those paths,those paths must be disrupted for cessati<strong>on</strong> to occur. Treatmentimmediately after an initial drug experience would therefore be c<strong>on</strong>tingentup<strong>on</strong> diagnosis of which path was involved.The n<strong>on</strong>socialized individual would be identified by appropriate pers<strong>on</strong>alitytests showing low scores <strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>formity and resp<strong>on</strong>sibility scales.In additi<strong>on</strong>, descripti<strong>on</strong>s of the initial drug experience would--insofaras they avoided rati<strong>on</strong>alizati<strong>on</strong> and self-justificati<strong>on</strong> of the “they mademe do it” type--show that availability and lack of percepti<strong>on</strong> of socialc<strong>on</strong>straining factors were prime features in the initial drug experience.Preventi<strong>on</strong> of further drug involvement and c<strong>on</strong>tinued drug use wouldbe possible either by developing the pers<strong>on</strong> into a more resp<strong>on</strong>siblemember of traditi<strong>on</strong>al society or by reducing drug availability. D<strong>on</strong>ote that the n<strong>on</strong>socialized individual does not have high levels ofmotivati<strong>on</strong> for c<strong>on</strong>tinued drug use, and so social c<strong>on</strong>trol techniqueswhich prevent access to the drugs through, for example, limiting383


friendships may be both appropriate and effective if they can bepermanently established. Motivati<strong>on</strong> for the n<strong>on</strong>socialized user toc<strong>on</strong>tinue drug use can be decreased if other methods of meeting theneeds of novelty and sensati<strong>on</strong> seeking are found.latrogenic users will c<strong>on</strong>tinue with drug use if the physical pain ormental anguish recurs and another mode of resolving the problems isnot available. This would seem, therefore, to be an effective groupfor traditi<strong>on</strong>al medical and psychotherapeutic treatment to provideother sources of help to resolve their problems.The usefulness of these interventi<strong>on</strong>s for cessati<strong>on</strong> of c<strong>on</strong>tinued druguse with n<strong>on</strong>socialized or iatrogenic users depends up<strong>on</strong> their remainingwithin their distinctive pathways. If it is <strong>on</strong>ly possible to obtain thedrug through participating in a distinctive drug subculture, thenthese individuals may well shift to the prodrug socializati<strong>on</strong> model. Apers<strong>on</strong> shifting to this model would be identified by positive pastexperiences with drugs--which by themselves could produce prodrugsocializati<strong>on</strong>--and by their involvements with others in the drug subculture.It is the prodrug socialized group for which c<strong>on</strong>tinued use is theoreticallymost likely. Internal processes and social support systems encouragesome use of illicit drugs. The individual’s commitment to thedrugs means that he or she is more likely to seek out a drug if it isnot readily available than some<strong>on</strong>e who is functi<strong>on</strong>ing under anothermodel. Further, active countersocializati<strong>on</strong> probably needs to exist inthe envir<strong>on</strong>ment for cessati<strong>on</strong> to occur.Since the models for psychological and physiological dependence arerelatively undeveloped because of a very limited research base, cessati<strong>on</strong>processes for addicts are also relatively unknown. But if addicti<strong>on</strong>were purely physiological, then the medical detoxificati<strong>on</strong> techniquesshould work reas<strong>on</strong>ably well. Psychological dependence would beexpected to develop from a l<strong>on</strong>g history of reinforcements and wouldneed to be offset by a l<strong>on</strong>g series of counterreinforcements. A newsubculture may be necessary for most addicts.Extensive and thorough analysis of the effects of c<strong>on</strong>temporary treatmentprograms by Sells and his associates (e.g., Sells and Simps<strong>on</strong> inpress) is in keeping with the model. They found that using <strong>on</strong>lydetoxificati<strong>on</strong> as a cessati<strong>on</strong> technique was relatively ineffectual. Thiswould be expected since c<strong>on</strong>temporary street addicts are psychologicallyas well as physiologically addicted. (But it should be noted thatdetoxificati<strong>on</strong> techniques are widely accepted in the medical world aseffective for individuals who are <strong>on</strong>ly physiologically addicted as aresult of medical treatment and not psychologically dependent.)Psychological dependence up<strong>on</strong> drugs necessitates treatment for thatdependence as well as for the physiological comp<strong>on</strong>ent. Sells andSimps<strong>on</strong> (in press) have found that methad<strong>on</strong>e maintenance, therapeuticcommunities, and drug-free treatments are all effective, but that theformer two are most effective for addicts and the latter for n<strong>on</strong>addictedc<strong>on</strong>tinual users. Though they both disrupt the psychological dependence<strong>on</strong> illicit drugs, methad<strong>on</strong>e maintenance provides an alternativedrug, and therapeutic communities c<strong>on</strong>trol access and provide countersocializati<strong>on</strong>.In methad<strong>on</strong>e maintenance, the prodrug community is nol<strong>on</strong>ger needed because the methad<strong>on</strong>e is supplied through legal channels,and the therapeutic community effectively c<strong>on</strong>trols the individual’s384


envir<strong>on</strong>ment to prevent such involvements. <strong>Drug</strong>-free treatment isless successful with addicts because the addict remains in the envir<strong>on</strong>mentand c<strong>on</strong>tinues to have both access to the drugs and, probably,social support for their use.The current models suggest that for the n<strong>on</strong>socialized user methad<strong>on</strong>emaintenance is most favorable from a l<strong>on</strong>g-term perspective. Thetherapeutic community can be expected to be more effective for theprodrug socialized user since it offers the greater possibility forresocializati<strong>on</strong>. The drug-free approaches are primarily orientedtoward psychological dependence. This means that they will be moreeffective with c<strong>on</strong>tinual use, but because they have problems with thephysiological addicti<strong>on</strong> that accompanies daily use, drug-free treatmentapproaches will be less effective when both physiological and psychologicaldependence occurs.EXISTENTIAL THEORY (p. 24)GreavesCessati<strong>on</strong> can be brought about in <strong>on</strong>ly three major ways: by c<strong>on</strong>trollingthe availability of abused substances (source factors), by creatingan envir<strong>on</strong>ment in which the sec<strong>on</strong>dary gain from drug use is madeexcessively painful (social factors), or by voliti<strong>on</strong> (pers<strong>on</strong>al factors).The first two are seen as transient and artificial in the case of individualswith severely disturbed pers<strong>on</strong>alities, who will simply relapse <strong>on</strong>cethe external c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s are removed, but may be of benefit in terms ofbringing about and sustaining a detoxified state in more healthy abusers.In any event, voluntary cessati<strong>on</strong> is the <strong>on</strong>ly form of cessati<strong>on</strong> whichholds forth any promise of sustained cessati<strong>on</strong>. Voluntary cessati<strong>on</strong>can occur under either of two n<strong>on</strong>exclusive c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s: (1) throughinsightful realizati<strong>on</strong> that drugs are positively destructive to theindividual and through resolve to avoid their use whatever the emoti<strong>on</strong>alcost, and (2) through treating and training the individual to secureemoti<strong>on</strong>al and phenomenal states that are pleasant and substitutive for<strong>on</strong>going drug-induced states.The problem with insight and resolve is that the drive for the drug ofchoice remains, much energy is expended in mere coping, and theopportunity for relapse is high. The problem with treating the pers<strong>on</strong>alitydisorder which gives rise to drug abusing behavior is that veryfew therapists are trained in dealing with problems of dysphoria andexistential ennui, prime “illnesses of the spirit” which c<strong>on</strong>tribute todrug dependence.ADAPTATIONAL THEORY (p. 195)Hendin<strong>Drug</strong> abusers who stop often say they became repelled by their ownc<strong>on</strong>fused functi<strong>on</strong>ing. “I would dial teleph<strong>on</strong>e numbers and actuallyforget whom I was calling” said <strong>on</strong>e young man in describing his385


decisi<strong>on</strong> to disc<strong>on</strong>tinue two years of daily marijuana abuse that kepthim in a semiclouded state. He was able to come to this c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong><strong>on</strong>ly after he had resolved a difficult emoti<strong>on</strong>al situati<strong>on</strong> involving hisschool work and his family.Since stress is a major part of the pattern of use, a diminuti<strong>on</strong> ofstress can cause the drug pattern to markedly abate or stop. Youngsterswho abuse marijuana as part of a maladaptati<strong>on</strong> to school frequentlystop when they stop going to school, particularly if their families havelearned to accept reduced expectati<strong>on</strong>s for academic achievement.Those whose parents c<strong>on</strong>tinue to treat them with disapproval or c<strong>on</strong>temptare more likely to c<strong>on</strong>tinue their drug abuse (Hendin et al., in press).Young people who abuse marijuana to deal with problems related tocompetiti<strong>on</strong> and aggressi<strong>on</strong> may cease to do so if they manage to structuretheir lives so as to ease the pressure <strong>on</strong> them (Hendin 1973a;Hendin et al., in press). Young women who push themselves into anunwanted pursuit of achievement with amphetamines will usually stop ifthey alter their goals (Hendin 1974b). Young men who need heroin tofuncti<strong>on</strong> in relati<strong>on</strong>ships with women often stop when they are nol<strong>on</strong>ger in the relati<strong>on</strong>ship (Hendin 1974a).These young people manage more than a change in the external envir<strong>on</strong>ment.Many use drugs to strengthen psychological defenses and waysof adapting, and they then learn to maintain these without the drug(Hendin 1975). For example, young people who use psychedelics tofragment experience and detach themselves in ways that make them feelsafer may stop when they have achieved a detachment and fragmentati<strong>on</strong>that they can maintain without the help of the drug. Their moodwithout drugs has come closer to their mood with drugs and madedrugs less necessary.BIOLOGICAL RHYTHM THEORY (p. 262)Hochhauser<strong>Drug</strong> use may cease when it no l<strong>on</strong>ger serves to provide internalc<strong>on</strong>trol for the individual. The acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of alternative (n<strong>on</strong>drug)ways of coping may result in the cessati<strong>on</strong> of drug use, or the c<strong>on</strong>tinueduse of drugs may disrupt the chr<strong>on</strong>obiological rhythms so much thatthe cessati<strong>on</strong> of drugs is necessary to bring the rhythms back underinternal c<strong>on</strong>trol. That is, some addicts may find that narcotics serveto regulate their chr<strong>on</strong>obiological rhythms; others may observe thatnarcotics disrupt such rhythms, depending <strong>on</strong> dosage, time (in therhythmic cycle) of administrati<strong>on</strong>, etc. Finally, not using drugs mayprovide the addict with a greater sense of internal c<strong>on</strong>trol over percepti<strong>on</strong>sof helplessness.386


INTERACTIVE FRAMEWORK (p. 95)Huba/Wingard/BentierWe believe that the cessati<strong>on</strong> of drug use is a less homogeneous processthan the initiati<strong>on</strong> of use because there seem to be groups of individualsfor whom different influences are important. N<strong>on</strong>etheless, these differentgroups of individuals may all be c<strong>on</strong>sidered within the general frameworkof our theory and many different systems must be simultaneouslystudied.<strong>On</strong>e group of individuals seems to cease taking drugs because ofbehavioral pressure from the intimate support system. For this group,the major reas<strong>on</strong> for ceasing to use drugs is that use fails to bevalued within that set of individuals defined as important sources ofmodeling and reinforcement. A sec<strong>on</strong>d group of individuals is perhapsmore likely to quit of their own voliti<strong>on</strong> as a result of realizing undesirablechanges in their psychological or organismic status. A thirdgroup of individuals may change their drug-taking behavior as afuncti<strong>on</strong> of some interventi<strong>on</strong> by the sociocultural influence system,usually arrest or forced treatment. This process may operate in partbecause of product unavailability. Finally, some small group of individualsmay cease taking a drug because of limited ec<strong>on</strong>omic resources.DRUG SUBCULTURES THEORY (p. 110)Johns<strong>on</strong><strong>Drug</strong> subcultures theory hypothesizes that drug use and abuse maydiminish or cease if and when commitments to subcultural values,norms, and rituals dec<strong>line</strong> or terminate for any combinati<strong>on</strong> of reas<strong>on</strong>s.Such diminuti<strong>on</strong> in use may be due to reducing interacti<strong>on</strong> or terminatingfriendships with drug-using peer groups or associates. Peer groupsand individuals may switch preferences in drug use because of changingdrug fads, dec<strong>line</strong>s in availability of a substance, or an increase inavailability of another drug. Individuals may switch reference groupsand orient themselves toward n<strong>on</strong>drug activities and associates. N<strong>on</strong>usingfriends (or those who are moderate users), parents, spouse, or legalauthorities may exert direct pressure to reduce or terminate use. Forthe heaviest drug users--who are frequently sellers--a decisi<strong>on</strong> to stopdealing or to sell <strong>on</strong>ly to close friends may reduce the amount of drugsc<strong>on</strong>sumed.Critical changes in the life cycle appear to be associated with l<strong>on</strong>g-termdiminuti<strong>on</strong> and almost complete cessati<strong>on</strong> of involvement in drug subculturesand drug use. Evidence from nati<strong>on</strong>al surveys (Abels<strong>on</strong> et al.1972, 1973, 1977; Abels<strong>on</strong> and Atkins<strong>on</strong> 1975; Abels<strong>on</strong> and Fishburne1976; O’D<strong>on</strong>nell et al. 1976; Johns<strong>on</strong> 1978) and local surveys (Kandel1978b; Brown et al. 1974; Divisi<strong>on</strong> of Substance <strong>Abuse</strong> Services 1978;Johns<strong>on</strong> and Uppal, in press) indicates that the assumpti<strong>on</strong> of adultroles significantly decreases participati<strong>on</strong> in the drug subculture forlarge segments of the regularly using populati<strong>on</strong>. Particularly importantto diminishing use are marriage, parenthood, full-time employment, andassociated changes in friends and peer groups (Brown et al. 1974).Involvement in these adult roles occupies major proporti<strong>on</strong>s of the387


working day, as well as leisure time activities. Little or no effort isexpended to seek drug supplies or associates with whom to use drugs.If, however, such pers<strong>on</strong>s attend social functi<strong>on</strong>s where drugs, especiallymarijuana, are being used, they may be influenced by the subculturalc<strong>on</strong>duct norms of that peer group to use again. But these will beisolated episodes, which will not occur until another similar socialoccasi<strong>on</strong> arises.SELF-DEROGATION THEORY (p. 128)KaplanCessati<strong>on</strong> of the drug abuse (or other deviant pattern) would be likelyto occur if and when self-devaluing outcomes outweigh self-enhancingoutcomes. In that case the subject would be likely to experiment withalternative modes of deviance, since normative patterns would c<strong>on</strong>tinueto be motivati<strong>on</strong>ally unacceptable as l<strong>on</strong>g as they were subjectively andin fact associated with self-devaluing experiences. But insofar asindividual maturati<strong>on</strong> and correlated changes in socioenvir<strong>on</strong>mentalexperiences (including social support systems) reduce the likelihood ofself-devaluing experiences, offer new opportunities for self-enhancement,and provide the pers<strong>on</strong> with effective coping mechanisms and a correlatedrealistic sense of c<strong>on</strong>trol over the envir<strong>on</strong>ment, the illicit drug use islikely to cease in favor of normative resp<strong>on</strong>se patterns.EGO/SELF THEORY (p. 29)KhantzianThe addict’s relati<strong>on</strong>ship with and dependence <strong>on</strong> a substance are theresult of failures to find more ordinary soluti<strong>on</strong>s to human problems ofcoping with emoti<strong>on</strong>al distress and seeking satisfacti<strong>on</strong> for <strong>on</strong>e’s needsand wants. <strong>Drug</strong>s have been substituted as an extraordinary soluti<strong>on</strong>for a range of problems, but particularly as a means to cope withmajor ego and self disturbances. However, the drug “soluti<strong>on</strong>s” are atbest short term and tenuous, and the l<strong>on</strong>g-term dependence <strong>on</strong> drugshas serious, maladaptive aspects and c<strong>on</strong>sequences. As a result,addicts understandably are very often ambivalent about their substances.Often c<strong>on</strong>sequences such as legal, medical, and interpers<strong>on</strong>al crisesthat result from l<strong>on</strong>g-term drug use break down the rati<strong>on</strong>alizati<strong>on</strong>sand denial that have supported c<strong>on</strong>tinuing drug use and dependency.At these times, alternative soluti<strong>on</strong>s and satisfacti<strong>on</strong>s become possibleand realizable and may, for the first time in some and <strong>on</strong>ce again inothers, make possible the replacement of drugs with human involvementssuch as alternative compulsive (but benign) activities, religious immersi<strong>on</strong>,relati<strong>on</strong>ships, and becoming the treator (versus the treated).This may occur with or without treatment interventi<strong>on</strong>s or relati<strong>on</strong>ships.388


GENERAL ADDICTION THEORY (p. 34)LindesmithTheoretical attenti<strong>on</strong> centers <strong>on</strong> voluntary cessati<strong>on</strong> of use when drugsare available to the user. The cognitive features of the proposedtheory offer two <strong>line</strong>s of explanati<strong>on</strong> for this phenomen<strong>on</strong>.The first is that in the process of getting hooked, a revoluti<strong>on</strong> occursin the addicts’ self-c<strong>on</strong>cept. They cannot escape the fact that theyhave become pariahs, viewed with disfavor and str<strong>on</strong>g disapproval inthe culture of which they are part. Prior to their own addicti<strong>on</strong>, theuser had usually shared these views. Beginning addicts thus face aloss of self-esteem and tend to become ambivalent. <strong>On</strong> the <strong>on</strong>e hand,they cannot help but crave the drug; <strong>on</strong> the other, they are unhappyabout bel<strong>on</strong>ging to a group viewed with str<strong>on</strong>g suspici<strong>on</strong> and dislike.They therefore resolve to kick the habit and sometimes succeed forvarying periods of time. During such periods of abstinence, the otherside of their ambivalence tends to take over and usually leads them toaband<strong>on</strong> the effort. With increasing age and durati<strong>on</strong> of addicti<strong>on</strong>, itappears that such periods of voluntary abstenti<strong>on</strong> become l<strong>on</strong>ger andmore frequent and more often permanent.The sec<strong>on</strong>d point is that as regular daily c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong> is c<strong>on</strong>tinued,users notice that they are getting less and less at a higher and highercost. The main effect of the drug is now to maintain “normalcy”between shots. “Highs” become progressively more brief and difficultto obtain. The ensuing and growing disillusi<strong>on</strong>ment may c<strong>on</strong>tribute toa decisi<strong>on</strong> to quit the habit, a decisi<strong>on</strong> made slightly more palatable bythe realizati<strong>on</strong> that even short-term abstenti<strong>on</strong> will restore the initialsedative-euphoric effects of the drug and reduce the size of the habit.HYPERACTIVE ADOLESCENTS THEORY (p. 132)L<strong>on</strong>eyWhile our theory is silent to date <strong>on</strong> the determinants of naturallyoccurring reducti<strong>on</strong> or cessati<strong>on</strong> of drug use, it does suggest thatpreventive and treatment efforts might aim at reducing children’saggressi<strong>on</strong> and/or at improving certain aspects of their envir<strong>on</strong>ments.Behaviorally oriented parent training (Patters<strong>on</strong> 1976) comes quickly tomind as a way to interrupt the processi<strong>on</strong> from temperamental irritabilityto childhood disobedience and fighting to adolescent substance abuseand delinquency. Treatment with CNS stimulants is often initiated inthe hope that it will prevent the development of a variety of sec<strong>on</strong>daryemoti<strong>on</strong>al and behavioral problems, including drug abuse, and it isoften withheld or disc<strong>on</strong>tinued for fear of harmful side effects, includingdrug abuse. In fact, available findings indicate that neither the hopenor the fear is warranted. When we are able to compare medicatedand n<strong>on</strong>medicated children at adolescence, we will be better able todetermine the c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s under which early treatment with CNS stimulantshas either iatrogenic or immunizing effects <strong>on</strong> subsequent drug abuse.389


COMBINATION-OF-EFFECTS THEORY (p. 137)McAuliffe/Gord<strong>on</strong>An addict comes to disc<strong>on</strong>tinue drugs in two ways: (1) by beingphysically prevented from c<strong>on</strong>tinuing, for example, through incarcerati<strong>on</strong>,and (2) by choosing to stop, at least temporarily. <strong>On</strong>ly the sec<strong>on</strong>drequires explanati<strong>on</strong>. According to our theory, drug taking stopsvoluntarily because of changes in c<strong>on</strong>tingencies of reinforcement. Thekey theoretical questi<strong>on</strong>s c<strong>on</strong>cern how the c<strong>on</strong>tingencies change.Much research suggests that opiate use stops initially because <strong>on</strong>e ormore of the numerous risks surrounding illicit drug use suddenlybecomes imminent (e.g., threat of incarcerati<strong>on</strong>, medical complicati<strong>on</strong>ssuch as overdose and hepatitis, aband<strong>on</strong>ment by spouse and family,loss of job, and psychological depressi<strong>on</strong>). Ordinarily, drug usepersists despite these risks because, in c<strong>on</strong>trast with the immediacyand certainty of the rewarding effects of opiates, the risks are usuallypsychologically remote and often discounted (Hendler and Stephens1977, p. 40). Moreover, throughout much of their careers, manyaddicts succeed in avoiding these unwanted outcomes; those hazardsthat are encountered are either relatively minor (e.g., a misdemeanorc<strong>on</strong>victi<strong>on</strong>) or are made tolerable as l<strong>on</strong>g as <strong>on</strong>e has heroin; and someof the difficulties are so gradual in <strong>on</strong>set that addicts are able toadjust to them.However, from time to time in the lives of most street addicts in oursamples (e.g., McAuliffe and Gord<strong>on</strong> 1974) and in Waldorf’s (1973),the addict is c<strong>on</strong>fr<strong>on</strong>ted by a crisis in which <strong>on</strong>e or more of the majorrisks suddenly impends. An example of a crisis in the life of a typicalstreet addict would be getting arrested for burglary and finding thatas a result his wife was leaving and he was being fired. With thec<strong>on</strong>tingencies of overall reinforcement so abruptly changed, the addictwill often alter his behavior if a reas<strong>on</strong>able path opens to him. Perhapsby entering a methad<strong>on</strong>e program he can avoid prosecuti<strong>on</strong> and placatehis wife and employer as well.Disc<strong>on</strong>tinuance of drug use occurs in similar ways for other kinds ofaddicts. Physician addicts generally c<strong>on</strong>tinue taking opiates untildiscovered by authorities (Winick 1961a). When then threatened withloss of their license to practice medicine and c<strong>on</strong>strained by suspensi<strong>on</strong>of their prescribing privileges, physicians ordinarily stop using drugs,at least temporarily (J<strong>on</strong>es and Thomps<strong>on</strong> 1958). Here, the unwelcomechanges in employment and lifestyle c<strong>on</strong>tingencies are drastic indeed,and easy access to the positive reinforcement of opiates can be terminatedeffectively by outside interventi<strong>on</strong>. Soldiers who became mildlyaddicted in Vietnam also experienced marked changes in their circumstanceswhen returned to the United States, and as a result, althoughthey had been euphoria-seeking users, most stopped using heroin(Robins 1974a; Gord<strong>on</strong> 1979). The changes of behavior by addictsunder adverse c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s are c<strong>on</strong>sistent with observati<strong>on</strong>s from laboratorystudies showing that both animals and humans reduce the frequencyof their drug-taking resp<strong>on</strong>se in the face of increased workrequirements and punishment (Griffiths et al. 1978, pp. 29-31).Addicts vary in the extent to which their stopping drug use indicatesan intenti<strong>on</strong> to abstain permanently. Our Baltimore street addictsreadily distinguished between what they termed “sincere” efforts at390


stopping and other occasi<strong>on</strong>s when, for example, entering a methad<strong>on</strong>eprogram was regarded as merely a temporary expedient adopted becauseof social pressure from family and the justice system or because ofexhausti<strong>on</strong> resulting from the hardships and demands of the addictlifestyle (Agar 1973; Preble and Casey 1969).Indicati<strong>on</strong>s are that street addicts, even when “sincere,” seldom disc<strong>on</strong>tinueopiates because they have lost interest in the positive effectsopiates provide. Street addicts rarely claim that they stopped becausethey no l<strong>on</strong>ger liked the high; it is the life that they can no l<strong>on</strong>gerabide (Brown et al. 1971, p. 641). Waldorf (1973, p. 147) points outthat most addicts use heroin heavily right up to the point of stopping--there is no gradual tapering off. (See also Robins 1974a, pp. 1, 35.)<strong>On</strong>ce in a methad<strong>on</strong>e program, addicts often use heroin, other drugs,or alcohol as supplementary or substitute intoxicants (Bazell 1973;Bourne 1975; McGlothlin 1977, tables 1 and 2; Stephens and Weppner1973, table 3; Weppner et al. 1972, table 3). Similarly, addicts receivingantag<strong>on</strong>ist therapy comm<strong>on</strong>ly stop taking the antag<strong>on</strong>ist so thatthey can again enjoy the effects of opiates (Curran and Savage 1976;Haas et al. 1976). This persistence of the potential for enjoyingopiate euphoria, in combinati<strong>on</strong> with the relative permanence of areinforcement history <strong>on</strong>ce acquired, plays a crucial role in relapseeven for earnest disc<strong>on</strong>tinuers, and by default places the major burdenfor motivating abstinence <strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tingencies located outside the drugeffects proper.Abstinence from heroin use does not always represent a radical readjustmentin lifestyle, for many abstaining addicts compensate by increasingtheir use of alcohol or other drugs, including less demanding opiatedrugs such as cough medicines c<strong>on</strong>taining codeine, and paregoric.<strong>Drug</strong> effects of somewhat lower quality are thus achieved at less costand risk. Waldorf (1973) found that 51 percent of his sample admittedsubstituting excessive use of other drugs or alcohol when stoppingheroin use: 24 percent drank heavily, 13 percent used drugs toexcess, and 14 percent did both. Methad<strong>on</strong>e maintenance may beviewed as an instituti<strong>on</strong>alized example of this substituti<strong>on</strong> method ofgiving up heroin, and it is noteworthy that methad<strong>on</strong>e programs havefound that many patients also supplement their methad<strong>on</strong>e with otherdrugs or alcohol (Bazell 1973; Bourne 1975, p. 101; McGlothlin 1977;Stephens and Weppner 1973; Weppner et al. 1972).It is important to recognize that by substituting “less serious” drugsfor heroin, addicts follow a pattern which Kandel (1975) has also foundam<strong>on</strong>g adolescent users of many different drugs. <strong>Drug</strong> users do notregress directly to n<strong>on</strong>use, but to lower categories of less seriousillicit drugs or to legal drugs. Thus, substituti<strong>on</strong> of less seriousillicit drugs may be an indicati<strong>on</strong> of partial rehabilitati<strong>on</strong>, even if it isnot the desired end point of the rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> process. (For a similarview, see Goldstein 1976b.)In our view, successful reintegrati<strong>on</strong> into c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al society, sharingin its rewards, and avoiding the active peer group are essential forl<strong>on</strong>g-term or permanent abstinence by addicts. When addicts weresuccessful in finding or reuniting with a spouse or girlfriend and infinding a job, this success was comm<strong>on</strong>ly cited as a factor in promotingabstinence. Most of the addicts found that they were happy livingmore c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al lives and felt no need for drugs or socializing withother addicts. Stephens and Cottrell (1972) point out that although391


addicts with jobs had a significantly better chance (14 percent) ofremaining abstinent, 81 percent did relapse.Goldstein (1976b) has emphasized the reciprocal effects of reducingdrug involvement and of social rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> each other. Sinceprogress al<strong>on</strong>g either of these dimensi<strong>on</strong>s can easily be upset by asetback <strong>on</strong> the other, this perspective helps, al<strong>on</strong>g with the psychopharmacologicalfactors of the preceding secti<strong>on</strong>, to account for theapparent fragility of abstinence (e.g., Ray 1961; Waldorf 1970).Individual differences in adopti<strong>on</strong> of the stereotypical addict lifestylehelp explain the abstaining addict’s subsequent readjustment to c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>alsociety. We (McAuliffe and Gord<strong>on</strong> 1974) and other researchers(Brotman and Freedman 1968; Stims<strong>on</strong> 1973) have found that addictsvary greatly in the extent to which they embrace the stereotypicaladdict lifestyle. Some addicts never become str<strong>on</strong>gly oriented towardheroin’s pleasures; they c<strong>on</strong>tinue to work and have a family, and theyrarely commit crimes. Other research has shown that such individualsare more likely to remain abstinent <strong>on</strong>ce they stop using heroin thanare addicts who are more like the hardcore addict stereotype (McAuliffeand Gord<strong>on</strong> 1974).Although many observers have noted that the most c<strong>on</strong>sistent predictorsof c<strong>on</strong>tinued abstinence are the addict’s age and length of addicti<strong>on</strong>(e.g., Waldorf 1970), there are a number of possible interpretati<strong>on</strong>s ofthis tendency. Winick (1962a) c<strong>on</strong>cluded that addicts stopped usingopiates as they matured because the crises of youth, which Winickassumed originally sparked drug use am<strong>on</strong>g most addicts, were nol<strong>on</strong>ger operative. There has been <strong>on</strong>ly some evidence to substantiateWinick’s theory, and other explanati<strong>on</strong>s may be offered. Anotherpotentially c<strong>on</strong>tributing factor is the tendency of an age cohort ofaddicts to be diminished in size by attriti<strong>on</strong> due to death, incarcerati<strong>on</strong>,and remissi<strong>on</strong> (Robins and Murphy 1967). Thus, the negative c<strong>on</strong>sequencesof addicti<strong>on</strong> also take their toll indirectly via their effects <strong>on</strong>the addict peer group as a whole. Older addicts, therefore, have aless potent subculture to resist, since their addict friends and closeacquaintances--pers<strong>on</strong>s most likely to offer them a shot--have becomefewer in number. Moreover, we have found that older addicts tend tosee the social aspects of drug use as less rewarding as time goes <strong>on</strong>.Whereas most of our resp<strong>on</strong>dents at first preferred shooting up withother addicts more than shooting up al<strong>on</strong>e, by the time of interviewthey preferred shooting al<strong>on</strong>e. Thus, for the older addict usingheroin may be less attractive than it was for the younger addict inmany respects.Being a heroin addict becomes harder and harder as the addict careerc<strong>on</strong>tinues (McAuliffe 1975b). <strong>On</strong>ce c<strong>on</strong>victed of several crimes, theaddict will be well known to the police. Subsequent c<strong>on</strong>victi<strong>on</strong>s arelikely to result in l<strong>on</strong>g sentences and little hope of parole. A numberof our resp<strong>on</strong>dents menti<strong>on</strong>ed that they have abstained because theyfelt certain that they would end up in jail again and they had hadenough of incarcerati<strong>on</strong>. The risk of pris<strong>on</strong> thus no l<strong>on</strong>ger seemspsychologically remote. Moreover, sources of m<strong>on</strong>ey for drugs otherthan crime also dry up. Jobs become harder to get, and family,spouse, and n<strong>on</strong>addict friends now refuse to help the addict anymore.Veins collapse so that intravenous use is difficult or impossible (e.g.,McAuliffe and Gord<strong>on</strong> 1974, p. 822), and the health of older addictsoften deteriorates to the point that they can no l<strong>on</strong>ger endure thehardships of the addict lifestyle. Many ex-addicts claim that they392


ecame tired of the demanding lifestyle of “ripping and running.”(For a descripti<strong>on</strong> of the demands, see Agar 1973; Preble and Casey1969.) The prospects of pursuing the life of the heroin addict againmust appear rather grim to the older abstaining addict. It is notsurprising that many find the normal life of an abstaining ex-addict,tame though it may seem, as the more desirable of the opti<strong>on</strong>s available.COPING THEORY (p. 38)Milkman/FroschCessati<strong>on</strong> of a pattern of substance abuse usually occurs in the c<strong>on</strong>textof cognitive/emoti<strong>on</strong>al reorganizati<strong>on</strong>. Fears of societal reprisal andphysical deteriorati<strong>on</strong> combined with increasingly sophisticated grouptreatment techniques may lead to the selecti<strong>on</strong> of alternate or substitutemodes of adaptati<strong>on</strong> and gratificati<strong>on</strong>.Prior to cessati<strong>on</strong>, an individual may change his or her drug of choice,c<strong>on</strong>current with intrapsychic redistributi<strong>on</strong>s. The amphetamine user,for example, may encounter repeated failure to achieve overinflatedself-expectati<strong>on</strong>s leading to increased deficits in self-esteem and theaband<strong>on</strong>ment of over-compensatory defense mechanisms. Heroin,barbiturates, or alcohol may become the subsequent drug of choice.In some cases (e.g., alcoholism), religi<strong>on</strong> may serve as a potent alternativeto former styles of living. In the case of heroin, identificati<strong>on</strong>with n<strong>on</strong>-drug-oriented members of the therapeutic community mayprovide an alternative sense of bel<strong>on</strong>ging and group identificati<strong>on</strong>. Insome instances, relatively sp<strong>on</strong>taneous recovery, with little or notherapeutic interventi<strong>on</strong>, is observed. The body may develop a physiologicintolerance for a particular chemical (e.g., alcohol), or theindividual may discover more developmentally mature mechanisms forcoping (e.g., new interpers<strong>on</strong>al relati<strong>on</strong>ships).ACHIEVEMENT-ANXIETY THEORY (p. 212)MisraThe cessati<strong>on</strong> of drug use is perhaps directly related to a decisi<strong>on</strong> tochange <strong>on</strong>e’s lifestyle. In a clinical sense, drug abuse is a variant ofcoping behavior. However, drug addicti<strong>on</strong> is indicative of a way oflife, with its own beliefs and values. Two unique features of thislifestyle are (a) complacency toward time and space and (b) denial ofresp<strong>on</strong>sibility. Therapeutic programs for drug addicts should c<strong>on</strong>sidersetting realistic goals for the clients. It must be emphasized thattreating addicti<strong>on</strong> is not the same as treating, say, a case of influenza.The target symptoms are not easy to identify. We have, perhaps, todeal with a whole lifestyle and not just a symptom or two.An addict is more or less a symptom of a “sick” social system. He orshe symbolizes the resp<strong>on</strong>se to the anxiety of achievement. Helpingaddicts should be a very slow and gradual process by which they(a) are encouraged to develop a sense of resp<strong>on</strong>sibility and (b) are393


persuaded to limit their behavior within the c<strong>on</strong>straints of time andstructure. This is not easy. It would not be unrealistic, for instance,to expect 60 percent of the clients in a drug program to exhibit a 40percent increase in their sense of respect for time (e.g., keeping thecounseling appointments) after being in the program for six m<strong>on</strong>ths.The goal of the drug programs should be to improve the employabilityof the clients, rather than to cure addicti<strong>on</strong>. Developing a sense ofrespect for time and structure seems to be a more realistic goal thanhelping addicts to stop abusing drugs.ADDICTIVE EXPERIENCES THEORY (p. 142)PeeleTo cease being addicted to a drug, <strong>on</strong>e must develop the ability toderive real rewards from the world to replace the unrealistic rewardsthat the drug provides. Such rewards include those which come frombasic competence, from the ability to carry out meaningful work whichis rewarded by others, from the capacity to form intimate relati<strong>on</strong>shipswith other people, and from having a comfortable and satisfying relati<strong>on</strong>shipgenerally with <strong>on</strong>e’s envir<strong>on</strong>ment. While it may be necessary torestrict or eliminate drug use in order to accomplish these goals,simple cessati<strong>on</strong> of use in no way implies that these goals are accomplished.A pers<strong>on</strong> will need to develop alternative means for gratificati<strong>on</strong> whichwill supersede the drug experience. This may be accomplished in anumber of ways, including an analysis of the feelings which led to useof the drug, explorati<strong>on</strong> of more functi<strong>on</strong>al methods of coping withthese feelings, and practicing acti<strong>on</strong>s which are incompatible withreliance <strong>on</strong> the drug experience. Initially, these behaviors may beirresolute and inadequate to offset the rewards the user feels the drugprovides. During this transiti<strong>on</strong> period, it may be necessary to utilizean artificial or therapeutic setting to help establish the new patternsof activity and self-reliance.There are instances of self-initiated programs for removing the reliance<strong>on</strong> a drug. These can occur with any drug--from cigarettes, toalcohol, to narcotics. The greatest amount of research has been d<strong>on</strong>e<strong>on</strong> those who cease to be addicted to a narcotic, the process of “maturingout.” What happens in these cases is that individuals--frequentlyadolescents--become addicted to heroin at a time when they are incapableof forming a solid relati<strong>on</strong>ship with the world <strong>on</strong> their own. Subsequently,they either replace the drug addicti<strong>on</strong> with a dependence <strong>on</strong>an instituti<strong>on</strong>--such as a hospital or a jail--or their capabilities andself-c<strong>on</strong>cepts mature to a point at which they can become drug free(Winick 1962a).394


SOCIAL NEUROBIOLOGICAL THEORY (p. 286)PrescottCessati<strong>on</strong> of use is dependent to a very large degree up<strong>on</strong> an individual’sability to change the social, physical, and cultural envir<strong>on</strong>mentthat would make possible the restorati<strong>on</strong> of somatosensory affecti<strong>on</strong>alexperiences within the c<strong>on</strong>text of meaningful human relati<strong>on</strong>ships.Without this change, cessati<strong>on</strong> of use becomes extremely difficult andshort lived. Purely cognitive strategies to induce change are unlikelyto be successful. The basic psychophysiology of attachment processesmust be treated so that affecti<strong>on</strong>al b<strong>on</strong>ds can be restored in order toeffectively realize cessati<strong>on</strong> of use. Psychopharmacological therapiesthat directly stimulate somatosensory and somatopleasure processes ofthe CNS/ANS may be a necessary first step in the process of somatosensoryaffecti<strong>on</strong>al rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> in particularly difficult cases. Thetransiti<strong>on</strong> from psychopharmacological therapies to somatosensory affecti<strong>on</strong>altherapies is a necessary and essential transiti<strong>on</strong> for the realizati<strong>on</strong>of cessati<strong>on</strong> of substance abuse. Altered vestibular functi<strong>on</strong>ing,hydroflotati<strong>on</strong> and hydrosuspensi<strong>on</strong> therapies, and massage and somesthetictherapies to reintegrate the vestibular-somesthetic and other sensoryprocesses appear necessary for the rec<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong> and rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> ofthe psychophysiological mechanisms of attachment behaviors. Thedegree to which those psychophysiological mechanisms can be rehabilitatedfor the purpose of establishing affecti<strong>on</strong>al b<strong>on</strong>ds will determine in largepart the nature and durati<strong>on</strong> of cessati<strong>on</strong> of substance abuse.NATURAL HISTORY PERSPECTIVE (p. 215)RobinsCross-secti<strong>on</strong>al studies of young people generally find more drug useam<strong>on</strong>g the single, and those without full-time jobs. <strong>Drug</strong> use is alsorare am<strong>on</strong>g those over 30. Together these facts suggest for thisnatural history of drug abuse up to the point of addicti<strong>on</strong> that druguse probably tends to diminish with aging and as young people takeup traditi<strong>on</strong>al roles of marriage and work. As yet, there are too fewl<strong>on</strong>gitudinal studies following drug users through the terminati<strong>on</strong> phaseto be certain that these are the correct inferences to draw. It ispossible that young people who enter adult roles early are just thosewho never used drugs.GENETIC THEORY (p. 297)SchuckitCessati<strong>on</strong> can be understood <strong>on</strong>ly in the c<strong>on</strong>text of the natural historyof substance abuse, especially alcoholism. Alcoholics do not get drunkin their mid-twenties and stay intoxicated until the day they die.Rather the natural history of this disorder appears to include periodsof abstinence, times of limited or “c<strong>on</strong>trolled” alcohol intake, andperiods of excessive alcohol intake with resultant problems. These395


individuals appear to move sp<strong>on</strong>taneously from <strong>on</strong>e state to another,and thus, whatever the causes of alcoholism in the first place, thecourse of the problem includes temporary cessati<strong>on</strong> of drinking whichalternates with periods of exacerbati<strong>on</strong> of problems (Schuckit 1979a;Smart 1976b; Ludwig 1972). The most likely explanati<strong>on</strong> for the seriesof exacerbati<strong>on</strong>s and remissi<strong>on</strong>s is a changing balance between factorspredisposing individuals to drink and those making them tend to stopor at least to cut back.Biologically mediated genetic factors may play a role in this temporaryremissi<strong>on</strong>. For example, genetically influenced metabolism of alcoholmight change over time, the development of tolerance might mandatethat a pers<strong>on</strong> stop or cut down <strong>on</strong> intake in order to be able to begindrinking or abusing drugs again at a lower level, genetically influencedorgan sensitivity to alcohol might lead to such severe illness that anindividual must stop to “take a breather,” etc. These hypothesizedfactors probably interact with envir<strong>on</strong>mental events which lead to acrisis, a reevaluati<strong>on</strong> of the cost versus the benefits of drug use ordrinking, and a resolve to (at least temporarily) stop the intake of thesubstance in order to preserve a marriage, keep a job, avoid problemswith the police, etc.Cessati<strong>on</strong> of abuse can be l<strong>on</strong>g-term or even permanent. L<strong>on</strong>g-termfollowups of drug abusers and alcoholics have dem<strong>on</strong>strated a rate ofpermanent “sp<strong>on</strong>taneous remissi<strong>on</strong>” (or at least resp<strong>on</strong>ses to n<strong>on</strong>specificinterventi<strong>on</strong>s) in 10 to 30 percent of substance abusers (Smart 1976b;Drew 1968; Vaillant 1973). This sp<strong>on</strong>taneous remissi<strong>on</strong> is, <strong>on</strong>ce again,probably due to a combinati<strong>on</strong> of genetically influenced biologicalfactors and envir<strong>on</strong>mental events. It may relate to changes in uniqueattributes of metabolism, acute reacti<strong>on</strong>s to the drug, subacute reacti<strong>on</strong>s,chr<strong>on</strong>ic vulnerabilities, or pers<strong>on</strong>ality factors associated with increasingage. Added to this might be the development of more end-stage organdisease, probably influenced by genetic factors, which make the individualso ill that c<strong>on</strong>tinued misuse is impossible. At the same time, therecogniti<strong>on</strong> with increasing age of <strong>on</strong>e’s own mortality coupled with thenumber of years invested in a job or in a marriage may combine tocreate an envir<strong>on</strong>mental force which, becoming str<strong>on</strong>ger each year,finally precludes any further substance abuse.<strong>On</strong>e final note must be said about the alcoholic or drug abuser whoseems to return to achieve “c<strong>on</strong>trolled” use of a substance. Evenwhen <strong>on</strong>e excludes the temporary periods of abstinence and low levelsof abuse which are seen in the course of most substance disorders asdescribed above, there remains an unknown percentage of individuals(probably around 10 percent) who do seem to be able to return toc<strong>on</strong>trolled use over a protracted period of time (Orford et al. 1976).A number of these individuals probably had sec<strong>on</strong>dary alcoholism,usually with primary affective disorder, with the result that theirability to drink or use drugs in a moderate manner returns as so<strong>on</strong> asthe primary disorder goes into remissi<strong>on</strong> (Schuckit and Winokur 1972).For the rare primary alcoholic or primary drug abuser who does returnto c<strong>on</strong>trolled substance use over an extended period of time, <strong>on</strong>e couldhypothetically invoke the same types of genetic and envir<strong>on</strong>mentalfactors discussed above regarding sp<strong>on</strong>taneous remissi<strong>on</strong>.396


AVAILABILITY AND PRONENESS THEORY (p. 46)SmartWhen availability disappears totally, all drug use must, by definiti<strong>on</strong>,cease. More problematic is what occurs when availability decreases bysmaller amounts. It would be anticipated that most curious or experimentingusers will be willing to make a limited amount of effort toobtain a drug. Likewise, they will be sensitive to price rises, whichare likely to discourage greatly their further drug use. The curiousstudent with no spare cash is unlikely to start using cocaine at $50per time unless it can be obtained free. Most experimenters whosought <strong>on</strong>ly a brief experience with a drug would desist from furtheruse if the price went up greatly, if far more effort was required toobtain it (e.g., going to a new city or social group), or if they hadto take more risk (e.g., associate with known criminals). Most wouldstop drug use altogether, wait for a more propitious time, or shift toanother more available drug. Probably changes in the availability ofparticular drugs explain the comm<strong>on</strong> finding of multidrug use am<strong>on</strong>gusers.With drug addicts (i.e., opiate addicts), cessati<strong>on</strong> of use will dependup<strong>on</strong> large changes in pr<strong>on</strong>eness or availability. Since they will havewithdrawal symptoms, they will be unlikely to stop usage because ofsmall price rises or decreases in physical accessibility. They will raisemore m<strong>on</strong>ey or shift to different dealers or locales or to a new drugwith similar effects (e.g., from opiates to alcohol or barbiturates).Total cessati<strong>on</strong> of use will, in practice, depend more up<strong>on</strong> zero or lowavailability than <strong>on</strong> reducti<strong>on</strong>s in pr<strong>on</strong>eness. Reducti<strong>on</strong>s in availabilityin the life of the addict occur because of supply problems (policeactivities), geographic changes (as in the case of Vietnam veterans),c<strong>on</strong>finement in jails, or admissi<strong>on</strong> to a drug treatment program fordetoxificati<strong>on</strong> or other l<strong>on</strong>g-term stay. Reducti<strong>on</strong>s in psychological orsocial pr<strong>on</strong>eness seem less likely for addicts, as they would result frommajor life readjustments, intensive and effective psychotherapy, orother rare events.PERCEIVED EFFECTS THEORY (p. 50)SmithWhatever the amount, frequency, and pattern of substance use, cessati<strong>on</strong>will not occur until the user perceives the disadvantages of use asoutweighing the benefits. The subjective character of this cost-benefitrelati<strong>on</strong>ship is emphasized because in many (perhaps most) instancesthe user perceives use as having a net positive effect l<strong>on</strong>g after mostoutside observers would have c<strong>on</strong>cluded that the cost-benefit relati<strong>on</strong>shiphad shifted from positive to negative.Cessati<strong>on</strong> is a single event, but it reflects the outcome of a protractedprocess of assessment that has been <strong>on</strong>going (c<strong>on</strong>sciously and unc<strong>on</strong>sciously)throughout the period of c<strong>on</strong>tinuing use. Factors thatdetermine when (if ever) cessati<strong>on</strong> will be perceived as being moreadvantageous than c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> include the following: changes in theuser’s life circumstances; increasing anxiety and c<strong>on</strong>cern regarding397


various potential losses associated with c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong>; reduced effectivenessof defenses that impair the reality testing processes by whichcosts and benefits of use are assessed; substituti<strong>on</strong> of more costeffectivesatisfacti<strong>on</strong>s for those previously obtained through substanceuse; increased attributi<strong>on</strong> of importance to l<strong>on</strong>ger term costs andbenefits associated with c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> of use; and a clearer recogniti<strong>on</strong>of the obstacles to achievement of important life goals posed by c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong>of use. Examples of altered life circumstances and specific anxietiesthat might facilitate cessati<strong>on</strong> are given in part 1.C<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> of use is sustained in part by the tendency to accordpresent satisfacti<strong>on</strong>s and costs disproporti<strong>on</strong>ately greater weight thanfuture <strong>on</strong>es. The probability of cessati<strong>on</strong> is increased by any shift inorientati<strong>on</strong> away from the present toward the future, or by any increasedcapacity to forego immediate gratificati<strong>on</strong>s to achieve more importantsubsequent <strong>on</strong>es. Cessati<strong>on</strong> is more likely if the user views c<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong>as being incompatible with achievement of l<strong>on</strong>g-term, significant lifegoals, especially if those goals are part of a clearly defined, carefullyc<strong>on</strong>sidered career plan that seems both achievable and likely to bringimportant future occupati<strong>on</strong>al, financial, social, and pers<strong>on</strong>al satisfacti<strong>on</strong>s.LIFE-THEME THEORY (p. 59)Spotts/Sh<strong>on</strong>tzDisc<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> may occur in resp<strong>on</strong>se to either extrinsic or intrinsicc<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. An important group of extrinsic c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s is related toavailability of the desired substance. When <strong>on</strong>e’s sources of supplydry up or when <strong>on</strong>e runs out of m<strong>on</strong>ey or other ways to obtain a drug(e.g., by theft), its use is, of necessity, disc<strong>on</strong>tinued. Generally,however, disc<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> under these c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s occurs easily <strong>on</strong>ly ifanother substitute substance can be found. Otherwise, in cases oftruly heavy usage, acts of desperati<strong>on</strong> may be attempted to maintainaccess to the needed drug.Intrinsic factors are of two types, physical and mental. Naturally,disc<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> of use follows the death of the user, a factor that isnot to be belittled in groups who live in a dangerous subculture orpractice heavy use of illicit substances. Disc<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> often followsalso when the pers<strong>on</strong> becomes physically unsuitable as a vehicle fordrug use, due to collapse of usable veins or, perhaps, to incapacityas a result of brain damage or physical illness.Mental causes of disc<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> seem to be of two main types, both ofwhich reflect changes in the status of the process of individuati<strong>on</strong>.The first is gradual and is actually organismic because it involves bothmental and physical factors. It could also be called existential becauseit may result from sheer aging, increased maturity, or the “burningout” of the c<strong>on</strong>flict(s) that maintained drug use in earlier years.Often, this type of change is accompanied by anxiety over the awarenessof pers<strong>on</strong>al deteriorati<strong>on</strong> and possible death, accompanied by thefeeling that “I wish to spend my last years in peace.”The sec<strong>on</strong>d type of mental change is sudden and has nearly all thefeatures of a religious c<strong>on</strong>versi<strong>on</strong>. The pers<strong>on</strong> realizes that the drughe has been taking is a false god that has been leading him into what398


he now feels were evil and sinful ways. He finds needed strength ina new source of power (perhaps a counselor, a parent, a wife, areligious figure, or a rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> program) and transfers all hisdevoti<strong>on</strong> from the drug to that new entity. Obviously, the success ofthis form of change depends up<strong>on</strong> the success with which the newgod-figure serves as an adequate symbol of selfhood and individuati<strong>on</strong>.FAMILY THEORY (p. 147)Stant<strong>on</strong>Lennard and Allen (1973) have emphasized that, in order for drugabuse treatment to “take hold,” the social c<strong>on</strong>text of the abuser mustbe changed. Applying drug abuse to its c<strong>on</strong>text in the family, <strong>on</strong>ecould assert, as have Bowen (1966), Haley (1962), and others, that inorder for the symptom to change, the family system must change.C<strong>on</strong>versely, treatment which changes an individual also affects thatpers<strong>on</strong>’s interpers<strong>on</strong>al system. However, if broader system change(rather than change primarily in the individual) does not occur, thechances for prol<strong>on</strong>ged cure are reduced, for there can be c<strong>on</strong>siderablepressure to revert to the old ways.The often-referred-to phenomen<strong>on</strong> of “maturing out” of drug abuse oraddicti<strong>on</strong> is relevant here. However, this c<strong>on</strong>cept does not go farenough. It is an individual-oriented c<strong>on</strong>cept and does not help toexplain why some addicts mature out and others do not, and why someare much older than others when they do. It is more instructive toexamine what is going <strong>on</strong> in the abuser’s life when use ceases, i.e.,what changes are taking place in the interpers<strong>on</strong>al systems--mostnotably the family. More explicitly, <strong>on</strong>e could ask what family lifecycle changes have occurred: Has either parent died? Has a siblingdeveloped problems? (Stant<strong>on</strong> 1977b). Has the abuser recently had afirst child? Has a new support system developed for the parent(s)?Some abusers have been known to “buy” freedom by substitutinganother pers<strong>on</strong> for themselves vis-a-vis their parents; they give theparent(s) a newborn or other child to raise as a replacement, thustaking pressure off themselves (Stant<strong>on</strong> et al. 1978). These andrelated questi<strong>on</strong>s about events in the abuser’s intimate interpers<strong>on</strong>alsystem must be answered in order to gain a more meaningful understandingof the critical variables surrounding cessati<strong>on</strong> of use.SELF-ESTEEM THEORY (p. 157)SteffenhagenIn the framework of the self-esteem theory, we explain cessati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> abasis of two sets of c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s, individual and situati<strong>on</strong>al. In the firstinstance, we postulate that if an individual’s self-esteem were raised(through therapy), he or she would quit using drugs because theywould no l<strong>on</strong>ger serve as a mechanism for coping with inferiority. Inthe sec<strong>on</strong>d case, an individual may quit drug abuse as a result of asuperimposed set of c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s, such as being forcefully detoxified inthe Army, being arrested and jailed, or being socially pressured into399


joining Alcoholics An<strong>on</strong>ymous or Synan<strong>on</strong>. <strong>Drug</strong> abuse may also ceaseif the social stress is removed or if interpers<strong>on</strong>al satisfacti<strong>on</strong>s areincreased so that the abuser’s fragile psychological balance does notrequire this primitive coping mechanism.Cessati<strong>on</strong> can take place <strong>on</strong> a microlevel or <strong>on</strong> a macrolevel. <strong>On</strong> amicrolevel, self-esteem can be increased so the neurotic coping mechanismis not necessary--the pers<strong>on</strong> would be cured. <strong>On</strong> the macrolevel, it isthe situati<strong>on</strong> which is resp<strong>on</strong>sible for cessati<strong>on</strong>, although the pers<strong>on</strong>alneed might remain--the individual would be rehabilitated, not cured.CONDITIONING THEORY (p. 174)WiklerIf it is accepted that c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing factors (classical and operant) andprotracted abstinence play an important role in relapse, then addicti<strong>on</strong>must be regarded as a disease sui generis, and regardless of antecedentetiological variables (e.g., premorbid pers<strong>on</strong>ality) its specific featuresmust be eliminated by appropriate procedures. As Wikler (1965)pointed out, mere detoxificati<strong>on</strong>, with or without c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al psychotherapyand enforced abstenti<strong>on</strong> from self-administrati<strong>on</strong> of opioids,will not prevent relapse when the former addict returns to his homeenvir<strong>on</strong>ment or other envir<strong>on</strong>ments where the c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed stimuli arepresent (drugs readily available; “pushers” and active addicts). Whatis needed in treatment after “detoxificati<strong>on</strong>” is active extincti<strong>on</strong> of bothclassically c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed abstinence and operantly c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed opioidself-administrati<strong>on</strong>. This would require repeated elicitati<strong>on</strong> of c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>edabstinence and repeated self-administrati<strong>on</strong> of opioids under c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>sthat prevent the reinforcing effects of opioids (producti<strong>on</strong> of “euphoria,”reestablishment of physical dependence). Under such c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s,c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed abstinence should eventually disappear and self-administrati<strong>on</strong>of opioids should eventually cease. With the introducti<strong>on</strong> of the orallyeffective, l<strong>on</strong>g-acting opioid antag<strong>on</strong>ist, cyclazocine, by Martin et al.(1966), it became possible to prevent the reinforcing effects of opioidsby daily administrati<strong>on</strong> of cyclazocine. If former addicts are maintained<strong>on</strong> blocking doses of an antag<strong>on</strong>ist for a sufficient length of time(e.g., over 30 weeks) to permit disappearance of protracted abstinence,and if active extincti<strong>on</strong> procedures are carried out during this period(Wikler 1973d), then administrati<strong>on</strong> of the antag<strong>on</strong>ist may be disc<strong>on</strong>tinued,with the expectati<strong>on</strong> that relapse will be much less likely to recur.ROLE THEORY (p. 225)WinickThe theory suggests that a populati<strong>on</strong> or subgroup will tend to ceasedrug dependence when (1) access to the substances dec<strong>line</strong>s, (2) negativeattitudes to their use become salient, and (3) role strain and/ordeprivati<strong>on</strong> are less prevalent. If all three of these trends are operative,the rate of drug dependence will dec<strong>line</strong> more rapidly than if <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>eor two trends are relevant.400


The narrow clustering of age at “maturing out” in different samples atdifferent times (mean ages of 33, 34, and 35 in, respectively, Ball andSnarr 1969; Snow 1974; and Winick 1962a) suggests that there areunderlying regularities in this process. Ethnicity, sex, residence,access to and salience of drugs, attitudes toward drugs in an area,and the extent to which n<strong>on</strong>drug-related roles are plausible and reinforced,c<strong>on</strong>tribute to cessati<strong>on</strong> of drug use, as does the extent towhich the user experiences less role strain and/or deprivati<strong>on</strong>.DEFENSE-STRUCTURE THEORY (p. 71)WurmserLike other neurotic symptoms, compulsive drug use can recede ordisappear--either “sp<strong>on</strong>taneously” or under the impact of outsideevents (including treatment). Wherever the earlier described circle(figure 1, p. 356) is interrupted, drug use recedes. When there is aradical change in the “narcissistic equilibrium,” i.e., when there isdramatic reas<strong>on</strong> to feel proud, not ashamed, not guilty anymore, thewheel may be stopped. Not rarely, however, is it precisely apparentsuccess that keeps it going, namely, when unc<strong>on</strong>scious guilt is animportant factor; then every triumph immediately has to be followed byan act of severe self-punishment and self-sabotage. In these frequentcases, actual suffering and punishment inflicted from the outside bringabout sudden stopping of the drug use. With the great need to depend<strong>on</strong> outside ideals as protectors and givers, the str<strong>on</strong>g interventi<strong>on</strong> bya cause or pers<strong>on</strong> that can functi<strong>on</strong> as a meaning-giving ideal maymake the dependency <strong>on</strong> a drug for increased self-esteem unnecessary.This is “cure” by displacement of idealizati<strong>on</strong>: c<strong>on</strong>versi<strong>on</strong> to a religi<strong>on</strong>or sect; entrance to a powerful organizati<strong>on</strong>; joining Alcoholics An<strong>on</strong>ymous,a political cause, or following a charismatic leader; an intenselove relati<strong>on</strong>ship; transference to a therapist--all are often observed tobring about cessati<strong>on</strong> of drug abuse.FIGURE 1.–Graphic representati<strong>on</strong> of the psychodynamicpattern of drug use401


RelapseADDICTION-TO-PLEASURE THEORY (p. 246)BejerotRepeated relapse is part of the picture in most addictive c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s,regardless of whether they are pharmacologically induced or of an<strong>on</strong>drug type (gambling, obesity, etc.). If sentiments are reactivatedthrough external stimuli and if dependent individuals c<strong>on</strong>sider thec<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s for a relapse to be favorable, they may decide that they canpermit themselves a relapse, particularly if they believe that they havenow gained c<strong>on</strong>trol over the addictive behavior (drinking, smoking,overeating, injecti<strong>on</strong>s, etc.).INCOMPLETE MOURNING THEORY (p. 83)ColemanFamily therapy offers a sense of “roots” and reinforces the c<strong>on</strong>tinuityof the generati<strong>on</strong>s. It also provides an opportunity for individuati<strong>on</strong>of each member. With optimal balance, future losses should be metwith more creative resp<strong>on</strong>ses. As Boszormenyi-Nagy and Spark (1973)suggest, “. . . death, loss and grief can be made into resources forsignificant relati<strong>on</strong>al gains.” Unfortunately, for those families that d<strong>on</strong>ot successfully change their structural and functi<strong>on</strong>al relati<strong>on</strong>ships,some relapses can be expected, particularly when the system is threatenedby additi<strong>on</strong>al loss or separati<strong>on</strong>s.METABOLIC DEFICIENCY PERSPECTIVE (p. 256)Dole/NyswanderImplicit in methad<strong>on</strong>e maintenance programs is an assumpti<strong>on</strong> thatheroin addicti<strong>on</strong> is a metabolic disease, rather than a psychologicalproblem. Although the reas<strong>on</strong>s for taking the initial doses of heroinmay be c<strong>on</strong>sidered psychological--adolescent curiosity or neurotic402


anxiety--the drug, for whatever reas<strong>on</strong> it is first taken, leaves itsimprint <strong>on</strong> the nervous system. This phenomen<strong>on</strong> is clearly seen inanimal studies: A rat, if addicted to morphine by repeated injecti<strong>on</strong>sat <strong>on</strong>e to two m<strong>on</strong>ths of age and then detoxified, will show a residualtolerance and abnormalities in brain waves in resp<strong>on</strong>se to challengedoses of morphine for m<strong>on</strong>ths, perhaps for the rest of its life. Simplystopping the drug does not restore the nervous system of this animalto its normal, preaddictive c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. Since all studies to date haveshown a close associati<strong>on</strong> between tolerance and physical dependence,and since the discomfort of physical dependence leads to drug-seekingactivity, a persistence of physical dependence would explain why bothanimals and men tend to relapse to use of narcotics after detoxificati<strong>on</strong>.This metabolic theory of relapse obviously has different implicati<strong>on</strong>s fortreatment than the traditi<strong>on</strong>al theory that relapse is due to moralweakness.LEARNED BEHAVIOR THEORY (p. 191)FrederickThe c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed drug behavior which is strengthened through reinforcementis weakened through extincti<strong>on</strong> in n<strong>on</strong>reinforcement, but recoveryrecurs through rest. Two additi<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>cepts are central to an understandingof learning principles inherent in drug-related behavior. Adifferent or newly c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed stimulus, which has not been reinforced,can evoke a c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed act up<strong>on</strong> its initial presentati<strong>on</strong>. The likelihoodthat this will occur increases when it is similar to a previously c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed,already reinforced, stimulus. Thus, the process of generalizati<strong>on</strong>becomes important in analyzing drug-taking behavior. When twoacts or resp<strong>on</strong>ses are alike but distinguishable, the individual can betaught to resp<strong>on</strong>d to <strong>on</strong>e and not the other. This principle of c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>eddiscriminati<strong>on</strong> can serve as a two-edged sword in carelesshands, since it can possess both addictive and therapeutic aspects.Whatever is useful to assist the drug abuser in the clinic can be usedto enhance and perpetuate a new addicti<strong>on</strong> out <strong>on</strong> the street, so tospeak. Pers<strong>on</strong>ality (P), motivati<strong>on</strong> (M), and habit (H) factors areparticularly important in bringing about a relapse to drug usage,although most values clearly will have been altered over time withc<strong>on</strong>tinued drug use. There is a sp<strong>on</strong>taneous recovery of past learnedaddictive habits, when the motivati<strong>on</strong> or drive to abstain is no l<strong>on</strong>gersuperior to the motivati<strong>on</strong> to engage in drug usage. While most destructiveand c<strong>on</strong>structive factors have been altered, due to reinforcementor n<strong>on</strong>reinforcement with time, the ratio is most affected by negativepers<strong>on</strong>ality, motivati<strong>on</strong>al, and habit factors, Mathematically, as theequati<strong>on</strong> shows, in principle, the proporti<strong>on</strong>al value now approaches 1,where drug usage unequivocally develops again:403


COGNITIVE CONTROL THEORY (p. 8)Gold<strong>Drug</strong>s exert a powerful effect <strong>on</strong> the user’s thinking, feelings, andbehavior. The drug abuser’s whole life is dominated by drug-relatedactivities: planning the next buy, talking about the last high, etc.With <strong>on</strong>e’s whole lifestyle centered around drug taking, it is not surprisingthat treatment is difficult and return to drug taking a frequentresp<strong>on</strong>se to stress. The drug abuser must be drug free to benefitfrom treatment. <strong>Drug</strong>s are a quick and readily available temporarysoluti<strong>on</strong> for the abuser, while treatment is slow, uneven, and difficult.To learn to cope with anxiety, the individual must experience it andnot always dampen the anxiety. It is through the repeated experienceof coping with anxiety that individuals learn they have c<strong>on</strong>trol overtheir emoti<strong>on</strong>s and behavior. A comprehensive “treatment package”aimed at helping abusers develop all the skills needed, both intrapers<strong>on</strong>aland interpers<strong>on</strong>al, to cope <strong>on</strong> their own is essential for lastingchange.BAD-HABIT THEORY (p. 12)GoodwinRelapse is at least partly due to stimulus generalizati<strong>on</strong>, the strengthof the reinforcers, and their slowness to extinguish.EXISTENTIAL THEORY (p. 24)GreavesRelapse to drug dependency is tied intimately to my noti<strong>on</strong>s of whatgives rise to cessati<strong>on</strong>. If cessati<strong>on</strong> is brought about through c<strong>on</strong>trolof the substance or through social sancti<strong>on</strong>s, relapse is virtuallycertain whenever such external “c<strong>on</strong>trols” are removed and opportunitypresents itself. The individual is, after all, using the drug because itserves a need. The <strong>on</strong>ly individuals likely to benefit over the l<strong>on</strong>ghaul from mere separati<strong>on</strong> from their drug of dependence are thosewho would likely have ceased use voluntarily to begin with. Thisasserti<strong>on</strong> is c<strong>on</strong>sistent with the very high rate of relapse reportedfollowing simple detoxificati<strong>on</strong>. The <strong>on</strong>ly way to lessen significantlythe probability of relapse is through a socially supportive, voluntaryprogram, such as Alcoholics An<strong>on</strong>ymous, or by treating and trainingpeople to secure the positive phenomenal states experienced by normalindividuals, such as through sensitivity training, existential psychotherapy,and biofeedback.404


ADAPTATIONAL THEORY (p. 195)HendinIf an individual has learned to use drugs to deal with a psychosocialcrisis, he or she is liable to return to drugs when back in the samesituati<strong>on</strong>. We have seen young men who found heroin necessary whenthey were emoti<strong>on</strong>ally involved with women; they stopped using thedrug when their relati<strong>on</strong>ship ended, but would return to heroin sixm<strong>on</strong>ths or a year later if they became involved with some<strong>on</strong>e else(Hendin 1974a). Young women who used amphetamines to help pushthemselves toward academic goals or relati<strong>on</strong>ships with men that theythought they should have, but did not really want, would stop excessiveuse of the drug when out of the situati<strong>on</strong>. Use would resume if theyreturned to an academic situati<strong>on</strong> or a comparable relati<strong>on</strong>ship (Hendin1974b).Almost any prior pattern of drug abuse can be used in resp<strong>on</strong>se tosevere depressi<strong>on</strong>--sometimes in an attempt to remove <strong>on</strong>eself from themood and sometimes in a more straightforward self-destructive “let theworst happen to me” mood. There of course are individuals who havebeen damaged so profoundly so early that life itself is a crisis fromwhich they need to retreat. Such individuals may be free of drugs<strong>on</strong>ly in a restricted, protected envir<strong>on</strong>ment.BIOLOGICAL RHYTHM THEORY (p. 262)HochhauserAlthough an individual may be able to give up drugs, subsequentfeelings of “helplessness” or a later disrupti<strong>on</strong> of chr<strong>on</strong>obiologicalrhythms may increase the likelihood of a relapse.INTERACTIVE FRAMEWORK (p. 95)Huba/Wingard/BentlerRelapse into drug taking may happen in much the same way as initiati<strong>on</strong>occurs, with three major dynamic excepti<strong>on</strong>s. First, since theindividual has previously used drugs, it is expected that there will beboth a direct and an indirect effect (through behavioral pressure) ofthe organismic status systems <strong>on</strong> behavior. That is, there will be acraving for those drugs which have produced dependencies. In somecases, behavior may occur automatically as a result of the craving,although in most cases the indirect c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> through self-perceivedbehavioral pressure will occur. However, the craving may not betranslated into drug-taking behavior if the psychological systems,intimate support system, or sociocultural influence system intervenethrough c<strong>on</strong>scious deliberati<strong>on</strong>, social disapproval, or sociolegal restraint.The self-perceived behavioral pressure may also be changed by productavailability; cravings may diminish and disappear entirely when thereis no product available for ingesti<strong>on</strong>. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, the pers<strong>on</strong>ality system405


may exert more influence <strong>on</strong> relapse than it does <strong>on</strong> initiati<strong>on</strong>. Theindividual may have developed coping and rati<strong>on</strong>alizati<strong>on</strong> styles duringprior drug use that serve to redefine intimate support, and because ofstr<strong>on</strong>g prior behavioral tendencies, more minimal cues for rejecti<strong>on</strong>,loss of self-esteem, etc., may cue further drug use. Finally, envir<strong>on</strong>mentalstress is seen to have a more vigorous role in relapse than ininitiati<strong>on</strong>, unless adequate counterdrug behavioral styles have beendeveloped by the individual.DRUG SUBCULTURES THEORY (p. 110)Johns<strong>on</strong>Return to drug use and abuse may occur if and when pers<strong>on</strong>s reorientthemselves toward subcultural values, c<strong>on</strong>duct norms, argot, andrituals, and then engage in subculture role behavior. Relapse occursfrequently because pers<strong>on</strong>s return to familiar patterns by participatingin old peer groups and so are familiar with group roles and behaviors.For many pers<strong>on</strong>s, relapse may be expected since disc<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> ofuse may have been involuntary (incarcerati<strong>on</strong>, legal or family pressureto enter treatment). In a sense, such pers<strong>on</strong>s may never have leftthe drug subculture and will revert quickly to old drug-using patternsand friends up<strong>on</strong> return to the community. Levels of use may increaseto abuse rapidly if the individual becomes involved in drug dealing andsales to derive an income and to obtain free drugs. Even when pers<strong>on</strong>shave voluntarily given up drug-using friends, regular drug use, andcompliance with subcultural c<strong>on</strong>duct norms, they may experience difficultyin finding new friends or in achieving new goals, thus increasingthe probability of a return to drug subculture friends, values, c<strong>on</strong>ductnorms, and behaviors.<strong>Drug</strong>-subculture theory does not directly incorporate the pharmacologicaleffects of drugs in predicting relapse, but it is compatible with perspectivessuch as Wikler’s (1953) c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing theory, and recent theoriesof endorphins and drug metabolism (Verebey et al. 1978). Theseperspectives hold that the drugs c<strong>on</strong>sumed alter body and brain biochemistryand metabolism so that a pers<strong>on</strong> who has previously been a heavyuser or was physically dependent up<strong>on</strong> a substance will exhibit physicalor psychological dependence (Lindesmith 1947; Chein et al. 1964; Eddyet al. 1965) and will seek out and return to drug use as previously.While such biological-psychological factors may be important motivati<strong>on</strong>sin returning to drug use, drug-subculture theory holds that relapsemay occur earlier and be more severe and l<strong>on</strong>g lasting through participati<strong>on</strong>in the drug subculture than where such subcultural supportsare weak or absent. Indeed, without drug subculture supports (exceptalcohol), especially access to illegal drug supplies via other users ordealers, pers<strong>on</strong>s who experience severe drug-induced craving for aparticular drug might be unable to satisfy that desire. Thus, drugsubcultures are critical in understanding relapse. Pers<strong>on</strong>s followingc<strong>on</strong>duct norms and role behaviors reinforce and promote pharmacologicallyinduced craving, provide drug supplies, and structure a patternof associati<strong>on</strong>s that channel biochemical and psychological desires.406


SELF-DEROGATION THEORY (p. 128)KaplanThe pers<strong>on</strong> is likely to relapse into the deviant resp<strong>on</strong>se pattern <strong>on</strong>lyin the face of erosi<strong>on</strong> of pers<strong>on</strong>al and social support mechanisms,pervasive self-devaluing experiences, and a history of self-enhancingc<strong>on</strong>sequences of earlier illicit drug use.EGO/SELF THEORY (p. 29)KhantzianIn my experience, it is the tenacity, persistence, and relative immutabilityof the character traits and pathology in the addict that predisposesto relapse. Very often, such relapses are precipitated byexperiences of rejecti<strong>on</strong>, loss, and stress.I have repeatedly observed the addict’s special problems in acceptingdependency and actively acknowledging and pursuing goals and satisfacti<strong>on</strong>srelated to needs and wants. The rigid character traits andalternating defenses employed by addicts are adopted against underlyingneeds and dependency in order to maintain a costly psychologicalequilibrium. Prominent defenses and traits include extreme repressi<strong>on</strong>,disavowal, self-sufficiency, activity, and assumpti<strong>on</strong> of aggressiveattitudes. These defenses (and the associated character traits) areemployed in the service of c<strong>on</strong>taining a whole range of l<strong>on</strong>gings andaspirati<strong>on</strong>s, but particularly those related to dependency and nurturanceneeds. It is because of massive repressi<strong>on</strong> of these needs that suchindividuals feel cut off, hollow, and empty. I suspect that the inabilityof addicts to acknowledge and pursue actively their needs to be admired,and to love and be loved, leaves them vulnerable to reversi<strong>on</strong> t<strong>on</strong>arcotic addicti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> at least two counts. First of all, failing to findsuitable outlets for their needs, they also fail to build up gradually anetwork of relati<strong>on</strong>ships, activities, and involvements that acts as abuffer against boredom, depressi<strong>on</strong>, and narcissistic withdrawal; thistriad of affects acts powerfully to compel such individuals to usedrugs. Furthermore, in failing to express and chance their wants andneeds, they are then subject to sporadic, uneven breakthroughs oftheir impulses and wishes in unpredictable and inappropriate ways thatare often doomed to frustrati<strong>on</strong> and failure. The resulting rage andanger that grow out of such disappointment also compel a reversi<strong>on</strong> todrugs (Khantzian 1978).GENERAL ADDICTION THEORY (p. 34)Lindesmith<strong>On</strong>ce established, the craving persists l<strong>on</strong>g after the c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s thatare necessary to produce it have been d<strong>on</strong>e away with. It may bedescribed as a basically subc<strong>on</strong>scious and irrati<strong>on</strong>al impulse combinedwith cognitive elements and with varied forms of rati<strong>on</strong>alizati<strong>on</strong>. It is407


something like the craving that produces relapse in the case of otherbad habits such as smoking, but it is probably much more powerfuland persistent, making virtually all allegedly permanent “cures” ofc<strong>on</strong>firmed addicti<strong>on</strong> problematic until the pers<strong>on</strong> dies.Since the euphoric effects <strong>on</strong> which addicts bestow so much ecstaticpraise and to which they often attribute their addicti<strong>on</strong> and theirrelapse are maximized by episodic use and minimized by regular dailyuse, and since the user knows this better than anybody else, relapseis an irrati<strong>on</strong>al acti<strong>on</strong> by the addicts’ own logic. They tend to c<strong>on</strong>cealthis irrati<strong>on</strong>ality from themselves with a wealth of rati<strong>on</strong>alizati<strong>on</strong>s that,to them, seem to reflect reality and to be the “truth.” They mayc<strong>on</strong>tend that during abstinence they suffer from discomforts and disorderswhich make it impossible to functi<strong>on</strong> or to enjoy life. They mayannounce that they are never going to use the drug regularly againbut <strong>on</strong>ly now and then, and then become readdicted in a few weeks.Impris<strong>on</strong>ed addicts often make such resoluti<strong>on</strong>s; others simply wait andlook forward to the day of release when they can resume use.Since sensitivity to the withdrawal phenomen<strong>on</strong> is greatly increasedduring addicti<strong>on</strong>, and since the very first dose taken after a period ofabstinence probably produces some mild withdrawal symptoms, theprocess of becoming readdicted is generally much more rapid than itwas initially. It is also facilitated, of course, by associati<strong>on</strong> withother addicts.COMBINATION-OF-EFFECTS THEORY (p. 137)McAuliffe/Gord<strong>on</strong>Relapse During Acute WithdrawalWaldorf’s data (1973) and our own (McAuliffe 1973) show that manystreet addicts report having made attempts to stop opiate use that so<strong>on</strong>end unsuccessfully during the acute phase of withdrawal. Usually,the addicts stopped for a few hours until they could tolerate withdrawaldistress no l<strong>on</strong>ger, at which point they would go out <strong>on</strong> the street toget a shot. Relapse in such cases thus stems from a simple escaperesp<strong>on</strong>se: taking heroin to relieve withdrawal symptoms. With socialsupport of the type found in therapeutic communities or with gradualwithdrawal therapy such as methad<strong>on</strong>e detoxificati<strong>on</strong>, relapses duringthe acute phase can be avoided.We distinguish this acute phase mainly because it occupies such aprominent place in the stereotyped public c<strong>on</strong>cepti<strong>on</strong> of relapse.Taking drugs to avoid withdrawal plays a more important role in settinga lower bound frequency of use--thereby imposing a regularity <strong>on</strong>users bey<strong>on</strong>d what they might prefer--than it does in relapse, becauseit is relatively easy to detoxify addicts and thus place them out ofreach of severe withdrawal discomfort. C<strong>on</strong>sequently, it is relapseafter having been detoxified and perhaps abstinent for a l<strong>on</strong>g period--after incarcerati<strong>on</strong>, for example, where withdrawal sickness is not afactor--that poses the more serious practical challenge to theorists andclinicians.408


Prol<strong>on</strong>ged Abstinence and RelapseFor the sake of discussi<strong>on</strong> it is c<strong>on</strong>venient to designate a degree ofrelapse that embodies aspects of the phenomen<strong>on</strong> of greatest practicalc<strong>on</strong>cern. In what follows, therefore, “relapse” will refer to the resumpti<strong>on</strong>of opiate use at rates sufficient to keep addicti<strong>on</strong>--the strength ofthe drug-taking resp<strong>on</strong>se--at a high level. Reacquisiti<strong>on</strong> of physicaldependence is not required for relapse to apply in this sense, althoughreacquisiti<strong>on</strong> would often occur, and when it did not, the risk of itsoccurring would always be great. This secti<strong>on</strong> treats relapses in thec<strong>on</strong>text of prol<strong>on</strong>ged abstinence and hence in situati<strong>on</strong>s in whichimpending withdrawal sickness is not a c<strong>on</strong>tributing factor. Relevantc<strong>on</strong>tingencies are c<strong>on</strong>sidered under two headings: those stemming frompsychopharmacological factors and those stemming from broader lifestylechanges.Psychopharmacological FactorsEven when addicts successfully pass through the acute phase of physicalwithdrawal, they are still usually str<strong>on</strong>gly addicted. Since the n<strong>on</strong>dependentor detoxified addict is no l<strong>on</strong>ger susceptible to unc<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>edwithdrawal sickness, drug taking stimulated by the need to avoidwithdrawal is no l<strong>on</strong>ger part of the resp<strong>on</strong>se picture. Thus, in theory,preventi<strong>on</strong> of relapse after acute withdrawal does not require extincti<strong>on</strong>of the addict’s withdrawal-avoidance resp<strong>on</strong>se.However, other opiate effects, especially euphoria, would still reinforcedrug taking after the acute phase of withdrawal. Since cues for theseeffects (e.g., friends experiencing euphoria, pain or anxiety troublingthe addict, and so <strong>on</strong>) are still operative in the addict’s envir<strong>on</strong>ment,the strength of the drug-taking resp<strong>on</strong>se that is associated with themmust be extinguished to complete the de-addicti<strong>on</strong> process. For thisextincti<strong>on</strong> to occur, the addict must be exposed repeatedly to the cuesthat cause craving for opiates, but <strong>on</strong>ly under circumstances when theoverall c<strong>on</strong>tingencies of reinforcement are so unfavorable that theaddict refrains from use. An example would be an abstaining addictwho when offered heroin by a friend resists his desire to use it becausehis wife would leave him if she noticed he was high, or because theurine sample required by his parole program would be found “dirty”(Kurland et al. 1969). Indicati<strong>on</strong>s are that extincti<strong>on</strong> takes approximatelya year (Hunt et al. 1971).Should the abstaining addict resp<strong>on</strong>d to craving by using opiates, thestrength of the drug-taking resp<strong>on</strong>se would again be increased.Although, as with addicti<strong>on</strong>, the first reinforcement is the most dangerousincrementally, sporadic use of heroin after withdrawal does notnecessarily lead to daily use (Zinberg and Jacobs<strong>on</strong> 1976). However,addicts in our study report that returning to a high level of addicti<strong>on</strong>is easier than acquiring it in the first place. Their observati<strong>on</strong> isc<strong>on</strong>sistent with experiments that show that <strong>on</strong>e relearns a resp<strong>on</strong>semore easily than <strong>on</strong>e learned it initially (Deese and Hulse 1967, pp.379-380). <strong>On</strong>ce acquired, a reinforcement history remains a permanentpart of <strong>on</strong>e’s makeup, and hence ex-addicts l<strong>on</strong>g remain vulnerable toreaddicti<strong>on</strong> after they embark up<strong>on</strong> abstinence.Substantial evidence shows. that abstaining street addicts resume heroinuse to obtain its euphoric effects and that desire for these effectscauses relapse. Alksne et al. (1955, pp. 63, 82) found that 41 percentof 135 adolescent addicts gave euphoria as a reas<strong>on</strong> for their relapsing409


after treatment. Stephens and Cottrell (1972, p. 51) found that“enjoyment of narcotics” was menti<strong>on</strong>ed as the reas<strong>on</strong> for relapsing by49 percent of their sample of 200 addicts, and this was also the mostfrequent reas<strong>on</strong>. We asked 47 street addicts who had been incarceratedduring their addicti<strong>on</strong> careers, “When you have been in jail for a l<strong>on</strong>gtime and off drugs, so that you were not strung out, how much doyou think about the following things when you think about drugs?You answer can be “a lot,” “a little,” or “not at all.” Four items wereinquired about: Item 1 measured the desire for euphoria; item 2, theimportance of subcultural involvement and social rewards; item 3, theuse of drugs for relief of unpleasant emoti<strong>on</strong>s; and item 4, the use ofdrugs for relief of withdrawal distress. Item 1, “the high,” wasthought of most. Item 4, “getting rid of withdrawal sickness,” wasthought of least. <strong>On</strong>ly 25 percent thought about withdrawal sickness“a lot,” which was half the percentage (51 percent) of those thinkingabout euphoria “a lot”; 47 percent did not think of withdrawal at all.(For a descripti<strong>on</strong> of this sample, see McAuliffe 1973.) Finally, experimentalevidence from a study by Lasagna et al. (1955) shows thateuphoria was the effect most often described by abstinent ex-addictswhen they received heroin and morphine under double-blind laboratoryc<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. Although <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e of the 30 ex-addicts reported a pleasantreacti<strong>on</strong> to placebo, 47 percent had euphoric reacti<strong>on</strong>s to heroin and 65percent to morphine. Positive reinforcement of this sort would naturallyincrease the probability of using heroin again under similar c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s.Thus, abstinent street addicts think a lot about opiate euphoria, mostoften return to using opiates for their euphoric effects, and experienceeuphoria when they use opiates. These facts provide a psychopharmacologicalbasis for relapse.Lifestyle ChangesSince we have shown that most abstaining street addicts would probablyfind a dose of heroin rewarding, additi<strong>on</strong>al factors must be proposedto explain why some addicts seek these rewards and eventually relapsewhereas others do not. In the early stages of a prol<strong>on</strong>ged period ofabstinence it seems likely that the main envir<strong>on</strong>mental forces affectingthe likelihood of drug use are the same as those negative <strong>on</strong>es thatwere originally decisive in getting the addict to stop using drugs, butas time goes <strong>on</strong> other, more positive, factors become increasinglyimportant. Much evidence suggests that the key to remaining abstinentis successful adjustment to a c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al lifestyle while avoidingc<strong>on</strong>tact with the addict subculture. Pers<strong>on</strong>ality traits, amount ofeducati<strong>on</strong>, developments in an addict’s career, and pure chance eventsin <strong>on</strong>e’s social network appear to determine these lifestyle changes(Goldstein 1976a; Ray 1961; Waldorf 1970).During the early stages of a period of abstinence many of the sameforces which originally led the addict to cease drug use c<strong>on</strong>tinueoperating to prevent relapse. An addict who stopped because he wasarrested may have to remain drug free to comply with the c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s ofcriminal probati<strong>on</strong> or parole. <strong>On</strong>e of our resp<strong>on</strong>dents reported that heremained abstinent for two years while <strong>on</strong> a parole department’s urinalysisprogram, but three weeks after discharge from the program hestarted using heroin again and so<strong>on</strong> relapsed. In this case, removalof the original reas<strong>on</strong> for stopping led promptly to relapse.Abstinence from heroin use does not always represent a radical readjustmentin lifestyle, for many abstaining addicts compensate by increasing410


their use of alcohol or other drugs, including less demanding opiatedrugs such as cough medicines c<strong>on</strong>taining codeine, and paregoric.<strong>Drug</strong> effects of somewhat lower quality are thus achieved at less costand risk. Waldorf (1973) found that 51 percent of his sample admittedsubstituting excessive use of other drugs or alcohol when stoppingheroin use: 24 percent drank heavily, 13 percent used drugs toexcess, and 14 percent did both. Methad<strong>on</strong>e maintenance may beviewed as an instituti<strong>on</strong>alized example of this substituti<strong>on</strong> method ofgiving up heroin, and it is noteworthy that methad<strong>on</strong>e programs havefound that many patients also supplement their methad<strong>on</strong>e with otherdrugs or alcohol (Bazell 1973; Bourne 1975, p. 101; McGlothlin 1977;Stephens and Weppner 1973; Weppner et al. 1972).It is important to recognize that by substituting “less serious” drugsfor heroin, addicts follow a pattern which Kandel (1975) has also foundam<strong>on</strong>g adolescent users of many different drugs. <strong>Drug</strong> users do notregress directly to n<strong>on</strong>use, but to lower categories of less seriousillicit drugs or to legal drugs. Thus, substituti<strong>on</strong> of less seriousillicit drugs may be an indicati<strong>on</strong> of partial rehabilitati<strong>on</strong>, even if it isnot the desired end point of the rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> process. !(For a similarview, see Goldstein 1976b.)In our view, successful reintegrati<strong>on</strong> into c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al society, sharingin its rewards, and avoiding the active addict peer group are essentialfor l<strong>on</strong>g-term or permanent abstinence by addicts. A number of ourresp<strong>on</strong>dents explained that they relapsed after brief periods of abstinencebecause either they were unable to find a job or they became l<strong>on</strong>elyafter withdrawing from the addict group and finding no suitable replacementgroup. When addicts were successful in finding or reunitingwith a wife or girlfriend and in finding a job, this success was comm<strong>on</strong>lycited as a factor in promoting abstinence. Most of the addicts foundthat they were happy living more c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al lives and felt no needfor drugs or socializing with other addicts, but there were some excepti<strong>on</strong>s--addictswho said that they had always felt that something wasmissing from their lives when they were not using drugs. In anyevent, if an addict resp<strong>on</strong>dent lost his job or broke up with his wife,he was likely to begin associating with other addicts again. Relapseusually followed within a brief period. Stephens and Cottrell’s (1972)resp<strong>on</strong>dents most often (31 percent) menti<strong>on</strong>ed “problems with familyor girlfriend” as a reas<strong>on</strong> for relapse, and 23 percent menti<strong>on</strong>ed “theinfluence of addict friends and envir<strong>on</strong>ment.” The authors determinedthat addicts with a job had a significantly better chance (14 percent)of remaining abstinent, although it should be noted that 81 percent didrelapse.Goldstein (1976b) has emphasized the reciprocal effects of reducingdrug involvement and of social rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> each other. Sinceprogress al<strong>on</strong>g either of these dimensi<strong>on</strong>s can easily be upset by asetback <strong>on</strong> the other, this perspective helps, al<strong>on</strong>g with the psychopharmacologicalfactors of the preceding secti<strong>on</strong>, to account for theapparent fragility of abstinence (e.g., Ray 1961; Waldorf 1970).C<strong>on</strong>tact with active addicts in particular appears to hold great dangersfor abstaining addicts even when their readjustment to c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>alsociety has been satisfactory. <strong>On</strong>e of our resp<strong>on</strong>dents who was abstinentfor 7 m<strong>on</strong>ths explained that he had not been associating withother addicts, but at a party he encountered an active addict whooffered him a dose of methad<strong>on</strong>e. The resp<strong>on</strong>dent claimed that he didnot feel a great need for the drug and everything in his life was411


going well (he was working, enjoying himself, and so <strong>on</strong>), but hedecided to take it anyway. As this case illustrates, it is especiallydifficult for an abstaining addict to resist the social pressure andtemptati<strong>on</strong> of an offer of a free dose, and active addicts seem pr<strong>on</strong>e torecruit ex-addicts back into their group.Individual differences in adopting of the stereotypical addict lifestylehelp explain the abstaining addict’s subsequent readjustment to c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>alsociety. We (McAuliffe and Gord<strong>on</strong> 1974) and other researchers(Brotman and Freedman 1968; Stims<strong>on</strong> 1973) have found that addictsvary greatly in the extent to which they embrace the stereotypicaladdict lifestyle. Some addicts never become str<strong>on</strong>gly oriented towardheroin’s pleasures; they c<strong>on</strong>tinue to work and have a family, and theyrarely commit crimes. Other research has shown that such individualsare more likely to remain abstinent <strong>on</strong>ce they stop using heroin thanare addicts who are more like the hardcore addict stereotype (McAuliffeand Gord<strong>on</strong> 1974).COPING THEORY (p. 38)Milkman/FroschIn additi<strong>on</strong> to envir<strong>on</strong>mental and physical c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing factors, druguse is difficult to extinguish because of the reinforcement achievedthrough recapitulati<strong>on</strong> of gratifying early childhood experiences. Inthe case of methad<strong>on</strong>e or LAAM, chemically altered ego states and peerculture are substituted for the heroin style of coping, with little directtherapeutic encounter or subsequent pers<strong>on</strong>ality reorganizati<strong>on</strong>.N<strong>on</strong>-drug-oriented treatment reduces the need for drug involvement byremoving the user from his or her characteristic envir<strong>on</strong>ment, wherestress may be great and drug use an accepted form of “getting over.”The treatment milieu or therapist may become need gratifying (parental,structured, safe), and the addictive dependency is transferred to thesurrogate experience. Therapeutic communities typically employ “forcedtherapy” models, temporarily adjusting the user’s self-regulati<strong>on</strong> systemthrough submissi<strong>on</strong> to external c<strong>on</strong>trols. However, the underlyingpercepti<strong>on</strong> of self as victim in a hostile and threatening envir<strong>on</strong>mentpersists. Outcome studies of therapeutic community participants arenot encouraging, and simple methad<strong>on</strong>e detoxificati<strong>on</strong> has generallyfailed, i.e., the majority of subjects relapse before completing thecustomary 21- to 30-day process.Relapse frequently occurs because c<strong>on</strong>temporary treatment does notprovide the user with alternative ways of defending against vulnerabilityand of satisfying the inner needs and wishes previously resolvedthrough drug use. Such alternatives may include new patterns ofdischarge, gratificati<strong>on</strong>, or defense. When detoxificati<strong>on</strong> is initiallysuccessful, the need-gratifying therapy should be gradually disc<strong>on</strong>tinuedthrough clinically m<strong>on</strong>itored and graded frustrati<strong>on</strong>s. The user shouldhave the necessary foundati<strong>on</strong> for replicating the n<strong>on</strong>drug, alternativelygratifying experiences in his or her characteristic envir<strong>on</strong>ment.412


ACHIEVEMENT-ANXIETY THEORY (p. 272)MisraThe fact that drug addicti<strong>on</strong> is a form of coping with the pressures ofachievement makes it highly likely that every time we c<strong>on</strong>fr<strong>on</strong>t anex-addict with the demands of achievement, we are risking relapse. Itis, then, all the more necessary to phase in a sense of resp<strong>on</strong>sibilityfor structure in helping addicts. Even then, a goal of 100 percentsuccess in the treatment of addicts can be no more than a quixoticdream.ADDICTIVE EXPERIENCES THEORY (p. 142)PeeleRelapse will occur when dependence needs and the dependent lifestyleare not addressed when drug use ceases. Thus, certain methods ofchemical treatment, such as methad<strong>on</strong>e maintenance and certain therapeuticcommunities which eliminate drug use without addressing theunderlying issues of the pers<strong>on</strong>’s addicti<strong>on</strong>, frequently produce eithera temporary cure or <strong>on</strong>e which is dependent <strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tinued participati<strong>on</strong>in the treatment program. When the pers<strong>on</strong> is reimmersed in thestresses which led to the addicti<strong>on</strong> in the first place without thesupport of the program, addicti<strong>on</strong> resumes.Certain addicti<strong>on</strong>s may be dependent <strong>on</strong> a given setting or level ofstress. As l<strong>on</strong>g as the pers<strong>on</strong> is not exposed to these settings, thereis no danger of addicti<strong>on</strong>. When these settings are excepti<strong>on</strong>al, suchas c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s of war or hospitalized illness, a pers<strong>on</strong> will not be addictedwhen removed from the setting. <strong>On</strong>e-time life crises, such as thoseproduced by adolescence and which are left behind when the individual“matures out,” are similar occurrences. However, when the stressfulsituati<strong>on</strong> is <strong>on</strong>e encountered regularly in the pers<strong>on</strong>’s life, then repeatedbouts with addicti<strong>on</strong> are likely.SOCIAL NEUROBIOLOGICAL THEORY (p. 286)PrescottRelapse into substance abuse will occur when cognitive behavioralrestructuring is achieved without c<strong>on</strong>comitant changes in the neuropsychobiologicalmechanisms of somatosensory affecti<strong>on</strong>al processes.The dissociati<strong>on</strong> of cognitive behaviors from psychophysiological behaviorsin the processes of rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> provides a basis for relapse. Theestablishment or reestablishment of neurointegrati<strong>on</strong> of somatosensoryaffecti<strong>on</strong>al processes with “higher brain centers” (altered states ofc<strong>on</strong>sciousness) would c<strong>on</strong>stitute an effective barrier to relapse. Ifearly deprivati<strong>on</strong>s are sufficiently severe that there is a permanentneur<strong>on</strong>al alterati<strong>on</strong> of the brain, then the neur<strong>on</strong>al dendritic networksnecessary for the integrati<strong>on</strong> of somatosensory affecti<strong>on</strong>al processeswith “higher brain centers” would be absent and, thus, would preclude413


a permanent rehabilitati<strong>on</strong>. Under such circumstances, c<strong>on</strong>tinuedenriched somatosensory affecti<strong>on</strong>al experiences would be required toprevent relapse. A useful analogy here is the diabetic’s c<strong>on</strong>tinuingneed of insulin <strong>on</strong> a daily basis so that normal functi<strong>on</strong>ing can bemaintained.GENETIC THEORY (p. 297)Schuckit<strong>On</strong>e aspect of relapse from temporary abstenti<strong>on</strong> or a period of apparent“c<strong>on</strong>trolled” use of alcohol or drugs has been discussed in the secti<strong>on</strong><strong>on</strong> cessati<strong>on</strong> of use. In short, the natural history of alcoholism ordrug abuse appears to include periods of active abuse alternating withperiods of abstinence and periods of modest use.As is true for initiati<strong>on</strong> of use in the first place, the individual whohas been abstinent may return to a use pattern through the influencesof both envir<strong>on</strong>mental and genetic factors. It is probable that socialpressures which were originally important in the selecti<strong>on</strong> of the substancemay <strong>on</strong>ce again exert their influence during a temporary abstinence.An additi<strong>on</strong>al factor important in relapse may be an extended (i.e., upto six m<strong>on</strong>ths or more) period of mild physical discomfort which mayfollow acute withdrawal from a drug (Schuckit 1979a; Johns<strong>on</strong> et al.1970; Martin et al. 1963). During a protracted abstinence, variousenvir<strong>on</strong>mental cues may remind alcoholics or drug abusers (in almost asubliminal way) that drugs may help them to feel more comfortable(Parker and Rado 1974). There is additi<strong>on</strong>al evidence, however, thateven in the absence of physical dependence, certain envir<strong>on</strong>mentalcues may themselves precipitate discomfort which may be perceived bythe individual as a withdrawal syndrome. This may lead to reinitiati<strong>on</strong>into the use of drugs even when no str<strong>on</strong>g physiological addicti<strong>on</strong> hadbeen established (Siegal 1975).Thus, it is possible that genetic factors may play a role in either thephysiological drive to return to drugs as mediated by a protractedabstinence syndrome or through psychological vulnerabilities to seekthe drug either to lessen peer pressure or to help alleviate a psychologicallymediated discomfort. <strong>On</strong>ce the individual has decided to try thedrug again, genetic factors similar to those described earlier may <strong>on</strong>ceagain be important in the transiti<strong>on</strong> from use to abuse.AVAILABILITY AND PRONENESS THEORY (p. 46)SmartRelapse to drug use or addicti<strong>on</strong> is comm<strong>on</strong> am<strong>on</strong>g former opiate addictswhen they leave the drug-free situati<strong>on</strong> and return to an envir<strong>on</strong>mentin which availability is greater, and most addicts do best in protectedn<strong>on</strong>drug-using therapeutic communities where drugs have a low availability.The best-known low-availability therapies are the therapeutic414


communities such as Phoenix, Daytop, Synan<strong>on</strong>, and the like. As l<strong>on</strong>gas addicts are in such programs they should not relapse, but difficultiesshould be expected when they leave them and return to high-availabilitysituati<strong>on</strong>s, such as to former friends and old neighborhoods. Availableresearch <strong>on</strong> outcomes from such programs certainly supports theseexpectati<strong>on</strong>s (Smart 1976a). <strong>On</strong> release from pris<strong>on</strong>, those addicts whoreturn to situati<strong>on</strong>s of high availability should also relapse, and evidencesupports this asserti<strong>on</strong>. In general, pr<strong>on</strong>eness should be less importantthan availability in maintaining drug use am<strong>on</strong>g addicts. However,after a l<strong>on</strong>g period of drug-free treatment or incarcerati<strong>on</strong>, pr<strong>on</strong>eness(al<strong>on</strong>g with availability) should again determine whether drug use isstarted again. Former addicts whose pr<strong>on</strong>eness (from whatever source)still exists may be expected to reestablish their addicti<strong>on</strong> or take up anew drug with similar effects.PERCEIVED EFFECTS THEORY (p. 50)SmithThe questi<strong>on</strong> of relapse does not apply to the pers<strong>on</strong> whose substanceuse is occasi<strong>on</strong>al, n<strong>on</strong>compulsive, and regulated in such a manner thatthe desired effects of use c<strong>on</strong>tinue to be perceived as outweighing theperceived undesired effects. Such a pers<strong>on</strong> may have periods ofabstinence, but use after such a period is not truly a relapse.The fascinating questi<strong>on</strong> regarding relapse is posed by the user whoescalates to compulsive use, fights and wins the ag<strong>on</strong>izing battle backto abstinence, but then becomes readdicted after a period of time.Many users repeat this process again and again. Why is <strong>on</strong>e suchexperience not enough to prevent its recurrence?<strong>On</strong>e possible explanati<strong>on</strong> lies in the fact that memory is highly selective,and the prior suffering may be remembered as being less intense thanit actually was. Or, alternatively, the past suffering may be accuratelyremembered, but the recollecti<strong>on</strong> may not offset the desire to reexperiencethe pleasure of use. It is also possible that the user is drivenby an unspecified biological craving that simply overpowers the fear ofbecoming readdicted.Still another possibility is that the user believes he or she is nowclearly aware of the warning signs that appear prior to the stage ofcompulsive use, will vigilantly heed any such warnings, and, in thatmanner, can achieve the pleasure of occasi<strong>on</strong>al, well-regulated, n<strong>on</strong>compulsiveuse without running the risk of readdicti<strong>on</strong>.Yet another possibility is that the individual’s abstinent periods arethemselves psychologically distressing (due to depressi<strong>on</strong>, anxiety,guilt, anger, etc.) and that substance use reduces those discomforts.Under such circumstances, it might be quite tempting for the user tobelieve that just enough substance can be taken to c<strong>on</strong>trol thosedistressing mood states without returning to the level of compulsiveuse.415


LIFE-THEME THEORY (p. 59)Spotts/Sh<strong>on</strong>tzWhat is defined as relapse depends up<strong>on</strong> what is regarded as genuinedisc<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong>. For how l<strong>on</strong>g and for what reas<strong>on</strong>s must a chr<strong>on</strong>icuser abstain from his drug of choice before reuse is regarded asrelapse? Has a pers<strong>on</strong> who has given up amphetamine relapsed if hec<strong>on</strong>tinues or substitutes excessive alcohol c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong> for use of hisdrug of choice? Does a pers<strong>on</strong> who gives up heroin relapse if he goes<strong>on</strong> a methad<strong>on</strong>e maintenance program, or is he simply substituting <strong>on</strong>ehabit-forming drug for another? Does a pers<strong>on</strong> who stops usingcocaine in pris<strong>on</strong>, because he cannot afford it there, relapse if hetakes it up again as so<strong>on</strong> as he is discharged? Users who are tryingdesperately to quit may be said to relapse every time they fail, thatis, several times a week, or even several times a day.From a theoretical point of view, relapse can occur in truly heavyusage <strong>on</strong>ly if the pers<strong>on</strong> not <strong>on</strong>ly gives up the use of drugs but alsotries to solve the problem of individuati<strong>on</strong> in a mature way. In mostcases of apparent disc<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong>, this probably does not happen. Ifsome<strong>on</strong>e stops taking cocaine when the supply dries up, he certainlydisc<strong>on</strong>tinues its use. But if he starts using cocaine again when thesupply is replenished, he can <strong>on</strong>ly be said to have relapsed if he gaveup cocaine as a soluti<strong>on</strong> to the problem of individuati<strong>on</strong> in the firstplace. As far as pers<strong>on</strong>alistic theory is c<strong>on</strong>cerned, disc<strong>on</strong>tinuati<strong>on</strong> ofphysical c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong> of a drug is a necessary but not a sufficientc<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> for relapse. It must be clear that something else has replacedthe drug in the pers<strong>on</strong>’s search for pers<strong>on</strong>al integrati<strong>on</strong>. <strong>On</strong>ly if thatsomething else fails and drugs then reenter the picture can truerelapse be diagnosed.FAMILY THEORY (p. 147)Stant<strong>on</strong>Most of the research and thinking about the phenomen<strong>on</strong> of “relapse”has not resulted in any satisfactory explanati<strong>on</strong>s. This is primarilybecause it has been anchored within a <strong>line</strong>ar framework. <strong>On</strong> the otherhand, applying a n<strong>on</strong><strong>line</strong>ar model which accounts for cyclic behaviorpatterns (e.g., A leads to B leads to C leads back to A), and whichencompasses homeostatic and human systems c<strong>on</strong>cepts, shows muchgreater promise. Observing a drug addict <strong>on</strong>ly at entry to or departurefrom the treatment center can provide <strong>on</strong>ly an inadequate picture,because it taps such a small porti<strong>on</strong> of the addicti<strong>on</strong>-readdicti<strong>on</strong> process.This myopic and naive view of addictive patterns has led to the attributingof relapse to such n<strong>on</strong>explanatory noti<strong>on</strong>s as “lack of motivati<strong>on</strong>,”which take no cognizance of the interpers<strong>on</strong>al (e.g., familial) pressuresand triangulati<strong>on</strong>s impinging <strong>on</strong> the abuser and encouraging, eitherovertly or covertly, premature departure from treatment.When <strong>on</strong>e widens <strong>on</strong>e’s lens to look, for instance, at the sequence ofbehaviors within the abuser’s family, the phenomen<strong>on</strong> of relapse fitsmore neatly into place. There is not space here to repeat the elementsin our homeostatic model, but suffice it to say that when addicts416


observe that their improvement or development of greater competenceresults in family crises (such as parents separating or a sibling developinga problem), it <strong>on</strong>ly makes sense--as it would to any loyal offspring--to take up drugs again, or to show some other sign of incompetence’ ordysfuncti<strong>on</strong>. This, then, is a family addictive cycle (whether acknowledgedas such by the addict or not), and efforts to bring aboutchange in the symptom are more likely to succeed if their interventi<strong>on</strong>sare directed toward changing the total family process surroundingdetoxificati<strong>on</strong> and readdicti<strong>on</strong> (Stant<strong>on</strong> 1979c; Stant<strong>on</strong> et al. 1978).It is also proposed in this model that the frequent dropouts (relapses?)seen in therapeutic communities and other types of drug programsresult from crises which occur outside the program. These serve assignals to abusers to pull out. Most comm<strong>on</strong>ly such crises occur inthe family, or certainly am<strong>on</strong>g people with whom abusers have relati<strong>on</strong>shipsthat are close enough and important enough to make them resp<strong>on</strong>d.This is perhaps the single most overlooked aspect of relapse and treatmentdropout.SELF-ESTEEM THEORY (p. 157)SteffenhagenSelf-esteem theory easily accounts for relapse or recidivism. Theetiological factor underlying the abuse is low self-esteem. Therefore,a social situati<strong>on</strong> which causes cessati<strong>on</strong> without raising self-esteem is<strong>on</strong>ly rehabilitative and not curative. Whenever the individual encountersan adverse social situati<strong>on</strong> he or she is likely to revert to the earliermode of coping.Individuals who remain drug free as a result of bel<strong>on</strong>ging to AlcoholicsAn<strong>on</strong>ymous, a group-support system, will most likely return to drugabuse when the support system is lost because the group never bolstersthe individual’s self-esteem but <strong>on</strong>ly provides a form of group self-esteem.CONDITIONING THEORY (p. 174)WiklerIn 1948, Wikler proposed that relapse is due to evocati<strong>on</strong> by drugrelatedenvir<strong>on</strong>mental stimuli (“bad associates,” neighborhoods whereopioids are illegally available) of fragments of the opioid-abstinencesyndrome that had become classically c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed to such stimuli duringprevious episodes of addicti<strong>on</strong>. As elaborated further over the years(Wikler 1961, 1965, 1973a,b,c,), this hypothesis may be stated asfollows. Reinforcement of opioid self-administrati<strong>on</strong> is c<strong>on</strong>tingent up<strong>on</strong>the prior existence of “needs” (or “sources of reinforcement”) whichare reduced by the pharmacological effects of the drug (e.g., heroin).The processes of addicti<strong>on</strong> and relapse may be divided into two successivephases, namely, “primary” and “sec<strong>on</strong>dary” pharmacological reinforcement.In the cases of young pers<strong>on</strong>s with prevailing moods ofhypophoria and anxiety and with str<strong>on</strong>g needs to bel<strong>on</strong>g to some417


identifiable group, self-administrati<strong>on</strong> of heroin is often practiced inresp<strong>on</strong>se to the pressure of a heroin-using peer group in a socialenvir<strong>on</strong>ment in which such a peer group exists. In primary pharmacologicalreinforcement, the pharmacological effects of heroin (miosis,respiratory depressi<strong>on</strong>, analgesia, etc.) are c<strong>on</strong>ceived as reflex resp<strong>on</strong>sesto the receptor acti<strong>on</strong>s of the drug, but its “direct” reinforcing propertiesare ascribed to acceptance by the peer groups and reducti<strong>on</strong> ofhypophoria and anxiety.With repetiti<strong>on</strong> of self-administrati<strong>on</strong> of heroin, tolerance developsrapidly to the direct pharmacological effects of the drug and physicaldependence begins (dem<strong>on</strong>strable by administrati<strong>on</strong> of narcotic antag<strong>on</strong>istsafter <strong>on</strong>ly a few doses of morphine, heroin, or methad<strong>on</strong>e; see Wikleret al. 1953). The prevailing mood of the heroin user is now predominantlydysphoric, and withholding of heroin now has as its reflexc<strong>on</strong>sequence the appearance of signs of heroin abstinence (mydriasis,hyperpnea, hyperalgesia, etc.), which generate a new need, experiencedas abstinence distress. Because of previous reinforcement of heroinself-administrati<strong>on</strong>, the heroin user engages in “hustling” for opioids--i.e., seeking “c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s,” earning or stealing m<strong>on</strong>ey, attempting tooutwit the law--which eventually becomes self-reinforcing, thoughinitially at least, it is maintained by acquiring heroin for selfadministrati<strong>on</strong>.In this stage, the “indirect” reinforcing properties ofheroin are attributed to its efficacy in suppressing abstinence distress.“<strong>On</strong> the street,” the heroin user who is both tolerant and physicallydependent frequently undergoes abstinence phenomena before he isable to obtain and self-administer the next dose. Given certain moreor less c<strong>on</strong>stant exteroceptive stimuli (street associates, neighborhoodcharacteristics, “strung out” addicts or leaders, “dope” talk) that aretemporally c<strong>on</strong>tiguous with such episodes, the cycle of heroin abstinenceand its terminati<strong>on</strong> can become classically c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed to such stimuli,while heroin-seeking behavior is operantly c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed. So<strong>on</strong>er orlater, the heroin user is detoxified, either in a hospital or in a jail.The well-known “acute” heroin-abstinence syndrome which is of relativelyshort durati<strong>on</strong> (about two to four weeks) is followed by the “protracted”abstinence syndrome which, in the case of morphine addicti<strong>on</strong>, hasbeen found to last about 30 weeks (Martin 1972). At least during thisperiod, the detoxified heroin user may be said to have still anothernew need. If, then, he is returned to his home envir<strong>on</strong>ment, he isexposed to the phase of sec<strong>on</strong>dary pharmacological reinforcement. Inresp<strong>on</strong>se to the c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed exteroceptive stimuli already described, hemay exhibit transient c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed abstinence changes, experienced asyet another new need, namely “narcotic hunger” or “craving.” Previouslyreinforced “hustling is also likely to appear now as a c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>edresp<strong>on</strong>se (self-reinforcing) to these same exteroceptive stimuli and leadto acquisiti<strong>on</strong> and self-administrati<strong>on</strong> of the drug with reestablishmentof physical dependence as in the “indirect” stage of primary pharmacologicalreinforcement, and the cycle of renewed c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing, detoxificati<strong>on</strong>,and sec<strong>on</strong>dary pharmacological reinforcement with relapse isrepeated again. Also, in the phase of primary pharmacological reinforcement,certain of the interoceptive acti<strong>on</strong>s of opioids, not involved inthe suppressi<strong>on</strong> of abstinence phenomena, can acquire c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>edproperties, inasmuch as in a tolerant and physically dependent individual,they are often followed by c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed abstinence phenomena,c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed abstinence distress, and c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed hustling leading toself-administrati<strong>on</strong> of heroin (relapse). Other interoceptive events canlikewise acquire the property of evoking c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ed self-administrati<strong>on</strong>of opioids. For example, anxiety is frequently associated with the418


opioid-abstinence syndrome, and probably the two phenomena aremediated, in part, by the same central nervous system pathways.Hence, the occurrence of anxiety for whatever reas<strong>on</strong> l<strong>on</strong>g after detoxificati<strong>on</strong>may result in relapse.ROLE THEORY (p. 225)WinickThe reas<strong>on</strong>s for relapse, in terms of this theory, would reflect thepers<strong>on</strong>’s inability to sustain the role of the n<strong>on</strong>user. Each period ofabstinence may represent a trying out of the n<strong>on</strong>user’s role, forvarying periods of time. It is likely that the most comm<strong>on</strong> pattern ofcessati<strong>on</strong> of drug dependence involves experimentati<strong>on</strong> with the n<strong>on</strong>user’srole until it is c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ant with other aspects of the pers<strong>on</strong>’s life.An earlier formulati<strong>on</strong> of the theory argued that drug-dependentpers<strong>on</strong>s “matured out” when there was a lessening of the role pressureswhich had led to the beginning of regular drug use (Winick 1962a).The process of “maturing out” was slow and typically involved a stopstartpattern of drug use until the pers<strong>on</strong> felt comfortable with therole of the n<strong>on</strong>user.In the original study which led to the formulati<strong>on</strong> of the “maturingout” theory, based <strong>on</strong> a nati<strong>on</strong>al sample, the mean age of “maturingout” was 35 (Winick 1962a). The narrow clustering of age at “maturingout” in different samples at different times suggests that there areunderlying regularities in the process. Ethnicity, sex, residence,access to and salience of drugs, attitudes toward drugs in an area,and the extent to which n<strong>on</strong>drug-related roles are plausible and reinforced,c<strong>on</strong>tribute to cessati<strong>on</strong> of drug use, as does the extent towhich the user experiences less role strain and/or deprivati<strong>on</strong>.DEFENSE-STRUCTURE THEORY (p. 71)WurmserSince the underlying c<strong>on</strong>flicts usually are not resolved, and the propensityto affect regressi<strong>on</strong> and ensuing defense by denial remains,any new, usually inevitably recurring disturbance of the narcissisticequilibrium gets the specific circular process of drug use <strong>on</strong>ce moreinto moti<strong>on</strong>. Quite often <strong>on</strong>e can find a displacement from the drugwithdrawal-relateddiscomfort <strong>on</strong>to all distress. The process is this:When I was anxious (etc.), drugs relieved the otherwise unmanageablefeelings. When the drugs ceased their effectiveness (e.g., in acutewithdrawal), all the suppressed feelings came back, usually withincreased vehemence, and coupled with all the added unpleasantness ofwithdrawal. Now, whenever I feel intense affective distress I also feelthe typical withdrawal symptoms. Such microc<strong>on</strong>versi<strong>on</strong> symptomsbased <strong>on</strong> displacement (from anxiety, shame, etc., <strong>on</strong>to physical symptoms<strong>on</strong>ce accompanying their resurgence) in form of chills, diarrhea,the “yen,” etc., weeks or years after detoxificati<strong>on</strong> from physicaladdicti<strong>on</strong>, can be observed in many compulsive drug users. The drug419


is seen as a specific relief for both: affective distress and the c<strong>on</strong>versi<strong>on</strong>symptoms in the form of pseudowithdrawal.420


ReferencesAbels<strong>on</strong>, H.; Cohen, R.; and Schrayer, D. A nati<strong>on</strong>wide study ofbeliefs, informati<strong>on</strong> and experience. In: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Commissi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>Marihuana and <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>. Marihuana: A Signal of Misunderstanding.Appendix, Vol. II. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.: Superintendent ofDocuments, U.S. Government Printing Office, 1972.Abels<strong>on</strong>, H.; Cohen, R.; Schrayer, D.; and Rappeport, M. <strong>Drug</strong>experience, attitudes and related behavior am<strong>on</strong>g adolescents andadults. In: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Commissi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> Marihuana and <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>.<strong>Drug</strong> Use in America: Problem in Perspective. Appendix, Vol. I.Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.: Superintendent of Documents, U.S. GovernmentPrinting Office, 1973.Abels<strong>on</strong>, H., and Atkins<strong>on</strong>, R. Public Experience with PsychoactiveSubstances: A Nati<strong>on</strong>wide Study Am<strong>on</strong>g Adults and Youth.Princet<strong>on</strong>, N.J.: Resp<strong>on</strong>se Analysis Corporati<strong>on</strong>, 1975.Abels<strong>on</strong>, H., and Fishburne, P.M. N<strong>on</strong>medical Use of PsychoactiveSubstances. Princet<strong>on</strong>, N.J.: Resp<strong>on</strong>se Analysis Corporati<strong>on</strong>,1976.Abels<strong>on</strong>, H.I.; Fishburne, P.M.; and Cisin, I. Nati<strong>on</strong>al Survey <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong><strong>Abuse</strong>: 1977. A Nati<strong>on</strong>wide Study--Youth, Young Adults, and OlderPeople. Vol. 1. Main Findings. Rockville, Md.: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute<strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>, 1977.Aberle, D.F. The Peyote Religi<strong>on</strong> Am<strong>on</strong>g the Navaho. Chicago, Ill.:Aldine, 1966.Abrams<strong>on</strong>, L.Y.; Seligman, M.E.P.; and Teasdale, J.D. Learnedhelplessness in humans: Critique and reformulati<strong>on</strong>. Journal ofAbnormal Psychology, 87(1):49-74, 1978.Ackerman, N.W. The Psychodynamics of Family Life: Diagnosis andTreatment of Family Relati<strong>on</strong>ships. New York: Basic Books, 1958.Ade, G. The Old Time Salo<strong>on</strong>: Not Wet--Not Dry, Just History.Detroit: Gale Research Corporati<strong>on</strong>, 1931.Adler, A. Ueber den nervosen Charakter. Munich: Bergmann, 1912;4th. ed., 1928.421


Adler, A. /<strong>On</strong> the origin of the striving for superiority and of socialinterest/ (Ger) . lnternati<strong>on</strong>ale Zeitschrift Fuer lndividualpsychologie,11:257-263, 1933. Reprinted in Ansbacher, H.L., and Ansbacher,R.R., eds. Superiority and Social Interest: A Collecti<strong>on</strong> of LaterWritings. Evanst<strong>on</strong>, III.: Northwestern University Press, 1964.Agar, M. Ripping and Running: A Formal Ethnography of Urban HeroinAddicts. New York: Seminar Press, 1973.Ainsworth, M.D.S. Attachment and dependency: A comparis<strong>on</strong>. In:Gewirtz, J.L., ed. Attachment and Dependency. Washingt<strong>on</strong>,D.C.: Winst<strong>on</strong>, 1972. pp. 97-138.Akers. R.L. Deviant Behavior: A Social Learning Approach. Belm<strong>on</strong>t,Calif.: Wadsworth, 1977.Akers, R.L.; Krohn, M.D.; Lanza-Kuduce, L.; and Radosevich, N.Social learning and deviant behavior. American Sociological Review,44(4):635-655, 1979.Akil, H.; Mayer, D.J.; and Liebeskind, J.C. Antag<strong>on</strong>ism of stimulati<strong>on</strong>producedanalgesia by nalox<strong>on</strong>e, a narcotic antag<strong>on</strong>ist. Science,191:961-962, 1976.Alarc<strong>on</strong>, R. de. The spread of heroin abuse in a community. Bulletin<strong>on</strong> Narcotics, 21(3):17-22, 1969.Alexander, B.K., and Dibb, G.S. Opiate addicts and their parents.Family Process, 14:499-514, 1975.Alexander, I., and Adlerstein, A. Death and religi<strong>on</strong>. In: Feifel,H., ed. The Meaning of Death. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1959.Alexander, P., and Bart<strong>on</strong>, D.H.R. Biochemical Journal, 37:463,1943.Alksne, H.; Trussell, R.E.; Elins<strong>on</strong>, J.; and Patrick, P. A Follow-UpStudy of Treated Adolescent Narcotics Users. New York: ColumbiaUniversity School of Public Health and Administrative Medicine,1955.Allen, A.A. Auk, 51:180, 1934.Allen, T.R., and West, L.J. Flight from violence: Hippies and thegreen rebelli<strong>on</strong>. American Journal of Psychiatry, 124:364-370,1968.Allgulander, C. Dependence <strong>on</strong> sedative and hypnotic drugs, a comparativeclinical and social study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica,Supplement 270, 1978.American Psychiatric Associati<strong>on</strong>, Committee <strong>on</strong> Nomenclature andStatistics. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual. Vol. II. Washingt<strong>on</strong>,D.C.: the Associati<strong>on</strong>, 1968.Andrews, K.H., and Kandel, D.B. Attitude and behavior: A specificati<strong>on</strong>of the c<strong>on</strong>tingent c<strong>on</strong>sistency hypothesis. American SociologicalReview, 44:298-310, 1979.422


Angrist, B., and Gersh<strong>on</strong>, S. Amphetamine abuse in N.Y.C., 1966-1968. Seminar in Psychiatry, 1:195-207, 1969.Annis, H.M. Patterns of intra-familial drug use. British Journal ofAddicti<strong>on</strong>, 69:361-369, 1974.Ansbacher, H.L., and Ansbacher, R.R. The Individual Psychology ofAlfred Adler. New York: Harper Torchbooks, 1956.Arnold, D.O. A process model of subcultures. In: Arnold, D.O.,ed. Subcultures. Berkeley, Calif.: Glendessary, 1970.Atweh, S.F., and Kuhar, M.J. Autoradiographic localizati<strong>on</strong> of opiatereceptors in rat brain. I. Spinal cord and lower medulla. BrainResearch, 124:53-67, 1977a.Atweh, S.F., and Kuhar, M.J. Autoradiographic localizati<strong>on</strong> of opiatereceptors in rat brain. II. The Brainstem. Brain Research,129:1-12, 1977b.Atweh, S.F., and Kuhar, M.J. Autoradiographic localizati<strong>on</strong> of opiatereceptors in rat brain. III. The telencephal<strong>on</strong>. Brain Research,134:393-406, 1977c.Ausubel, D.P. “Pers<strong>on</strong>ality Predispositi<strong>on</strong>s in <strong>Drug</strong> Addicti<strong>on</strong>: APsychometric Study of 80 Matched Addicts and C<strong>on</strong>trols.” Unpublishedpaper. Lexingt<strong>on</strong>, Ky.: U.S. Public Health Service Hospital,1947.Ausubel, D.P. The psychopathology and treatment of drug addicti<strong>on</strong>in relati<strong>on</strong> to the mental hygiene movement. Psychiatric Quarterly,22(Supp., Part II): 219-250, 1948.Ausubel, D.P. An evaluati<strong>on</strong> of recent adolescent drug addicti<strong>on</strong>.Mental Hygiene, 36:373-382, 1952a.Ausubel, D.P. Ego Development and the Pers<strong>on</strong>ality Disorders. NewYork: Grune & Stratt<strong>on</strong>, 1952b.Ausubel, D.P. C<strong>on</strong>troversial issues in the management of drug addicti<strong>on</strong>:Leaalizati<strong>on</strong>, ambulatory treatment, and the British system.Mental Hygiene, 44:535-544, 1958a.Ausubel, D.P. <strong>Drug</strong> Addicti<strong>on</strong>: Physiological, Psychological, and SociologicalAspects. New York: Random House, 1958b.Ausubel, D.P. Causes and types of drug addicti<strong>on</strong>: A psychosocialview. Psychiatric Quarterly, 35:523-531, 1961.Ausubel, D.P. The case for compulsory closed ward treatment ofnarcotic addicts. In: Medical Views of the Narcotics Problem.Annual Judicial C<strong>on</strong>ference of the Sec<strong>on</strong>d Judicial Circuit of theUnited States. Federal Rules Decisi<strong>on</strong>s, 1962. pp. 53-59.Ausubel, D.P. The Dole-Nyswander treatment of heroin addicti<strong>on</strong>.Journal of the American Medical Associati<strong>on</strong>, 195:949-950, March14, 1966.423


Ausubel, D.P. The role of race and social class in the psychiatricdisorders of treated narcotic addicts. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of theAddicti<strong>on</strong>s, 15:303-307, 1980a.Ausubel, D. P. What Every Well-Informed Pers<strong>on</strong> Should Know About<strong>Drug</strong> Addicti<strong>on</strong>. Chicago: Nels<strong>on</strong>-Hall, 1980b.Ausubel, D.P., and Ausubel, P. Research <strong>on</strong> ego deveopment am<strong>on</strong>gsegregated Negro children. In: Passow, A.H., ed. Educati<strong>on</strong> inDepressed Areas. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University,1963. pp. 109-141.Ausubel, D.P., and Spalding, W.B. Alcohol and Narcotic <strong>Drug</strong>s: ATeachers’ Manual. Springfield, Ill.: Superintendent of PublicInstructi<strong>on</strong>, 1956.Babor, T.C.; Meyer, R.E.; Mirin, S.M.; Davies, M.; Valentine, N.;and Rawlins, M. Interpers<strong>on</strong>al behavior in a small group settingduring the heroin addicti<strong>on</strong> cycle. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of theAddicti<strong>on</strong>s, 11:513-523, 1976.Bac<strong>on</strong>, S. Introducti<strong>on</strong>. In: Cahalan, D.; Cisin, I.H.; and Crossley,H.M. American Drinking Practices: A Nati<strong>on</strong>al Survey of Behaviorand Attitudes. Rutgers Center Alcohol <strong>Studies</strong> No. 6. NewBrunswick, N.J.: Rutgers University, 1969.Ball, J. <strong>On</strong> the treatment of drug dependence. American Journal ofPsychiatry, 198:107-108, 1972.Ball, J.C. Two patterns of opiate addicti<strong>on</strong>. In: Ball, J.C., andChambers, C.D., eds. The Epidemiology of Opiate Addicti<strong>on</strong> inthe United States. Springfield, Ill.: Charles C Thomas, 1970.pp. 81-94.Ball, J.C., and Bates, W.M. Nativity, parentage and mobility ofopiate addicts. In: Ball, J.C., and Chambers, C.D., eds.The Epidemiology of Opiate Addicti<strong>on</strong> in the United States.Springfield, III.: Charles C Thomas, 1970. pp. 95-111.Ball, J.C., and Snarr, R.W. A test of the maturati<strong>on</strong> hypothesis withrespect to opiate addicti<strong>on</strong>. Bulletin <strong>on</strong> Narcotics, 21:9-13, 1969.Barkley, R.A. Hyperactivity. In: Mash, E., and Terdal, L., eds.Behavioral Assessment of Childhood Disorders. New York:Guilford Press, in press.Barr, H.L.; Langs, R.J.; Holt, R.R.; Goldberger, L.; and Klein,G.S. LSD: Pers<strong>on</strong>ality and Experience. New York: Wiley lnterscience,1972.Barry, H. Psychological factors in alcoholism. In: Kissin, B., andBegleiter, H., eds. The Biology of Alcoholism. Vol. 3. NewYork: Plenum, 1974.Barry, H.; Barry, H., III; and Blane, H.T. Birth order of delinquentboys and alcohol involvement. Quarterly Journal of <strong>Studies</strong> <strong>on</strong>Alcohol, 30:408-413, 1969.424


Barry, H., III. Cross cultural evidence that dependency c<strong>on</strong>flictmotivates drunkenness. In: Everett, M.W.; Waddell, J.O.;Heath, D.B., eds. Cross-Cultural Approaches to the Study ofAlcohol. Chicago: Aldine, 1976.Bazell, R.J. <strong>Drug</strong> abuse: Methad<strong>on</strong>e becomes the soluti<strong>on</strong> and theproblem. Science, 179:772-775, 1973.Beck, L.; Langford, W.; MacKay, M.; and Sum, G. Childhood chemotherapyand later drug abuse and growth curve: A follow-upstudy of 30 adolescents. American Journal of Psychiatry, 132:436-438, 1975.Becker, E. The Denial of Death. New York: Free Press, 1973.Becker, H.S. Becoming a marijuana user. American Journal of Sociology,59:235-243, 1953.Becker, H.S. Outsiders. <strong>Studies</strong> in the Sociology of Deviance. NewYork: Academic Press, 1963.Becker, H. History, culture, and subjective experience: An explorati<strong>on</strong>of the social bases of drug-induced experiences. Journal of Healthand Social Behavior, 8:163-176, 1967.Becker, H. C<strong>on</strong>sciousness, power and drug effects. Journal of Psychedelic<strong>Drug</strong>s, 6:67-76, 1974.Becker, H.S., and Strauss, A. Careers, pers<strong>on</strong>ality, and adultsocializati<strong>on</strong>. American Journal of Sociology, 62:253-263, NOv.1956.Behling, D.W. Alcohol abuse as encountered in 51 instances of reportedchild abuse. Clinical Pediatrics, 18(2):87-91, 1979.Bejerot, N. /Current problems of drug addicti<strong>on</strong>./(Swed)Lakartidningen, 62(50):4231-4238, 1965.Bejerot, N. Addicti<strong>on</strong>: An Artificially Induced Drive. Springfield,Ill.: Charles C Thomas, 1972.Bejerot, N. The biological and social character of drug dependence.In: Kisker, K.P.; Meyer, J.E.; Muller, C.; and Stromgren, E.,eds. Psychiatric der Gegenwart, Forshung und Praxis. Vol. I II.2nd ed. Berlin: Springer Verlag, 1975. pp. 488-518.Bell, D., and Trethowan, W. Amphetamine abuse and disturbed sexuality.Archives of General Psychiatry, 4:74-78, 1961.Bellak, L.; Hurvich, M.; and Gediman, H.K. Ego Functi<strong>on</strong>s in Schizophrenics,Neurotics and Normals, A Systematic Study of C<strong>on</strong>ceptual,Diagnostic and Therapeutic Aspects. New York: Wiley, 1973.Benedict, R. C<strong>on</strong>tinuities and disc<strong>on</strong>tinuities in cultural c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing.Psychiatry, 1:161-168, 1938.Benni<strong>on</strong>, L.J., and Li, T. Alcohol metabolism in American Indians andwhites. New England Journal of Medicine, 294:9-13, 1976.425


Bentler, P.M. The interdependence of theory, methodology, andempirical data: Causal modeling as an approach to c<strong>on</strong>structvalidati<strong>on</strong>. In: Kandel, D.B., ed. L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal Research <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong>Use: Empirical Findings and Methodological Issues. Washingt<strong>on</strong>,D.C.: Hemisphere, 1978.Bentler, P.M. Multivariate analysis with latent variables: Causalmodeling. Annual Review of Psychology, 31:419-456, 1980.Bentler, P.M., and Speckart, G. Models of attitude-behavior relati<strong>on</strong>s.Psychological Review, 86:452-464, 1979.Berman, A.J., and Berman, D. Ne<strong>on</strong>atal anoxia in the rhesus m<strong>on</strong>key:A behavioral and neurophysiological evaluati<strong>on</strong>. In: Prescott,J.W.; Read, M.S.; and Coursin, D.B., eds. Brain Functi<strong>on</strong> andMalnutriti<strong>on</strong>: Neuropsychological Methods of Assessment. NewYork: Wiley, 1975.Berman, A.J.; Berman, D.; and Prescott, J.W. The effect of cerebellarlesi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> emoti<strong>on</strong>al behavior in the rhesus m<strong>on</strong>key. In: Cooper,I.S.; Riklan, M.; and Snider, R., eds. The Cerebellum, Epilepsy,and Behavior. New York: Plenum Press, 1974. pp. 277-284.Berzins, J.R., and Ross, W.F. Locus of c<strong>on</strong>trol am<strong>on</strong>g opiate addicts.Journal of C<strong>on</strong>sulting and Clinical Psychology, 40:84-91, 1973.Beuthin, P., and Bosquet, W. L<strong>on</strong>g term variati<strong>on</strong> in basal andphenobarbital-stimulated oxidative drug metabolism in the rat.Biochemical Pharmacology, 19:620-625, 1970.Bihari, B. <strong>Drug</strong> dependency: Some etiological c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s. AmericanJournal of <strong>Drug</strong> and Alcohol <strong>Abuse</strong>, 3(3):409-423, 1976.Blachly, P.H. Seducti<strong>on</strong>. A C<strong>on</strong>ceptual Model in the <strong>Drug</strong> Dependenciesand Other C<strong>on</strong>tagious Ills. Springfield,Ill.: Charles C Thomas,1970.Blakemore, C., and Cooper, G.F. Development of the brain depends<strong>on</strong> visual experience. Nature, 228:477-478. 1970.Bloom, F.; Segal, D.; Ling, N.; and Guillemin, R. Endorphins:Profound behavioral effects in rats suggest new etiological factorsin mental illness. Science, 194:630-632, 1976.Blouin, A.G.A.; Bornstein, R.A.; and Trites, R.L. Teenage alcoholuse am<strong>on</strong>g hyperactive children: A five year follow-up study.Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 3:188-194, 1978.Blum, R. “A New Perspective <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong>.” Paper presenteda the meeting of the Nati<strong>on</strong>al Coordinating Council <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong>,Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C., June 1972.Blum, R.H., and Associates. Utopiates. New York: Athert<strong>on</strong>, 1964.Blum, R.H., and Associates. The Dream Sellers. San Francisco:Jossey-Bass, 1972a.Blum, R.H., and Associates. Horatio Alger’s Children. San Francisco:Jossey-Bass, 1972b.426


Blum, R.H., and Blum, E.M. A cultural case study. In: Blum,R.H., and Associates. <strong>Drug</strong>s I: Society and <strong>Drug</strong>s. San Francisco:Jossey-Bass, 1969.Blumberg, H.; Dayt<strong>on</strong>, H.B.; and Wolf, P.S. Analgesic and narcoticantag<strong>on</strong>ist properties of noroxymorph<strong>on</strong>e derivatives. Toxicologyand Applied Pharmacology, 10:406, 1967.Bohman, M. “Some Genetic Aspects of Alcoholism and Criminality--SeenThrough a Material of Adoptees.” Work supported by the SwedishMedical Research Council, University of Umea, Sweden, 1977.B<strong>on</strong>net, K.A.; Miller, J.M.; and Sim<strong>on</strong>, E.J. The effects of chr<strong>on</strong>icopiate treatment and social isolati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> opiate receptors in therodent brain. In: Kosterlitz, H.W., ed. Opiates and EndogenousOpioid Peptides. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 1976.Bornschein, R.L. Diurnal variati<strong>on</strong>s in morphine induced analgesia,locomotor activati<strong>on</strong> and toxicity in mice. Dissertati<strong>on</strong> AbstractsInternati<strong>on</strong>al, 36(2-b):665-666, 1975.Bornschein, R.L.; Crockett, R.S.; and Smith, R.P. Diurnal variati<strong>on</strong>sin the analgesic effectiveness of morphine in mice. Pharmacology,Biochemistry and Behavior, 6(6):621-626, 1977.Boszormenyi-Nagy, I., and Spark, G.M. Invisible Loyalties. NewYork: Harper and Row, 1973.Bourne, P.G. Methad<strong>on</strong>e: Benefits and Shortcomings. Washingt<strong>on</strong>,D.C.: <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong> Council, 1975.Bowen, M. The use of family therapy in clinical practice. ComprehensivePsychiatry, 7:345-374, 1966.Bowen, M. Family Therapy in Clinical Practice. New York: Ar<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>,1978.Bowers, W.J. Normative c<strong>on</strong>straints <strong>on</strong> deviant behavior in the collegec<strong>on</strong>text. Sociometry, 31:370-385, 1968.Bowlby, J. Attachment and Loss. Vol. I. Attachment. New York:Basic Books, 1969.Braucht, G.N.; Brakarsh, D.; Follingstad, D.; and Berry, K.L.Deviant drug use in adolescence: A review of psychosocial correlates.Psychological Bulletin, 79(2):92-106, 1973.Brecher, E.M. Licit and Illicit <strong>Drug</strong>s. Mt. Vern<strong>on</strong>, N.Y.: C<strong>on</strong>sumersUni<strong>on</strong>, 1972.Breder, C.M., Jr., and Coates, C.W. Copeia, 1932. p. 147.Brill, H. Medical and delinquent addicts or drug abusers: A medicaldistincti<strong>on</strong> of legal significance. The Hastings Law Journal,19(3):783-801, 1968.Brill, H., and Hirose, T. The rise and fall of a methamphetamineepidemic: Japan 1945-1955. Seminars in Psychiatry, 1(2):179-194,1969.427


Bromberg, W. Marihuana: A psychiatric study. Journal of the AmericanMedical Associati<strong>on</strong>, 113:4-12, July 1939.Br<strong>on</strong>fenbrenner, U. The origins of alienati<strong>on</strong>. Scientific American,231:53-57,60-61, 1974.Brook, J.S.; Lukoff, I.F.; and Whiteman, M. Correlates of adolescentmarijuana use as related to age, sex and ethnicity. The YaleJournal of Biology and Medicine, 50:383-390, 1977a.Brook, J.S.; Lukoff, I.F.; and Whiteman, M. Peer, family, andpers<strong>on</strong>ality domains as related to adolescents’ drug behavior.Psychological Reports, 41:1095-1102, 1977b.Brook, J.S.; Lukoff, I.F.; and Whiteman, M. Family socializati<strong>on</strong> andadolescent pers<strong>on</strong>ality and their associati<strong>on</strong> with adolescent use ofmarijuana. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 133:261-271, 1978.Broom, L., and Selznick, P. Sociology. 4th ed. New York: Harperand Row, 1968.Brotman, R., and Freedman, A. A Community Mental Health Approachto <strong>Drug</strong> Addicti<strong>on</strong>. JD Publicati<strong>on</strong> No. 9005. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.:Department of Health, Educati<strong>on</strong>, and Welfare, Social and Rehabilitati<strong>on</strong>Service, Office of Juvenile Delinquency and Youth Development,1968.Brought<strong>on</strong>, R. Biorhythmic variati<strong>on</strong>s in c<strong>on</strong>sciousness and psychologicalfuncti<strong>on</strong>s. Canadian Psychological Review, 16(4):217-239,1975.Brown, B.; Green, M.; and Turner, N. The spread of addicti<strong>on</strong>--Therole of the “average addict.” American Journal of <strong>Drug</strong> and Alcohol<strong>Abuse</strong>, 3(4):521-528, 1976.Brown, B.S.; Gauvey, S.K.; Meyers, M.B.; and Stark, S.D. Intheir own words: Addicts’ reas<strong>on</strong>s for initiating and withdrawingfrom heroin. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of the Addicti<strong>on</strong>s, 6:635-645,1971.Brown, G.M. Endocrine aspects of psychosocial dwarfism. In: Sachar,E.J., ed. Horm<strong>on</strong>es, Behavior and Psychopathology. New York:Raven Press, 1976.Brown, J.W.; Glaser, D.; Waxer, E.; and Geis, G. Turning off:Cessati<strong>on</strong> of marijuana use after college. Social Problems, 21(4):527-538, 1974.Brunswick, A.F., and Boyle, J.M. Patterns of drug involvement.Developmental and secular influences <strong>on</strong> age at initiati<strong>on</strong>. Youthand Society, 11:139-162, 1979.Buchsbaum. M.S. Neurophysiological studies of reducti<strong>on</strong> and augmentati<strong>on</strong>.In: Petrie, A. Individuality in Pain and Suffering. 2nded. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1978.Buchsbaum, M.S.; Davis, G.C.; Bunney, W.E., Jr. Nalox<strong>on</strong>e alterspain percepti<strong>on</strong> and somatosensory evoked potentials in normalsubjects. Nature 270:620-622, 1977.428


Buchsbaum, M.S.; Davis, G.C.; and van Kahmen, D.P. Diagnosticclassificati<strong>on</strong> and the endorphin hypothesis of schizophrenia:Individual differences and psychopharmacological strategies. In:Baxter, C.F., ed. VA Advisory C<strong>on</strong>ference <strong>on</strong> Chr<strong>on</strong>ic Schizophrenia.New York: Raven, in press.Buchsbaum, M.S., and Ludwig, A.M. Effects of sensory input andalcohol administrati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> visual evoked potentials in normal subjectsand alcoholics. In: Begleiter, H., ed. Biological Effects of Alcohol.New York: Plenum Press, in press.Burch, N. R.; Dossett, R.G.; Berman, A.J.; and Berman, D. Periodanalysis of the electroencephalogram: Maturati<strong>on</strong> and anoxia. In:Prescott, J.W.; Read, M.S.; and Coursin, D.B., eds. BrainFuncti<strong>on</strong> and Malnutriti<strong>on</strong>: Neuropsychological Methods of Assessment.New York: Wiley, 1975.Burke, G., I II., and Milkman, H. “Co-Treatment of Groups of DifferentiatedSubstance <strong>Abuse</strong>rs.” Paper presented at Nati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>Drug</strong><strong>Abuse</strong> C<strong>on</strong>ference, Seattle, 1978.Cahalan, D. Problem Drinkers. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1970.Cahalan, D., and Room, R. Problem drinking am<strong>on</strong>g American menaged 21-59. American Journal of Public Health, 62:1473-1482,1972.Cairns, R.B. Attachment behavior of mammals. Psychological Review,73:409-426, 1966.Cairns, R.B. Attachment and dependency: A psychobiological andsocial-learning synthesis. In: Gewirtz, J.L., ed. Attachmentand Dependency. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.: Winst<strong>on</strong>, 1972. pp. 29-80.Calhoun, J.B. Populati<strong>on</strong> density and social pathology. ScientificAmerican, 206(2):139-148, 1962.Calhoun, J.B. In: Mayer, W.V., and Van Gelder, R.G., eds. PhysiologicalMammology. Vol. 1. New York: Academic Press, 1963.p. 1.Camer<strong>on</strong>, N., and Magaret, A. Behavior Pathology. Bost<strong>on</strong>: Hought<strong>on</strong>Mifflin, 1951.Campbell, J. The Hero with a Thousand Faces. Princet<strong>on</strong>, N.J.:Princet<strong>on</strong> University Press, 1949.Cann<strong>on</strong>, W.B. A law of denervati<strong>on</strong>. American Journal of MedicalScience, 198:737-749, 1939.Cann<strong>on</strong>, W.B., and Rosenbleuth, A. The Supersensitivity of DenervatedStructures. New York: Macmillan, 1949.Cantwell, D.P. Psychiatric illness in the families of hyperactive children.Archives of General Psychiatry, 70:414-417, 1972.Cantwell, D.P. Hyperactivity and antisocial behavior. Journal of theAmerican Academy of Child Psychiatry, 17:252-262, 1978.429


Carey, J. The College <strong>Drug</strong> Scene. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1968.Carey, J.T., and Mandel, J. A San Francisco Bay area “speed”scene. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 9:164-174, 1968.Carney, R. E. Summary of Results from 1969-1972 Values Oriented <strong>Drug</strong><strong>Abuse</strong> Preventi<strong>on</strong> Programs. Santa M<strong>on</strong>ica, Calif.: Educati<strong>on</strong>alAssistance Institute, 1972.Carr-Saunders, A. The Populati<strong>on</strong> Problem. Oxford: Oxford, 1926.Carstairs, G.M. Bhang and alcohol: Cultural factors in the choice ofintoxicants. In: Solom<strong>on</strong>, D., ed. The Marijuana Papers.Indianapolis, Ind.: Bobbs-Merrill, 1966.Catt<strong>on</strong>, K., and Shain, M. Heroin users in the community: A reviewof the drug use and life-styles of addicts and users not in treatment.Addictive Diseases, 2:421-440, 1976.Chapman, R.N. Ecology, 9:111, 1928.Chein, I. The use of narcotics as a pers<strong>on</strong>al and social problem. In:Wilner, D.M., and Kassebaum, G.G., eds. Narcotics. NewYork: McGraw-Hill, 1965.Chein, I. Psychological, social and epidemiological factors in drugaddicti<strong>on</strong>. In: Rehabilitating the Narcotic Addict. Fort Worth,Tex.: Institute <strong>on</strong> New Developments in the Rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> of theNarcotic Addict, 1966. pp. 53-72.Chein, I., Gerard, D. L.; Lee, R.S.; and Rosenfeld, E. The Road to H.New York: Basic Books, 1964.Chein, I., and Rosenfeld, E. Juvenile narcotics use. Law and C<strong>on</strong>temporaryProblems, 22:52-68, 1957.Childers, S.R.; Simantov, R.; and Snyder, S.H. Enkephalin: Radioimmunoassayand radioreceptor assay in morphine dependent rats.European Journal of Pharmacology, 46:289-293, 1977.Clausen, J.A. Social and psychological factors in narcotic addicti<strong>on</strong>.Law and C<strong>on</strong>temporary Problems, 22:34-51, 1957.Clements, J.E., and Simps<strong>on</strong>, R. Envir<strong>on</strong>mental and behavioral aspectsof glue sniffing in a populati<strong>on</strong> of emoti<strong>on</strong>ally disturbed adolescents.Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of the Addicti<strong>on</strong>s, 13: 129-134, 1978.Clements, S.D., and Peters, J. Minimal brain dysfuncti<strong>on</strong> in theschool-age child. Archives of General Psychiatry, 6:185-197,1962.Cl<strong>on</strong>inger, C.R.; Christiansen, K.O.; Reich T.; and Gottesman, I.Implicati<strong>on</strong>s of sex differences in the prevalences of antisocialpers<strong>on</strong>ality, alcoholism, and criminality for familial transmissi<strong>on</strong>.Archives of General Psychiatry, 35:941-951, 1978.Cloward, R.A., and Ohlin, L.E. Delinquency and Opportunity: ATheory of Delinquent Gangs. New York: The Free Press, 1960.430


Coghlan, A.J.; Gold, S.R.; Dohrenwend, E.F.; and Zimmerman, R.S.A psychobehavioral residential drug abuse program: A new adventurein adolescent psychiatry. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of the Addicti<strong>on</strong>s,8(5):767-777, 1973.Cohen, A.K. Delinquent Boys; the Culture of the Gang. Glencoe,III.: Free Press, 1955.Cohen, S. Lysergic acid diethylamide: Side effects and complicati<strong>on</strong>s.Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 130:30-40, Jan. 1960.Cohen, S. The <strong>Drug</strong> Dilemma. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1969.Cohen, S., and Ditman, K.S. Complicati<strong>on</strong>s associated with lysergicacid diethylamide (LSD-25). Journal of the American MedicalAssociati<strong>on</strong>, 181:161-162, 1962.Cohen, S., and Ditman, K.S. Prol<strong>on</strong>ged adverse reacti<strong>on</strong>s to lysergicacid diethylamide. Archives of General Psychiatry, 8:475-480,1963.Cole, S. The growth of scientific knowledge: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Theories</str<strong>on</strong>g> of deviance asa case study. In: Coser, L.A., ed. The Idea of Social Structure.New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, 1975. pp. 175-220.Coleman, M. Platelet serot<strong>on</strong>in in disturbed m<strong>on</strong>keys and children.Clinical Proceedings of the Children’s Hospital, 7:187-194, 1971.Coleman, P.D., and Riesen, A. H. Envir<strong>on</strong>mental effects <strong>on</strong> corticaldendritic fields. I. Rearing in the dark. Journal of Anatomy,102:363-374, 1968.Coleman, S.B. “Death--The Facilitator of Family Integrati<strong>on</strong>.” Paperpresented at the American Psychological Associati<strong>on</strong>, Chicago,III., Sept. 1975.Coleman, S.B. “Final Report: A Nati<strong>on</strong>al Study of Family Therapy inthe Field of <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>.” Grant No. 3H81-DA-01478-0151.Rockville, Md.: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>, Dec. 1976.(Document available through Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong><strong>Library</strong>, Rockville, Md.)Coleman, S.B. Sib group therapy: A preventi<strong>on</strong> program for siblingsfrom drug addict programs. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of the Addicti<strong>on</strong>s,13:115-127, 1978a.Coleman, S.B. Siblings in sessi<strong>on</strong>. In: Kaufman, E., and Kaufmann,P., eds. From Enmeshed Enemy to Ally: The Family Treatment of<strong>Drug</strong> and Alcohol <strong>Abuse</strong>rs. New York: Gardner, 1978b.Coleman, S.B. The family trajectory: A circular journey to drugabuse. In: Ellis, B., ed. Family Factors and Substance <strong>Abuse</strong>.Rockville, Md.: Office of Program Development and Analysis,Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>, March 1979a.Coleman, S.B. “Cross-Cultural Approaches to Working With AddictFamilies.” Journal of <strong>Drug</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong>, 9:293-299, 1979b.431


Coleman, S.B., and Davis, D. I. Family therapy and drug abuse: Anati<strong>on</strong>al survey. Family Process, 17:21-29, 1978.Coleman, S.B., and Stant<strong>on</strong>, M.D. The role of death in the addictfamily. Journal of Marriage and Family Counseling, 4:79-91,1978.Collier, H.O.J. Supersensitivity and dependence. Nature, 220:228-231,1968.Commissi<strong>on</strong> of Inquiry into the N<strong>on</strong>-Medical Use of <strong>Drug</strong>s. Final Report.Ottawa: Informati<strong>on</strong> Canada, 1973.C<strong>on</strong>ger, J.J. Reinforcement theory and the dynamics of alcoholism.Quarterly Journal of <strong>Studies</strong> <strong>on</strong> Alcohol, 17:296-305, 1956.C<strong>on</strong>roy, R.T.W.L., and Mills, J.N. Human Circadian Rhythms.L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>: Churchill, 1970.Coopersmith, S. The Antecedents of Self Esteem. San Francisco:W.H. Freeman, 1967.Coser, R.L. Insulati<strong>on</strong> from observability and types of social c<strong>on</strong>formity.American Sociological Review, 26:28-39, 1961.Coss, R.G., and Globus, A. Social experience affects the developmentof dendritic spines and branches <strong>on</strong> tectal interneur<strong>on</strong>es in thejewel fish. Developmental Psychobiology, 12: 347-359, 1979.Costa, E., and Trabucchi, M., eds. The Endorphins. New York:Raven, 1978.Cott<strong>on</strong>, N.S. The familial incidence of alcoholism. Journal of <strong>Studies</strong><strong>on</strong> Alcohol, 40:89-116, 1979.Crowley, T. The reinforcers for drug abuse: Why people take drugs.Comprehensive Psychiatry, 13:51-62, 1972.Curran, S., and Savage, C. Patient resp<strong>on</strong>se to naltrex<strong>on</strong>e: Issues ofacceptance, treatment effects, and frequency of administrati<strong>on</strong>.In: Julius, D., and Renault, P., eds; Narcotic Antag<strong>on</strong>ists:Naltrex<strong>on</strong>e. Research M<strong>on</strong>ograph Series, Vol. 9. Rockville, Md.:Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>, 1976.Curtis, H.C. Psychosis following the use of marihuana with report ofcases. Journal of the Kansas Medical Society, 40:515-517, 1939.Dai, B. Opium Addicti<strong>on</strong> in Chicago. M<strong>on</strong>tclair, N.J.: Patters<strong>on</strong>Smith, 1937.Davis, V. E., and Walsh, M.J. Alcohol, amines, and alkaloids: Apossible biochemical basis for alcohol addicti<strong>on</strong>. Science, 167:1005-1007, 1970.Davis, W. Day-night periodicity in phenobarbital resp<strong>on</strong>se of mice andthe influence of socio-psychological c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. Experientia,18:235-236, 1962.432


Davis, W.M., and Nichols, J.R. Physical dependence and sustainedopiate-directed behavior in the rat. Psychopharmacologia, 3:139-145, 1962.Deese, J., and Hulse, S.H. The Psychology of Learning. 3rd ed.New York: McGraw-Hill, 1967.De Feudis, F.V., and Marks, J.H. Brain to serum distributi<strong>on</strong> ofradioactivity of injected ( 3 H)-d-amphetamine in differentiallyhoused mice. Biological Psychiatry, 6:85-88, 1973.DeL<strong>on</strong>g, J.V. The methad<strong>on</strong>e habit. New York Times Magazine,March 16, 1975.Deneau, G.A. Psychogenic dependence in m<strong>on</strong>keys. In: Steinberg,H., ed. Scientific Basis of <strong>Drug</strong> Dependence. New York: Grune& Stratt<strong>on</strong>, 1969.Denhoff, E., and Stern, L. Minimal Brain Dysfuncti<strong>on</strong>--A DevelopmentalApproach. New York: Mass<strong>on</strong> Publishing, 1979.Divisi<strong>on</strong> of Substance <strong>Abuse</strong> Services (DSAS). Substance Use Am<strong>on</strong>gPublic and Parochial School Students in Grades 7 Through 12.Albany, N.Y.: the Divisi<strong>on</strong>, 1978.Dokecki, P.R. When the bough breaks . . . what will happen tobaby? Review of Rock-a-Bye Baby, Lothar Wolff, exec. prod.,Time-Life Films, Inc. C<strong>on</strong>temporary Psychology, 18:64, 1973.Dole, V.P., and Nyswander, M.E. A medical treatment for diacetylmorphine(heroin) addicti<strong>on</strong>. Journal of the American MedicalAssociati<strong>on</strong>, 193:646-650, 1965.Dole, V.P., and Nyswander, M.E. Rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> of heroin addictsafter blockade. with methad<strong>on</strong>e. New York State Journal of Medicine,55:2011-2017, 1966.Dole, V.P., and Nyswander, M.E. Addicti<strong>on</strong>--A metabolic disease.Archives of Internal Medicine, 120:19-24, 1967.Dole, V.P.; Nyswander, M.E.; and Kreek, M.J. Narcotic blockade.Archives of Internal Medicine, 118:304-309, 1966.Doust, J.W.L. A metabolic basis for drug dependence. CanadianPsychiatric Associati<strong>on</strong> Journal, 19: 478-494, 1974.Drew, L.R.H. Alcoholism as a self-limiting disease. Quarterly Journal<strong>Studies</strong> <strong>on</strong> Alcohol, 29:956-967, 1968.Dublin, R. Deviant behavior and social structure. American SociologicalReview, 24:152, Apr. 1959.Duncan, D.F. Life stress as a precursor to adolescent drug dependence.Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of the Addicti<strong>on</strong>s, 12(8):1047-1056,1977.Eddy, N.B.; Halbach, H.; Isbell, H.; and Seevers, M.H. <strong>Drug</strong>dependence: Its significance and characteristics. Bulletin of theWorld Health Organizati<strong>on</strong>, 32:721-733, 1965.433


Edinger, E.F. Ego and Archetype: lndividuati<strong>on</strong> and the ReligiousFoundati<strong>on</strong> of the Psyche. Baltimore, Md.: Penguin, 1973.Edwards, G . <strong>Drug</strong>s, drug dependence and the c<strong>on</strong>cept of plasticity.Quarterly Journal of <strong>Studies</strong> <strong>on</strong> Alcohol, 35:176-195, 1974.Eisenstadt, J.M. Parental loss and genius. American Psychologist,33:211-223, 1978.Ellinwood, E.H.; Smith, W.C.; and Vaillant, G.E. Narcotic addicti<strong>on</strong>in males and females: A comparis<strong>on</strong>. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of theAddicti<strong>on</strong>s, 1:33-45, 1966.El-Sobky, A.; Dostrovsky, J.D.; and Wall, P.D. Lack of effect ofnalox<strong>on</strong>e <strong>on</strong> pain percepti<strong>on</strong> in humans. Nature 263:783-784,1976.Eriks<strong>on</strong>, E.H. Childhood and Society. New York: W.W. Nort<strong>on</strong>, 1950;reprinted 1956.Erikss<strong>on</strong>, K., and Kiianmaa, K. Genetic analysis of susceptibility tomorphine addicti<strong>on</strong> in inbred mice. Annales Medicinae Experimentaliset Biologiae Fenniae, 49:73-78, 1971.Erlenmeyer, A. Zur Theorie und Therapie des Morphinismus. [<strong>On</strong>the theory and therapy of morphine addicti<strong>on</strong>.] Zeitschrift furdie gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatric, 103:705-718, 1926.Er<strong>on</strong>, L. “Prescripti<strong>on</strong> for the Reducti<strong>on</strong> of Aggressive Behavior.”Paper presented at the 12th Annual Meeting of the Associati<strong>on</strong> forthe Advancement of Behavior Therapy, Chicago, Nov. 1978.Erway, L.E. Otolith formati<strong>on</strong> and trace elements: A theory of schizophrenicbehavior. The Journal of Orthomolecular Psychiatry,4:1-11, 1975.Essman, W.B. Neurochemical changes associated with isolati<strong>on</strong> andenvir<strong>on</strong>mental stimulati<strong>on</strong>. Biological Psychiatry, 3:141-147, 1971.Essman, W.B. Biochemical plasticity am<strong>on</strong>g differentially housed mice.In: Neuropsychoplasmacology. Excerpta Medica Internati<strong>on</strong>alC<strong>on</strong>gress Series 359. Amsterdam: Excerpta Medica, 1974. pp.704-719.Essman, W.B. Neurochemical changes in aggressive behavior. BiologicalPsychiatry Today, 1979. pp. 1290-1314.Essman, W.B., and Casper, W.B. Serot<strong>on</strong>in and early development.In: Essman, W.B., ed. Serot<strong>on</strong>in in Health and Disease. Vol. II.Physiological Regulati<strong>on</strong> and Pharmacological Acti<strong>on</strong>. New York:Spectrum, 1978. pp. 63-157Evans, H.L.; Ghiselli, W.B.; and Patt<strong>on</strong>, R.A. Diurnal rhythm inbehavioral effects of methamphetamine. p-chloromethamphetamineand scopolamine. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics,186(1):10-17, 1973.Ewing, J.A.; Rouse, B.A.; and Pellizar, E.D. Alcohol sensitivity andethnic background. American Journal of Psychiatry, 131:206-210,1974.434


Feifel, H. Attitudes toward death in some family and mentally illpopulati<strong>on</strong>s. In: Feifel, H., ed. The Meaning of Death. NewYork: McGraw-Hill, 1959.Feldman, H.W. Ideological supports to becoming and remaining aheroin addict. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 9:131-139,1968.Feldman, H.W.; Agar, M.H.; and Beschner, G. Angel Dust, An EthnographicStudy of PCP Users. Lexingt<strong>on</strong>, Mass.: Lexingt<strong>on</strong> Books,1979.Fenichel, O. The Psychoanalytic Theory of Neurosis. New York:Nort<strong>on</strong>, 1945.Fenna, D.; Mix, L.; Schaefer, O.; and Gilbert, J. Ethanol metabolismin various racial groups. Canadian Medical Associati<strong>on</strong> Journal,105:472-475, 1971.Fine, C.A., and Kleinman, S. Rethinking subculture: An interacti<strong>on</strong>istanalysis. American Journal of Sociology, 85(1):1-20, 1979.Finest<strong>on</strong>e, H. Cats, kicks, and color. Social Problems, 5:3-13, 1957.Fishman, J., ed. The Basis of Addicti<strong>on</strong>: Report of the Dahlem Workshop<strong>on</strong> the Bases of Addicti<strong>on</strong>. Berlin: Abak<strong>on</strong> Verlagsgesellschaft,1978.Floeter, M.K., and Greenough, W.T. Cerebellar plasticity: Modificati<strong>on</strong>of Purkinje cell structure by differential rearing in m<strong>on</strong>keys.Science, 206:227-232, 1979.Fraiberg, S. The Magic Years. New York: Scribner, 1959.Frankl, V.E. Man’s Search for Meaning. New York: Beac<strong>on</strong> Press,1963.Frederick, C. J. <strong>Drug</strong> abuse as self-destructive behavior. <strong>Drug</strong>Therapy, 2:49-68, 1972.Frederick, C.J. <strong>Drug</strong> abuse: A self-destructive enigma. MarylandState Medical Journal, 22:19-21, 1973.Frederick, C.J., and Resnik, H.L.P. How suicidal behaviors arelearned. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 25:37-55, 1971.Frederick, C.J.; Resnik, H.L.P.; and Wittlin, B.J. Self-destructiveaspects of hardcore addicti<strong>on</strong>. Archives of General Psychiatry,28:570-585, 1973.Fredericks<strong>on</strong>, R.C.A.; Burgis, V.; and Edwards, J.D. Hyperalgesiainduced by nalox<strong>on</strong>e follows diurnal rhythm in resp<strong>on</strong>sivity topainful stimuli. Science, 198:756-758, 1977.Fredericks<strong>on</strong>, R.C.A., and Norris, F.H. Enkephalin-induced depressi<strong>on</strong>of single neur<strong>on</strong>s in brain areas with opiate receptors--Antag<strong>on</strong>ismby nalox<strong>on</strong>e. Science, 194:440-442, 1976.435


Freedman, D.A. The influence of c<strong>on</strong>genital and perinatal sensorydeprivati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> later development. Psychosomatics, 9(5):272-277.1968.Freud, A. The Writings of Anna Freud. Vol. VI. Normality and Pathologyin Childhood: Assessment of Development. New York: Internati<strong>on</strong>alUniversal Press, 1965.Freud, S. Inhibiti<strong>on</strong>s, symptoms and anxiety. In: Strachey, J., ed.and trans. Standard Editi<strong>on</strong> of the Complete Psychological Worksof Sigmund Freud. Vol. 20. L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>: Hogarth Press, 1953.(Originally published, 1926.) pp. 75-174.Freud, S. Bey<strong>on</strong>d the Pleasure Principle. Strachey, J., trans. NewYork: Liverright, 1929.Friedman, A.H. Serendipity and chr<strong>on</strong>obiology in pharmacology. In:Scheving, L.E.; Halberg, F.; and Pauly, J.E., eds. Chr<strong>on</strong>obiology.Tokyo: lgaku Shoin, Ltd., 1974.Friedman, C.J.; Sibinga, M.S.; Steisel, I.M.; and Sinnam<strong>on</strong>, H.M.Sensory restricti<strong>on</strong> and isolati<strong>on</strong> experiences in children withphenyl ket<strong>on</strong>uria. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 73(4):294-303,1968.Frosch, W.A.. and Milkman, H. Ego functi<strong>on</strong>s in drug users, psychodynamicsof drug dependence. In: Blaine, J.D., and Julius,D.A., eds. Psychodynamics of <strong>Drug</strong> Dependence. ResearchM<strong>on</strong>ograph 12. Rockville, Md.: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>,1977. pp. 142-156.Frosch, W.A.; Robbins, E.S.; and Stern, M. Untoward reacti<strong>on</strong>s tolysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) resulting in hospitalizati<strong>on</strong>. NewEngland Journal of Medicine, 273:1235-1239, 1965.Fuller, J.L. Experimental deprivati<strong>on</strong> and later behavior. Science,158:1645-1652, 1967.Ganger, R., and Shugart, G. The heroin addict’s pseudo-assertivebehavior and family dynamics. Social Casework, 47:643-649,1966.Gans, H.J. The Urban Villagers. New York: Free Press, 1962.Gearing, F.R. Successes and failures in methad<strong>on</strong>e maintenance treatmentof heroin addicts in New York City. In: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Associati<strong>on</strong>for the Preventi<strong>on</strong> of Addicti<strong>on</strong> to Narcotics. Proceedings of theThird Nati<strong>on</strong>al C<strong>on</strong>ference <strong>on</strong> Methad<strong>on</strong>e Treatment. New York:the Associati<strong>on</strong>, 1971.Geer, J.H.; Davis<strong>on</strong>, G.C.; and Gatchel, R.I. Reducti<strong>on</strong> of stress inhumans through n<strong>on</strong>veridical perceived c<strong>on</strong>trol of aversive stimulati<strong>on</strong>.Journal of Pers<strong>on</strong>ality and Social Psychology, 16:731-738,1970.Gendreau, P., and Gendreau, L. P. The “addicti<strong>on</strong>-pr<strong>on</strong>e” pers<strong>on</strong>ality:A study of Canadian heroin addicts. Canadian Journal of BehavioralScience, 2:18-25, 1970.436


Gerard, D., and Kornetsky, C. Adolescent opiate addicti<strong>on</strong>: A studyof c<strong>on</strong>trol and addict subjects. Psychiatric Quarterly, 29:457-486,1955.Gerstein, D.R. The structure of heroin communities (in relati<strong>on</strong> tomethad<strong>on</strong>e maintenance). American Journal of <strong>Drug</strong> and Alcohol<strong>Abuse</strong>, 3:571-587, 1976.Gesa, G.L.; Paglietti, E.; and Quarantotti, B.P. Inducti<strong>on</strong> of copulatorybehavior in sexually inactive rats by nalox<strong>on</strong>e. Science,204:203-205, 1979.Ghodse, S.E., et al. Circadian rhythm of plasma corticosteriods inheroin dependent subjects. Clinical Endocrinology, 7(2):175-178,1977.Gilbert, P.E., and Martin, W.R. The effects of morpine- and nalorphinelikedrugs in the n<strong>on</strong>dependent, morpine-dependent and cyclazocinedependentchr<strong>on</strong>ic spinal dog. Journal of Pharmacology and ExperimentalTherapeutics, 198:66-82, 1976.Ginsberg, I., and Greenley, J.R. Completing theories of marijuanause: A l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal study. Journal of Health and Social Behavior,19(1):22-34, 1978.Gispen, W.H.; Wiegant, V.M.; Bradbury, A.F.; Hulme, E.C.; Smyth,D.G.; Snell, C.R.; and DeWied, L. Inducti<strong>on</strong> of excessivegrooming in the rat fragments of lipotropin. Nature, 264:794,1976.Glaser, D. Criminality theories and behavioral images. AmericanJournal of Sociology, 61:433-444, 1956.Glaser, D.; Lander, B.; and Abbott, W. Opiate addicted and n<strong>on</strong>addictedsiblings in a slum area. Social Problems, 18(4):510-521,1971.Glasscote, R.M., et al. The Treatment of <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>: Programs,Problems, Prospects. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D. C.: The Joint Informati<strong>on</strong>Service of the American Psychiatric Associati<strong>on</strong> and the Nati<strong>on</strong>alAssociati<strong>on</strong> for Mental Health, 1972.Globus, A.; Rosenzweig, M.R.; Bennet, E.L.; and Diam<strong>on</strong>d, M.C.Effects of differential experience and dendritic spine counts in ratcerebral cortex. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology,82:175-181, 1973.Gold, M.; Redm<strong>on</strong>d, D.E., Jr.; and Kleber, H.D. Cl<strong>on</strong>idine blocksacute opiate-withdrawal symptoms. The Lancet, Sept. 16, 1978.pp. 599-601.Gold, S.R., and Coghlan, A.J. Locus of c<strong>on</strong>trol and self-esteemam<strong>on</strong>g adolescent drug abusers: Effects of residential treatment.<strong>Drug</strong> Forum, 5(2):185-191, 1976.Goldberg, L. <strong>Drug</strong> abuse in Sweden. Bulletin <strong>on</strong> Narcotics, 20:1-12,1968.437


Goldstein, A. Naltrex<strong>on</strong>e in the management of heroin addicti<strong>on</strong>:Critique of the rati<strong>on</strong>ale. In: Julius, D., and Renault, P., eds.Narcotic Antag<strong>on</strong>ists: Naltrex<strong>on</strong>e. Research M<strong>on</strong>ograph Series,Vol. 9. Rockville, Md.: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>, 1976a.Goldstein, A. Heroin addicti<strong>on</strong>: Sequential treatment employing pharmacologicsupports. Archives of General Psychiatry, 33:353-358,1976b.Goldstein, A. Opioid peptides (endomorphins) in pituitary and brain.Science, 193(4228):1081-1086, 1976c.Goldstein, A., and Hilgard, E.R. Failure of the opiate antag<strong>on</strong>istnalox<strong>on</strong>e to modify hypnotic analgesia. Proceedings of the Nati<strong>on</strong>alAcademy of Sciences, USA, 72:2041-2043, 1975.Goldstein, A., and Kaiser, S. Psychotropic effects of caffeine in man:III. A questi<strong>on</strong>naire survey of coffee drinking and its effects in agroup of housewives. Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics,10:477-488, 1969.Goldstein, A.; Lowney, L. I.; and Pal, B.K. Stereospecific and n<strong>on</strong>specificinteracti<strong>on</strong>s of the morphine c<strong>on</strong>gener levorphanol insubcellular fracti<strong>on</strong>s of mouse brain. Proceedings of the Nati<strong>on</strong>alAcademy of Sciences, USA, 68:1742-1747, 1971.Goldstein, A.; Tachibana, S.; Lowney, L.I.; Hunkapillar, M.; andHood, L. Dynorphin-(1-13), an extraordinarily potent opioidpeptide. Proceedings of the Nati<strong>on</strong>al Academy of Sciences, USA,76:6666-6670, 1979.Goldstein, J.W.; Gleas<strong>on</strong>, T.C.; and Korn, J.H. Whither the epidemic?Psychoactive drug-use career patterns of college students.Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 5:16-33, 1975.Goode, E. The Marijuana Smokers. New York: Basic Books, 1970.Goode, E. <strong>Drug</strong>s in American Society. New York: Alfred Knopf,1972.Goode, E. Marihuana use and the progressi<strong>on</strong> to dangerous drugs.In: Miller, L.L., ed. Marihuana: Effects <strong>on</strong> Human Behavior.New York: Academic Press, 1974. pp. 303-338.Goodwin, D. Is Alcoholism Hereditary? New York: Oxford UniversityPress, 1976.Goodwin, D.W. Alcoholism and heredity. Archives of General Psychiatry,36:57-61, 1979.Goodwin, D.W.; Powell, B.; Bremer, D.; Hoine, H.; and Stern, J.Alcohol and recall: State dependent effects in man. Science,163:1358-1361, 1969.Goodwin, D.W.; Schulsinger, F.; Hermansen, L.; Guze, S.B.; andWinokur, G. Alcoholism and the hyperactive child syndrome.Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 160:349-353, 1975.438


Goodwin, D.W.; Schulsinger, F.; Moller, N.; Hermansen, L.; Winokur,G.; and Guze, S. Drinking problems in adopted and n<strong>on</strong>adopteds<strong>on</strong>s of alcoholics. Archives of General Psychiatry, 31:164-169,1974.Gord<strong>on</strong>, R.A. Social level, social disability, and gang interacti<strong>on</strong>.American Journal of Sociology, 73:42-62, 1967.Gord<strong>on</strong>, R.A. “An Interpretati<strong>on</strong> of the High Vietnam Remissi<strong>on</strong> Ratesin Light of a Comprehensive Theory of Opiate Addicti<strong>on</strong>.” Papersubmitted for publicati<strong>on</strong>, 1979.Gorsuch, R. L. Evaluating community based behavioral programs: Caseexample. In: Schinke, P., Jr., ed. Community Applicati<strong>on</strong>s ofBehavioral Methods: A Sourcebook for Social Workers. Chicago:Aldine, in press.Gorsuch, R.L., and Butler, M. Initial drug abuse: A review ofpredisposing social psychological factors. Psychological Bulletin,83:120-137, 1976a.Gorsuch, R.L., and Butler, M. Toward developmental models ofn<strong>on</strong>-medical drug use. In: Sells, S.B., ed. The Effectiveness of<strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong> Treatment. Vol. III. Cambridge, Mass.: Ballinger,1976b. pp. 29-76.Goss, A., and Morosko, I. Relati<strong>on</strong>s between a dimensi<strong>on</strong> of internalexternalc<strong>on</strong>trol and the MMPI with an alcoholic populati<strong>on</strong>.Journal of C<strong>on</strong>sulting and Clinical Psychology, 34:189-192, 1970.Gossop, M.R., and Roy, A. Hostility in drug dependent individuals:Its relati<strong>on</strong> to specific drugs and oral or intravenous use. BritishJournal of Psychiatry, 128:188-193, 1976.Goyer, P.F.; Davis, G.C.; and Rapoport, J.L. <strong>Abuse</strong> of prescribedstimulant medicati<strong>on</strong> by a 13-year-old hyperactive boy. Journal ofAmerican Academy of Child Psychiatry, 18:170-175, 1979.Gozali, J., and Sloan, J. C<strong>on</strong>trol orientati<strong>on</strong> as a pers<strong>on</strong>ality dimensi<strong>on</strong>am<strong>on</strong>g alcoholics. Quarterly Journal of <strong>Studies</strong> <strong>on</strong> Alcohol, 32:159-161, 1971.Greaves, G. MMPI correlates of drug abuse in hospitalized adolescents.Psychological Reports, 29:1222, 1971.Greaves, G. Sexual disturbances am<strong>on</strong>g chr<strong>on</strong>ic amphetamine users.Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 155:363-365, 1972.Greaves, G. Toward an existential theory of drug dependence.Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 159:263-274, 1974.Greaves, G. Psychosocial aspects of amphetamine abuse. In: Caldwell,J., ed. Amphetamines and Related Stimulants. West Palm Beach,Fla.: CRC Press, in press.Green, J., and Jaffe, J.H. Alcohol and opiate dependence. Journal of<strong>Studies</strong> <strong>on</strong> Alcohol, 38:1274-1293, 1977.439


Greenblatt, M., and Schuckit, M.A., eds. Alcohol Problems in Womenand Children. New York: Grune & Stratt<strong>on</strong>, 1976.Greenough, W.T. Experiential modificati<strong>on</strong> of the developing brain.American Scientist, 63:37-46, 1975.Greenough, W.T., and Juraska, J.M. Experience-induced changes inbrain fine structure: Their behavioral implicati<strong>on</strong>s. In: Hahn,M.E.; Jensen, C.; and Dydek, B.P., eds. Development and Evoluti<strong>on</strong>of Brain Size. New York: Academic Press, 1979.Grevert, P., and Goldstein, A. Endorphins: Nalox<strong>on</strong>e fails to alterexperimental pain or mood in humans. Science, 199:1093-1095,1978.Griffiths, R.R.; Bigelow, G.E.; and Liebs<strong>on</strong>, I. Experimental drugself-administrati<strong>on</strong>: Generality across species and type of drug.In: Krasnegor, N.A., ed. Self-Administrati<strong>on</strong> of <strong>Abuse</strong> Substances:Methods for Study. Research M<strong>on</strong>ograph Series, Vol. 20. Rockville,Md.: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>, 1978.Groves, W.E. Students’ drug use and life styles. In: Josephs<strong>on</strong>, E.,and Carroll, E., eds. <strong>Drug</strong> Use. New York: Winst<strong>on</strong>-Wiley,1974.Guarner, E. Psychodynamic aspects of drug experiences. BritishJournal of Medical Psychology, 39:157-162, 1966.Gusfield, J. Symbolic Crusade. Urbana, Ill.: University of IllinoisPress, 1963.Gusfield, J. The structural c<strong>on</strong>text of college drinking. In: Maddox,G.L., ed. The Domesticated <strong>Drug</strong>: Drinking Am<strong>on</strong>g Collegians.New Haven, C<strong>on</strong>n.: College and University Press, 1970.Haas, N.; Ling, W.; Holmes, E.; Blakis, M.; Litaker, M. Naltrex<strong>on</strong>ein methad<strong>on</strong>e maintenance patients electing to become “drug free.”In: Julius, D., and Renault, P., eds. Narcotic Antag<strong>on</strong>ists:Naltrex<strong>on</strong>e. Research M<strong>on</strong>ograph Series, Vol. 9. Rockville, Md.:Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>, 1976.Haertzen, C.A. Development of scales based <strong>on</strong> the patterns of drugeffects using the Addicti<strong>on</strong> Research Center. Psychological Reports,18:163-194, 1966.Haertzen, C.A. Estimating specificity of drug and alcohol subculturalgroups with slang names for drugs. Journal of C<strong>on</strong>sulting andClinical Psychology, 49(3):592-594, 1979.Haertzen, C.A., and Hill, H.E. Effects of morphine and pentobarbital<strong>on</strong> differential MMPI profiles. Journal of Clinical Psychology,15:434-437, 1959.Haertzen, C.A., and Hooks, N.T. Changes in pers<strong>on</strong>ality and subjectiveexperience associated with the chr<strong>on</strong>ic administrati<strong>on</strong> andwithdrawal of opiates. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease,148:606-614, 1969.440


Haglund, R.M.J., and Schuckit, M.A. The epidemiology of alcoholism.In: Estes, N., and Heinemann, E., eds. Alcoholism: Development,C<strong>on</strong>sequences and Interventi<strong>on</strong>s. St. Louis, MO.: Mosby, 1977.Halberg, F.; Caradente, F.; Cornelissen, G.; and Katinas, G.S.Glossary of chr<strong>on</strong>obiology. Chr<strong>on</strong>obiologia, 4(1):1-189, 1977.Haley, J. Whither family therapy. Family Process, 1:69-100, 1962.Haley, J. Toward a theory of pathological systems. In: Zuk, G.H.,and Boszormenyi-Nagy, I., eds. Family Therapy and DisturbedFamilies. Palo Alto, Calif.: Science and Behavior Books, 1967.Haley, J. Uncomm<strong>on</strong> Therapy. New York: Nort<strong>on</strong>, 1973.Haley, J. Problem-Solving Therapy. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass,1976.Hall, J.N. Relati<strong>on</strong>ship between locus of c<strong>on</strong>trol and drug effects inusers of narcotics, stimulants, hypnotics-sedatives, and hallucinogens.Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of the Addicti<strong>on</strong>s, 13(1):143-148,1978.Hamovitch, M.B. The Parent and the Fatally III Child. Los Angeles:Pelmar, 1964.Harbin, H.T., and Maziar, H.M. The families of drug abusers: Aliterary review. Family Process, 14:411-431, 1975.Harding, W.M., and Zinberg, N.E. The effectiveness of the subculturein developing rituals and social sancti<strong>on</strong>s for c<strong>on</strong>trolled drug use.In: du Toit, B.M., ed. <strong>Drug</strong>s, Rituals, and Altered States ofC<strong>on</strong>sciousness. Netherlands: A.A. Balkema, 1977.Harlow, H.F. Learning to Love. San Francisco: Albien, 1971.Harlow, H.F.; Harlow, M.K.; and Hansen, E.W. The maternal affecti<strong>on</strong>alsystem of rhesus m<strong>on</strong>keys. In: Rheingold, H.L., ed.Maternal Behaviors in Mammals. New York: Wiley, 1963.Harney, M., and Cross, J. The Narcotic Officer’s Notebook. Springfield,III.: Charles C Thomas, 1961.Harris, R., and Linn, M.W. Differential resp<strong>on</strong>se of heroin andn<strong>on</strong>heroin abusers to inpatient treatment. American Journal of<strong>Drug</strong> and Alcohol <strong>Abuse</strong>, 5(2):125-150, 1978.Hartmann, D. A study of drug taking adolescents. PsychoanalyticStudy of the Child, 24:384-398, 1969.Harvey, J.A., and Yunger, L.M. Relati<strong>on</strong>ship between telencephaliccenters of serot<strong>on</strong>in and pain sensitivity. In: Barchas, J., andUsdin, E.A., eds. Serot<strong>on</strong>in and Behavior. New York: HarcourtBrace Jovanovich, 1973. pp. 179-190.Haus, E., and Halberg, F. 24-hour rhythm in susceptibility of C miceto toxic dose of ethanol. Journal of Applied Physiology, 14:878-880, 1959.441


Heath, R.G. Pleasure and brain activity in man: Deep and surfaceelectroencephalograms during orgasm. Journal of Nervous andMental Disease, 154:3-18, 1972.Heath, R.G. Maternal-social deprivati<strong>on</strong> and abnormal brain development:Disorders of emoti<strong>on</strong>al and social behavior. In: Prescott,J.W.; Read, M.S.; and Coursin, D.B., eds. Brain Functi<strong>on</strong> andMalnutriti<strong>on</strong>: Neuropsychological Methods and Assessment. NewYork: Wiley, 1975a.Heath, R.G. Brain functi<strong>on</strong> and behavior: I. Emoti<strong>on</strong> and sensoryphenomena in psychotic patients and in experimental animals.Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 160:159-175, 1975b.Heath, R.G. Brain functi<strong>on</strong> in epilepsy: Midbrain, medullary, andcerebellar interacti<strong>on</strong> with the rostral forebrain. Journal of Neurology,Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 39:1037-1051, 1976.Heath, R.G. Modulati<strong>on</strong> of emoti<strong>on</strong> with a brain pacemaker: Treatmentfor intractable psychiatric illness. Journal of Nervous and MentalDisease, 165:300-317, 1977.Heath, R.G.; Dempesy, C.W.; F<strong>on</strong>tana, C.J.; and Myers, W.A.Cerebellar stimulati<strong>on</strong>: Effects <strong>on</strong> septal regi<strong>on</strong>, hippocampus, andamygdala of cats and rats. Biological Psychiatry, 13:501-529,1978.Heath, R.G.; Llewellyn, R.C.; and Rouchell, A.M. Brain mechanismsin psychiatric illness: Rati<strong>on</strong>ale for and results of treatment withcerebellar stimulati<strong>on</strong>. In: Hitchcock, E.R.; Ballantine, H.T.,Jr.; and Meyers<strong>on</strong>, B.A., eds. Modern C<strong>on</strong>cepts in PsychiatricSurgery. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 1979. pp. 77-84.Hechtman, L.; Weiss, G.; Perlman, T.; Hopkins, J.; and Wener, A.Hyperactives as young adults: Prospective 10 year follow-up. In:Gadow, K., and L<strong>on</strong>ey, J., eds. Psychosocial Aspects of <strong>Drug</strong>Treatment for Hyperactivity. American Associati<strong>on</strong> for the Advancementof Science Selected Symposia Series. Boulder, Colo.: WestviewPress, in press.Helmer, J., and Vietoriez, T. <strong>Drug</strong> Use, the Labor Market, and ClassC<strong>on</strong>flict. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.: The <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong> Council, 1974.Hendin, H. Suicide and Scandinavia: A Psychoanalytic Study of Cultureand Character. New York: Grune & Stratt<strong>on</strong>, 1964.Hendin, H. Marijuana abuse am<strong>on</strong>g college students. The Journal ofNervous and Mental Disease, 156:259-270, 1973a.Hendin, H. College students and LSD: Who and why? The Journal ofNervous and Mental Disease, 156:249-258, 1973b.Hendin, H. Students <strong>on</strong> heroin. The Journal of Nervous and MentalDisease, 158:240-255, 1974a.Hendin, H. Amphetamine abuse am<strong>on</strong>g college students. The Journal ofNervous and Mental Disease, 158:256-267, 1974b.442


Hendin, H. Bey<strong>on</strong>d alienati<strong>on</strong>: The end of the psychedelic road.American Journal of <strong>Drug</strong> and Alcohol <strong>Abuse</strong>, 1:11-23, 1974c.Hendin, H. The Age of Sensati<strong>on</strong>. New York: Nort<strong>on</strong>, 1975.Hendin, H.; Carr, A.; Siegel, K.; and Ulman, R. “Adolescent <strong>Drug</strong><strong>Abuse</strong>rs and Their Families: A Preliminary Report,” in press.Hendin, H.; Gaylin, W.; and Carr, A. Psychoanalysis and SocialResearch: The Psychoanalytic Study of the N<strong>on</strong>-Patient. NewYork: Doubleday, 1965.Hendler, H.I., and Stephens, R.C. The addict odyssey: From experimentati<strong>on</strong>to addicti<strong>on</strong>. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of the Addicti<strong>on</strong>s,12:25-42. 1977.Henik, W., and Domino, G. Changes in locus of c<strong>on</strong>trol in heroinaddicts. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of the Addicti<strong>on</strong>s, 9(6):989-992,1974.Henker, B.; Whalen, C.K.; Bugental, D.B.; and Barker, C. Licitand illicit substance use patterns in stimulant treated childrenand their peers. In: Gadow, K., and L<strong>on</strong>ey, J., eds. PsychosocialAspects of <strong>Drug</strong> Treatment for Hyperactivity. AmericanAssociati<strong>on</strong> for the Advancement of Science Selected SymposiaSeries. Boulder, Colo.: Westview Press, in press.Herman, B.H., and Panksepp, J. Effects of morphine and nalox<strong>on</strong>e <strong>on</strong>separati<strong>on</strong> distress and approach attachment: Evidence for opiatemediati<strong>on</strong> of social affect. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior,9:213-220, 1978.Hill, H.E.; Haertzen, C.A.; and Glaser, R. Pers<strong>on</strong>ality characteristicsof narcotic addicts as indicated by the MMPI. Journal of GeneralPsychology, 62:127-139, 1960.Hiller, J.M.; Pears<strong>on</strong>, J.; and Sim<strong>on</strong>, E.J. Distributi<strong>on</strong> of stereospecificbinding of the potent narcotic analgesic etorphine in the humanbrain: Predominance in the limbic system. Research Communicati<strong>on</strong>sin Chemical Pathology and Pharmacology, 6:1052-1062, 1973.Himmelsbach, C.K. Clinical studies of drug addicti<strong>on</strong>, physical dependence,withdrawal and recovery. Archives of Internal Medicine,69:766, 1942.Himmelsbach, C.K. Morphine, with reference to physical dependence.Federati<strong>on</strong> Proceedings, 2:201-203, 1943.Hinrichsen, J.J. Locus of c<strong>on</strong>trol am<strong>on</strong>g alcoholics: Some empiricaland c<strong>on</strong>ceptual issues. Journal of <strong>Studies</strong> <strong>on</strong> Alcohol, 37:908-916,1976.Hirsch, H.V.B., and Spinelli, D.N. Visual experience modifies distributi<strong>on</strong>of horiz<strong>on</strong>tally and vertically oriented receptive fields incats. Science, 168:869-871, 1970.Hochhauser, M. <strong>Drug</strong>s as agents of c<strong>on</strong>trol. Journal of Psychedelic<strong>Drug</strong>s, 10(1):65-69, 1978a.443


Hochhauser, M. Adolescent drug abuse and the development of behavior.Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of the Addicti<strong>on</strong>s, 13(6):1013-1019, 1978b.Hoffer, A. D-lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD): A review of its presentstatus. Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics, 6:183-255, 1965.Hoffman, L. Breaking the homeostatic cycle. In: Guerin, P., ed.Family Therapy: Theory and Practice. New York: Gardner, 1976.Horn, G.; Rose, S.P.R.; and Bates<strong>on</strong>, P.P.G. Experience and plasticityin the central nervous system. Science, 203:75-78, 1979.Hosobuchi, Y.; Adams, T.F.; and Linchitz, R. Pain relief by electricalstimulati<strong>on</strong> of the central gray matter in humans and its reversalby nalox<strong>on</strong>e. Science 197:183-186, 1977.Houck, J.E.; Kimball, C.; Chang, C.; Pedigo, N.W.; and Yamamura,N.W. Placental beta-endorphin-like peptides. Science, 207:78-80,1980.Howard, J., and Borges, P.B. Needle sharing in the Haight: Somesocial and psychological functi<strong>on</strong>s. Journal of Health and SocialBehavior, 11:220-230, 1970.Huba, G.J. Daydreaming. In: Woody, R.H., ed. Encyclopedia ofClinical Assessment. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1980.Huba, G.J., and Bentler, P.M. Phencyclidine use in high school:Tests of models. Journal of <strong>Drug</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong>, 9:285-291, 1979.Huba, G.J.; Wingard, J.A.; and Bentler, P.M. Adolescent drug useand peer and adult interacti<strong>on</strong> patterns. Journal of C<strong>on</strong>sulting andClinical Psychology, 47:265-276, 1979a.Huba, G.J.; Wingard, J.A.; and Bentler, P.M. A L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal Analysisof the Role of Peer Support, Adult Models, and Peer Subculturesin Beginning Adolescent Substance Use. TechnIcal Report. LOSAngeles, Calif.: UCLA/NIDA Center for Adolescent <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>Etiologies, 1979b.Huba, G.J.; Wingard, J.A.; and Bentler, P.M. Adolescent drug useand intenti<strong>on</strong>s to use drugs in the future: A c<strong>on</strong>current analysis.Journal of <strong>Drug</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong>, 9:145-150, 1979c.Huba, G.J.; Wingard, J.A.; and Bentler, P.M. Models for Adolescent<strong>Drug</strong> Use. Technical Report. Los Angeles, Calif.: UCLA/NIDACenter for Adolescent <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong> Etiologies, 1979d.Huba, G.J.; Wingard, J.A.; and Bentler, P.M. Intenti<strong>on</strong>s to usedrugs am<strong>on</strong>g adolescents: A l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal analysis. Internati<strong>on</strong>alJournal of the Addicti<strong>on</strong>s, in press.Hubel, D.H., and Wiesel, T.N. The period of susceptibility to thephysiological effects of unilateral eye closure in kittens. Journalof Physiology (L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>), 206:419-436, 1970.Huesmann, L.R., ed. Learned helplessness as a model of depressi<strong>on</strong>.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87(1):1-198, 1978.444


Hughes, J. Isolati<strong>on</strong> of an endogenous compound from the brain withproperties similar to morphine. Brain Research, 88:295-308,1975.Hughes, J.; Smith, T.W.; Kosterlitz, H.; Fothergill, L.A.; Morgan,B.A.; and Morris, H.R. Identificati<strong>on</strong> of two related pentapeptidesfrom the brain with potent opiate antag<strong>on</strong>ist activity. Nature,258:577-579. 1975.Hughes, P.H., and Crawford, G.A. A c<strong>on</strong>tagious disease model forresearching and intervening in heroin epidemics. Archives ofGeneral Psychiatry, 27:149-155, 1972.Hughes, P.H.; Crawford, G.A.; Barker, N.A.; Schumann, S.; andJaffe, J.H. The social structure of a heroin copping community.American Journal of Psychiatry, 128:551-558, 1971.Hull, C.I. The c<strong>on</strong>cept of the habit-family hierarchy and maze learning.Psychological Review, 41:33-54, 1934.Hunt, W.A.; Barnett, L.W.; and Branch, L.G. Relapse rates inaddicti<strong>on</strong> programs. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 27:455-456,1971.Huxley, A. The Doors of Percepti<strong>on</strong>. New York: Perennial <strong>Library</strong>,1970.Ingoglia, N.A., and Dole, V.P. Localizati<strong>on</strong> of d- and l-methad<strong>on</strong>eafter intraventricular injecti<strong>on</strong> into rat brains. Journal of Pharmacologyand Experimental Therapeutics, 175:84-87. 1970.Jacks<strong>on</strong>, D.D. The questi<strong>on</strong> of family homeostasis. Psychiatric QuarterlySupplement, 31 (Part 1):79-90, 1957.Jacob, J.J.; Tremblay, E.C.; and Colombel, M.C. Facilitati<strong>on</strong> dereacti<strong>on</strong>s nociceptives par la nalox<strong>on</strong>e chez le souris et chez lerat. Psychopharmacologia, 37:217-223, 1974.Jacobs<strong>on</strong>, R., and Zinberg, N.E. The Social Basis of <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong> Preventi<strong>on</strong>.Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.: <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong> Council, 1975.Jacquet, Y.F., and Marks, N. The C-fragment of B-lipotropin: Anendogenous neuroleptic of anti-psychotogen. Science, 194:632,1976.Jaffe, J.H. <strong>Drug</strong> addicti<strong>on</strong> and drug abuse. In: Goodman, L.S., andGilman, A., eds. The Pharmacological Basis of Therapeutics.New York: Macmillan, 1970a. pp. 276-312.Jaffe, J.H. Narcotic analgesics. In: Goodman, L.S., and Gilman, A.,eds. The Pharmacological Basis of Therapeutics. New York:Macmillan, 1970b. pp. 237-275.James, W. Principles of Psychology. Vol. I. New York: Henry Holt,1890; reprint ed. New York: Dover, 1950.James, W. The Varieties of Religious Experience: A Study of HumanNature. Reprint (1907). New York: New American <strong>Library</strong>,1958.445


Jarvik, M.E. Further observati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> nicotine as the reinforcingagent in smoking. In: Dunn, W.L., Jr., ed. Smoking Behavior:Motives and Incentives. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.: V.H. Winst<strong>on</strong>, 1973.Jasinski, D.R. Assessment of the dependence liability of opiates andsedative-hypnotics. In: Goldberg, L., and Hoffmeister, F., eds.Psychic Dependence, Bayer Symposium IV. New York: Springer,1973. pp. 160-170.Jasinski, D.R.; Martin, W.R.; and Haertzen, C.A. The human pharmacologyand abuse potential of N-allylnoroxymorph<strong>on</strong>e (nalox<strong>on</strong>e).Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 157:420-426, 1967.Jasinski, D.R.; Martin, W.R.; and Hoeldtke, R.D. Effects of shortandl<strong>on</strong>g-term administrati<strong>on</strong> of pentazocine in man. ClinicalPharmacology and Therapeutics, 11:385-403, 1970.Jasinski, D.R.; Martin, W.R.; and Hoeldtke, R.D. <strong>Studies</strong> of thedependence-producing properties of GPA-1657, profadol, andpropiram in man. Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics, 12:613-640, 1971.Jasinski, D.R.; Martin, W.R.; and Sapira, J.D. Antag<strong>on</strong>ism of thesubjective, behavioral, pupillary and respiratory depressant effectof cyclazocine by nalox<strong>on</strong>e. Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics,9:215-222, 1968.Jel<strong>line</strong>k, E.M. The Disease C<strong>on</strong>cept of Alcoholism. New Brunswick,N.J.: Hillhouse, 1960.Jeri, F.R.; Sanchez, C.C.; del Pozo, T.; Fernandez, M.; and Carbajal,C. Further experience with the syndromes produced by cocapaste smoking. United Nati<strong>on</strong>s Bulletin <strong>on</strong> narcotics, 30(3):1-11,1978.Jessop, D.; Kandel, D.; Elins<strong>on</strong>, J.; Josephs<strong>on</strong>, E.; and Zanes, A.Comparative Analyses of Stages of <strong>Drug</strong> Use in Different EthnicGroups: Center Cross-Study I I (Nati<strong>on</strong>al Teenage <strong>Drug</strong> Study andNew York State Sample). New York: Columbia University, Centerfor Socio-Cultural Research <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> Use, 1977.Jessop, D.; Kandel, D.; and Lukoff, I. Comparative Analyses of Stagesof <strong>Drug</strong> Use in Different Ethnic Groups: Center Cross-Study I(Bedford Stuyvesant and New York State). New York: ColumbiaUniversity, Center for Socio-Cultural Research <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> Use,1976.Jessor, R. Predicting time of <strong>on</strong>set of marijuana use: A developmentalstudy of high school youth. Journal of C<strong>on</strong>sulting and ClinicalPsychology, 44(1):125-134, 1976.Jessor, R. Marihuana: A review of recent psychosocial research. In:DuP<strong>on</strong>t, R.L.; Goldstein, A.; and O’D<strong>on</strong>nell, J., eds. Handbook<strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>. Rockville, Md.: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong><strong>Abuse</strong>, 1979. pp. 337-355.Jessor, R.; Chase, J.A.; and D<strong>on</strong>ovan, J.E. Psychosocial correlatesof marijuana use and problem drinking in a nati<strong>on</strong>al sample ofadolescents. American Journal of Public Health, in press.446


Jessor, R.; Graves, T.D.; Hans<strong>on</strong>, R.C.; and Jessor, S.L. Society,Pers<strong>on</strong>ality, and Deviant Behavior: A Study of a Tri-Ethnic Community.New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winst<strong>on</strong>, 1968; reprint ed.Huntingt<strong>on</strong>, N.Y.: Kreiger, 1968Jessor, R., and Jessor, S.L. A social psychology of marihuana use.Journal of Pers<strong>on</strong>ality and Social Psychology, 26: 1-15, 1973.Jessor, R., and Jessor, S.L. Problem Behavior and Psychosocial Development--AL<strong>on</strong>gitudinal Study of Youth. New York: AcademicPress, 1977.Jessor, R., and Jessor, S.L. Theory testing in l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal research<strong>on</strong> marihuana use. In: Kandel, D.B., ed. L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal Research<strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> Use. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.: Hemisphere, 1978. pp. 41-71.Jessor, S.L., and Jessor, R. Transiti<strong>on</strong> from virginity to n<strong>on</strong>virginityam<strong>on</strong>g youth: A social-psychological study over time. DevelopmentalPsychology, 11:473-484, 1975.Johns<strong>on</strong>, B.D. Marijuana Users and <strong>Drug</strong> Subcultures. New York:Wiley, 1973.Johns<strong>on</strong>, B.D. <strong>On</strong>ce an addict, seldom an addict. C<strong>on</strong>temporary <strong>Drug</strong>Problems, Spring:35-53, 1978.Johns<strong>on</strong>, B.D.; Goldstein, P.; and Duchaine, N. “What Is an Addict.Theoretical Perspectives and Empirical Patterns of Opiate Use.”Paper presented to the Society for the Study of Social Problems,Bost<strong>on</strong>, Aug. 1979.Johns<strong>on</strong>, B.D., and Preble, E. “Final Report. Ethnography of <strong>Drug</strong>Use Am<strong>on</strong>g Two White Ethnic Groups.” Submitted to the Nati<strong>on</strong>alInstitute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>, 1978.Johns<strong>on</strong>, B.D., and Uppal, P.S. Marijuana and youth: A generati<strong>on</strong>g<strong>on</strong>e to pot. In: Scarpitti, F., and Datesman, S., eds. <strong>Drug</strong>sand the Youth Culture. Beverly Hills, Calif.: Sage Publicati<strong>on</strong>s,in press.Johns<strong>on</strong>, L.C.; Burdick, J.A.; and Smith, J. Sleep during alcoholicwithdrawal in the chr<strong>on</strong>ic alcoholic. Archives of General Psychiatry,22:406-418, 1970.Johnst<strong>on</strong>, L. <strong>Drug</strong>s and American Youth. Ann Arbor, Mich.: Institutefor Social Research, The University of Michigan, 1973.Johnst<strong>on</strong>, L.D.; Bachman, J.G.; and O’Malley, P.M. <strong>Drug</strong> Use Am<strong>on</strong>gAmerican High School Students 1975-1977. Rockville, Md.: Nati<strong>on</strong>alInstitute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>, 1977.Johnst<strong>on</strong>, L.D.; Bachman, J.G.; and O’Malley, P.M. <strong>Drug</strong>s and theClass of ‘78: Behaviors, Attitudes and Recent Nati<strong>on</strong>al Trends.Rockville, Md.: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>, 1979.Johnst<strong>on</strong>, L.D.; O’Malley, P.M.; and Eveland, L.K. <strong>Drug</strong>s and delinquency:A search for causal c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s. In: Kandel, D.B., ed.L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal Research <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> Use. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.: Hemisphere,1978. pp. 137-156.447


J<strong>on</strong>as, A.D., and J<strong>on</strong>as, D.F. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 12:33, 1975.J<strong>on</strong>as, D.F., and J<strong>on</strong>as, A.D. A bioanthropological overview of addicti<strong>on</strong>.Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, Spring:345-354,1977.J<strong>on</strong>es, B.M. Circadian variati<strong>on</strong> in the effects of alcohol <strong>on</strong> cognitiveperformance. Quarterly Journal of <strong>Studies</strong> <strong>on</strong> Alcohol, 35(4):1212-1219, 1974.J<strong>on</strong>es, B.M., and J<strong>on</strong>es, M.K. Male and female intoxicati<strong>on</strong> levels forthree alcohol doses or do women really get higher than men?Alcohol Technical Reports, 5:11-14, 1976.J<strong>on</strong>es, B.M., and Paredes, A. Circadian variati<strong>on</strong> of ethanol metabolismin alcoholics. British Journal of Addicti<strong>on</strong>s, 69(1):3-10, 1974.J<strong>on</strong>es, L. E., and Thomps<strong>on</strong>, W. How 92% beat the dope habit. Bulletinof the Los Angeles County Medical Associati<strong>on</strong>, 88(April 3):19,37-40,1958.J<strong>on</strong>es, M.C. Pers<strong>on</strong>ality correlates and antecedents of drinking patternsin adult males. Journal of C<strong>on</strong>sulting and Clinical Psychology,32:2-12, 1968.J<strong>on</strong>es, R.E. A study of 100 physician psychiatric inpatients. AmericanJournal of Psychiatry, 134:1119-1122, 1977.Jung, C.G. <strong>On</strong> the relati<strong>on</strong> of analytical psychology to poetry. In:Campbell, J., ed. The Portable Jung. Hull, R.F.C, trans.New York: Viking, 1971. pp. 302-322.Kaij, L. “<strong>Studies</strong> <strong>on</strong> the Etiology and Sequels of <strong>Abuse</strong> of Alcohol.”Sweden: University of Lund, Department of Psychiatry, 1960.Kales, A.; Bixler, E.O.; Tjiauw-Ling, T.; Scharf, M.B.; and Kales,J.D. Chr<strong>on</strong>ic hypnotic-drug use--ineffectiveness, drug-withdrawalinsomnia, and dependence. Journal of the American Medical Associati<strong>on</strong>,227:513-517, 1974.Kandel, D. The role of parents and peers in adolescent marihuanause. Science, 181:1067-1070, 1973.Kandel, D. Interpers<strong>on</strong>al influences <strong>on</strong> adolescent illegal drug use.In: Josephs<strong>on</strong>, E., and Carroll, E., eds. <strong>Drug</strong> Use: Epidemiologicaland Sociological Approaches. Washingt<strong>on</strong>: Hemisphere, 1974.Kandel, D. Stages in adolescent involvement in drug use. Science,190:912-914, 1975.Kandel, D.B. Adolescent involvement in illicit drug use: A multipleclassificati<strong>on</strong> analysis. Social Forces, 55:438-458, 1976.Kandel, D.B. C<strong>on</strong>vergences in prospective l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal surveys ofdrug use in normal populati<strong>on</strong>s. In: Kandel, D.B., ed. L<strong>on</strong>gitudinalResearch <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> Use. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.: Hemisphere,1978a. pp. 3-38.448


Kandel, D.B., ed. L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal Research <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> Use: Empirical Findingsand Methodological Issues. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.: Hemisphere,1978b.Kandel, D.B., and Faust, R. Sequence and stages in patterns ofadolescent drug use. Archives of General Psychiatry, 32:923-932,1975.Kandel, D.B.; Kessler, R.; and Margulies, R.Z. Antecedents ofadolescent initiati<strong>on</strong> into stages of drug use: A developmentalanalysis. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 7(1):13-40, 1978.Also in: Kandel, D.B., ed. L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal Research <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> Use:Empirical Findings and Methodological Issues. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.:Hemisphere, 1978. pp. 73-99.Kandel, D.B.; Treiman, D.; Faust, R.; and Single, E. Adolescentinvolvement in legal and illegal drug use: A multiple classificati<strong>on</strong>analysis. Social Forces, 54:438-458, 1976.Kaplan, H.B. Toward a general theory of psychosocial deviance: Thecase of aggressive behavior. Social Science and Medicine, 6:593-617, 1972.Kaplan, H.B. Increase in self-rejecti<strong>on</strong> as an antecedent of deviantresp<strong>on</strong>ses. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 4:281-292, 1975a.Kaplan, H.B. Self-Attitudes and Deviant Behavior. Pacific Palisades,Ca.: Goodyear Publishing, 1975b.Kaplan, H.B. Sequelae of self-derogati<strong>on</strong>: Predicting from a generaltheory of deviant behavior. Youth and Society, 7:171-197, 1975c.Kaplan, H.B. The self-esteem motive and change in self-attitudes.Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 161:265-275, 1975d.Kaplan, H.B. Antecedents of negative self-attitudes: Membershipgroup devaluati<strong>on</strong> and defenselessness. Social Psychiatry, 11:15-25, 1976a.Kaplan, H.B. Self-attitudes and deviant resp<strong>on</strong>se. Social Forces,54:788-801, 1976b.Kaplan, H.B. Antecedents of deviant resp<strong>on</strong>ses: Predicting from ageneral theory of deviant behavior. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,6:89-101, 1977a.Kaplan, H.B. Increase in self-rejecti<strong>on</strong> and c<strong>on</strong>tinuing/disc<strong>on</strong>tinueddeviant resp<strong>on</strong>se. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 6:77-87,1977b.Kaplan, H.B. Social class, self-derogati<strong>on</strong> and deviant resp<strong>on</strong>se.Social Psychiatry, 13:19-28, 1978a.Kaplan, H.B. Deviant behavior and self-enhancement in adolescence.Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 7:253-277, 1978b.Kaplan, H.I., and Kaplan, H.S. The psychosomatic c<strong>on</strong>cept of obesity.Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 125(2):181-201, 1957.449


Kastenbaum, R., and Aisenberg, P. The Psychology of Death. NewYork: Springer, 1972.Keats, A.S., and Telford, J. Narcotic antag<strong>on</strong>ists as analgesics. In:Gould, R.F., ed. Molecular Modificati<strong>on</strong> of <strong>Drug</strong> Design. Advancesin Chemistry Series, No. 45. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.: American ChemicalSociety, 1964. pp. 170-176.Kellam, S.G.; Sim<strong>on</strong>, M.B.; and Ensminger, M.E. Antecedents ofteenage drug use and psychological well-being: A ten-yearcommunity-wide prospective study. In: Ricks, D., and Dohrenwend,B.S., eds. Origins of Psychopathology: Research and Public Policy.Cambridge, Mass.: Cambridge University Press, in press.Keller, M. <strong>On</strong> the loss-of-c<strong>on</strong>trol phenomen<strong>on</strong> in alcoholism. BritishJournal of Addicti<strong>on</strong>, 67:153-166, 1972.Kernberg, O.F. Border<strong>line</strong> C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s and Pathological Narcissism.New York: Ar<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>, 1975.Kettlewell, B. The Evoluti<strong>on</strong> of Melanism. Oxford: Clarend<strong>on</strong>, 1973.Khantzian, E.J. A preliminary dynamic formulati<strong>on</strong> of the psychopharmacologicacti<strong>on</strong> of methad<strong>on</strong>e. In: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Associati<strong>on</strong> for thePreventi<strong>on</strong> of Addicti<strong>on</strong> to Narcotics. Proceedings of the FourthNati<strong>on</strong>al Methad<strong>on</strong>e C<strong>on</strong>ference. New York: the Associati<strong>on</strong>, 1972.Khantzian, E.J. Opiate addicti<strong>on</strong>: A critique of theory and someimplicati<strong>on</strong>s for treatment. American Journal of Psychotherapy,28:59-70, 1974.Khantzian, E.J. Self selecti<strong>on</strong> and progressi<strong>on</strong> in drug dependence.Psychiatry Digest, 10:19-22, 1975.Khantzian, E.J. The ego, the self and opiate addicti<strong>on</strong>: Theoreticaland treatment c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Review of Psychoanalysis,5:189-198, 1978.Khantzian, E.J.; Mack, J.E.; and Schatzberg, A.F. Heroin use as anattempt to cope: Clinical observati<strong>on</strong>s. American Journal of Psychiatry,131(2):160-164, 1974.Khantzian, E.J., and Treece, C.J. Psychodynamic aspects of drugdependence: An overview. In: Blaine, J.D., and Julius, D.A.,eds. Psychodynamics of <strong>Drug</strong> Dependence. Research M<strong>on</strong>ographNo. 12. Rockville, Md.: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>, 1977.pp. 11-25.Khantzian, E.J., and Treece, C.J. Heroin addicti<strong>on</strong>: The diagnosticdilemma for psychiatry. In: Pickens, R.W., and Hest<strong>on</strong>, L.L.,eds. Psychiatric Factors in <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>. New York: Crune &Stratt<strong>on</strong>, 1979. pp. 21-45.Kielholz, P., and Battegay, R. The treatment of drug addicts inSwitzerland. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 4:225-238, 1963.King, R. The <strong>Drug</strong> Hangup. New York: Nort<strong>on</strong>, 1972.450


Klagsbrun, M., and Davis, D.I. Substance abuse and family interacti<strong>on</strong>.Family Process, 16:149-173, 1977.Kleinman, P.A., and Lukoff, I.F. Ethnic differences in factors relatedto drug use. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 19:190-199,1978.Kluckhohn, C. Culture and Behavior. New York: Free Press, 1962.Kohlberg, L. C<strong>on</strong>tinuities in childhood and adult moral developmentrevisited. In: Baltes, P., and Schaie, K.W., eds. Life-SpanDevelopmental Psychology. New York: Academic Press, 1973.Kohut, H. The Analysis of the Self. New York: Internati<strong>on</strong>al UniversitiesPress, 1971.Kolb, L. <strong>Drug</strong> addicti<strong>on</strong> and its relati<strong>on</strong> to crime. Mental Hygiene,9:74-89, 1925a.Kolb, L. Types and characteristics of drug addicts. Mental Hygiene,9:300-313, 1925b.Kosterlitz, H.W.; Waterfield, A.A.; and Berthoud, V. “Assesssmentof Ag<strong>on</strong>ist and Antag<strong>on</strong>ist Properties of Narcotic Analgesic <strong>Drug</strong>sby Their Acti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> the Morphine Receptor in the Guinea PigIleum.” Presented to committee <strong>on</strong> Problems of <strong>Drug</strong> Dependence,Nati<strong>on</strong>al Research Council, Chapel Hill, N.C., 1973.Kramer, J., and L<strong>on</strong>ey, J. “Predicting Adolescent Antisocial BehaviorAm<strong>on</strong>g Hyperactive Boys.” Paper presented as part of a symposium<strong>on</strong> Juvenile Delinquency: Predictors, Prognosis, and ClinicalPractice at the annual meeting of the American PsychologicalAssociati<strong>on</strong>, Tor<strong>on</strong>to, Canada, Aug. 1978.Kripke, D.F.; Mullaney, D.J.; Atkins<strong>on</strong>, M.; and Wolf, S. Circadianrhythm disorders in manic depressives. Biological Psychiatry,13(3):335-351, 1978.Krohn, M.D. An investigati<strong>on</strong> of the effect of parental and peerassociati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> marijuana use: An empirical test of differentialassociati<strong>on</strong> theory. In: Riedel, M., and Thornberry, T.P., eds.Crime and Delinquency: Dimensi<strong>on</strong>s of Deviance. New York:Praeger, 1974. pp. 75-89.Krystal, H. The genetic development of affects and affect regressi<strong>on</strong>.Annual of Psychoanalysis, 2:93-126, 1974.Krystal, H., and Raskin, H.A. <strong>Drug</strong> Dependence: Aspects of EgoFuncti<strong>on</strong>s. Detroit: Wayne State University Press, 1970.Kubie, L.S. The fundamental nature of the distincti<strong>on</strong> between normalityand neurosis. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 23:167-204, 1954.Kubler-Ross, E. <strong>On</strong> Death and Dying. New York: Macmillan, 1969.Kubler-Ross, E. Death, the Final Stage of Growth. Englewood Cliffs,N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1975.451


Kuhar, M.J.; Pert, C.B.; and Snyder, S.H. Regi<strong>on</strong>al distributi<strong>on</strong> ofopiate receptor binding in m<strong>on</strong>key and human brain. Nature245:447-450, 1973.Kurland, A.A.; Kerman, F.; Wurmser, L.; and Kokosky, R. Intermittentpatterns of narcotic usage. In: Cole, J.O., and Wittenborn,J.R., eds. <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>: Social and Psychopharmacological Aspects.Springfield, Ill.: Charles C Thomas, 1969.Lack, D. The Natural Regulati<strong>on</strong> of Animal Numbers. Oxford: Oxford,1954.Lal, H.; DeFeo, J.J.; Pitterman, A.; Patel, G.; and Baumel, I.Effects of prol<strong>on</strong>ged social deprivati<strong>on</strong> or enrichment <strong>on</strong> neur<strong>on</strong>alsensitivity for CNS depressants and stimulants. In: Singh, J.M.,and Miller, L., eds. <strong>Drug</strong> Addicti<strong>on</strong>: Experimental Pharmacology.Vol. 1. New York: Futura Publishing, 1972. pp. 255-266.Langer, J. <strong>Drug</strong> entrepreneurs and dealing culture. Social Problems,23(3):377-386, 1977.Langhorne, J.E., Jr., and L<strong>on</strong>ey, J. A four-fold model for subgroupingthe hyperkinetic/MBD syndrome. Child Psychiatry and HumanDevelopment, 9:153-159, 1979.Langhorne, J.E., Jr.; L<strong>on</strong>ey, J.; Paternite, C.E.; and Bechtoldt,H.P. Childhood hyperkinesis: A return to the source. Journal ofAbnormal Psychology, 85:201-209, 1976.Larsen, S. The Shaman’s Doorway. New York: Harper and Row,1976.Lasagna, L. <strong>Drug</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong> in the field of analgesic drugs. Proceedingsof the Royal Society of Medicine, 58:978-983, 1965.Lasagna, L.; v<strong>on</strong> Felsinger, J.M.; and Beecher, H.K. <strong>Drug</strong>-inducedmood changes in man. 1. Observati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> healthy subjects,chr<strong>on</strong>ically-ill patients, and “postaddicts.” Journal of the AmericanMedical Associati<strong>on</strong>, 157:1006-1020, 1955.Laufer, M.W. L<strong>on</strong>g-term management and some follow-up findings <strong>on</strong>the use of drugs with minimal cerebral syndromes. Journal ofLearning Disabilities, 4:55-58, 1971.Laufer, M.W., and Denhoff, E. Hyperkinetic behavior syndrome inchildren. The Journal of Pediatrics, 50:463-474, 1957.Laws<strong>on</strong>, T.R., and Winstead, D.K. Toward a theory of drug use.British Journal of Addicti<strong>on</strong>, 73:149-154, 1978.Lazarus, A. Multi-Modal Behavior Therapy. New York: Springer,1976.Lazarus, A. Has behavior therapy outlived its usefulness? AmericanPsychologist, 32:550-554, 1977.Lemert, E.M. Human Deviance, Social Problems, and Social C<strong>on</strong>trol.Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1972.452


Lennard, H.L. Mystificati<strong>on</strong> and <strong>Drug</strong> Misuse. New York: Harperand Row, 1972.Lennard, H.L., and Allen, S.D. The treatment of drug addicti<strong>on</strong>:Toward new models. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of the Addicti<strong>on</strong>s,8:521-535, 1973.Lettieri, D.J., ed. Predicting Adolescent <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>: A Review ofIssues, Methods and Correlates. Rockville, Md.: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute<strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>. 1975.Levine, J.D.; Gord<strong>on</strong>, N.C.; and Fields, H.L. The mechanism ofplacebo analgesia. The Lancet, Sept. 23, 1978. pp. 654-657.Levine, J.D.; Gord<strong>on</strong>, N.C.; J<strong>on</strong>es, R.T.; and Fields, H.L. Thenarcotic antag<strong>on</strong>ist nalox<strong>on</strong>e enhances clinical pain. Nature272:826-827, 1978.Levi-Strauss, C. Social structure. In: Kroeber, A.L., ed. AnthropologyToday. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1953.Lewin, L. Phantastica. L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1924.Rev. ed. 1964.Lewis, O. La Vida: The Puerto Rican Family in the Culture of Poverty.New York: Random House, 1966.Li, C.H. Lipotropin: A new active peptide from pituitary glands.Nature, 201:924, 1964.Lichstein, L., and Sackett, G.P. Reacti<strong>on</strong>s by differentially raisedrhesus m<strong>on</strong>keys to noxious stimulati<strong>on</strong>. Developmental Psychobiology,4:339-352, 1971.Lieberman, M.A.; Yalom, I.D.; and Miles, M.B. Encounter Groups:First Facts. New York: Basic Books, Inc., 1973.Light, A.B., and Torrance, E.G. Opium addicti<strong>on</strong>: VI. The effects ofabrupt withdrawal followed by readministrati<strong>on</strong> of morphine inhuman addicts, with special reference to the compositi<strong>on</strong> of theblood, the circulati<strong>on</strong> and the metabolism. Archives of InternalMedicine, 44:1-16, 1929.Linden, R., and Currie, R. Religiosity and drug use: A test of socialc<strong>on</strong>trol theory. Canadian Journal of Criminology and Correcti<strong>on</strong>s,19:346-355, 1977.Lindesmith, A.R. Opiate Addicti<strong>on</strong>. Bloomingt<strong>on</strong>, Ind.: PrincipiaPress, 1947; reprint ed., Addicti<strong>on</strong> and Opiates. New York:Aldine, 1968.Lindesmith, A.R. The Addict and the Law. New York: RandomHouse, 1965.Lindesmith, A.R. A reply to McAuliffe and Gord<strong>on</strong>’s “A test of Lindesmith’stheory of addicti<strong>on</strong>.” American Journal of Sociology,81:147-153, 1975.453


Ling, N.; Burgus, R.; and Guillemin, R. Isolati<strong>on</strong>, primary structure,and synthesis of alpha-endorphin and gamma-endorphin, twopeptides of hypothalamic-hypophysical origin with morphinomimeticactivities. Proceedings of the Nati<strong>on</strong>al Academy of Sciences, USA,73:3942-3946, 1976.Little, R.B. Hazards of drug dependency am<strong>on</strong>g physicians. Journalof the American Medical Associati<strong>on</strong>, 218:1533-1535, 1971.Loiselle, P., and Whitehead, P.C. Scaling drug use: An examinati<strong>on</strong>of the popular wisdom. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science,3:347-356, 1971.L<strong>on</strong>ey, J.; Kramer, J.; and Milich, R. The hyperkinetic child growsup: Predictors of symptoms, delinquency, and achievement atfollow-up. In: Gadow, K., and L<strong>on</strong>ey , J., eds. PsychosocialAspects of <strong>Drug</strong> Treatment for Hyperactivity. American Associati<strong>on</strong>for the Advancement of Science Selected Symposia Series.Boulder, Colo.: Westview Press, in press a.L<strong>on</strong>ey, J.; Langhorne, J.E., Jr.; and Paternite, C.E. An empiricalbasis for subgrouping the hyperkinetic/MBD syndrome. Journalof Abnormal Psychology, 87:431-441, 1978.L<strong>on</strong>ey, J.; Langhorne, J.E., Jr.; Paternite, C.E.; Whaley-Klahn,M.A.; Blair-Broeker, C.; and Hacker, M. In: Sells, S.; Roff,M.; Strauss, J.; and Pollin, W., eds. Human Functi<strong>on</strong>ing in L<strong>on</strong>gitudinalPerspective: <strong>Studies</strong> of Normative and Psychopathologic Populati<strong>on</strong>s.New York: Williams & Wilkins, in press b.Lord, J.A.H.; Waterfield, A.A.; Hughes, J.; and Kosterlitz, W.H.Endogenous opioid peptides: Multiple ag<strong>on</strong>ists and receptors.Nature, 267:495-499, 1977.Luce, G.G. Biological Rhythms in Human and Animal Physiology. NewYork: Dover, 1971.Ludwig, A.M. <strong>On</strong> and off the wag<strong>on</strong>: Reas<strong>on</strong>s for drinking and abstainingby alcoholics. Quarterly Journal <strong>Studies</strong> <strong>on</strong> Alcohol, 33:91-96,1972.Ludwig, A.M., and Wikler, A. Craving and relapse to drink. QuarterlyJournal of <strong>Studies</strong> <strong>on</strong> Alcohol, 35:108-130, 1974.Ludwig, A.M.; Wikler, A.; and Stark, L.H. The first drink: Psychobiologicalaspects of craving. Archives of General Psychiatry,30:539-547, 1974.Lukoff, I.F. “Social and Ethnic Patterns of Reported Heroin Use andC<strong>on</strong>tiguity with <strong>Drug</strong> Users.” New York: The Addicti<strong>on</strong> Researchand Treatment Corporati<strong>on</strong> Evaluati<strong>on</strong> Team, 1972.Lukoff, I.F. Issues in the evaluati<strong>on</strong> of heroin treatment. In: Josephs<strong>on</strong>,E., and Carroll, E.E., eds. <strong>Drug</strong> Use: Epidemiological and SociologicalApproaches. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.: Hemisphere, 1974. pp.129-157.454


Lukoff, I.F. Analysis of the Gearing study of the methad<strong>on</strong>e maintenancetreatment program. In: Research and Program PlanningInformati<strong>on</strong>, Research Utilizati<strong>on</strong> Briefs. New York: CommunityCouncil of Greater New York, Oct. 1975.Lukoff, I.F. C<strong>on</strong>sequences of use: Heroin and other narcotics. In:Rittenhouse, J.D., ed. The Epidemiology of Heroin and OtherNarcotics. NIDA Research M<strong>on</strong>ograph 16. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.:Superintendent of Documents, U.S. Government Printing Office,1977. pp. 195-227.Lukoff, I.F., and Brook, J.S. A sociocultural explorati<strong>on</strong> of reportedheroin use. In: Winick, C., ed. Sociological Aspects of <strong>Drug</strong>Dependence. Cleveland: CRC Press, 1974. pp. 35-56.Lukoff, I.F.; Quatr<strong>on</strong>e, D.; and Sardell, A. Some Aspects of the Epidemiologyof Heroin Use in a Ghetto Community: A PreliminaryReport. Mimeo. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.: U.S. Department of Justice,Law Enforcement Assistance Administrati<strong>on</strong>, Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute ofLaw Enforcement and Criminal Justice, July 1972.Lutsch, E.F., and Morris, R.W. Light reversal of a morphine-inducedanalgesia susceptibility rhythm in mice. Experientia, 27:420-421,1971.Lutsch, E.F., and Morris, R.W. Effect of c<strong>on</strong>stant lighting <strong>on</strong> themorphine susceptibility rhythm. Experientia, 28:673-674, 1972.Lynch, J.J. Psychophysiology and development of social attachment.Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 151:231-244, 1970.MacLagan, D. S. Proceedings of the Royal Society [B], 111:437, 1962.Maddox, G. Drinking prior to college. In: Maddox, G.L., ed.The Domesticated <strong>Drug</strong>: Drinking Am<strong>on</strong>g Collegians. New Haven,C<strong>on</strong>n.: College and University Press, 1970.Mahler, M. <strong>On</strong> human symbiosis and the vicissitudes of individuati<strong>on</strong>.Journal of American Psychoanalytic Associati<strong>on</strong>, 15:740-760, 1967.Mah<strong>on</strong>ey, M.J. Reflecti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> the cognitive learning trend in psychotherapy.American Psychologist, 32:5-13, 1977.Maier, S.F., and Seligman, M.E.P. Learned helplessness: Theory andevidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 105:3-46,1976.Maloff, D.; Becker, H.S.; F<strong>on</strong>aroff, A.; and Rodin, J. Informalsocial c<strong>on</strong>trols and their influence <strong>on</strong> substance use. Journal of<strong>Drug</strong> Issues, 9:161-184, 1979.Marra, E.F. Intoxicant <strong>Drug</strong>s. Buffalo: State University of New Yorkat Buffalo, 1967.Marte, E., and Halberg, F. Circadian susceptibility rhythm of mice toLibrium. Federati<strong>on</strong> Proceedings, 20:305, 1961.Martin, W.R. Opioid antag<strong>on</strong>ists. Pharmacological Reviews, 19:463-521,1967.455


Martin, W.R. A homeostatic and redundancy theory of tolerance anddependence to narcotic analgesics. Research Publicati<strong>on</strong>s of theAssociati<strong>on</strong> for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease, 46:206-225,1968.Martin, W.R. Pharmacological redundancy as an adaptive mechanism inthe central nervous system. Federati<strong>on</strong> Proceedings, 29: 13-18,1970.Martin, W.R. Pathophysiology of narcotic addicti<strong>on</strong>: Possible roles ofprotracted abstinence in relapse. In: Zaraf<strong>on</strong>etis, C.J.D., ed.<strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>--Proceedings of the Internati<strong>on</strong>al C<strong>on</strong>ference. Philadelphia:Lea and Febiger, 1972. pp. 153-159.Martin, W.R., and Eades, C.G. A comparative study of the effect ofdrugs <strong>on</strong> activating and vasomotor resp<strong>on</strong>ses evoked by midbrainstimulati<strong>on</strong>: atropine, pentobarbital, chlorpromazine, and chlorpromazinesulfoxide. Psychopharmacologia, 1:303-335, 1960.Martin, W.R.; Eades, C.G.; Thomps<strong>on</strong>, J.A.; Huppler, R.E.; andGilbert, P.E. The effects of morphine- and nalorphine-like drugsin the n<strong>on</strong>dependent and morphine-dependent chr<strong>on</strong>ic spinal dog.Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 197:517-532,1976.Martin, W.R.; Eades, C.G.; Thomps<strong>on</strong>, W.O.; Thomps<strong>on</strong>, J.A.; andFlanary, H.G. Morphine physical dependence in the dog. Journalof Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 189:759-771,1974.Martin, W.R., and Eisenman, A.J. Interacti<strong>on</strong>s between nalorphineand morphine in the decerebrate cat. Journal of Pharmacology andExperimental Therapeutics, 138:113-119, 1962.Martin, W.R.; Fraser, H.F.; Gorodetzky, C.W.; and Rosenberg, D.E.<strong>Studies</strong> of the dependence-producing potential of the narcoticantag<strong>on</strong>ist 2-cyclopropylmethyl-2’-hydroxy-5, 9-dimethyl-6, 7-benzomorphan(cyclazocine, WIN 20, 740; ARC II-C-3). Journal ofPharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 150:426-436, 1965.Martin, W.R., and Gorodetzky, C.W. Dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> of tolerance toand physical dependence <strong>on</strong> N-allylnormorphine (nalorphine).Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 150:437-442, 1965.Martin, W.R.; Gorodetzky, C.W.; and McClane, T.K. An experimentalstudy in the treatment of narcotic addicts with cyclazocine.Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics, 7:455-465, 1966.Martin, W.R.; Hewett, B.B.; Baker, A.J.; and Haertzen, C.A.Aspects of the psychopathology and pathophysiology of addicti<strong>on</strong>.<strong>Drug</strong> and Alcohol Dependence, 2:185-202, 1977.Martin, W.R., and Jasinski, D.R. Physiological parameters of morphinedependence in man--tolerance, early abstinence, protracted abstinence.Journal of Psychiatric Research, 7:9-17, 1969.Martin, W.R.; Jasinski, D.R.; Haertzen, C.A.; Kay, D.C.; J<strong>on</strong>es,B.E.; Mansky, P.A.; and Carpenter, R.W. Methad<strong>on</strong>e--a reevaluati<strong>on</strong>.Archives of General Psychiatry, 28:286-295, 1973.456


Martin, W.R.; Jasinski, D. R.; Sapira, J.D.; Flanary, H.G.; Kelly,O.A.; Thomps<strong>on</strong>, A.K.; and Logan, C.R. The respiratoryeffects of morphine during a cycle of dependence. Journal ofPharmacology and Experimental Therapy, 162:182-189, 1968.Martin, W.R.; Sloan, J.W.; Sapira, J.D.; and Jasinski, D.R. Physiologic,subjective, and behavioral effects of amphetamine, methamphetamine,ephedrine, phenmetrazine and methylphenidate in man.Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics, 12:245-258, 1971.Martin, W.R.; Wikler, A.; Eades, C.G.; and Pescor, F.T. Toleranceto and physical dependence <strong>on</strong> morphine in rats. Psychopharmacologia,4:247, 1963.Maslow, A. Motivati<strong>on</strong> and Emoti<strong>on</strong>. New York: Harper & Row, 1954.Maslow, A. Toward a Psychology of Being. Princet<strong>on</strong>: Van Nostrand,1962.Mas<strong>on</strong>, W.A. Early social deprivati<strong>on</strong> in the n<strong>on</strong>-human primates:Implicati<strong>on</strong>s for human behavior. In: Glass, D.E., ed. Envir<strong>on</strong>mentalInfluences. New York: The Rockefeller University Pressand Russell Sage Foundati<strong>on</strong>, 1968.Mas<strong>on</strong>, W.A. Motivati<strong>on</strong>al factors in psychosocial development. In:Arnold, W.J., and Page, M.M., eds. Nebraska Symposium <strong>on</strong>Motivati<strong>on</strong>. Lincoln, Neb.: University of Nebraska Press, 1971.Mas<strong>on</strong>, W.A., and Berks<strong>on</strong>, G. Effects of maternal mobility <strong>on</strong> thedevelopment of rocking and other behaviors in rhesus m<strong>on</strong>keys: Astudy with artificial mothers. Developmental Psychobiology,8:197-211, 1975.Mas<strong>on</strong>, W.A., and Kenney, M.D. Re-directi<strong>on</strong> of filial attachments inrhesus m<strong>on</strong>keys: Dogs as mother surrogates. Science, 183:1209-1211, 1974.Mayer, D.J.; Price, D.D.; and Rafii, A. Antag<strong>on</strong>ism of acupunctureanalgesia in man by the narcotic antag<strong>on</strong>ist nalox<strong>on</strong>e. BrainResearch, 121:368-372, 1977.Mayfield, D., and Allen, D. Alcohol and affect: A psychopharmacologicalstudy. American Journal of Psychiatry, 123:1346-1351, 1967.McAree, C.P.; Steffenhagen, R.A.; and Zheutlin, S. Pers<strong>on</strong>alityfactors in college drug users. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of Social Psychiatry,15(2):102-106, 1969.McAree, C.P.; Steffenhagen, R.A.; and Zheutlin, S. Pers<strong>on</strong>alityfactors and patterns of drug use in college students. AmericanJournal of Psychiatry, 128(7):890-892, 1972.McAuliffe, W.E. “A Test of Lindesmith’s Theory of Addicti<strong>on</strong>.”Doctoral dissertati<strong>on</strong>. Baltimore, Md.: Johns Hopkins University,Department of Social Relati<strong>on</strong>s, 1973.McAuliffe, W. E. A sec<strong>on</strong>d look at first effects: The subjective effectsof opiates <strong>on</strong> n<strong>on</strong>addicts. Journal of <strong>Drug</strong> Issues, 5:369-399,1975a.457


McAuliffe, W.E. Bey<strong>on</strong>d sec<strong>on</strong>dary deviance: Negative labelling andits effect <strong>on</strong> the heroin addict. In: Grove, W.R., ed. The Labellingof Deviance. Beverly Hills: Sage, 1975b.McAuliffe, W.E. “Two Forms of Opiate Addictive Disease: A ProposedStudy of Opiate Addicts in the United States and Great Britain.”Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard School of Public Health, Departmentof Behavioral Sciences, 1979.McAuliffe, W.E., and Gord<strong>on</strong>, R.A. A test of Lindesmith’s theory ofaddicti<strong>on</strong>: The frequency of euphoria am<strong>on</strong>g l<strong>on</strong>g-term addicts.American Journal of Sociology, 79:795-840, 1974.McAuliffe, W.E., and Gord<strong>on</strong>, R.A. Issues in testing Lindesmith’stheory. American Journal of Sociology, 81:154-163, 1975.McAuliffe, W.E., and Gord<strong>on</strong>, R.A. “C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing and the Combinati<strong>on</strong>of-Effects:A Comprehensive Reinforcement Theory of OpiateAddicti<strong>on</strong>.” Paper submitted for publicati<strong>on</strong>, 1979.McClane, T.K., and Martin, W.R. Antag<strong>on</strong>ism of the spinal cordeffects of morphine and cyclazocine by nalox<strong>on</strong>e and thebaine.Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of Neuropharmacology, 6:325-327, 1967.McClane, T.K., and Martin, W.R. Subjective and physiologic effectsof morphine, pentobarbital, and meprobamate. Clinical Pharmacologyand Therapeutics. 20:192-198. 1976.McClelland, D.C.; Davis, W.N.; Kalin, R.; and Wanner, E. The DrinkingMan. New York: Free Press, 1972.McC<strong>on</strong>nell, J.V. Biorhythms: A report and analysis. Journal of BiologicalPsychology, 20(1):13-24, 1978.McGlothlin, W.H. Critique of “C<strong>on</strong>sequences of use: Heroin and othernarcotics,” by I.F. Lukoff. In: Rittenhouse, J.D., ed. TheEpidemiology of Heroin and Other Narcotics. Research M<strong>on</strong>ographSeries, Vol. 16. Rockville, Md.: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong><strong>Abuse</strong>, 1977.McKenzie, J.D. Trends in Marihuana Use. College Park, Md.: Universityof Maryland Counseling Center, 1969.Meehle, P.E. Article 31. In: Dahlstrom, W.G., ed. Basic Readings <strong>on</strong>the MMPI in Psychology and Medicine. Minneapolis: MinnesotaUniversity Press, 1956.Meichenbaum, D. Cognitive-Behavior Modificati<strong>on</strong>. New York: Plenum,1977.Meites, J.; Brani, J.F.; Van Vugt, D.A.; and Smith, A.F. Relati<strong>on</strong>of endogenous opioid peptides and morphine to neuroendocrinefuncti<strong>on</strong>s. Life Sciences, 24:1325-1336, 1979.Mellinger, G.D. Use of licit drugs and other coping alternatives:Some pers<strong>on</strong>al observati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> the hazards of living. In: Lettieri,D.J., ed. <strong>Drug</strong>s and Suicide: When Other Coping Strategies Fail.Beverly Hills: Sage Publicati<strong>on</strong>s, 1978.458


Mellinger, G.D.; Somers, R.H.; Bazell, S.; and Manheimer, D.I.<strong>Drug</strong> use, academic performance, and career indecisi<strong>on</strong>: L<strong>on</strong>gitudinaldata in search of a model. In: Kandel, D.B., ed. L<strong>on</strong>gitudinalResearch <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> Use. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.: Hemisphere, 19pp. 157-177.Mellinger, G.D.; Somers, R.H.; and Manheimer, D.I. <strong>Drug</strong> useresearch items pertaining to pers<strong>on</strong>ality and interpers<strong>on</strong>al relati<strong>on</strong>s:A workinq paper for research investiqators. In: Lettieri. D.J..ed. Predicting Adolescent <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>: A Review of Issues,Methods, and Correlates. Rockville, Md.: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong><strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>, 1975. pp. 299-342.Mello, N.K. A semantic aspect of alcoholism. In: Cappell, H.D., andLeBlanc, A.E., eds. Biological and Behavioral Approaches to <strong>Drug</strong>Dependence. Tor<strong>on</strong>to: Addicti<strong>on</strong> Research Foundati<strong>on</strong> of <strong>On</strong>tario,1975.Melzack, R., and Burns, S.K. Neurophysiological effects of earlysensory restricti<strong>on</strong>. Experimental Neurology, 13:163-175, 1965.Melzack, R., and Scott, T.H. The effects of early experience <strong>on</strong> theresp<strong>on</strong>se to pain. Journal of Comparative Physiology and Psychology,50:155-161, 1957.Melzack, R., and Thomps<strong>on</strong>, W.R. Effects of early experience <strong>on</strong>social behavior. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 10:82-90, 1956.Mendels<strong>on</strong>, W.; Johns<strong>on</strong>, N.; and Stewart, M. Hyperactive childrenas teen-agers: A follow-up study. Journal of Nervous and MentalDiseases, 153:273-279, 1971.Menninger, K . Man Against Himself. New York: Harcourt, Brace &World, 1938.Mert<strong>on</strong>, R.K. Social structure and anomie. American Sociological Review,3:672-682, Oct. 1938.Mert<strong>on</strong>, R.K. Social Theory and Social Structure. New York: FreePress, 1957.Metzner, R.; Litwin, G.; and Weil, G.M. The relati<strong>on</strong> of expectati<strong>on</strong>and mood to psilocybin reacti<strong>on</strong>s: A questi<strong>on</strong>naire study. PsychedelicReview, 5:3-39, 1965.Meunier, J.E., and Zajah, J.M. Cerebellar opiate receptors in lagomorphs.Dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong>, characterizati<strong>on</strong> and regi<strong>on</strong>al distributi<strong>on</strong>.Brain Research, 168:331-321, 1979.Meyers<strong>on</strong>, B.J., and Terenius, L. Endorphin and male sexual behavior.European Journal of Pharmacology, 42:191-192, 1977.Milich, R., and L<strong>on</strong>ey, J. The role of hyperactive and aggressivesymptomatology in predicting adolescent outcome am<strong>on</strong>g hyperactivechildren. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 4:93-112, 1979.Milkman, H. Addictive processes: An introductory formulati<strong>on</strong>. StreetPharmacologist, 2(4):1-5, 1979.459


Milkman, H., and Frosch, W.A. <strong>On</strong> the preferential abuse of heroinand amphetamine. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 156:242-248, 1973.Milkman, H., and Metcalf, D. An innovative approach to methad<strong>on</strong>edetoxificati<strong>on</strong>. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of the Addicti<strong>on</strong>s, 15(6), inpress.Miller, D. Adolescence, Psychology, Psychopathology and Psychotherapy.New York: Ar<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>, 1974.Miller, N.E., and Dollard, J. Social Learning and Imitati<strong>on</strong>. NewHaven, C<strong>on</strong>n.: Yale University Press, 1941.Miller, W.B. Lower class culture as a generating milieu of gang delinquency.Journal of Social Issues, 14:5-19, 1958.Minde, K.; Lewin, D.; Weiss, G.; Lavigueur, H.; Douglas, V.; andSykes, E. The hyperactive child in elementary school: A fiveyear c<strong>on</strong>trolled follow-up. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 38:215-221,1971.Minuchin, S. Families and Family Therapy. Cambridge, Mass.: HarvardPress, 1974.Mirin, S.M.; Meyer, R.E.; McNamee, H.B.; and McDougle, H. Psychopathology,craving and mood during heroin acquisiti<strong>on</strong>: An experimentalstudy. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of the Addicti<strong>on</strong>s, 11:525-544,1976.Misra, R.K. Mental health: A cross-cultural point of view. In:Berry, J.W., and L<strong>on</strong>ner, W.J., eds. Applied Cross-CulturalPsychology. The Netherlands: Swets and Zeitlinger, 1975. pp.155-160.Misra, R.K. <strong>Drug</strong> addicti<strong>on</strong>: Problems and prospects. <strong>Drug</strong> Forum,5(3).:283-288, 1976.Mitchell, G. What m<strong>on</strong>keys can tell us about human violence. TheFuturist, 9(2):75-80, 1975.Mitchell, G.D. Persistent behavior pathology in rhesus m<strong>on</strong>keysfollowing early social isolati<strong>on</strong>. Folia Primatology, 8:132-147,1968.Mitchell, G.D. Abnormal behavior in primates. In: Rosenblum, L.A.,ed. Primate Behavior: Developments in Field and LaboratoryResearch. New York: Academic Press, 1970. pp. 195-249.Mitchell, G.D., and Clark, D.L. L<strong>on</strong>g-term effects of social isolati<strong>on</strong>in n<strong>on</strong>socially adapted rhesus m<strong>on</strong>keys. The Journal of GeneticPsychology, 113:117-128, 1968.Mogar, R.E., and Savage, C. Pers<strong>on</strong>ality change associated withpsychedelic (LSD) therapy: A preliminary report. Psychotherapy:Theory, Research and Practice, 1:154-162, 1954.460


M<strong>on</strong>ey, J., and Wolff, G. Late puberty, retarded growth and reversiblehyposomatotropinism (psychosocial dwarfism). Adolescence,9:121-134, 1974.M<strong>on</strong>ey, J.; Wolff, G.; and Annecillo, C. Pain agnosia and self-injuryin the syndrome of reversible somatotropin deficiency (psychosocialdwarfism). Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia,2:127-139, 1972.M<strong>on</strong>roe, J.J.; Ross, W.F.; and Berzins, J.I. The dec<strong>line</strong> of theaddict as “psychopath”: Implicati<strong>on</strong>s for community care. Internati<strong>on</strong>alJournal of the Addicti<strong>on</strong>s, 6:601-608, 1971.Morris, R.W., and Lutsch, E.F. Daily susceptibility rhythm to morphineanalgesia. Journal of Pharmaceutical Sciences, 58:374-376, 1969.Morris<strong>on</strong>, J. R., and Stewart, M.A. A family study of the hyperactivechild syndrome. Biological Psychiatry, 3:189-195, 1971.Muller, O. Circadian rhythmicity in resp<strong>on</strong>se to barbiturates. In:Scheving, L.E.; Halberg, F.; and Pauly, J.E., eds. Chr<strong>on</strong>obiology.Tokyo: lgaku Shoin, Ltd., 1974.Musto, D. The American Disease, the Origins of Narcotic C<strong>on</strong>trol.New Haven, C<strong>on</strong>n.: Yale University Press, 1973.Nail, R.L.; Gunders<strong>on</strong>, E.K.; and Kolb, D. Family characteristicsassociated with heroin dependence am<strong>on</strong>g Navy men in Viet Nam.Military Medicine, 139:967-970, 1974.Nair, V. Circadian rhythm in drug acti<strong>on</strong>: A pharmacological, biochemical,and electromicroscopic study. In: Scheving, L.E.;Halberg, F.; and Pauly, J.E., eds. Chr<strong>on</strong>obiology. Tokyo:lgaku Shoin, Ltd., 1974.Nathan, P.E., and Lansky, D. Comm<strong>on</strong> methodological problems inresearch <strong>on</strong> the addicti<strong>on</strong>s. Journal of C<strong>on</strong>sulting and ClinicalPsychology, 46:713-726, 1978.Nati<strong>on</strong>al Commissi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> Marihuana and <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>. <strong>Drug</strong> Use inAmerica: Problem in Perspective. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.: Superintendentof Documents, U.S. Government Printing Office, 1973.Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>. <strong>Drug</strong> Use and Crime: Report of thePanel <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> Use and Criminal Behavior. Springfield, Va.:Nati<strong>on</strong>al Technical Informati<strong>on</strong> Service, 1976.Nels<strong>on</strong>, W., and Halberg, F. An evaluati<strong>on</strong> of time-dependent changesin susceptibility of mice to pentabarbital injecti<strong>on</strong>. Neuropharmacology,12:509:524, 1973.Nesbitt, M. Psychosis due to exogenous pois<strong>on</strong>s. Illinois MedicalJournal, 77:278-281, 1940.Nesbitt, P.D. Chr<strong>on</strong>ic smoking and emoti<strong>on</strong>ality. Journal of AppliedSocial Psychology, 2:187-196, 1972.461


Neumann, E. The Great Mother. An Analysis of the Archetype.Manhein, R., trans. Bollingen Series XLVII (2nd ed.). Princet<strong>on</strong>,N.J.: Princet<strong>on</strong> University Press, 1972.Newcomb, T.M. The Acquaintance Process. New York: Holt, Rinehartand Winst<strong>on</strong>, 1961New York City Mayor’s Committee <strong>on</strong> Marihuana. The Marihuana Problemin New York City. Lancaster, Pa.: Jacques Cattell Press, 1944.p. 13.Newmeyer, J.A., and Johns<strong>on</strong>, G.L. <strong>Drug</strong> emergencies in crowds: Ananalysis of “rock medicine,” 1973-1977. Journal of <strong>Drug</strong> Issues,9:235-245, 1979.Nichols, J.R. A procedure which produces sustained opiate-directedbehavior in the rat. Psychological Reports, 13(3):895-904, 1963.Nichols, J.R. How opiates change behavior. Scientific American,212(2):80-88, 1965.Nichols, J.R. The children of addicts: What do they inherit? Annalsof the New York Academy of Sciences, 197:60-65, 1972.Nichols, J.R., and Hsiao, S. Addicti<strong>on</strong> liability of albino rats: Breedingfor quantitative differences in morphine drinking. Science,157:561-563, 1967.Nowlis, V., and Nowlis, H.H. The descripti<strong>on</strong> and analysis of mood.Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 65:345-355, 1956.Nurco, D.N. Etiological aspects of drug abuse. In: DuP<strong>on</strong>t, R.L.;Goldstein, A.; and O’D<strong>on</strong>nell, J., eds. Handbook <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>.Rockville, Md.: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>, 1979. pp.315-324.Nurco, D.N., and Lerner, M. Occupati<strong>on</strong>al skills and life-styles ofnarcotic addicts. In: Winick, C., ed. Sociological Aspects of<strong>Drug</strong> Dependence. Cleveland: CRC Press, 1974. pp. 253-279.Obitz, F.W.; Cooper, K.; and Madeiros, D.C. General and specificperceived locus of c<strong>on</strong>trol in heroin addicts. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journalof the Addicti<strong>on</strong>s, 9(5):757-760, 1974.Obitz, F.W., and Swans<strong>on</strong>, M.K. C<strong>on</strong>trol orientati<strong>on</strong> in women alcoholics.Journal of <strong>Studies</strong> <strong>on</strong> Alcohol, 37:694-697, 1976.O’Brien, C. Pers<strong>on</strong>al communicati<strong>on</strong>, 1978.O’D<strong>on</strong>nell, J., and Ball, J. Narcotic Addicti<strong>on</strong>. New York: Harperand Row, 1966.O’D<strong>on</strong>nell, J.A., and Clayt<strong>on</strong>, R.C. “The Stepping-St<strong>on</strong>e Hypothesis--Marihuana, Heroin and Causality.” Paper presented at the Meetingsof the American Sociological Associati<strong>on</strong>, San Francisco,1978.462


O’D<strong>on</strong>nell, J.A., and Clayt<strong>on</strong>, R.C. Determinants of early marihuanause. In: Beschner, G.M., and Friedman, A.S., eds. Youth<strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>. Lexingt<strong>on</strong>, Mass.: Lexingt<strong>on</strong> Books, 1979. pp.63-104.O’D<strong>on</strong>nell, J.A.; Voss, H.L.; Clayt<strong>on</strong>, R.R.; Slatin, G.T.; and Room,R.G.W. Young Men and <strong>Drug</strong>s: A Nati<strong>on</strong>wide Study. ResearchM<strong>on</strong>ograph Series, Vol. 5. Rockville, Md.: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong><strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>, 1976.Olds, J., and Milner, P. Positive reinforcement produced by electricalstimulati<strong>on</strong> of septal areas and other. regi<strong>on</strong>s of rat brain. Journalof Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 47:419-427, 1954.Ols<strong>on</strong>, D. H.; Sprenkle, D.H.; and Russell, C.S. Circumplex model ofmarital and family systems: Cohesi<strong>on</strong> and adaptability dimensi<strong>on</strong>s,family types, and clinical applicati<strong>on</strong>s. Family Process, 18:3-28,1979.Olweus, D. Aggressi<strong>on</strong> in the Schools. New York: Wiley, 1978.Omenn, G.S. Alcoholism. In: Mendlewicz, J., ed. Recent Developmentsin Genetics and Psychopharmacology Modern Problems PharmacopsychologyBasel, Switz.: Karger, 1975.Orcutt, J.D. Normative definiti<strong>on</strong>s of intoxicated states: A test ofseveral sociological theories. Social Problems, 25(4):385-396,1978.Orford, J.; Oppenheimer, E.; and Edwards, G. Abstinence or c<strong>on</strong>trol:The outcome for excessive drinkers two years after c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong>.Behavior Research and Therapy, 14:409-418, 1976.Orr, W.C. Biological rhythms and drug addicti<strong>on</strong>. Alcohol TechnicalReports--Oklahoma City, 5:15-18, 1976.Oziel, L.J., and Obitz, F.W. C<strong>on</strong>trol orientati<strong>on</strong> in alcoholics relatedto extent of treatment. Journal of <strong>Studies</strong> <strong>on</strong> Alcohol, 36:158-161,1975.Oziel, L.J.; Obitz, F.W.; and Keys<strong>on</strong>, M. General and specific perceivedlocus of c<strong>on</strong>trol in alcoholics. Psychological Reports,30:957-958, 1972.Panksepp, J.; Herman, B.; C<strong>on</strong>ner, R.; Bishop, P.; and Scott, J.P.The biology of social attachments: Opiates alleviate separati<strong>on</strong>distress. Biological Psychiatry, 13:607-618, 1978.Parker, L.F., and Radow, B.L. Morphine-like physical dependence: Apharmacologic method for drug assessment using the rat. PharmacologyBiochemistry and Behavior, 2:613-618, 1974.Partanen, J.; Bruun, K; and Markkanen, T. Inheritance of DrinkingBehavior. New Brunswick, N.J.: Rutgers University Center ofAlcohol <strong>Studies</strong>, 1966.Pasternak, G.W.; Goodman, R.; and Snyder, S.H. An endogenousmorphine-like factor in mammalian brain. Life Sciences, 16:1765-1769, 1975.463


Patters<strong>on</strong>, G. The aggressive child: Victim and architect of a coercivesystem. In: Mash, E.J.; Hamerlynck, L.A.; and Handy, L.C.,eds. Behavior Modificati<strong>on</strong> and Families. New York: Bruner/Mazel,1976.Pattis<strong>on</strong>, E.M. The Experience of Dying. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.:Prentice-Hall, 1977.Patt<strong>on</strong>, R.G., and Gardner, L.I. Growth Failure in Maternal Deprivati<strong>on</strong>Springfield, Ill.: Charles C Thomas, 1963Patt<strong>on</strong>, R.G., and Gardner, L.I. Deprivati<strong>on</strong> dwarfism (psychosocialdeprivati<strong>on</strong>): Disordered family envir<strong>on</strong>ment as cause of so-calledidiopathic hypopituitarism. In: Gardner, L.I., ed. Endocrine andGenetic Diseases of Childhood and Adolescence. 2nd ed. Philadelphia:W.B. Saunders, 1975.Pauly, J., and Scheving, L. Temporal variati<strong>on</strong>s in the susceptibilityof white rats to phenobarbital sodial and Tremorine. Internati<strong>on</strong>alJournal of Neuropharmacology, 3:651-658, 1964.Peele, S. Redefining addicti<strong>on</strong>: I. Making addicti<strong>on</strong> a scientificallyand socially useful c<strong>on</strong>cept. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of Health Services,7:103-124, 1977.Peets, J.M., and Pomeranz, B. CXBK mice deficient in opiate receptorsshow poor electroacupuncture analgesia. Nature, 273:675-676,1978.Pert, C.B.; Aposhian, D.; and Snyder, S.H. Phylogenetic distributi<strong>on</strong>of opiate receptor binding. Brain Research, 75:356-361, 1974.Pert, C.B.; Kuhar, M.J.; and Snyder, S.H. Autoradiographic localizati<strong>on</strong>of the opiate receptor in rat brain. Life Sciences, 16:1849-1854, 1975.Pert, C.B., and Snyder, S.H. Opiate receptor: Dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> innervous tissue. Science, 179:1011-1014, 1973.Perzel, J.F., and Lam<strong>on</strong>, S. “Enmeshment Within Families of Poly-<strong>Drug</strong><strong>Abuse</strong>rs.” Paper presented at the Nati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong> C<strong>on</strong>ference,New Orleans, Aug. 26-31, 1979.Pescor, M.J. The Kolb classificati<strong>on</strong> of drug addicts. Public HealthReports, Supplement No. 155, 1939.Pescor, M.J. Physician drug addicts. Diseases of the Nervous System,3:2-3, 1942.Pescor, M.J. A statistical analysis of the clinical records of hospitalizeddrug addicts. Public Health Reports, Supplement 143, 1943a.Pescor, M.J. Follow-up study of treated drug addicts. Public HealthReports, Supplement No. 170, 1943b.Petren, T., and Sollberger, A. Developmental rhythms. In: V<strong>on</strong>Mayersbach, H., ed. The Cellular Aspects of Biorhythms.Berlin: Springer-Verlag, 1967.464


Petrie, A.S. Individuality in Pain and Suffering. Chicago: Universityof Chicago Press, 1976; revised ed. 1978.Philipp, M., and Marneros, A. Chr<strong>on</strong>obiology and its implicati<strong>on</strong>s forpharmacotherapy of endogenous depressi<strong>on</strong>. Pharmakopsychiatrie-Neuropsychopharmakologie, 11:235-240, 1978.Phillips, D.C., and Kelly, M.E. Hierarchical theories of developmentin educati<strong>on</strong> and psychology. Harvard Educati<strong>on</strong>al Review, 45:351-375, 1975.Piaget, J. The C<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong> of Reality in the Child. New York:Basic Books, 1954.Pickens, R.; Meisch, R.; and McGuire, L.E. Methamphetamine reinforcementin rats. Psych<strong>on</strong>omic Science, 8:371-372, 1967.Pittel, S.M.; Calef, V.; Gryler, R.; Hilles, L.; Hofer, R.; and Kempner,P. Developmental factors in adolescent drug use. A study ofpsychedelic drug users. Journal of the American Academy of ChildPsychiatry, 10:640-660, 1971.Platt, J.J. , and Labate, C. Heroin Addicti<strong>on</strong>: Theory, Research andTreatment. New York: Wiley, 1976.Plumb, M.M.; D’Amanda, C.; and Taintor, Z. Chemical substanceabuse and perceived locus of c<strong>on</strong>trol. In: Lettieri, D.J., ed.Predicting Adolescent <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>: A Review of Issues, Methodsand Correlates. Rockville, Md.: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong><strong>Abuse</strong>, 1975.Pomeranz, B., and Chiu, D. Nalox<strong>on</strong>e blockade of acupuncture:Endorphin implicated. Life Sciences, 19:1757-1762, 1976.Poplar, J.F. Characteristics of nurse addicts. American Journal ofNursing, 69:117-119, 1969.Powell, D. H. A pilot study of occasi<strong>on</strong>al heroin users. Archives ofGeneral Psychiatry, 28:586-594, 1973.Powell, G.F.; Brasel, J.A.; and Blizzard, R. Emoti<strong>on</strong>al deprivati<strong>on</strong>and growth retardati<strong>on</strong> simulating idiopathic hypopituitarism. I.Clinical evaluati<strong>on</strong> of the syndrome. New England Journal of Medicine,276:1271-1278, 1967a.Powell, G.F.; Brasel, J.A.; Raiti, S.; and Blizzard, R.M. Emoti<strong>on</strong>aldeprivati<strong>on</strong> and growth retardati<strong>on</strong> simulating idiopathic hypopituitarism.II. Endocrinologic evaluati<strong>on</strong> of the syndrome. NewEngland Journal of Medicine, 276:1279-1283, 1967b.Pradelles, P.; Cros, C.; Humbert, F.; Dray, F.; and Ben-Ari, Y.Visual deprivati<strong>on</strong> decreases Met-enkephalin and substance Pc<strong>on</strong>tent of various forebrain structures. Brain Research, 166:191-193, 1979.Preble, E., and Casey, J.J. Taking care of business--the heroinuser’s life <strong>on</strong> the street. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of the Addicti<strong>on</strong>s,4:1-24, 1969.465


Preble, E., and Miller, T. Methad<strong>on</strong>e, wine, and welfare. In: Weppner,R.S., ed. Street Ethnography. Beverly Hills: Sage, 1977. pp.229-248.Prescott, J.W. Invited commentary <strong>on</strong> “Central nervous system functi<strong>on</strong>ingin altered sensory envir<strong>on</strong>ments,” by S.I. Cohen. In:Appley, M.H.; and Trumbull, R., eds. Psychological Stress.New York: Applet<strong>on</strong>-Century-Crofts, 1967. pp. 113-118.Prescott, J.W. Early somatosensory deprivati<strong>on</strong> as an <strong>on</strong>togeneticprocess in the abnormal development of the brain and behavior.In: Goldsmith, I.E., and Moor-Jankowski, J., eds. Medical Primatology1970. Basel: Karger, 1971a.Prescott, J.W. Sensory deprivati<strong>on</strong> vs. sensory stimulati<strong>on</strong> duringearly development: A comment <strong>on</strong> Berkowitz’s study. The Journalof Psychology, 77:189-191, 1971b.Prescott, J.W. Before ethics and morality. The Humanist, Nov./Dec.:19-21, 1972a.Prescott, J.W. Cann<strong>on</strong>’s Law of Denervati<strong>on</strong> Supersensitivity: Implicati<strong>on</strong>sfor psychophysiological assessment. Psychophysiology,9:279, 1972b.Prescott, J.W. Sexual behavior in the blind. In: Gillman, A.E., andGord<strong>on</strong>, A.R., eds. Medical Aspects of Human Sexuality, June1973. pp. 59-60.Prescott, J.W. Body pleasure and the origins of violence. The Futurist,9(2):64-74, 1975.Prescott, J.W. Somatosensory deprivati<strong>on</strong> and its relati<strong>on</strong>ship to theblind. In: Jastrzembska, Z.S., ed. The Effects of Blindness andOther Impairments <strong>on</strong> Early Development. New York: AmericanFoundati<strong>on</strong> for the Blind, 1976aPrescott, J.W. Violence, pleasure and religi<strong>on</strong>. The Bulletin of theAtomic Scientists, 31(9), 1976b.Prescott, J.W. Phylogenetic and <strong>on</strong>togenetic aspects of human affecti<strong>on</strong>aldevelopment. In: Gemme, R., and Wheeler, C.C., eds. SelectedProceedings of the 1976 Internati<strong>on</strong>al C<strong>on</strong>gress of Sexology. NewYork: Plenum Press, 1977.Prescott, J.W. “Why Men Dehumanize Women: Patriarchy, Dualism andM<strong>on</strong>otheism Revisited.” Paper presented at the Western PsychologicalAssociati<strong>on</strong> Annual Meeting, San Francisco, April 21, 1978.Prescott, J.W. Alienati<strong>on</strong> of affecti<strong>on</strong>. Psychology Today, Dec.1979a. p. 124.Prescott, J.W. Deprivati<strong>on</strong> of physical affecti<strong>on</strong> as a primary processin the development of physical violence: A comparative and crossculturalperspective. In: Gil, D.G., ed. Child <strong>Abuse</strong> and Violence.New York: American Orthopsychiatric Associati<strong>on</strong>, AMSPress, 1979b. pp. 66-137.466


Prescott, J.W., and Wallace, D. “Developmental Sociobiology and theOrigins of Aggressive Behavior.” Paper presented at the 21stInternati<strong>on</strong>al C<strong>on</strong>gress of Psychology, Paris, July 1976.Prescott, J.W., and Wallace, D. Role of pain and pleasure in thedevelopment of destructive behaviors: A psychometric study ofparenting, sexuality, substance abuse and criminality. In: Centerfor the Study of the Correlates of Crime and the Determinants ofCriminal Behavior, Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute of Law Enforcement andCriminal Justice. Colloquium <strong>on</strong> the Correlates of Crime and theDeterminants of Criminal Behavior. Otten, L., ed. Washingt<strong>on</strong>,D.C.: Law Enforcement Assistance Administrati<strong>on</strong>, 1978.Quay, H. Classificati<strong>on</strong>. In: Quay, H.C., and Werry, J.S., eds.Psychopathological Disorders of Childhood. New York: Wiley,1979.Rado, S. The psychic effects of intoxicants. The Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journalof Psychoanalysis, 7:396-413, 1926.Rado, S. The psychoanalysis of pharmacothymia (drug addicti<strong>on</strong>).Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 2:1-23, 1933.Radzialowski, F.M., and Bosquet, W. Daily rhythmic variati<strong>on</strong> inhepatic drug metabolism in the rat and mouse. Journal of Pharmacologyand Experimental Therapeutics, 163:229-238, 1968.Rapoport, J.L.; Buchsbaum, M.S.; Zahn, T.P.; Weingartner, H.;Ludlow, C.; and Mikkelsen, E.J. Dextroamphetamine: Cognitiveand behavioral effects in normal prepubertal boys. Science,199:560-563, 1978.Ray, M.B. The cycle of abstinence and relapse am<strong>on</strong>g heroin addicts.Social Problems, 9:132-140, 1961.Reich, A. Pathologic forms of self-esteem regulati<strong>on</strong>. PsychoanalyticStudy of the Child, 15:215-232, 1960.Reich, W. The Mass Psychology of Fascism. New York: Farrar,Straus and Ciroux, 1973.Reilly, D.M. Family factors in the etiology and treatment of youthfuldrug abuse. Family Therapy, 2:149-171, 1976.Reinberg, A. Chr<strong>on</strong>opharmacology. In: Mills, J.N., ed. BiologicalAspects of Circadian Rhythms. New York: Plenum, 1973.Reinberg, A., and Halberg, F. Circadian chr<strong>on</strong>opharmacology.Annual Review of Pharmacology, 11:455-492, 1971.Research Center for Human Relati<strong>on</strong>s. “The Ecology of Juvenile <strong>Drug</strong>Use, 1949-1952.” Report No. I. New York: the Center, NewYork University, 1954a.Research Center for Human Relati<strong>on</strong>s. “Delinquency Trends.” ReportNo. I-A. New York: the Center, New York University, 1954b.Research Center for Human Relati<strong>on</strong>s. “Heroin Use and Street Gangs.”Report No. III. New York: the Center, New York University,1954c.467


Research Center for Human Relati<strong>on</strong>s. “The Family of the Addict.”Report No. V. New York: the Center, New York University,1956.Research Center for Human Relati<strong>on</strong>s. Pers<strong>on</strong>al Backgrounds of <strong>Drug</strong>Users, Delinquents, and C<strong>on</strong>trols. Report No. II. New York:New York University, 1957a.Research Center for Human Relati<strong>on</strong>s. “Post-Hospitalizati<strong>on</strong> Adjustmentof Addicts Treated at Riverside Hospital.” Report No. VI. NewYork: the Center, New York University, 1957b.Reynolds, T.D.; L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>, W.P.; and Yorke, J.A. Behavioral rhythmsin schizophrenia. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 166(7):489-499, 1978.Richard, C.M. Physiological Zoology, 31:138, 1958.Riesen, A.H. Effects of stimulus deprivati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the development andatrophy of the visual sensory system. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,30:23-36, 1960.Riesen, A.H. Excessive arousal effects of stimulati<strong>on</strong> after earlysensory deprivati<strong>on</strong>. In: Solom<strong>on</strong>, P.; Kubzansky, P.E.; Leiderman.P.H.: Mendels<strong>on</strong>, J.H.; Trumbull, R.; and Wexler, D.,eds. Sensory Deprivati<strong>on</strong>. Cambridge; Mass.: Harvard UniversityPress, 1961a. pp. 34-40.Riesen, A.H. Stimulati<strong>on</strong> as a requirement for growth and functi<strong>on</strong>.In: Fiske, D.W., and Maddi, S.R. Functi<strong>on</strong>s of Varied Experience.Homewood, Ill.: Dorsey Press, 1961 b.Riesen, A.H. Effects of visual deprivati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> perceptual functi<strong>on</strong> andthe neural substrate. In: de Ajuriaguerra, J., ed. Desafferentati<strong>on</strong>Experimentale et Clinique Symposium, Bel Air, Geneva, 1964.Geneva, Switz.: Georg, 1965. pp. 47-66.Riesen, A.H., ed. The Developmental Neuropsychology of SensoryDeprivati<strong>on</strong>. New York: Academic Press, 1975.Riverside Hospital. “Annual Report 2.” New York: the Hospital,1954.Robbins, E.S.; Frosch, W.A.; and Stern, M. Further observati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong>untoward reacti<strong>on</strong>s to LSD. American Journal of Psychiatry,124:393-395, 1967.Robins, L.N. Deviant Children Grown Up: A Sociological and PsychiatricStudy of Sociopathic Pers<strong>on</strong>ality. Baltimore: Williams &Wilkins, 1966. Reprint ed., Huntingt<strong>on</strong>, N.Y.: Robert E. KriegerPublishing Co., Inc., 1974.Robins, L.N. A Follow-Up of Vietnam <strong>Drug</strong> Users. Washingt<strong>on</strong>,D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office, 1973.Robins, L.N. “Comment at Third Vanderbilt Sociology C<strong>on</strong>ference,Vanderbilt University,” 1974a.468


Robins, L.N. The Vietnam <strong>Drug</strong> User Returns. Special Acti<strong>on</strong> OfficeM<strong>on</strong>ograph, Series A. No. 2. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.: Superintendentof Documents, U.S. Government Printing Office, May 1974b.Robins, L.N. Alcoholism and labelling theory. In: Gove, W.R., ed.The Labelling of Deviance. New York: Wiley, 1975a. pp. 21-33.Robins, L.N. “Discriminating the Roles of Three Kinds of Predictorsof Rare Life History Outcomes: Demographic Variables, FamilyHistory and Pers<strong>on</strong>al History.” Paper presented at the Societyfor Life History Research in Psychopathology, Rochester, N.Y.,May 1975b.Robins, L.N. Estimating addicti<strong>on</strong> rates and locating target populati<strong>on</strong>s:How decompositi<strong>on</strong> into States helps. In: Rittenhouse, J., ed.The Epidemiology of Heroin and Other Narcotics. Research M<strong>on</strong>ographSeries 16. Rockville, Md.: Nati<strong>on</strong>al lnstitute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong><strong>Abuse</strong>, 1976. pp. 25-39.Robins, L.N. Sturdy childhood predictors of adult antisocial behaviour:Replicati<strong>on</strong> from l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal studies. Psychological Medicine,8:611-622, 1978.Robins, L.N. Addict careers. In: DuP<strong>on</strong>t, R.L.; Goldstein, A.; andO’D<strong>on</strong>nell, J., eds. Handbook <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>. Rockville, Md.:Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>, 1979. pp. 325-336.Robins, L.N.; Darvish, H.S.; and Murphy, G.E.A. Follow-up studyof 76 users and 146 n<strong>on</strong>users. In: Zubin, J., and Freedman,A., eds. Psychopathology of Adolescence. New York: Grune &Stratt<strong>on</strong>, 1970. pp. 159-178.Robins, L.N.; Davis, D. H.; and Goodwin, D.W. <strong>Drug</strong> use by U.S.Army enlisted men in Vietnam: A follow-up <strong>on</strong> their return home.American Journal of Epidemiology 99:235-249, 1974a.Robins, L.N.; Davis, D.H.; and Nurco, D.N. How permanent wasViet Nam drug addicti<strong>on</strong>. American Journal of Public Health(Supplement), 64:38-43, 1974b.Robins, L.N., and Helzer, J.E. <strong>Drug</strong> use am<strong>on</strong>g Vietnam veterans--Three years later. Medical World News, Oct. 1975. pp. 44-45,49.Robins, L.N.; Helzer, J.E.; and Davis, D.H. Narcotic use in southeastAsia and afterward. Archives of General Psychiatry, 2:955-961,1975.Robins, L.N.; Helzer, J.E.; Hesselbrock, M.; and Wish, E. Vietnamveterans three years after Vietnam: How our study changed ourview of heroin.. In: Harris, L., ed. Problems of <strong>Drug</strong> Dependence.Proceedings of the Committee <strong>on</strong> Problems of <strong>Drug</strong> Dependence.Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Academy of Sciences, 1977. Also toappear in: Brill, L., and Winick, C., eds. Yearbook of Substance<strong>Abuse</strong>. New York: Human Sciences Press, in press.Robins, L.N., and Murphy, G.E. <strong>Drug</strong> use in a normal populati<strong>on</strong> ofyoung Negro men. American Journal of Public Health, 57(9):1580-1596, 1967.469


Rogers, C. <strong>On</strong> Becoming a Pers<strong>on</strong>. Bost<strong>on</strong>: Hought<strong>on</strong> Mifflin, 1962.Rohsenow, D.J., and O’Leary, M.R. Locus of c<strong>on</strong>trol research <strong>on</strong>alcoholic populati<strong>on</strong>s: A review. I. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of theAddicti<strong>on</strong>s, 13(1):55-68, 1978a.Rohsenow, D.J., and O’Leary, M.R. Locus of c<strong>on</strong>trol research <strong>on</strong>alcoholic populati<strong>on</strong>s: A review. II. Relati<strong>on</strong>ship to other measures.Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of the Addicti<strong>on</strong>s, 13(2):213-226, 1978b.Roitzen, R.; Cahalan, D.; and Shanks, P. “Sp<strong>on</strong>taneous remissi<strong>on</strong>”am<strong>on</strong>g untreated problem drinkers. In: Kandel, D.B., ed.L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal Research <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> Use. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.: Hemisphere,1978. pp. 197-221.Rosenthal, H.S. Persistent hallucinosis following repeated administrati<strong>on</strong>of hallucinogenic drugs. American Journal of Psychiatry, 121:238-244, 1964.Rosenzweig, M.R. Resp<strong>on</strong>siveness of brain size to individual experience:Behavioral and evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary implicati<strong>on</strong>s. In: Hahn, M.E.;Rensen, C.; and Dudek, B.C., eds. Development and Evoluti<strong>on</strong>of Brain Size. New York: Academic Press, 1979.Rosenzweig, M.R.; Krech, D.; Bennett, E.L.; and Diam<strong>on</strong>d, M.C.Modifying brain chemistry and anatomy by enrichment or impoverishmentof experience. In: Newt<strong>on</strong>, G., and Levine, S., eds.Early Experience and Behavior. Springfield, Ill.: Charles CThomas, 1968.Ross, D.M., and Ross, S.A. Hyperactivity. New York: Wiley, 1976.Rossier, J.; Vargo, T.M.; Minick, S.; Ling, N.; Bloom, F.E.; andGuillemin, R. Regi<strong>on</strong>al dissociati<strong>on</strong> of beta-endorphin and enkephalinc<strong>on</strong>tents in rat brain and pituitary. Proceedings of the Nati<strong>on</strong>alAcademy of Sciences, USA, 74:5162-5165, 1977.Roszak, T. Unfinished Animal. New York: Harper & Row, 1975.Roth, J. Timetables. Indianapolis, Ind.: Bobbs-Merrill, 1963.Roth, L.M., and Howland, R.B. Annals of the Entomological Society ofAmerica, 34:151, 1958.Rotter, J.B. Generalized expectancies for internal versus externalc<strong>on</strong>trol of reinforcement. Psychological M<strong>on</strong>ographs, 80 (1), wholeNo. 609, 1966.Rubingt<strong>on</strong>, E., and Weinberg, M.S. Deviance: The lnteracti<strong>on</strong>ist Perspective.2nd ed. New York: MacMillan, 1973.Sackett, G.P. Unlearned resp<strong>on</strong>ses, differential rearing experiences,and the development of social attachments by rhesus m<strong>on</strong>keys.In: Rosenblum; L.A., ed. Primate Behavior: Developments in Fieldand Laboratory Research. New York: Academic Press, 1970. pp.111-140.Sadava, S.W. Research approaches in illicit drug use: A criticalreview. Genetic Psychology M<strong>on</strong>ograph, 91:3-59, 1975.470


Sadava, S.W., and Forsyth, R. Pers<strong>on</strong>-envir<strong>on</strong>ment interacti<strong>on</strong> andcollege student drug use: A multivariate l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal study.Genetic Psychology M<strong>on</strong>ograph, 96:211-245, 1977.Sadava, S.W.; Thistle, R.; and Forsyth, R. Stress, escapism andpatterns of alcohol and drug use. Journal of <strong>Studies</strong> <strong>on</strong> Alcohol,39(5):725-736, 1978.Saltzberg, B. A model for relating ripples in the EEG power spectraldensity to transient patterns of brain electrical activity inducedby subcortical spiking. IEEE Transacti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> Bio-Medical Engineering,23(4):355-356, 1976.Saltzberg, B., and Lustick, L.S. Signal analysis: An overview ofEEG applicati<strong>on</strong>s. In: Prescott, J.W.; Read, M.S.; and Coursin,D.B., eds. Brain Functi<strong>on</strong> and Malnutriti<strong>on</strong>: NeuropsychologicalMethods of Assessment. New York: Wiley, 1975.Saltzberg, B.; Lustick, L.S.; and Heath, R.G. Detecti<strong>on</strong> of focaldepth spiking in the scalp EEG of m<strong>on</strong>keys. Electroencephalographyand Clinical Neurophysiology, 31:327-333, 1971.Sandberg, S.T.; Rutter, M.; and Taylor, E. Hyperkinetic disorder inpsychiatric clinic attenders. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology,20:279-299, 1978.Schachter, S., and Singer, J.E. Cognitive, social and physiologicaldeterminants of emoti<strong>on</strong>al state. Psychological Review, 69:379-399,1962.Schaffer, H.R., and Emers<strong>on</strong>, P.E. Patterns of resp<strong>on</strong>se to physicalc<strong>on</strong>tact in early human development. Journal of Child Psychologyand Psychiatry and Allied Discip<strong>line</strong>s, 5:1-13, 1964a.Schaffer, H.R., and Emers<strong>on</strong>, P.E. The development of social attachmentin infancy. M<strong>on</strong>ographs of the Society for Research in ChildDevelopment, 3:1-77, 1964b.Schasre, R. Cessati<strong>on</strong> patterns am<strong>on</strong>g neophyte heroin users. Internati<strong>on</strong>alJournal of the Addicti<strong>on</strong>s, 1:23-32, 1966.Scheving, L.E. Circadian variati<strong>on</strong> in susceptibility of the rat tod-amphetamine sulfate. Anatomical Record, 160:422, 1969.Schuckit, M.A. Alcoholism and sociopathy--Diagnostic c<strong>on</strong>fusi<strong>on</strong>.Quarterly Journal <strong>Studies</strong> <strong>on</strong> Alcohol, 34:157-164, 1973.Schuckit, M.A. Geriatric alcoholism and drug abuse. The Ger<strong>on</strong>tologist,17:168-174, 1977.Schuckit, M.A. <strong>Drug</strong> and Alcohol <strong>Abuse</strong>: A Clinical Guide to Diagnosisand Treatment. New York: Plenum, 1979a.Schuckit, M.A. Alcoholism and affective disorder: Diagnostic c<strong>on</strong>fusi<strong>on</strong>.In: Goodwin, D.W., ed. Alcoholism and Depressi<strong>on</strong>. New York:Spectrum, 1979b.471


Schuckit, M.A. Ethanol intoxicati<strong>on</strong>: Differences between young menwith alcoholic relatives and c<strong>on</strong>trols. Submitted to Journal of<strong>Studies</strong> <strong>on</strong> Alcohol, 1979c.Schuckit, M.A.; Goodwin, D.A.; and Winokur, G. A study of alcoholismin half siblings. American Journal of Psychiatry, 128:1132-1136,1972.Schuckit, M.A., and Haglund, R.M.J. An overview of the etiologictheories <strong>on</strong> alcoholism. In: Estes, N., and Heinemann, E., eds.Alcoholism: Development, C<strong>on</strong>sequences and Interventi<strong>on</strong>s. St.Louis, MO.: Mosby, 1977.Schuckit, M.A.; Petrich, J.; and Chiles, J. Hyperactivity: Diagnosticc<strong>on</strong>fusi<strong>on</strong>. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 166:79-87,1978.Schuckit, M.A., and Rayses, V. Differences in acetaldehyde levels inrelatives of alcoholics and c<strong>on</strong>trols. Science, 203:54-55, 1979.Schuckit, M.A., and Winokur, G. A short term follow up of womenalcoholics. Diseases of the Nervous System, 33:672-678, 1972.Schuster, C.R., and Thomps<strong>on</strong>, T. Self administrati<strong>on</strong> of and behavioraldependence <strong>on</strong> drugs. Annual Review of Pharmacology,9:483-502, 1969.Schuster, C.R., and Thomps<strong>on</strong>, T. Self administrati<strong>on</strong> of and behavioraldependence <strong>on</strong> drugs. Annual Review of Pharmacology,9:483-502, 1969.Segal, B. Locus of c<strong>on</strong>trol and drug and alcohol use in college students.Journal of Alcohol and <strong>Drug</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong>, 19(3):1-5, 1974.Segal, B. Pers<strong>on</strong>ality factors related to drug and alcohol use. In:Lettieri, D.J., ed. Predicting Adolescent <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>: A Reviewof Issues, Methods, and Correlates. Rockville, Md.: Nati<strong>on</strong>alInstitute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>, 1975. pp. 165-191.Seldin, N.E. The family of the addict: A review of the literature.Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of the Addicti<strong>on</strong>s, 7:97-107, 1972.Seligman, M.E.P. Helplessness: <strong>On</strong> Depressi<strong>on</strong>, Development and Death.San Francisco: W.H. Freeman, 1975.Sells, S.B., and Simps<strong>on</strong>, D.D. The case for drug abuse treatmenteffectiveness based <strong>on</strong> the DARP research program. BritishJournal of Addicti<strong>on</strong>, in press.Selye, H. The Physiology and Pathology of Exposure to Stress.M<strong>on</strong>treal: M<strong>on</strong>treal Acta, 1950.Seto, A.; Tricomi, S.; Goodwin, D.W.; Kolodney, R.; and Sullivan,T. Biochemical correlates of ethanol induced flushing in Orientals.Journal of <strong>Studies</strong> <strong>on</strong> Alcohol, 39:1-11, 1978.Sharpless, S.K. Disuse supersensitivity. In: Riesen, A.H., ed.The Developmental Neuropsychology of Sensory Deprivati<strong>on</strong>. NewYork: Academic Press, 1975.472


Sheppard, C.W.; Smith, D.E.; and Gay, G.R. The changing face ofheroin addicti<strong>on</strong> in the Haight-Ashbury. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal ofthe Addicti<strong>on</strong>s, 7:109-122, 1972.Sherif, M., and Sherif, C. Reference Groups. Chicago: Regnery,1964.Sh<strong>on</strong>tz, F.C. Research Methods in Pers<strong>on</strong>ality. New York: Applet<strong>on</strong>-Century-Crofts, 1965.Sh<strong>on</strong>tz, F.C. Single organism designs. In: Bentler, P.M., andLettieri, D.J., eds. Data Analysis Strategies and Designs forSubstance <strong>Abuse</strong>. Research Issues Series 13. Rockville, Md.:Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>, 1976. pp. 25-44.Short, J.F. Gang delinquency and anomie. In: Clinard, M.B., ed.Anomie and Deviant Behavior. New York: Free Press, 1964.Short, J.F., Jr., and Strodtbeck, F.L. Group Process and GangDelinquency. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1965.Siegal, S. Evidence from rats that morphine tolerance is a learnedresp<strong>on</strong>se. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology,89:498-506, 1975.Siegel, A.J. The heroin crisis am<strong>on</strong>g U.S. Forces in Southeast Asia.Journal of the American Medical Associati<strong>on</strong>, 223:1258-1261, 1973.Sim<strong>on</strong>, E.J., and Hiller, J.M. The opiate receptors. Annual Reviewsof Pharmacology and Toxicology, 18:371-394, 1978.Sim<strong>on</strong>, E.J.; Hiller, J.M.; and Edelman, I. Stereospecific binding ofthe potent narcotic analgesic 3 H-etorphine to rat brain homogenate.Proceedings of the Nati<strong>on</strong>al Academy of Sciences, USA, 70:1947-1949, 1973.Sim<strong>on</strong>, E.J., and van Praag, D. <strong>Studies</strong> <strong>on</strong> the intracellular distributi<strong>on</strong>and tissue binding of dihydromorphine-7,8- 3 H in the rat.Proceedings of the Society for Experimental Biology and Medicine,122:6-11, 1966.Simantov, R., and Snyder, S.H. Elevated levels of enkephalin inmorphine-dependent rats. Nature 262:505-507, 1976.Sinclair, J.D.; Adkins, J.; and Walker, S. Morphine-induced suppressi<strong>on</strong>of voluntary alcohol drinking in rats. Nature, 246:425-427,1973.Singer, J.L. The Inner World of Daydreaming. New York: Harperand Row, 1975.Single, E., and Kandel, D. The role of buying and selling in illicitdrug use. In: Trebach, A., ed. <strong>Drug</strong>s, Crime, and Politics.New York: Praeger, 1978. pp. 118-128.Single, E.; Kandel, D.; and Faust, R. Patterns of multiple drug usein high school. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 15(4):344-357, 1974.473


Sinnett, E.R., and Morris, J.B. Temporal patterns of the use ofn<strong>on</strong>-prescribed drugs. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 45:1239-1245,1977.Sinnett, E.R.; Wampler, K.S.; and Harvey, W.M. C<strong>on</strong>sistency ofpatterns of drug use. Psychological Reports, 31:143-152, 1972.Slater, P.E. Prolegomena to a psychoanalytic theory of aging anddeath. In: Kastenbaum, R., and Aisenbert, R., eds. The Psychologyof Death. New York: Springer, 1964.Smart, R.G. Outcome studies of therapeutic community and halfwayhouse treatment for addicts. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of the Addicti<strong>on</strong>s,11:143-159, 1976a.Smart, R.G. Sp<strong>on</strong>taneous recovery in alcoholics: A review and analysisof the available research. <strong>Drug</strong> and Alcohol Dependence, 1:277-285,1976b.Smart, R.G. Perceived availability and the use of drugs. Bulletin <strong>on</strong>Narcotics, 29:59-63, 1977.Smart, R.G., and Fejer, D. <strong>Drug</strong> use am<strong>on</strong>g adolescents and theirparents: Closing the generati<strong>on</strong> gap in mood modificati<strong>on</strong>. Journalof Abnormal Psychology, 79:153-160, 1972.Smith, G.M. Correlates of Pers<strong>on</strong>ality and <strong>Drug</strong> Use-l. RAUS ClusterReview, No. 3. Rockville, Md.: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>,1977.Smith, G.M., and Fogg, C.P. Psychological antecedents of teenagedrug use. In: Simm<strong>on</strong>s, R.G., ed. Research in Community andMental Health: An Annual Compilati<strong>on</strong> of Research. Vol. 1.Greenwich, C<strong>on</strong>n.: Al Press, 1977. pp. 87-102.Smith, G.M., and Fogg, C.P. Psychological predictors of early use,late use, and n<strong>on</strong>use of marijuana am<strong>on</strong>g teenage students. In:Kandel, D.B., ed. L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal Research <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> Use: EmpiricalFindings and Methodological Issues. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.: Hemisphere,1978. pp. 101-113.Smith, R.B., and Stephens, R.C. <strong>Drug</strong> use and “hustling”: A studyof their interrelati<strong>on</strong>ship. Criminology, 14(2):155-176, 1976.Snoek, J. Role strain in diversified role sets. American Journal ofSociology, 71:363-372, 1966.Snow, M. Maturing out of narcotic addicti<strong>on</strong> in New York City.Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of the Addicti<strong>on</strong>s, 8:917-933, 1974.Snyder, C.R. Alcohol and the Jews. Glencoe, Ill.: Free Press, 1958.Snyder, S.H. Opiate receptors in the brain. New England Journal ofMedicine, 296:267-291, 1977.Spealman, R.D. Behavior maintained by terminati<strong>on</strong> of a schedule ofself-administered cocaine. Science, 204:1231-1232, 1979.474


Spindler, G. Pers<strong>on</strong>ality and peyotism in Menomini Indian occulturati<strong>on</strong>.Psychiatry, 15:151-159, 1952.Spinelli, D.N., and Jensen, F.E. Plasticity: The mirror of experience.Science, 203:75-78, 1979.Spitz, R.A. Hospitalism: An inquiry into the genesis of psychiatricc<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s in early childhood. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child,1:53-74, 1945.Spitz, R.A. The First Year of Life. New York: Internati<strong>on</strong>al UniversityPress, 1965.Spotts, J.V., and Sh<strong>on</strong>tz, F.C. Cocaine Users: A Representative CaseApproach. New York: Free Press, 1980.Stamatoyannopoulas, G.; Chen, S.H.; and Fukui, J. Liver alcoholdehydrogenase in Japanese. High populati<strong>on</strong> frequency of atypicalform and its possible role in alcohol sensitivity. American Journalof Human Genetics, 27:789-796, 1975.Stant<strong>on</strong>, M.D. “<strong>Drug</strong> Misuse and the Family.” Paper prepared forthe White House Office of <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong> Policy, Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C..1977a.Stant<strong>on</strong>, M.D. The addict as savior: Heroin, death and the family.Family Process, 16:191-197, 1977b.Stant<strong>on</strong>, M.D. The family and drug misuse: A bibliography. AmericanJournal of <strong>Drug</strong> and Alcohol <strong>Abuse</strong>, 5(2):151-170, 1978a.Stant<strong>on</strong>, M.D. Forum: Family therapy for the drug user: C<strong>on</strong>ceptualand practical c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s. <strong>Drug</strong> Forum, 6:203-205, 1978b.Stant<strong>on</strong>, M.D. The client as family member: Aspects of c<strong>on</strong>tinuingtreatment. In: Brown, B.S., ed. Addicts and Aftercare: CommunityIntegrati<strong>on</strong> of the Former <strong>Drug</strong> User. Beverly Hills, Calif.:Sage Publicati<strong>on</strong>s, 1979a.Stant<strong>on</strong>, M.D. <strong>Drug</strong>s and the family. Marriage and Family Review,2(1):1-10, 1979b.Stant<strong>on</strong>, M.D. Family treatment approaches to drug abuse problems:A review Family Process, 18:251-280, 1979c.Stant<strong>on</strong>, M.D. Family treatment of drug problems: A review. In:DuP<strong>on</strong>t, R.L.; Goldstein, A.; and O’D<strong>on</strong>nell, J., eds. Handbook<strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong><strong>Abuse</strong>, 1979d.Stant<strong>on</strong>, M.D. Aspects of the family and drug abuse. In: Ellis, B.,ed. <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong> from the Family Perspective: Coping Is a FamilyAffair. Rockville, Md.: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>, 1980.Stant<strong>on</strong>, M.D., and Coleman, S.B. The participatory aspects ofindirect self-destructive behavior: The addict family as a model.In: Farberow, N., ed. The Many Faces of Suicide. New York:McGraw-Hill, 1979.475


Stant<strong>on</strong>, M.D.; Todd, T.C.; Heard, D.B.; Kirschner, S.; Kleiman,J.I.; Mowatt, D.T.; Riley, P.; Scott, S.M.; and Van Deusen,J.M. Heroin addicti<strong>on</strong> as a family phenomen<strong>on</strong>: A new c<strong>on</strong>ceptualmodel. American Journal of <strong>Drug</strong> and Alcohol <strong>Abuse</strong>, 5(2):125-150,1978.Steffenhagen, R.A. <strong>Drug</strong> abuse and related phenomena: An Adlerianapproach. Journal of Individual Psychology, 30:238-250, Nov.1974.Steinglass, P. Family therapy in alcoholism. In: Kissin, B., andBegleiter, H., eds. The Biology of Alcoholism. Vol. V. NewYork: Plenum, 1976.Stephens, R., and Cottrell, E. A follow-up study of 200 narcoticaddicts committed for treatment under the Narcotic Addict Rehabilitati<strong>on</strong>Act (NARA). British Journal of Addicti<strong>on</strong>, 67:45-53, 1972.Stephens, R.C., and Levine, S. The street addict role: Towards thedefiniti<strong>on</strong> of a type. Psychiatry, 34:351-357, 1971.Stephens, R.C., and McBride, D.C. Becoming a street addict.Human Organizati<strong>on</strong>, 35(1):87-93, 1976.Stephens, R.C., and Smith, R.B. Copping and caveat emptor: Thestreet addict as c<strong>on</strong>sumer. Addictive Diseases, 2(4):585-600,1976.Stephens, R.C., and Weppner, R.S. Patterns of “cheating” am<strong>on</strong>gmethad<strong>on</strong>e maintenance patients. <strong>Drug</strong> Forum, 2:357-366, 1973.Stims<strong>on</strong>, G.V. Heroin and Behavior. New York: Wiley, 1973.Stinchcombe, A.L. Mert<strong>on</strong>’s theory of social structure. In: Coser,L.A., ed. The Idea of Social Structure. New York: HarcourtBrace Jovanovich, 1975. pp. 11-33.Street, L. I Was a <strong>Drug</strong> Addict. New York: Random House, 1953.Strickland, B.R. Internal-external expectancies and health relatedbehavior. Journal of C<strong>on</strong>sulting and Clinical Psychology, 46(6):1192-1211, 1978.Struble, R.G., and Riesen, A.H. Changes in cortical dendritic branchingsubsequent to partial social isolati<strong>on</strong> in stumptailed m<strong>on</strong>keys.Developmental Psychobiology, 11:479-486, 1978.Suchman, E.A. The “hang-loose” ethic and the spirit of drug use.Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 9:146-155, 1968.Su, C.Y.; Lin, S.H.; Wang, Y.T.; Li, C.H.; Hung, L.H.; Lin,C.S.; and Lin, B.C. Effects of beta-endorphin <strong>on</strong> narcoticabstinence syndrome in man. Journal of the Formosan MedicalAssociati<strong>on</strong>, 77:133-141, 1978.Suomi, S.J., and Harlow, H.F. Social rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> of isolate-rearedm<strong>on</strong>keys. Developmental Psychology, 6:487-496, 1972.476


Sutherland, E. Principles of Criminology. Philadelphia: Lippincott,1939.Sutherland, E.H., and Cressey, D.R. Criminology. 8th ed. Philadelphia:Lippincott, 1970.Sykes, G.M., and Matza, D. Techniques of neutralizati<strong>on</strong>: A theoryof delinquency. American Sociological Review, 22:667-670, Dec.1957.Szasz. T. The Myth of Mental Illness. New York: Paul B. Hoeber,1961.Tart, C., ed. Altered States of C<strong>on</strong>sciousness. New York: Wiley,1969.Teilhard de Chardin, P. Les Directi<strong>on</strong>s de I’Avenir. Paris, France:Editi<strong>on</strong>s du Seuil, 1973. Reprinted in: Hague, R., trans.Toward the Future. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, 1975.Tennant, F.S. Dependency traits am<strong>on</strong>g parents of drug abusers.Journal of <strong>Drug</strong> Dependence, 6(1):83-88, 1976.Terenius, L. Stereospecific interacti<strong>on</strong> between narcotic analgesicsand a synaptic plasma membrane fracti<strong>on</strong> of rat cerebral cortex.Acta Pharmacologia et Toxicologia, 32:317-320, 1973.Terenius, L. Endogenous peptides and analgesia. Annual Reviews ofPharmacology and Toxicology, 18:189-204, 1978.Terenius, L., and Wahlstrom, A. Inhibitor(s) of narcotic receptorbinding in brain extracts and cerebrospinal fluid. Acta Pharmacologicaet Toxicologia, 35(Suppl. 1):55, 1974.Terry, C.E., and Pellens, M. The Opium Problem. M<strong>on</strong>tclair, N.J.:Patters<strong>on</strong> Smith, 1928.Teschemacher, H.; Opheim, K.E.; Cox, B.M.; and Goldstein, A. Apeptide-like substance from pituitary that acts like morphine. IIsolati<strong>on</strong>. Life Sciences, 16:1771-1776, 1975.Thomps<strong>on</strong>, H.V., and Worden, A.N. The Rabbit. New Naturalistm<strong>on</strong>ograph (L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>), 1956.Tomkins, S.S. Affect, Imagery and C<strong>on</strong>sciousness. Vols. I and II.New York: Springer, 1962-1963.Topaz, P.M. Report <strong>on</strong> the Journal of Learning Disabilities preliminarystudy of drug abuse and minimal brain dysfuncti<strong>on</strong>. Journal ofLearning Disabilities, 4:39-42, 1971.Turk, D. “Cognitive Modificati<strong>on</strong> of Pain.” <strong>On</strong>tario, Canada: Universityof Waterloo, 1975.Unger, S.M. Mesca<strong>line</strong>, LSD, psilocybin and pers<strong>on</strong>ality change. In:Solom<strong>on</strong>, D., ed. The C<strong>on</strong>sciousness-Expanding <strong>Drug</strong>. NewYork: Berkley Publishing, 1966. p. 206.477


Ungerleider, J.T.; Fisher, D.D.; and Fuller, M. The dangers ofLSD: Analysis of seven m<strong>on</strong>ths’ experience in a university hospital’spsychiatric service. Journal of the American Medical Associati<strong>on</strong>,197:389-392, 1966.U.S. Department of Labor. State Child-Labor Laws. Washingt<strong>on</strong>,D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office, 1966.Vaillant, G.E. A twelve year follow-up of New York narcotic addicts.III. Some social and psychiatric characteristics. Archives of GeneralPsychiatry, 15:559-609, 1966a.Vaillant, G.E. Parent-child cultural disparity and drug addicti<strong>on</strong>.Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 142:534-539, 1966b.Vaillant, G.E. A twelve year follow-up study of New York narcoticaddicts. I. The relati<strong>on</strong> of treatment to outcome. AmericanJournal of Psychiatry, 122:727-737, 1966c.Vaillant, G.E. A 20-year follow-up of New York narcotic addicts.Archives of General Psychiatry, 29:237-241, 1973.Valzelli, L. <strong>Drug</strong>s and aggressiveness. Advances in Pharmacology,5:79-108, 1967.Vaz, E.W. Middle Class Juvenile Delinquency. New York: Harper andRow, 1967.Veith, J.L.; Sandman, C.A.; Walker, J.M.; Coy, D. H.; and Kastin,A.J. Systematic administrati<strong>on</strong> of endorphins selectively altersopen field behavior of rats. Physiology and Behavior, 20:539-542,1978.Verebey, K.; Volanka, J.; and Clouet, D. Endorphins in psychiatry.Archives of General Psychiatry, 35:877-888, 1978.Vernadakis, A., and Weiner, N., eds. <strong>Drug</strong>s and the Developing Brain.New York: Plenum, 1974.Vessell, E. Genetic and envir<strong>on</strong>mental factors affecting hexobarbitalmetabolism in mice. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,151:900-912, 1968.Vesell, E.S.: Paae. J.F.: and Passananti. G.T. Genetic and envir<strong>on</strong>mentalfactors affecting ethanol metabolism in man. Clinical Pharmacologyand Therapeutics, 12:192-197, 1971.v<strong>on</strong> Knorring, L., and Oreland, L. Visual evoked resp<strong>on</strong>ses andplatelet MAO as an aid to identify a risk group for alcoholicabuse. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology, 2:385-392, 1978.V<strong>on</strong> Mayersbach, H., ed. The Cellular Aspects of Biorhythms.Berlin: Springer-Verlag, 1967.Wahl, C. Diagnosis and treatment of status medicamentosis. Diseases ofthe Nervous System, 28:318-322, 1967.Wahl, C.W. Helping the Dying Patient and His Family. New York:Family Service Associati<strong>on</strong> of America, 1960.478


Waldorf, D. Life without heroin: Some social adjustments duringl<strong>on</strong>g-term periods of voluntary abstenti<strong>on</strong>. Social Problems,18:228-243, 1970.Waldorf, D. Careers in Dope. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall,1973.Waldorf, D., and Daily, D.W. Debunking popular myths about addictsand addicti<strong>on</strong>. In: Coombs, R.H., ed. Junkies and Straights.Lexingt<strong>on</strong>, Mass.: D.C. Heath, 1975. pp. 37-56.Waldorf, D.; Murphy, S.; Reinarman, C.; and Joyce, B. Doing Coke:An Ethnography of Cocaine Users and Sellers. Washingt<strong>on</strong>,D.C.: <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong> Council, 1977.Walter, R.; Ritzmann, R.F.; Bhargava, H.N.; Rainbow, T.C.; Flexner,L.B.; and Krivoy, W.A. Inhibiti<strong>on</strong> by Z-Pro-D-Leu of developmentof tolerance to and physical dependence <strong>on</strong> morphine in mice.Proceedings of the Nati<strong>on</strong>al Academy of Sciences, USA, 75:4573-4576.1978.Wats<strong>on</strong>, S.J.; Barchas, J.D.; and Li, C.H. Beta-lipotropin: Localizati<strong>on</strong>of cells and ax<strong>on</strong>s in rat brain by immunocytochemistry. Proceedingsof the Nati<strong>on</strong>al Academy of Sciences, USA, 74:5155-5158,1977.Weber, M. The Theory of Social and Ec<strong>on</strong>omic Organizati<strong>on</strong>. Henders<strong>on</strong>,A.M., and Pars<strong>on</strong>s, T., trans. New York: Oxford UniversityPress, 1974.Weech, A.A. The narcotic addict and “the street.” Archives of GeneralPsychiatry, 14:299-306, 1966.Weil, A. The Natural Mind. New York: Hought<strong>on</strong> Mifflin, 1972.Weiss, G.; Kruger, E.; Daniels<strong>on</strong>, U.; and Elman, M. Effect ofl<strong>on</strong>g-term treatment of hyperactive children with methylphenidate.Canadian Medical Associati<strong>on</strong> Journal, 112:159-165, 1975.Weiss, G.; Minde, K.; Werry, J.; Douglas, V.; and Nemeth, E. Thehyperactive child. VIII. Five year follow-up. Archives of GeneralPsychiatry, 24:409-414, 1971.Weissbach, T.A.; Vogler, R.E.; and Compt<strong>on</strong>, J.V. Comments <strong>on</strong> therelati<strong>on</strong>ship between locus of c<strong>on</strong>trol and alcohol abuse. Journal ofClinical Psychology, 32(2):484-486, 1976.Welch, B.L., and Welch, A.S. Aggressi<strong>on</strong> and the biogenic amineneurohumors. In: Garattini, S., and Sigg, E.B., eds. AggressiveBehavior. Amsterdam: Excerpta Medica Foundati<strong>on</strong>, 1969.Wender, P. Minimal Brain Dysfuncti<strong>on</strong> in Children. New York: Wiley,1971.Weppner, R.S.; Stephens, R.C.; and C<strong>on</strong>rad, H.T. Methad<strong>on</strong>e: Someaspects of its legal and illegal use. American Journal of Psychiatry,129:451-455, 1972.479


Werry, J.S. Organic factors. In: Quay, H.C., and Werry, J.S.,eds. Psychopathological Disorders in Childhood., New York:Wiley, 1979.Westermeyer, J. Use of alcohol and opium by the Meo of Laos. AmericanJournal of Psychiatry, 127: 1019-1023, 1971.Wetzel, R.D. Hopelessness, depressi<strong>on</strong> and suicide intent. Archivesof General Psychiatry, 33: 1069-1073, 1976.Whalen, C.K., and Henker, B. Psychostimulants and children: Areview and analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 83:1113-1130, 1976.Whitehead, P.C., and Cabral, R.M. Scaling the sequence of drugusing behaviours: A test of the stepping-st<strong>on</strong>e hypothesis.<strong>Drug</strong> Forum, 5:45-54. 1975-76.Whyte, W.F. Street Corner Society. Chicago: University of ChicagoPress, 1943.Wieder, H., and Kaplan, E.H. <strong>Drug</strong> use in adolescents: Psychodynamicmeaning and pharmacogenic effect. Psychoanalytic Study of theChild, 24:399-431, 1969.Wikler, A. Recent progress in research <strong>on</strong> the neurophysiologicalbasis of morphine addicti<strong>on</strong>. American Journal of Psychiatry,105:329-338, 1948.Wikler, A. A psychodynamic study of a patient during self-regulatedreaddicti<strong>on</strong> to morphine. Psychiatric Quarterly, 26:270-293, 1952.Wikler, A. Opiate Addicti<strong>on</strong>. Springfield, Ill.: Charles C Thomas,1953.Wikler, A. <strong>On</strong> the nature of addicti<strong>on</strong> and habituati<strong>on</strong>. British Journalof Addicti<strong>on</strong>, 57:73-80, 1961.Wikler, A. C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing factors in opiate addicti<strong>on</strong> and relapse. In:Wilner, D.M., and Kassebaum, G.G., eds. Narcotics. NewYork: McGraw-Hill, 1965. pp. 85-100.Wikler, A. Sources of reinforcement for drug using behavior. Atheoretical formulati<strong>on</strong>. In: Pharmacology and the Future of Man.Proceedings of the 5th Internati<strong>on</strong>al C<strong>on</strong>gress <strong>on</strong> Pharmacology,San Francisco, 1972. Vol. 1. Basel, Switz.: Karger, 1973a.pp. 18-30.Wikler, A. Dynamics of drug dependence: Implicati<strong>on</strong>s of a c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ingtheory for research and treatment. Archives of General Psychiatry,28:611-616, 1973b.Wikler, A. C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>ing of successive adaptive resp<strong>on</strong>se to the initialeffects of drugs. C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>al Reflex, 8:193-210, 1973c.Wikler, A. Requirements for extincti<strong>on</strong> of relapse-facilitating variablesand for rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> in a narcotic-antag<strong>on</strong>ist treatment program.In: Braude, M.C.; Harris, L.S.; May, E.L.; Smith, J.P.; andVillareal, J.E., eds. Narcotic Antag<strong>on</strong>ists. Advances in BiochemicalPsychopharmacology. Vol. 8. New York: Raven, 1973d.pp. 399-414.480


Wikler, A.; Fraser, H.F.; and Isbell, H. N-allylnormorphine: Effectsof single doses and precipitati<strong>on</strong> of acute “abstinence syndromes”during addicti<strong>on</strong> to morphine, methad<strong>on</strong>e or heroin in man (postaddicts).Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics,109:8-20, 1953.Wikler, A., and Rasor, R.W. Psychiatric aspects of drug addicti<strong>on</strong>.American Journal of Medicine, 14:566-570, 1953.Willis, J.H. The natural history of drug dependence: Some comparativeobservati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> United Kingdom and United States subjects. In:Steinberg, H., ed. Scientific Basis of <strong>Drug</strong> Dependence. NewYork: Grune & Stratt<strong>on</strong>, 1969.Wingard, J.A.; Huba, G.J.; and Bentler, P.M. The relati<strong>on</strong>ship ofpers<strong>on</strong>ality structure to patterns of adolescent drug use. MultivariateBehavioral Research, 14:131-143, 1979a.Wingard, J.A.; Huba, G.J.; and Bentler, P.M. A l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal analysisof pers<strong>on</strong>ality structure and adolescent substance use. TechnicalReport. Los Angeles, Calif.: UCLA/NIDA Center for Adolescent<strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong> Etiologies, 1979b.Wingard, J.A.; Huba, C.J.; and Bentler, P.M. “<strong>Drug</strong> Use and PsychosomaticSymptomatology in N<strong>on</strong>-Instituti<strong>on</strong>alized Adult Women.”Paper presented at Nati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong> C<strong>on</strong>ference, New Orleans,1979c.Winick, C. Narcotics addicti<strong>on</strong> and its treatment. Law and C<strong>on</strong>temporaryProblems, 22:9-33, 1957.Winick, C. The use of drugs by jazz musicians. Social Problems,7:240-253, 1960.Winick, C. Physician narcotic addicts. Social Problems, 9:174-186,1961a.Winick, C. How high the mo<strong>on</strong>. Antioch Review, 21:53-68, 1961b.Winick, C. Maturing out of narcotic addicti<strong>on</strong>. Bulletin <strong>on</strong> Narcotics,14:1-7, 1962a.Winick, C. The taste of music. Jazz M<strong>on</strong>thly, 9:8-11, 1962b.Winick, C. The life cycle of the narcotic addict and of addicti<strong>on</strong>.Bulletin <strong>on</strong> Narcotics, 16:1-11, 1964.Winick, C. The New People. New York: Pegasus, 1968.Winick, C. Some reas<strong>on</strong>s for the increases in drug dependence am<strong>on</strong>gmiddle-class youths. In: Silverstein, H., ed. Sociology of Youth.New York: Macmillan, 1973. pp. 433-440.Winick, C. <strong>Drug</strong> dependence am<strong>on</strong>g nurses. In: Winick, C., ed.Sociological Aspects of <strong>Drug</strong> Dependence. Cleveland, Ohio: CRCPress, 1974a. pp. 155-168.481


Winick, C. A sociological theory of the genesis of drug dependence.In: Winick, C., ed. Sociological Aspects of <strong>Drug</strong> Dependence.Cleveland: CRC Press, 1974b. pp. 314.Winick, C. Note <strong>on</strong> a theory of the genesis of drug dependence am<strong>on</strong>gadolescents. Addictive Diseases, 1:5-6, 1974c.Winick, C., and Goldstein, J. The Glue Sniffing Problem. New York:American Social Health Associati<strong>on</strong>, 1965.Winick, C., and Nyswander, M. Psychotherapy with successful musicianswho are drug addicts. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,31:622-636, 1961.Wohlwill, J.F. The Study of Behavioral Development. New York:Academic Press, 1973.Wolff, G., and M<strong>on</strong>ey, J. Relati<strong>on</strong>ship between sleep and growth inpatients with reversible somatotropin deficiency (psychosocialdwarfism). Psychological Medicine, 3:18-27, 1973.Wolff, P.H. Vasomotor sensitivity to ethanol in diverse m<strong>on</strong>goloidpopulati<strong>on</strong>s. American Journal of Human Genetics, 25:193-199,1973.Wolfgang, M. The culture of youth. In: President’s Commissi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>Law Enforcement. Task Force Report: Juvenile Delinquency andYouth Crime. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C. Superintendent of Documents,U.S. Government Printing Office: 1967.Wolfgang, M.E., and Ferricutti, F. The Subculture of Violence.L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>: Tavistock, 1967.Woodruff, R.A.; Goodwin, D.W.; and Guze, S.B. Psychiatric Diagnosis.New York: Oxford University Press, 1974.Wurmser, L. Psychoanalytic c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s of the etiology of compulsivedrug use. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Associati<strong>on</strong>,22:820-843, 1974.Wurmser, L. The Hidden Dimensi<strong>on</strong>. Psychodynamics in Compulsive<strong>Drug</strong> Use. New York: Ar<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>, 1978.Wynne-Edwards, V.C. Animal Dispersi<strong>on</strong> in Relati<strong>on</strong> to Social Behavior.Edinburgh: Oliver & Boyd, 1962.Yaffe, S.; Krasner, J.; and Catz, C. Variati<strong>on</strong>s in detoxifying enzymesduring mammalian development. Annals of the New York Academyof Sciences, 151:887-899, 1968.Yinger, M. C<strong>on</strong>traculture and subculture. American Sociological Review,25:625-635, Oct. 1960.Yorke, C. A critical review of some psychoanalytic literature <strong>on</strong> drugaddicti<strong>on</strong>. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 43:141-159,1970.Young, R.D. Developmental psychopharmacology: A beginning.Psychological Bulletin, 67:73-86, 1967.482


Zeiner. A.R., and Paredes. A. Racial differences in circadian variati<strong>on</strong>of ethanol metabolism. Alcoholism: Clinical and ExperimentalResearch, 2(1):71-75, 1978.Zimmering, P.; Toolin, J.; Safrin, R.; and Wortis, S.B. Heroinaddicti<strong>on</strong> in adolescent boys. Journal of Mental and NervousDisease, 114:19-34, 1951.Zimmering, P.; Toolin, J.; Safrin, R.; and Wortis, S.B. <strong>Drug</strong> addicti<strong>on</strong>in relati<strong>on</strong> to problems of adolescence. American Journal ofPsychiatry, 109:272-278, 1952.Zinberg, N.E. Gl’s and OJ’s in Vietnam. New York Times, Magazinesecti<strong>on</strong>, Dec. 5, 1971.Zinberg, N.E. Heroin use in Vietnam and the United States: A c<strong>on</strong>trastand a critique. Archives of General Psychiatry, 26:486-488,1972.Zinberg, N.E. The search for rati<strong>on</strong>al approaches to heroin use. In:Bourne, P.G., ed. Addicti<strong>on</strong>. New York: Academic Press,1974a.Zinberg, N.E. “High” States: A Beginning Study. Publicati<strong>on</strong> SS-3.Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.: <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong> Council, 1974b.Zinberg, N.E. Addicti<strong>on</strong> and ego functi<strong>on</strong>. Psychoanalytic Study ofthe Child, 30:567-588, 1975.Zinberg, N.E. N<strong>on</strong>addictive opiate use. In: DuP<strong>on</strong>t, R.L.; Goldstein,A.; and O’D<strong>on</strong>nell, J., eds. Handbook <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>. Rockville,Md.: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>, 1979. pp. 303-313.Zinberg, N.E., and DeL<strong>on</strong>g, J.V. Research and the drug issue.C<strong>on</strong>temporary <strong>Drug</strong> Problems, 3:71-100, 1974.Zinberg, N.E., and Fraser, K.M. The role of the social setting in thepreventi<strong>on</strong> and treatment of alcoholism. In: Mendels<strong>on</strong>, J.H.,and Mello, N.K., eds. The Diagnosis and Treatment of Alcoholism.New York: McGraw-Hill, 1979.Zinberg, N.E., and Harding, W.M. C<strong>on</strong>trol over intoxicant use: Atheoretical and practical overview. Journal of <strong>Drug</strong> Issues,9:121-143, 1979.Zinberg, N.E.; Harding, W.M.; and Winkeller, M. A study of socialregulatory mechanisms in c<strong>on</strong>trolled illicit drug users. Journal of<strong>Drug</strong> Issues, 7:117-133, 1977.Zinberg, N.E., and Jacobs<strong>on</strong>, R.C. The natural history of chipping.American Journal of Psychiatry, 133:37-40, 1976.Zinberg, N.E.; Jacobs<strong>on</strong>, R.C.; and Harding, W.M. Social sancti<strong>on</strong>sand rituals as a basis for drug abuse preventi<strong>on</strong>. The AmericanJournal of <strong>Drug</strong> and Alcohol <strong>Abuse</strong>, 2:165-181, 1975.Zinberg, N.E., and Roberts<strong>on</strong>, J.A. <strong>Drug</strong>s and the Public. NewYork: Sim<strong>on</strong> and Schuster, 1972.483


Zinner, J., and Shapiro, R. The family group as a single psychicentity: Implicati<strong>on</strong>s for acting out in adolescence. Internati<strong>on</strong>alReview of Psycho-Analysis, 1:179-186, 1974.Zinner, J., and Shapiro, R. Splitting in families of border<strong>line</strong> adolescents,In: Mack, J.E., ed. Border<strong>line</strong> States in Psychiatry.New York: Grune & Stratt<strong>on</strong>, 1975. pp. 103-122.Zubek, J.P., ed. Sensory Deprivati<strong>on</strong>: Fifteen Years of Research.New York: Applet<strong>on</strong>-Century-Crofts, 1969.Zuckerman, M. Sensati<strong>on</strong> Seeking: Bey<strong>on</strong>d the Optimal Level of Arousal.Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc., 1979.484


While limited supplies last, single copies of the m<strong>on</strong>ographs maybe obtained free of charge from the Nati<strong>on</strong>al Clearinghouse for <strong>Drug</strong><strong>Abuse</strong> Informati<strong>on</strong> (NCDAI). Please c<strong>on</strong>tact NCDAI also for informati<strong>on</strong>about availability of coming issues and other publicati<strong>on</strong>s ofthe Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong> relevant to drug abuse research.Additi<strong>on</strong>al copies may be purchased from the U.S. Government PrintingOffice (GPO) and/or the Nati<strong>on</strong>al Technical Informati<strong>on</strong> Service (NTIS)as indicated. NTIS prices are for paper copy. Microfiche copies,at $3.50, are also available from NTIS. Prices from either sourceare subject to change.Addresses are:NCDAINati<strong>on</strong>al Clearinghouse for <strong>Drug</strong> <strong>Abuse</strong>Room 10A-565600 Fishers LaneRockville, Maryland 20857Informati<strong>on</strong>GPOSuperintendent of DocumentsU.S. Government Printing OfficeWashingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C. 20402NTISNati<strong>on</strong>al Technical Informati<strong>on</strong>ServiceU.S. Department of CommerceSpringfield, Virginia 221611 FINDINGS OF DRUG ABUSE RESEARCH. Not available from NCDAI.Vol. 1: GPO out of stock NTIS PB #272 867/AS $22Vol. 2: GPO out of stock NTIS PB #272 868/AS $202 OPERATIONAL DEFINITIONS IN SOCIO-BEHAVIORAL DRUG USE RESEARCH1975. Jack Elins<strong>on</strong>, Ph.D., and David Nurco, Ph.D., eds. Notavailable from NCDAI.GPO out of stock NTIS PB #246 338/AS $113 AMINERGIC HYPOTHESES OF BEHAVIOR: REALITY OR CLICHE? Bruce J.Bernard, Ph.D., ed.GPO Stock #017-024-00486-3 $2.25 NTIS PB #246 687/AS $11485


4 NARCOTIC ANTAGONISTS: THE SEARCH FOR LONG-ACTING PREPARATIONS.Robert Willette, Ph.D., ed.GPO Stock #017-024-00488-0 $1.10 NTIS PB #247 096/AS $65 YOUNG MEN AND DRUGS: A NATIONWIDE SURVEY. John A. O'D<strong>on</strong>nell,Ph.D., et al.GPO Stock #017-024-00511-8 $2.25 NTIS PB #247 446/AS $116 EFFECTS OF LABELING THE "DRUG ABUSER": AN INQUIRY. Jay R.Williams, Ph.D.GPO Stock #017-024-00512-6 $1.05 NTIS PB #249 092/AS $67 CANNABINOID ASSAYS IN HUMANS. Robert Willette, Ph.D., ed.GPO Stock #017-024-00510-0 $1.95 NTIS PB #251 905/AS $108 Rx: 3x/WEEK LAAM-ALTERNATIVE TO METHADONE. Jack Blaine, M.D.,and Pierre Renault, M.D., eds.Not available from GPO NTIS PB #253 763/AS $109 NARCOTIC ANTAGONISTS: NALTREXONE PROGRESS REPORT. DemetriosJulius, M.D., and Pierre Renault, M.D., eds.GPO Stock #017-024-00521-5 $2.55 NTIS PB #255 833/AS $1210 EPIDEMIOLOGY OF DRUG ABUSE: CURRENT ISSUES. Louise G. Richards,Ph.D., and Louise B. Blevens, eds. Examines methodological issuesin surveys and data collecti<strong>on</strong>. Not available from NCDAI.GPO Stock #017-024-00571-1 $2.60 NTIS PB #266 691/AS $1311 DRUGS AND DRIVING. Robert Willette, Ph.D., ed. Reviews research<strong>on</strong> effects of drugs <strong>on</strong> psychomotor performance, focusing <strong>on</strong> measuresof impairment by different drugs at various levels.GPO Stock #017-024-00576-2 $1.70 NTIS PB #269 602/AS $1112 PSYCHODYNAMICS OF DRUG DEPENDENCE. Jack D. Blaine, M.D., andDemetrios A. Julius, M.D., eds. Theoretical and clinical papers c<strong>on</strong>cernedwith the intrapsychic determinants of drug addicti<strong>on</strong>.GPO Stock #017-024-00642-4 $2.75 NTIS PB #276 084/AS $1213 COCAINE: 1977. Robert C. Petersen, Ph.D., and Richard C. Stillman,M.D., eds. Reports the extent and limits of current knowledgeabout cocaine, its use and misuse.GPO Stock #017-024-00592-4 $3 NTIS PB #269 175/AS $1314 MARIHUANA RESEARCH FINDINGS: 1976. Robert C. Petersen, Ph.D.,ed. Technical papers <strong>on</strong> which the 6th Marihuana and Health reportto C<strong>on</strong>gress was based.GPO Stock #017-024-00622-0 $3 NTIS PB #271 279/AS $1515 REVIEW OF INHALANTS: EUPHORIA TO DYSFUNCTION. Charles Wm. Sharp,Ph.D., and Mary Lee Brehm, Ph.D., eds. Review of inhalant abuse,including an extensive bibliography.GPO Stock #017-024-00650-5 $4.25 NTIS PB #275 798/AS $19486


16 THE EPIDEMIOLOGY OF HEROIN AND OTHER NARCOTICS. Joan DunneRittenhouse, Ph.D., ed. Task Force report <strong>on</strong> research technologiesand implicati<strong>on</strong>s for studying heroin-narcotic use.GPO Stock #017-024-00690-4 $3.50 NTIS PB #276 357/AS $1417 RESEARCH ON SMOKING BEHAVIOR. Murray E. Jarvik, M.D., Ph.D., etal., eds. Includes epidemiology, etiology, c<strong>on</strong>sequences of use, and approachesto behavioral change. From a NIDA-supported UCLA c<strong>on</strong>ference.GPO Stock #017-024-00694-7 4.50 NTIS PB #276 353/AS $2018 BEHAVIORAL TOLERANCE: RESEARCH AND TREATMENT IMPLICATIONS.Norman A. Krasnegor, Ph.D., ed. Theoretical and empirical studiesof n<strong>on</strong>pharmacologic factors in development of drug tolerance.GPO Stock #017-024-00699-8 $2.75 NTIS PB #276 337/AS $1119 THE INTERNATIONAL CHALLENGE OF DRUG ABUSE. Robert C. Petersen,Ph.D., ed. Papers from the VI World C<strong>on</strong>gress of Psychiatry whichdeal with drug issues of particular interest worldwide.GPO Stock #017-024-00822-2 $4.50 NTIS PB #293 807/AS $1920 SELF-ADMINISTRATION OF ABUSED SUBSTANCES: METHODS FOR STUDY.Norman A. Krasnegor, Ph.D., ed. Techniques used to study basicprocesses underlying abuse of drugs, ethanol, food, and tobacco.Not available from GPO NTIS PB #288 471/AS $1521 PHENCYCLIDINE (PCP) ABUSE: AN APPRAISAL. Robert C. Petersen,Ph.D., and Richard C. Stillman, M.D., eds. Pi<strong>on</strong>eering volume forclinicians and researchers assessing what is known about the problemof PCP abuse.GPO Stock #017-024-00785-4 $4.25 NTIS PB #288 472/AS $1722 QUASAR: QUANTITATIVE STRUCTURE ACTIVITY RELATIONSHIPS OF ANALGE-SICS, NARCOTIC ANTAGONISTS, AND HALLUCINOGENS. Gene Barnett, Ph.D.;Milan Trsic, Ph.D.; and Robert Willette, Ph.D.; eds. Reports froman interdisciplinary c<strong>on</strong>ference <strong>on</strong> the molecular nature of drugreceptorinteracti<strong>on</strong>s.GPO Stock #017-024-00786-2 $5.25 NTIS PB #292 265/AS $2423 CIGARETTE SMOKING AS A DEPENDENCE PROCESS. Norman A. Krasnegor,Ph.D., ed. Discusses factors involved in the <strong>on</strong>set, maintenance,and cessati<strong>on</strong> of the cigarette smoking habit. Includes an agendafor future research.GPO Stock #017-024-00895-8 $4.50 NTIS PB #297 721/AS $1324 SYNTHETIC ESTIMATES FOR SMALL AREAS: STATISTICAL WORKSHOP PAPERSAND DISCUSSION. Joseph Steinberg, ed. Papers from a workshop cosp<strong>on</strong>soredby NIDA and the Nati<strong>on</strong>al Center for Health Statistics <strong>on</strong>a class of statistical approaches that yield needed estimates ofdata for States and local areas.GPO Stock #017-024-00911-3 $5 NTIS PB #299 009/AS $16487


25 BEHAVIORAL ANALYSIS AND TREATMENT OF SUBSTANCE ABUSE. Norman A.Krasnegor, Ph.D., ed. Papers present comm<strong>on</strong>alities and implicati<strong>on</strong>sfor treatment of dependency <strong>on</strong> drugs, ethanol, food, and tobacco.GPO Stock #017-024-00939-3 $4.50 NTIS PB #80-112428 $1526 THE BEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF SMOKING. Norman A. Krasnegor, Ph.D.,ed. Reprint of the behavioral secti<strong>on</strong> of the 1979 Report of theSurge<strong>on</strong> General <strong>on</strong> Smoking and Health, with an introducti<strong>on</strong> bythe editor.GPO Stock #017-024-00947-4 $4.25 NTIS PB #80-118755 $1227 PROBLEMS OF DRUG DEPENDENCE, 1979: PROCEEDINGS OF THE 41ST AN-NUAL SCIENTIFIC MEETING, THE COMMITEE ON PROBLEMS OF DRUG DEPEN-DENCE, INC. Louis S. Harris, Ph.D., ed. Comprehensive assemblageof <strong>on</strong>going research <strong>on</strong> drug abuse, addicti<strong>on</strong>, and new compounds.GPO Stock #017-024-00981-4 $8NTIS PB #to be assigned28 NARCOTIC ANTAGONISTS: NALTREXONE PHARMACOCHEMISTRY AND SUSTAINED-RELEASE PREPARATIONS (DRUG DEVELOPMENT VOLUME V). Gene Barnett,Ph.D., and Robert Willette, Ph.D., eds. Papers report research <strong>on</strong>inserted sustained-release and l<strong>on</strong>g-acting drug devices, and <strong>on</strong>possible use with the narcotic antag<strong>on</strong>ist naltrex<strong>on</strong>e.In Preparati<strong>on</strong>29 DRUG ABUSE DEATHS IN NINE CITIES: A SURVEY REPORT. Louis A.Gottschalk, M.D., et al. Epidemiologic study providing data <strong>on</strong>drug-involved deaths and procedures for their investigati<strong>on</strong>s.GPO Stock #to be assignedNTIS PB #to be assigned488

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!