22.06.2015 Views

Savory - Arachnida 1977

Savory - Arachnida 1977

Savory - Arachnida 1977

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

80 11. DE ARACHNIDIS<br />

results from sheltering behind words. Some advance has now been made<br />

from that position, so that we now look upon instinctive actions as being<br />

reflex acts, following one another in predetermined succession, coming<br />

often under the influence of external circumstances, so that a symmetry<br />

of activity is produced, and often also under the influence of<br />

internal conditions, so that new types of activity constantly appear.<br />

This union of external and internal factors and the realization of the<br />

nature of the latter is important. Instincts are something more than<br />

reflexes, but this "something" is of a material nature, secreted by glands;<br />

it acts as a chemical compound distributed by the blood. Thus it becomes<br />

at once subject to direct study, and mere difficulties of technique<br />

are the obstacles to a full knowledge of the nature of every hormone, of<br />

the way in which it is produced and the physicochemical nature of its<br />

mode of action.<br />

An exceptionally fine analysis of an instinctive action in this way has<br />

been made by Peters ( 1931-3), in his study of the capture of insects by<br />

the common spider Araneus diadematus. He concludes that a series of<br />

stimuli, each followed by a characteristic reaction, produces the habit,<br />

and he has broken down a process, which many have watched and<br />

which appears to be a continuous operation, into the following steps:<br />

STIMULUS RECEIVED<br />

I. Vibration of threads of web<br />

2. Struggling of prey<br />

3. Contact with prey<br />

4. Contact with silk wrapping<br />

S. Chemical stimulus of short bite<br />

REFLEX RESPONSE<br />

Movement to centre of disturbance<br />

Long bite<br />

\'\Trapping or enshrouding<br />

Short bite<br />

Wrapped prey is carried off<br />

An analysis, similar to this in every essential, has been made by<br />

Homann ( 1928), who studied the approach of the jumping spider<br />

Evarcha blancardi towards its victim. Here again the outward appearance<br />

is one of a steady stalking, and here again the separate steps have been<br />

recognized and isolated:<br />

STIMULUS RECEIVED<br />

I. Image in posterior lateral eye<br />

2. Image in anterior lateral eye<br />

3. Images in both anterior lateral<br />

eyes<br />

4. Images in anterior median eyes<br />

REFLEX RESPONSE<br />

Turning of spider's body towards<br />

object<br />

Turning of spider's body continues<br />

Spider begins to move forwards<br />

Spider begins to creep towards<br />

victim, or to court a female<br />

8. ETHOLOGY: BEHAVIOUR 81<br />

Finally, an aspect of arachnid behaviour which appears as a characteristic<br />

feature in the lives of many is due to the rhythmical nature of<br />

individual activity. It has been said above that an arachnid spends a<br />

large proportion of its life waiting, and doing nothing; to this there<br />

must be added a recognition of the fact that idleness is often interrupted<br />

at regular intervals.<br />

Rhythmic behaviour, which is by no means limited to the <strong>Arachnida</strong>,<br />

is of two kinds. There arc exogenous rhythms, which are responses to<br />

regular recurring changes in the environment; and these are to be<br />

contrasted with endogenous rhythms, consequent upon internal changes<br />

within the organism. The former tend to disappear if the particular<br />

stimuli are removed; the latter persist even when the animal is kept<br />

under constant conditions, artificially maintained.<br />

Probably the most familiar example of rhythmic behaviour among<br />

<strong>Arachnida</strong> is the simple observation that the spiders in our gardens are<br />

usually seen to be spinning their webs in the evenings, while there are<br />

other species that are equally accustomed to spin th~ir ~ebs just befo~e<br />

the dawn. An example of precise observations of th1s kmd of rhythmic<br />

behaviour is that described by Nlarples ( 1971) concerning the common<br />

species ,Zygiella atrica. The habit of this spider is to spend the day _in<br />

shelter, though in communication with its web, and to take up 1ts<br />

position in the centre of its web during the night.<br />

Hourly observations of 127 webs gave the following result:<br />

5.30 a.m.-1 0.00 p.m.<br />

10.00 p.m.-10.30 p.m.<br />

10.30 p.m.-11.00 p.m.<br />

4.00 a.m.- 5.30 a.m.<br />

No spiders in the webs<br />

Spiders beginning to occupy hub<br />

All spiders occupying hubs<br />

Spiders spinning webs<br />

Behaviour of this kind, clearly associated with the intensity of the<br />

light, is common enough among <strong>Arachnida</strong> of many orders. It is<br />

particularly obvious among Opiliones, as may be seen by anyone who<br />

keeps harvestmen in cages. Their rhythms were examined by ~dgar<br />

and Yuan ( 1968), working on Phalangium opilio and seven speCies of<br />

Leiobunum, with the use of a kymograph. Their results showed that<br />

under all ordinary conditions 90% of their activity occurred between<br />

6.00 p.m. and 6.00 a.m. .<br />

Fowler and Goodnight ( 1966), working with Leiobunum longzpes,<br />

compared its activity with the secretion of 5-hydroxytryptamine. This<br />

compound is widespread in animals and plants, its presence can be<br />

readily shown and the amount measured.<br />

The results obtained showed that activity began about two hours<br />

before sunset rose to a maximum at about 2.00 a.m., and then rapidly<br />

'

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!