22.06.2015 Views

Savory - Arachnida 1977

Savory - Arachnida 1977

Savory - Arachnida 1977

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

76 11. DE ARACHNIDIS<br />

Telotaxis is an orientation of the animal followed by movement<br />

towards the source of the stimulus, which is acting as if it were a goal<br />

or an end to be sought. Telotactic movements haYe therefore an outward<br />

appearance of purpose, since there is a minimum of deviation in<br />

the path taken.<br />

It has been asserted that true telotaxis is shown only by movements<br />

towards or away from a source oflight, but there can be no doubt that<br />

vibrotaxis, which is so conspicuous a part of the behaviour of spiders,<br />

is an equally good example. It is readily to be seen whenever a web<br />

spider runs across its web to seize its prey. At the same time it should be<br />

remembered that when vibrotaxis was first recognized by Barrows<br />

( 1915) he also described the result of simultaneous oscillation of the<br />

web at two foci, when the spider took a path between them. This is<br />

typical tropotaxis.<br />

Vibrotaxis is not confined to web spiders, for spiders that hunt on the<br />

surface of water respond in the same way to the ripples produced either<br />

by struggling insects or by the prong of a vibrating tuning fork. It can<br />

readily be seen that this tropism is a direct consequence of the flexor<br />

reflex. Let it be supposed that the spider is standing with all eight legs<br />

on the surface, as shown in Fig. 25. The ripples passing under the right<br />

Frc. 25. Diagram to illustrate the mechanism of vibrotaxis.<br />

legs will have a smaller amplitude than those passing under the left<br />

legs, and thus there is a more intense stimulation of the right legs,<br />

followed by a greater degree of flexion of the femoral muscles. The consequence<br />

of this forced movement is that the extended left legs exert a<br />

turning effect relatively to the less extended right legs so that as the<br />

spider runs it is automatically directed towards the origin of the<br />

disturbance.<br />

The distinction between kineses and taxes is well illustrated by considering<br />

movements determined by the force of gravity. An arachnid<br />

which habitually climbs upwards or downwards shows a discrimination<br />

between "up" and "down", which is a response to the direction of the<br />

stimulus. This is pure geotaxis. As it climbs up (or down) the force of<br />

gravity does not alter by an appreciable amount. Therefore the animal<br />

does not move more and more quickly as it approaches the top (or the<br />

8. ETHOLOGY: BEHAVIOUR 77<br />

bottom) of a tree trunk. If it did so, it would be exhibiting geokinesis.<br />

Because it does not, there is no geokinesis in arachnid behaviour.<br />

The essential difference between the tropism theory and the taxis<br />

theory is a difference in interpretation, the former being mechanistic,<br />

the latter teleological. If a biologist can convince himself, in spite of the<br />

impossibility of obtaining direct evidence of the animal's subjective<br />

state, that the animal has a conscious appreciation of sensations, then<br />

the taxis theory will afford an acceptable interpretation of many facts.<br />

For instance, Ki.ihn states that flies move towards the window if they are<br />

chased and that on the theory of tropisms such a sudden change in the<br />

sign of the reaction is inexplicable, as in any other such change when it<br />

appears as a result of suddenly approaching danger. As an innate<br />

reaction to "the simple sensation of danger", such behaviour may be<br />

intelligible.<br />

The present position with regard to the interpretation of the behaviour<br />

of invertebrates like <strong>Arachnida</strong> may be summarized as follows.<br />

The mechanistic theories, such as the tropism theory, were valuable<br />

because they pointed the way to a great deal of experimental work, most<br />

if not all of which may be broken down into measurement of the reactions<br />

of the sense organs and the response of the muscles. It may be<br />

useful-perhaps it may be necessary-to know the facts about these<br />

actions, but their relation to any psychic events is obscure and experiment<br />

tells us nothing about these. The tactic view of behaviour may lead<br />

a biologist to ask whether a creature which, for example, lurks under<br />

stones does so for some conscious reason, and then force him to give a<br />

negative answer simply because the taxes which guided it have been<br />

recognized and named.<br />

Mechanistic theories of behaviour carry us very little further than<br />

this. Despite the persuasiveness of the mechanists, an animal does not<br />

seem to be a mechanical device; it seems to be much more obviously a<br />

sentient being, trying, now in one way, now in another, to survive amid<br />

the perils of existence.<br />

The position is no more than a logical consequence of a very simple<br />

dilemma. Either there is nothing in animals that corresponds to the<br />

origin of thoughts in man, or there is something in animals in which<br />

originate thoughts, or such homologues of thought as may occur in<br />

cerebral ganglia. The nature of these thoughts and of the mind in which<br />

they are developed is a fundamental problem of biopsychology, to which<br />

this chapter can do no more than serve as one path along which that<br />

problem may be approached.<br />

Reflexes, kineses and taxes may well be looked upon as patterns of<br />

mechanical behaviour, evolved and retained in response to the environmental<br />

conditions in which <strong>Arachnida</strong> can survive. One is inclined to

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!