22.06.2015 Views

Savory - Arachnida 1977

Savory - Arachnida 1977

Savory - Arachnida 1977

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

38<br />

Arachnophobia<br />

Many of the <strong>Arachnida</strong> inspire fear; scorpions are commonly avoided as<br />

if they were more dangerous than snakes, Solifugae are shunned, and<br />

countless people are terrified of spiders. These facts are interesting<br />

because there is no obvious justification for so disproportionate, so widespread,<br />

so illogical a horror, which may well have been an obstacle to<br />

the progress of serious arachnology. vVhat is the reason for and the origin<br />

of this fear?<br />

What is fear? Fear is of two kinds. Subjective fear, or, in Spencer's<br />

words, "the revival on a given stimulus of past experience of pain", is<br />

the consciousness of the changes in blood pressure and muscle tone<br />

consequent upon the activity of the adrenal glands, and is therefore<br />

rightly described as "unfled flight". Objective fear is the actual secretion<br />

of adrenalin, and is an unconditioned reflex normally elicited by<br />

an unexpected noise, a sudden shock, or a loss of support, threatening<br />

stability of posture.<br />

The tonic reflexes which maintain equilibrium respond to tactile<br />

impulses from the soles of the feet, to impulses of pressure from the<br />

semicircular canals of the ear, and to visual impulses from the retinas<br />

of the eyes. Here we are concerned only with the last of these. From<br />

them there arises a tendency, well developed in the lower animals, to<br />

keep the images of moving bodies on the same place in each retina.<br />

This results in an orientation and movement of their bodies known as<br />

rheotropism or rheotaxis.<br />

It is dear that if the body is moved or the eye is turned so as to keep<br />

the image of a moving object more or less stationary on the retina, the<br />

of the background and surroundings must, at the same time, be<br />

across the retina. This is exactly what happens during an<br />

unexpected fall and the primitive response is a secretion of adrenalin to<br />

fit the body to meet the sudden change of circumstances. This is fear.<br />

Just as, according to the James-Lange theory of the emotions we "feel<br />

38. ARACH~OPHOBIA 317<br />

sorry because we cry", so do we feel frightened because of the physical<br />

changes in blood pressure and muscle tension.<br />

The sudden running of a spider across the floor, of a scorpion across<br />

the tent-cloth or of a solifuge across the desert sand attracts the eye, and<br />

as the attempt is automatically made to "keep the eye on" the moving<br />

animal, images of the background pass across the retina. The reflex<br />

response follows, and, since all motion is relative, the conscious mind has<br />

no difficulty in projecting the origin of fear to the escaping creature.<br />

This explanation of the phenomenon of arachnophobia has received<br />

criticism during the past three decades, but little of the criticism has<br />

been constructive, offering an alternative hypothesis.<br />

During the same period, however, phobias of all kinds, and there are<br />

many, have been officially included in the International Classification<br />

of Diseases, and considerable advances have been made in understanding<br />

and treating them.<br />

Animal phobias, which include a fear of birds, moths, butterflies,<br />

wasps, mice, snakes, cats and dogs, have usually been found to have<br />

appeared first in childhood. When 18,000 children answered a BBC<br />

question, "Which animal do you dislike most?", the snake came first,<br />

with 27% of the replies, the spider second with 9·5% and well ahead of<br />

the third animal, the lion, which scored 4·5%.<br />

The childish dislike may persist into adult life, when it is sometimes<br />

overcome by unprejudiced reason showing the patient that it is simply<br />

silly; at other times it is allowed to develop into a serious phobia.<br />

Statistics reveal that most of the sufferers are women.<br />

During a period of 12 years in which I was concerned with countless<br />

women students, I made a practice of asking them whether they were<br />

afraid of spiders, and if so, why? Their replies fell into three groups, each<br />

with its qualification: fear of black spiders, not of the gaily-coloured<br />

ones; (ii) fear of large spiders, not of the little ones; and (iii) fear of<br />

long-legged spiders, not of those with short legs. In every case the<br />

answer provoked a second question: just how dark, or how large, or<br />

how long in the leg must a spider be before it passes the threshold and<br />

becomes frightening? Is there a critical value for each characteristic?<br />

There was never a precise answer to any of these questions, but there<br />

was unmistakably a general agreement that colour, size and legginess<br />

are all contributory causes, and that most certainly movement cannot<br />

be discounted: in fact it becomes clear that the phobia is complex and<br />

no one explanation will cover all cases.<br />

One particularly significant confession by a very clever woman<br />

traced her fear back to a toy, a papier mache spider with legs of spirally<br />

coiled wire. When the spider was hung up, all its legs trembled in a<br />

characteristically purposeless manner, inexplicable and terrifying. To

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!