06.04.2015 Views

Presentation

Presentation

Presentation

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

A multi-level perspective of science teaching<br />

and the role of video based analysis<br />

Hans E. Fischer<br />

University Duisburg-Essen<br />

Germany<br />

Hans.Fischer@uni-due.de


Outline<br />

1. Modeling (science) instruction<br />

2. The role of video based analysis<br />

3. Recent results of video based research<br />

4. Further aims<br />

0 1 2 3 4


Conditions for science instruction<br />

0 1 2 3 4<br />

In general: Discipline, organization of lessons,<br />

pacing, monitoring …<br />

Specific structure of subject matter<br />

(logic of content, scientific literacy, NOS, SI)<br />

Science specific teaching processes<br />

(experimental work, professional knowledge)<br />

Science specific learning processes<br />

(pre-knowledge, problem solving, conceptual<br />

development)<br />

System conditions<br />

(specific sequences, science or subjects, curricula)<br />

Quality of instruction


Paradigms of Quality of Instruction<br />

Behaviorism<br />

Mandl & Hron, Carroll (from 1970): Quality was operationalized as<br />

time on task<br />

Direct instruction, cognitivism<br />

Mayer, Rosenshine, Sweller (from 1980): information processing,<br />

cognitive structure, cognitive load<br />

Teacher personality and styles of instruction,<br />

subjective Theories<br />

Dann, Wahl, Pallasch, Shulman (from 1980): Interaction analysis,<br />

teacher knowledge<br />

Process-product-paradigm<br />

Helmke & Weinert (from 1990): Quality as model of instruction<br />

connected with students‘ performance<br />

0 1 2 3 4


Paradigms of Quality of Instruction<br />

Process-mediation-product-paradigm<br />

Baumert & Köller, Leutner (from 1990): processes of<br />

cognitive assimilation, strategies of learning and self<br />

regulated learning<br />

Offer-mediation-use-paradigm<br />

Klieme (from 2000): student as co-producer of his/her<br />

own knowledge, vertical fit of cognitive concepts,<br />

cognitive activation<br />

0 1 2 3 4


Important for quality of Instruction<br />

Time on task<br />

Information processing, cognitive structure,<br />

cognitive load<br />

Classroom interaction, teacher knowledge<br />

Students‘ performance<br />

Cognitive assimilation, strategies of learning<br />

and self regulated learning<br />

Student as co-producer of knowledge, vertical<br />

fit of cognitive concepts, cognitive activation


Offer-mediation-use-paradigm<br />

Family<br />

Content of the<br />

family<br />

environement<br />

Parents as:<br />

Pedagoges<br />

Example<br />

Model of<br />

language<br />

Teacher<br />

Instruction<br />

(offer)<br />

Quality of<br />

instruction<br />

Teachers‘<br />

personality<br />

and expertise<br />

Teaching and<br />

learning material<br />

Individual pre-conditions<br />

Mediations-<br />

Processes<br />

Perception and<br />

interpretation<br />

of teacher and<br />

students<br />

School- and class context<br />

Subject and curriculum context<br />

Learning<br />

activity<br />

(use)<br />

Learning<br />

processes during<br />

instruction<br />

Out of school<br />

activities<br />

Effect<br />

(return)<br />

Subject specific:<br />

Knowkedge<br />

Literacy<br />

Abilities<br />

Competencies<br />

Others:<br />

Key competencies<br />

Socialisation<br />

0 1 2 3 4<br />

Klieme


Offer-mediation-use-paradigm<br />

Family<br />

Content of the<br />

family<br />

environement<br />

Parents as:<br />

Pedagoges<br />

Example<br />

Model of<br />

language<br />

Teacher<br />

Instruction<br />

(offer)<br />

Quality of<br />

instruction<br />

Teachers‘<br />

personality<br />

and expertise<br />

Teaching and<br />

learning material<br />

Individual pre-conditions<br />

Mediations-<br />

Processes<br />

Perception and<br />

interpretation<br />

of teacher and<br />

students<br />

School- and class context<br />

Subject and curriculum context<br />

Learning<br />

activity<br />

(use)<br />

Learning<br />

processes during<br />

instruction<br />

Out of school<br />

activities<br />

Effect<br />

(return)<br />

Subject specific:<br />

Knowkedge<br />

Literacy<br />

Abilities<br />

Competencies<br />

Others:<br />

Key competencies<br />

Socialisation<br />

0 1 2 3 4<br />

Klieme


Research on instruction<br />

Teacher<br />

Professional<br />

knowledge<br />

Practical experience<br />

Background<br />

Instruction<br />

Pre-knowledge<br />

Motivation/Interest<br />

Background<br />

(social/ cognitiv)<br />

Students<br />

School/family<br />

environment<br />

Planning and<br />

performing<br />

Offer<br />

Mediation<br />

Use<br />

Experience<br />

Learning processes<br />

Practical<br />

experience<br />

Critical reflection<br />

Further education<br />

Performance<br />

Expectation<br />

Motivation/interest<br />

0 1 2 3 4


TIMSS, PISA<br />

Teacher<br />

Professional<br />

knowledge<br />

Practical experience<br />

Background<br />

Instruction<br />

School/family<br />

environment<br />

Planning and<br />

performing<br />

Practical<br />

experience<br />

Critical reflection<br />

Further education<br />

Pre-knowledge<br />

Motivation/Interest<br />

Background<br />

(social/cognitiv)<br />

Students<br />

Experience<br />

Learning processes<br />

Performance<br />

Expectation<br />

Motivation/interest<br />

01 2 3 4


TIMSS-Video, TIMSS-R<br />

Teacher<br />

Professional<br />

knowledge<br />

Practical experience<br />

Background<br />

Instruction<br />

School/family<br />

environment<br />

Planning and<br />

performing<br />

Practical<br />

experience<br />

Critical reflection<br />

Further education<br />

Pre-knowledge<br />

Motivation/Interest<br />

Background<br />

(social/cognitiv)<br />

Students<br />

Experience<br />

Learning processes<br />

Performance<br />

Expectation<br />

Motivation/interest<br />

01 2 3 4


Duit, Fischer, Klieme, Labudde, IPN,<br />

nwu<br />

Teacher<br />

Professional<br />

knowledge<br />

Practical experience<br />

Background<br />

Instruction<br />

School/family<br />

environment<br />

Planning and<br />

performing<br />

Practical<br />

experience<br />

Critical reflection<br />

Further education<br />

Pre-knowledge<br />

Motivation/Interest<br />

Background<br />

(social/cognitiv)<br />

Students<br />

Experience<br />

Learning processes<br />

Performance<br />

Expectation<br />

Motivation/interest<br />

01 2 3 4


Variables of instruction<br />

Teacher<br />

Cognition<br />

•PK<br />

•PCK<br />

•CK<br />

Attitudes<br />

• Motivation/Interest<br />

•Personality<br />

• Individual concepts<br />

Instruction<br />

Students<br />

Cognition<br />

• Knowledge, competencies<br />

• Learning processes<br />

• Behaviour<br />

Attitudes<br />

• Self-Efficacy<br />

• Motivation/Interest<br />

• Intelligence/Aptitude<br />

01 2 3 4


Including Instruction<br />

Surface structure (Duit, Fischer, Klieme,<br />

Labudde, Prenzel/Seidel, Reusser …)<br />

Culture specific patterns, no correlation with performance<br />

Deep structure (Duit, Fischer, Klieme, Labudde,<br />

Sumfleth, Tiberghien, Viiri …)<br />

Rating of quality of instruction<br />

Level of teachers‘ offer<br />

Level of cognitive activation of students<br />

Structure of subject matter<br />

Communication structure, argumentation<br />

01 2 3 4


Cognitive activation<br />

Learning processes<br />

Argumentation


Variables of instruction<br />

Teacher<br />

Cognition<br />

•PK<br />

•PCK<br />

•CK<br />

Attitudes<br />

• Motivation/Interest<br />

•Personality<br />

• Individual concepts<br />

Instruction<br />

Teaching and learning aims, intentions of the teacher<br />

lesson design and teaching methods<br />

Teacher’s behaviour, action, media<br />

Teacher-student interaction<br />

Students’ behaviour, action, media<br />

Content related operations, development of meaning<br />

learning paths, cognitive activation<br />

Students<br />

Cognition<br />

• Knowledge, competencies<br />

• Learning processes<br />

• Behaviour<br />

Attitudes<br />

• Self-Efficacy<br />

• Motivation/Interest<br />

• Intelligence/Aptitude<br />

01 2 3 4


Variables of instruction<br />

Teacher<br />

Instruction<br />

Students<br />

Cognition<br />

•PK<br />

•PCK<br />

•CK<br />

Teaching and learning aims, intentions of the teacher<br />

lesson design and teaching methods<br />

Teacher’s behaviour, action, media<br />

Teacher-student interaction<br />

Students’ behaviour, action, media<br />

Content related operations, development of meaning<br />

learning paths, cognitive activation<br />

Cognition<br />

• Knowledge, competencies<br />

• Learning processes<br />

• Behaviour<br />

Offer<br />

Attitudes<br />

• Motivation/Interest<br />

•Personality<br />

Mediation<br />

Use<br />

• Individual concepts<br />

Attitudes<br />

• Self-Efficacy<br />

• Motivation/Interest<br />

• Intelligence/Aptitude<br />

01 2 3 4


Methods


Surface and deep structure of instruction<br />

Surface Structure (low inferent coding)<br />

Teacher‘s behaviour, actions, media<br />

Students‘ behaviour, actions, media<br />

Teacher-student interactions<br />

…<br />

Deep Structure (high inferent coding)<br />

Teacher‘s aims and intentions<br />

Teacher‘s instructional design and methods<br />

Students‘ content related activities (cognitive activation)<br />

Students‘ learning path<br />

Interaction among students-teachers and students-students<br />

Structure of classroom talk<br />

…<br />

01 2 3 4


Developing a coding procedure<br />

1. Knowing the theoretical basis<br />

2. Developing a research question<br />

3. Producing data<br />

Video recording, Data compression, data<br />

segmentation, on-line or/and transcripts, …<br />

4. Applying the constructs of the model<br />

a. Determination of categories<br />

b. Determination of indicators<br />

• As clear, precise and extensive as possible<br />

• On the intended level of observation


Developing a coding procedure<br />

5. Modeling the transfer from categories to<br />

indicators<br />

− First order coding manual containing rules,<br />

categories, indicators and examples<br />

6. Testing the coding procedure<br />

−<br />

−<br />

−<br />

−<br />

Estimation of reliability<br />

Discussing problematic coding<br />

Revising the coding manual<br />

Until the coding procedure works reliable<br />

(or until you have to be satisfied because of the<br />

timetable or lacking of money)


Developing a coding procedure<br />

7. Application of the coding procedure to the<br />

video-recordings of interest<br />

8. Validity check by correlating external criteria<br />

if possible<br />

01 2 3 4


Example of a Coding Manual<br />

Category/Value Definition Description/Indicators Examples Dissociation<br />

No Instruction/<br />

Interruption<br />

Pre-Instruction<br />

Instruction<br />

01 2 3 4<br />

Refers to sequences in<br />

which no instruction<br />

takes place.<br />

Instruction already<br />

took place before and<br />

continued later.<br />

All intervals are coded<br />

in which instruction<br />

does not yet take<br />

place. The amount of<br />

pre-instruction intervals<br />

is an indicator for<br />

how much time was<br />

actually used for<br />

instruction.<br />

All intervals are coded<br />

in which instruction<br />

takes place. The<br />

amount of coded<br />

intervals is an indicator<br />

for how much time was<br />

actually used for<br />

instruction (time on<br />

task).<br />

The Interruption<br />

usually is marked by<br />

non-teaching and nonlearning<br />

activity.<br />

Typically such<br />

Interruptions take<br />

longer than a coding<br />

interval.<br />

Teacher did not yet<br />

start with instruction.<br />

At the most teacher<br />

and students prepare<br />

for instruction.<br />

No specific description:<br />

Instruction takes<br />

place.<br />

Important: All<br />

additional coding<br />

refers only to intervals<br />

coded as instruction.<br />

External event or<br />

disruption like announcements<br />

or door<br />

knocking. Teacher<br />

leaves class-room,<br />

students remain without<br />

instructions.<br />

Clarification of formal<br />

or organizational<br />

matter.<br />

Students arrive at the<br />

classroom after the<br />

bell, unpack their<br />

books, wait for the<br />

teacher.<br />

Teacher indicating the<br />

beginning of instruction:<br />

„Lets start now“.<br />

Students stand up at<br />

the beginning of the<br />

lesson.<br />

Noise or disciplinary<br />

problems do not<br />

generally count as<br />

interruption; the<br />

action must be<br />

different from what<br />

usually happens<br />

during instruction.<br />

Separation from<br />

Instruction, Pre-<br />

Instruction is<br />

coded, until the<br />

teacher starts<br />

instruction.<br />

Mind connection<br />

with pre- and postinstruction.<br />

Instruction only<br />

takes place one<br />

time and is embraced<br />

by pre- and<br />

post-instruction<br />

phases.<br />

Georg Trendel, Rainer Wackermann, nwu-essen


Example of a Coding Manual<br />

Category/Value Definition Description/Indicators Examples Dissociation<br />

Instruction<br />

All intervals<br />

are coded in<br />

which<br />

instruction<br />

takes place.<br />

The amount of<br />

coded intervals<br />

is an indicator<br />

for time on<br />

task.<br />

No specific<br />

description:<br />

Instruction<br />

takes place.<br />

Important: All<br />

additional<br />

coding refers<br />

only to<br />

intervals coded<br />

as instruction.<br />

Teacher<br />

indicating the<br />

beginning of<br />

instruction:<br />

„Lets start<br />

now“.<br />

Students stand<br />

up at the<br />

beginning of<br />

the lesson.<br />

Mind<br />

connection<br />

with preand<br />

postinstruction.<br />

Instruction<br />

only takes<br />

place one<br />

time and is<br />

embraced by<br />

pre- and<br />

postinstruction<br />

phases.<br />

01 2 3 4<br />

Georg Trendel, Rainer Wackermann, nwu-essen


Results<br />

01 2 3 4


Results of Large Scale Assessments<br />

01 2 3 4<br />

Performance & culture (TIMSS/PISA):<br />

Competences are distributed country specific<br />

Strong influences from out of school (e.g. social<br />

background)<br />

Reference to Instruction (TIMSS-Video study,<br />

COAKTIV)<br />

„Scripts“ are culture specific<br />

Structure of lessons is dependent on conceptions of the<br />

teacher<br />

PCK and CK of the teachers are very strong variables to<br />

predict students‘ performance (mathematics/PISA)<br />

Not clarified:<br />

Are specific scripts responsible for specific competences ?<br />

How does PCK and CK influence instruction?


Expectation<br />

Video analysis should track the effect from<br />

teachers’ professional knowledge and beliefs –<br />

action during the lessons – students’<br />

performance, knowledge and beliefs<br />

Video analysis as methodology to close the gap<br />

between teachers’ knowledge, students’<br />

assessment and quality of instruction<br />

01 2 3 4


Descriptive Analysis (coding)<br />

45,00<br />

Surface structure: modes of interaction<br />

0,00<br />

Mean period of time per lesson [min]<br />

40,00<br />

35,00<br />

30,00<br />

25,00<br />

20,00<br />

7,25<br />

5,18<br />

2,33 0,80<br />

1,43<br />

1,52<br />

16,69<br />

2,78<br />

1,63<br />

8,84<br />

1,98<br />

2,61<br />

12,57<br />

0,14<br />

2,27<br />

5,05<br />

0,63<br />

35,63<br />

0,25<br />

3,25<br />

0,55<br />

34,55<br />

15,00<br />

28,45 29,36<br />

26,68 26,34<br />

10,00<br />

5,00<br />

0,00<br />

18,13<br />

groupwork<br />

seatwork<br />

transition<br />

classroom discourse<br />

cross section course A course B course C course D course E course F<br />

01 2 3 4<br />

Reyer & Fischer, 2004


Descriptive Analysis (coding)<br />

Goals of the lessons<br />

Learning by experience<br />

18,54 min<br />

Concept development<br />

13,05 min<br />

Diverse<br />

6,77 min<br />

Top down<br />

4,45 min<br />

Reproduction<br />

4,40 min<br />

Activation/ performance<br />

control<br />

Problem solving<br />

4,24 min<br />

3,44 min<br />

Routine work<br />

1,57 min<br />

Discipline<br />

0,79 min<br />

Conceptual change<br />

0,42 min<br />

No interpretation<br />

0,05 min<br />

mean per lesson [min]<br />

01 2 3 4<br />

Reyer & Fischer, 2004


Conclusions Surface Analysis<br />

Two types of lessons<br />

Student oriented teaching including experimental work<br />

(short and indiscriminate tasks, experiments in groups)<br />

Teacher centered classroom discussions, demonstrating<br />

experiments (mostly short answers of the students)<br />

Rarely students’ lectures and home work<br />

No correlation between surface structure and<br />

TIMSS-test results (TIMSS-Video, TIMSS-R, nwu)<br />

Correlation between surface structure and<br />

motivation (TIMSS-R)<br />

01 2 3 4


Conclusions Deep Structure<br />

Fact, action and apply oriented<br />

Factual knowledge is partly worked out by students’<br />

guessing and teachers’ confirming the guessed word as<br />

being correct<br />

Action is planned in advance as step to solve a task<br />

(also experimental)<br />

Experience and concept development is applied<br />

incompletely<br />

Problem solving and conceptual change is applied very<br />

rarely<br />

01 2 3 4


Results<br />

Teacher level<br />

01 2 3 4


COAKTIV<br />

(Baumert, Blum, Mathematik)<br />

Teacher<br />

Professional<br />

knowledge<br />

Experience<br />

Background<br />

School background<br />

Lesson planning & -<br />

performing<br />

Practical<br />

experience<br />

Critical reflection<br />

Further education<br />

Instruction<br />

Pre-knowledge<br />

Motivation/interest<br />

Background<br />

(social/cognitiv)<br />

Students<br />

Lesson experiences<br />

Learning processes<br />

Performance<br />

Expectation<br />

Motivation/interest<br />

01 2 3 4


Intervention<br />

Training<br />

(effects)<br />

Teacher beliefs and ideas<br />

(effects)<br />

Interaction during lessons<br />

(teacher and students)<br />

(effects)<br />

Rainer Wackermann<br />

nwu<br />

Students perception,<br />

-emotion and<br />

performance<br />

01 2 3 4


Content of the training<br />

Types of teaching aims (Oser)<br />

Learning through experience<br />

Conceptual change<br />

Problem solving<br />

Conceptual development<br />

Training<br />

Hypertext learning (top down)<br />

Motility<br />

Reproduction<br />

activation/control of learning processes and<br />

performance<br />

Discipline<br />

01 2 3 4


Conclusion<br />

Teacher training was successful on all levels<br />

and for all teachers and classes<br />

Sub-group of teachers performed even better<br />

than the average<br />

Basic models are an effective strategy for<br />

teaching physics<br />

01 2 3 4


Results<br />

Lesson level<br />

01 2 3 4


Linking of content during lessons<br />

Analysis of biology, chemistry and physics<br />

lessons<br />

Questions<br />

In which way teachers are going to link content?<br />

On which level students use the offered oportunities<br />

to learn?<br />

Is there a correlation between the level of teachers‘<br />

offer and students‘ test results?<br />

01 2 3 4


Results biology<br />

Positive Effects of linkage regarding<br />

- test results<br />

- interest<br />

- willingness to make efforts<br />

Julia Wadouh<br />

Birgit Neuhaus<br />

Angela Sandmann<br />

01 2 3 4


Results chemistry<br />

Ina Glemnitz<br />

Elke Sumfleth<br />

Correlation between level of teachers offer and<br />

linking level of students‘ answers<br />

Correlation between linking level of students‘<br />

answers and the structure of their knowledge<br />

More than 70% of students‘ response were on a<br />

low level of linkage<br />

01 2 3 4


Results physics<br />

Correlation between linking level of teachers<br />

offer (difficulty) and linking level of students‘<br />

answers<br />

But: teachers tend to expect too much<br />

And: Students of high ranking teachers don‘t show<br />

better test results<br />

Anna Lau<br />

Knut Neumann<br />

Hans E. Fischer<br />

Conclusions: The difficulty (level of linkage)<br />

should be better adapted to students‘ abilities<br />

01 2 3 4


New research question<br />

Anna Lau<br />

Knut Neumann<br />

Alexander Kauertz<br />

Hans E. Fischer<br />

Elke Sumfleth<br />

Which variable is decisive for predicting students<br />

learning outcomes at school, verticale linkage or<br />

fit between level of teachers’ offer and level of<br />

students’ answer?<br />

01 2 3 4


Difficulty characteristics of tasks<br />

measuring competence in physics<br />

Conceptual and integrated thinking<br />

should lead to more complex answers<br />

Describing complexity by variing tasks accordingly<br />

Complexity as a measure to predict the difficulty of<br />

Structuring physics content by means of big<br />

ideas (basic concepts)<br />

Are German standards for physics able to structure physics<br />

content for middle school teaching?<br />

Is it possible to vary those structures using tasks?<br />

Are those tasks a measure of difficulties?<br />

Alexander Kauertz<br />

Hans E. Fischer<br />

01 2 3 4


Results<br />

Complexity correlates<br />

with diffulty<br />

Different basic<br />

concepts lead to<br />

different diffuculties<br />

The results are basis<br />

for a competence<br />

model which can<br />

predict students<br />

abilities


New research question<br />

Hendrik Notarp<br />

Alexander Kauertz<br />

Knut Neumann<br />

Hans E. Fischer<br />

Developing and validating a model for describing<br />

students‘ competencies in the three science<br />

subjects regarding content knowledge and NOS<br />

and SI


Perspective<br />

01 2 3 4


QuIP<br />

Quality of Instruction in Physics<br />

comparing Finland, Germany and Switzerland<br />

(Jouni Viiri, Hans E. Fischer, Peter Labudde)<br />

01 2 3 4


Important for quality of Instruction<br />

Time on task<br />

Information processing, cognitive structure,<br />

cognitive load<br />

Classroom interaction, teacher knowledge<br />

Students‘ performance<br />

Cognitive assimilation, strategies of learning<br />

and self regulated learning<br />

Student as co-producer of knowledge, vertical<br />

fit of cognitive concepts, cognitive activation


Important for quality of Instruction<br />

Time on task, structure of the lessons<br />

Information processing, cognitive structure,<br />

content structure, cognitive load<br />

Classroom interaction, teacher knowledge and<br />

beliefs<br />

Students‘ performance and motivation/interest<br />

Cognitive assimilation, strategies of learning<br />

and self regulated learning<br />

Student as co-producer of knowledge, vertical<br />

fit of cognitive concepts, cognitive activation


Design of the project<br />

Educational Background<br />

Family Background<br />

(OECD, 2002)<br />

Professional Knowledge<br />

(Schulman,1986)<br />

Teaching Enthusiasm<br />

(Kunter et al., 2005)<br />

Working Conditions<br />

Surface Structure<br />

Type of instruction and interaction<br />

Experimental groupwork ,.. (Seidel et al., 2006)<br />

Classroom<br />

Management<br />

Teacherbehavior<br />

Classroom Interaction<br />

(Scott & Mortimer,2005)<br />

Deep Structure<br />

CognitiveActivation<br />

(Lau et al., 2007;<br />

Trendel et al., 2007)<br />

Content Structure<br />

Bruckmann, 2007)<br />

Use of Experiments<br />

Tesch, 2007)<br />

Cognitive Abilities<br />

(Weiß, 2006)<br />

Competence<br />

(Kauertz, 2007)<br />

General Motivation<br />

(OECD, 2002)<br />

Situational Motivation<br />

(Decy & Ryan, 2000;<br />

Eccles, 2005)<br />

01 2 3 4<br />

Competence<br />

(Kauertz, 2007)<br />

Output<br />

Motivation<br />

(OECD, 2002)


Teacher<br />

Instruction<br />

Student<br />

Educational Background<br />

Family Background<br />

(OECD, 2002)<br />

Professional Knowledge<br />

(Schulman,1986)<br />

Teaching Enthusiasm<br />

(Kunter et al., 2005)<br />

Working Conditions<br />

Surface Structure<br />

Type of instruction and interaction<br />

Experimental groupwork ,.. (Seidel et al., 2006)<br />

Classroom<br />

Management<br />

Teacherbehavior<br />

Classroom Interaction<br />

(Scott & Mortimer,2005)<br />

Deep Structure<br />

CognitiveActivation<br />

(Lau et al., 2007;<br />

Trendel et al., 2007)<br />

Content Structure<br />

Bruckmann, 2007)<br />

Use of Experiments<br />

Tesch, 2007)<br />

Cognitive Abilities<br />

(Weiß, 2006)<br />

Competence<br />

(Kauertz, 2007)<br />

General Motivation<br />

(OECD, 2002)<br />

Situational Motivation<br />

(Decy & Ryan, 2000;<br />

Eccles, 2005)<br />

01 2 3 4<br />

Competence<br />

(Kauertz, 2007)<br />

Output<br />

Motivation<br />

(OECD, 2002)


General research perspective using<br />

video analysis<br />

Developing a model of quality of instruction for<br />

sciences<br />

Describing cause-effect relations by connecting the<br />

different levels of science teaching<br />

Describing and developing didactic competences of<br />

teachers using video analysis and video feedback<br />

Connecting field studies, laboratory studies,<br />

experimental studies and large scale assessments<br />

01 2 3 4


For further information<br />

WWW.nwu-duisburg-essen.de


Question<br />

Some colleagues tell that …<br />

Is this true?


Research question<br />

How does teacher enthusiasm influence<br />

students‘ motivation and interest in physics?<br />

Is enthusiasm represented as social-supportive<br />

or subject-supportive behavior or else?<br />

Is there an influence of social-supportive or<br />

subject-supportive teacher behavior on<br />

students‘ interest and/or motivation?


What do we need to know?<br />

Model of enthusiasm<br />

Which variables are influencing enthusiasm?<br />

Which variables should be controlled?<br />

When do we know?


Tracking<br />

Educational Background<br />

Family Background<br />

(OECD, 2002)<br />

Professional Knowledge<br />

(Schulman,1986)<br />

Teaching Enthusiasm<br />

(Kunter et al., 2005)<br />

Working Conditions<br />

Surface Structure<br />

Type of instruction and interaction<br />

Experimental groupwork (Seidel et al., 2006)<br />

Classroom<br />

Management<br />

Teacherbehavior<br />

Classroom Interaction<br />

(Scott & Mortimer,2005)<br />

Deep Structure<br />

CognitiveActivation<br />

(Lau et al., 2007;<br />

Trendel et al., 2007)<br />

Content Structure<br />

Bruckmann, 2007)<br />

Use of Experiments<br />

Tesch, 2007)<br />

Cognitive Abilities<br />

(Weiß, 2006)<br />

Competence<br />

(Kauertz, 2007)<br />

General Motivation<br />

(OECD, 2002)<br />

Situational Motivation<br />

(Decy & Ryan, 2000;<br />

Eccles, 2005)<br />

Competence<br />

(Kauertz, 2007)<br />

Output<br />

Motivation<br />

(OECD, 2002)


Tracking<br />

Educational Background<br />

Family Background<br />

(OECD, 2002)<br />

Professional Knowledge<br />

(Schulman,1986)<br />

Teachers‘ Enthusiasm<br />

(Kunter et al., 2005)<br />

Working Conditions<br />

Surface Structure<br />

Type of instruction and interaction<br />

Experimental groupwork (Seidel et al., 2006)<br />

Classroom<br />

Management<br />

Teacherbehavior<br />

Classroom Interaction<br />

(Scott & Mortimer,2005)<br />

Deep Structure<br />

CognitiveActivation<br />

(Lau et al., 2007;<br />

Trendel et al., 2007)<br />

Content Structure<br />

Bruckmann, 2007)<br />

Use of Experiments<br />

Tesch, 2007)<br />

Cognitive Abilities<br />

(Weiß, 2006)<br />

Competence<br />

(Kauertz, 2007)<br />

General Motivation<br />

(OECD, 2002)<br />

Situational Motivation<br />

(Decy & Ryan, 2000;<br />

Eccles, 2005)<br />

Competence<br />

(Kauertz, 2007)<br />

Output<br />

Motivation<br />

(OECD, 2002)


HLM<br />

Individual units on different aggregation levels or<br />

independent clusters<br />

There are structures of influence between hierarchic<br />

levels and also clusters to estimate the effect of the<br />

general context (culture)<br />

Multi level analysis allows to take into account group<br />

specific components of the mistakes<br />

The strategy leads to „correct estimated values“ of the<br />

mistakes regarding the sample cluster<br />

Variance of the regression coefficient is explained with<br />

characteristics of the aggregation unit


Path analysis<br />

More than one dependent variable<br />

Recursive model of variables<br />

Partial correlation<br />

Partial regression<br />

A B C D non recursive<br />

A B C D recursive

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!