27.02.2015 Views

Embodying who we are: Leader group prototypicality and leadership ...

Embodying who we are: Leader group prototypicality and leadership ...

Embodying who we are: Leader group prototypicality and leadership ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly 22 (2011) 1078–1091<br />

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect<br />

The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly<br />

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/leaqua<br />

<strong>Embodying</strong> <strong>who</strong> <strong>we</strong> <strong>are</strong>: <strong>Leader</strong> <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness<br />

Daan van Knippenberg<br />

Rotterdam School of Management, Erasmus University Rotterdam, PO Box 1738, 3000 DR Rotterdam, The Netherl<strong>and</strong>s.<br />

article info abstract<br />

Available online 22 October 2011<br />

Keywords:<br />

<strong>Leader</strong>ship categorization<br />

<strong>Leader</strong>ship effectiveness<br />

<strong>Leader</strong> fairness<br />

<strong>Leader</strong> <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong><br />

Social identity<br />

Social identification<br />

Trust<br />

Uncertainty<br />

<strong>Leader</strong>ship is a process enacted in the context of a sh<strong>are</strong>d <strong>group</strong> membership, <strong>and</strong> <strong>leadership</strong><br />

effectiveness is contingent on follo<strong>we</strong>rs' perceptions of the leader as a <strong>group</strong> member. Addressing<br />

this role of <strong>group</strong> membership, the social identity theory of <strong>leadership</strong> puts leader <strong>group</strong><br />

<strong>prototypicality</strong>, the extent to which the leader is perceived to embody <strong>group</strong> identity, centerstage<br />

in <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness. I review empirical research in leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong>,<br />

concluding there is a robust empirical basis for the key propositions of the social identity theory<br />

of <strong>leadership</strong>. I also identify ne<strong>we</strong>r developments that extend <strong>and</strong> enrich the social identity analysis<br />

of <strong>leadership</strong>, including attention to the roles of uncertainty, leader fairness, leader–follo<strong>we</strong>r<br />

relationship, leader self-perceived <strong>prototypicality</strong>, <strong>and</strong> <strong>leadership</strong> of creativity <strong>and</strong> innovation.<br />

© 2011 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.<br />

1. Introduction<br />

<strong>Leader</strong>ship is on the agenda of behavioral research for more than a hundred years. This is not surprising if <strong>we</strong> consider that<br />

<strong>leadership</strong> is such a natural <strong>and</strong> basic element of social <strong>group</strong>s. Indeed, it is hard to think of human <strong>group</strong>s without some form<br />

of <strong>leadership</strong> structure, even if only informally (cf. Halevy, Chou, & Galinsky, 2011). Moreover, typically no member of a social<br />

<strong>group</strong> – be it a work team, organization, nation, or other social <strong>group</strong>ing – is better positioned to impact the functioning of the<br />

<strong>group</strong> than a leader. Not surprisingly then, the question of what makes leaders effective in mobilizing <strong>and</strong> motivating follo<strong>we</strong>rs<br />

lies at the core to <strong>leadership</strong> research. To address this question <strong>leadership</strong> research has covered a wide range of influences on<br />

<strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness including leader personality (e.g., Judge,Bono,Ilies,&Gerhardt,2002), leader behavioral styles (e.g., Bass,<br />

1985; Conger & Kanungo, 1987; Judge, Piccolo, & Ilies, 2004), <strong>and</strong> social exchange relationships bet<strong>we</strong>en leaders <strong>and</strong> follo<strong>we</strong>rs<br />

(e.g., Graen & Uhl-Bien, 1995; Holl<strong>and</strong>er, 1958).<br />

In view of the fact that <strong>leadership</strong> is so inextricably embedded in <strong>group</strong>s, it is perhaps somewhat ironic that none of these<br />

perspectives on <strong>leadership</strong> really engages with the implications of the fact that leaders <strong>are</strong> also members of the <strong>group</strong>s they<br />

lead. <strong>Leader</strong>ship is a process that is enacted in the context of a <strong>group</strong> membership sh<strong>are</strong>d by leader <strong>and</strong> follo<strong>we</strong>rs — the President<br />

of the United States is an American citizen, the CEO is a member of the organization, the team leader is a member of the team (or<br />

at least of the same organization), <strong>and</strong> so forth. Accordingly, responses to <strong>leadership</strong> may be informed by characteristics of the<br />

leader as <strong>group</strong> member (e.g., <strong>who</strong> is the President as an American?) <strong>and</strong> an underst<strong>and</strong>ing of the psychology of <strong>group</strong> membership<br />

may be important to our underst<strong>and</strong>ing of <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness. This is the issue that assumes center-stage in the social identity<br />

theory of <strong>leadership</strong> (Hogg, 2001; Hogg & van Knippenberg, 2003; van Knippenberg & Hogg, 2003).<br />

In this review, I outline the social identity theory of <strong>leadership</strong> from its first conceptual articulation in Hogg (2001), via the integration<br />

of two independent streams of research by Hogg <strong>and</strong> colleagues (Fielding & Hogg, 1997; Hains, Hogg, & Duck, 1997; Hogg, Hains, &<br />

Mason, 1998) <strong>and</strong> van Knippenberg <strong>and</strong> colleagues (Platow & van Knippenberg, 2001; van Knippenberg & van Knippenberg, 2005a;<br />

van Knippenberg, van Knippenberg, & van Dijk, 2000) in the most full-blown articulation of the social identity theory of <strong>leadership</strong><br />

(van Knippenberg & Hogg, 2003), to more recent developments <strong>and</strong> extensions that took place after the publication of the 2003<br />

E-mail address: dvanknippenberg@rsm.nl.<br />

1048-9843/$ – see front matter © 2011 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.<br />

doi:10.1016/j.leaqua.2011.09.004


D. van Knippenberg / The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly 22 (2011) 1078–1091<br />

1079<br />

formulation of the theory. I only present a concise review of the earlier work in the social identity analysis of <strong>leadership</strong>, because such<br />

reviews can also be found elsewhere (Hogg & van Knippenberg, 2003; van Knippenberg & Hogg, 2003), <strong>and</strong> the current review prioritizes<br />

more recent empirical developments that took place after, or had a smaller presence in, the 2003 articulation of the theory.<br />

Core to the social identity theory of <strong>leadership</strong> is the proposition that leaders <strong>are</strong> more effective in mobilizing <strong>and</strong> influencing<br />

follo<strong>we</strong>rs the more they <strong>are</strong> seen as <strong>group</strong> prototypical — to embody what is defining of the <strong>group</strong> identity (Hogg, 2001). <strong>Leader</strong> <strong>group</strong><br />

<strong>prototypicality</strong> therefore is the unifying theme in this review, <strong>and</strong> the aim of this review is to take stock of the state of the science in<br />

empirical research in leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong>. This is not to say that the social identity theory is limited to the role of leader <strong>group</strong><br />

<strong>prototypicality</strong>. It is not, <strong>and</strong> also highlights such influences as leader <strong>group</strong>-oriented behavior (e.g., leader self-sacrifice; van Knippenberg<br />

& van Knippenberg, 2005a; cf.Platow, Haslam, Foddy, & Grace, 2003; Reicher, Haslam, & Hopkins, 2005). Such influences <strong>are</strong> not<br />

unique to the social identity analysis of <strong>leadership</strong>, though (cf. Lord & Brown, 2004; Shamir, House, & Arthur, 1993; van Knippenberg,<br />

van Knippenberg, De Cremer, & Hogg, 2004), <strong>and</strong> will be covered here mainly, or only, in relationship to leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong><br />

to highlight the unique contribution of the social identity theory of <strong>leadership</strong> to our underst<strong>and</strong>ing of <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness.<br />

This review is structured as follows. First, I briefly outline the basic propositions of the social identity theory of <strong>leadership</strong>. I follow<br />

this up with an updated review of the evidence for these basic propositions that also highlights <strong>are</strong>as where evidence may be sparser<br />

than ideally it would be. Subsequently, I review a series of more recent developments in the social identity analysis of <strong>leadership</strong>. In<br />

conclusion, I take stock of where this combined evidence leaves us in terms of the current state of our underst<strong>and</strong>ing of the role of<br />

leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> in <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness as <strong>we</strong>ll as in terms of an agenda for future research to develop this core aspect<br />

of the social identity theory of <strong>leadership</strong>.<br />

2. Social identity <strong>and</strong> leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong><br />

To underst<strong>and</strong> how the <strong>group</strong> membership sh<strong>are</strong>d by leader <strong>and</strong> follo<strong>we</strong>rs informs follo<strong>we</strong>rs' responses to <strong>leadership</strong> (i.e., <strong>and</strong><br />

thus <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness) it is important to realize that <strong>group</strong> memberships have identity implications. Group memberships<br />

reflect on how <strong>we</strong> see ourselves <strong>and</strong> others. Social identity captures this <strong>group</strong> membership-based aspect of identity or selfconception<br />

(identity <strong>and</strong> self can be used interchangeably here). It refers to that part of an individual's sense of self that is rooted<br />

in the individual's <strong>group</strong> membership — the part that gives people a sense of “<strong>we</strong>” <strong>and</strong> includes other members of the <strong>group</strong> in<br />

one's sense of self (Ashforth & Mael, 1989; Tajfel & Turner, 1986; Turner, Hogg, Oakes, Reicher, & Wetherell, 1987). This is not<br />

to say that all <strong>group</strong> memberships <strong>are</strong> equally self-defining for all people. The concept of social identification (i.e., including<br />

organizational identification, team identification, etc.; Ashforth & Mael, 1989; van Knippenberg & van Schie, 2000) captures the<br />

extent to which an individual defines the self in terms of a particular <strong>group</strong> membership. The higher the identification with a particular<br />

<strong>group</strong>, the more the individual sees the self in terms of the membership in that <strong>group</strong>, the more the individual has a sense<br />

of “<strong>we</strong>” connected to that <strong>group</strong> (i.e., rather than of “I” as a unique individual). The higher identification, the more the individual<br />

perceives the self in terms of characteristics that <strong>are</strong> typical of the <strong>group</strong>, <strong>and</strong> the more perceptions, attitudes, <strong>and</strong> behavior <strong>are</strong><br />

governed by the <strong>group</strong> membership (Hogg, 2003; Turner et al., 1987).<br />

Two influences associated with social identification in particular <strong>are</strong> relevant to an underst<strong>and</strong>ing of <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness.<br />

First, social identification leads one to experience the <strong>group</strong> identity as not only self-describing but also as self-guiding. Group<br />

identity includes a sh<strong>are</strong>d sense of social reality that reflects what the <strong>group</strong> believes, values, <strong>and</strong> sees as appropriate <strong>and</strong> important:<br />

it has a normative element to it, <strong>and</strong> this <strong>group</strong>-normative influence is internalized through a process of social identification.<br />

Put differently, through social identification <strong>group</strong> identity becomes a source of social influence — a source of information about<br />

social reality <strong>and</strong> about what is appropriate <strong>and</strong> desirable (Abrams & Hogg, 1990; Turner et al., 1987; van Knippenberg, 2000a).<br />

Second, because it implies seeing the self through the lens of <strong>group</strong> membership, social identification entails taking the <strong>group</strong>'s<br />

best interest to heart — indeed, experiencing the <strong>group</strong> interest as the self interest (i.e., the interest of an inclusive “<strong>we</strong>”, of the<br />

collective self; Ashforth & Mael, 1989; Turner et al., 1987; van Knippenberg, 2000b). These two influences provide an important<br />

link with responses to <strong>leadership</strong> as they <strong>are</strong> informed by social identification.<br />

Building on research in cognitive psychology (Rosch, 1978), the social identity approach outlines how social <strong>group</strong>s <strong>are</strong> mentally<br />

represented as <strong>group</strong> prototypes, fuzzy sets of characteristics that capture the characteristics that define the <strong>group</strong> (Hogg,<br />

2001; Turner et al., 1987). Group prototypes capture what is <strong>group</strong>-defining <strong>and</strong> in that sense represent the ideal-type of the<br />

<strong>group</strong> more than the <strong>group</strong> average. Translated to a hypothetical <strong>group</strong> member, the <strong>group</strong> prototypical <strong>group</strong> member would<br />

not be Joe Sixpack, but rather embody what is perceived to be the ideal-type of “<strong>who</strong> <strong>we</strong> <strong>are</strong>”. In this respect, it may also be important<br />

to note that prototypes <strong>are</strong> not restricted to – indeed do not even need to include – demographic attributes. Group prototypes<br />

capture the socially sh<strong>are</strong>d reality of the <strong>group</strong>, <strong>and</strong> thus include what the <strong>group</strong> values, believes, <strong>and</strong> considers<br />

important, <strong>and</strong> what <strong>are</strong> seen as appropriate <strong>and</strong> desirable behaviors <strong>and</strong> courses of action. In effect, <strong>group</strong> prototypes capture<br />

what is <strong>group</strong>-normative. The notion of <strong>group</strong> identity as a source of social influence can thus be rephrased as the <strong>group</strong> prototype<br />

as a source of social influence for those <strong>who</strong> identify with the <strong>group</strong>.<br />

2.1. <strong>Leader</strong> <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong><br />

Core to the social identity theory of <strong>leadership</strong> is the notion that <strong>group</strong> leaders, like all <strong>group</strong> members, differ in the extent to<br />

which they <strong>are</strong> perceived to represent or embody the <strong>group</strong> prototype — <strong>are</strong> <strong>group</strong> prototypical (i.e., social identity is individual<br />

self-definition, <strong>and</strong> the perception of <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> thus at its root is individual cognition even when such perceptions<br />

often <strong>are</strong> socially sh<strong>are</strong>d). To the extent that a leader is perceived to be <strong>group</strong> prototypical – that is, to embody the <strong>group</strong>


1080 D. van Knippenberg / The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly 22 (2011) 1078–1091<br />

(team, organizational) identity, to embody “<strong>who</strong> <strong>we</strong> <strong>are</strong>”–the leader derives influence from the implicit perception that he or she<br />

represents what is <strong>group</strong>-normative (Hogg, 2001; Hogg & van Knippenberg, 2003; van Knippenberg & Hogg, 2003; cf. van<br />

Knippenberg, 2000a; van Knippenberg, Lossie, & Wilke, 1994). In addition, leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> speaks to follo<strong>we</strong>rs' motivation<br />

to pursue the <strong>group</strong>'s interest. Because <strong>group</strong> prototypical leaders <strong>are</strong> perceived to embody the <strong>group</strong> identity <strong>and</strong> thus<br />

also what the <strong>group</strong> values <strong>and</strong> considers important, <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> induces trust in the leader's motivation to pursue the<br />

<strong>group</strong>'s best interest (cf. the benevolence aspect of trustworthiness; Mayer, Davis, & Schoorman, 1995) <strong>and</strong> therefore follo<strong>we</strong>r<br />

openness to the leader's influence (Giessner & van Knippenberg, 2008; van Knippenberg & Hogg, 2003; van Knippenberg &<br />

van Knippenberg, 2005a; cf. Dirks & Ferrin, 2002). <strong>Leader</strong>s should thus be more effective the more follo<strong>we</strong>rs perceive them to<br />

be <strong>group</strong> prototypical.<br />

For similar reasons, individuals should also be more likely to emerge as leaders the more they <strong>are</strong> <strong>group</strong> prototypical. Group<br />

<strong>prototypicality</strong> gives <strong>group</strong> members the influence to emerge as leaders (cf. van Knippenberg et al., 1994), <strong>and</strong> in interacting<br />

<strong>group</strong>s other <strong>group</strong> members may also implicitly convey this expectation to <strong>group</strong> prototypical members, in a sense “giving<br />

the floor” to them to emerge as leaders (cf. Ridgeway, 2003). While clearly the emphasis in research in the social identity theory<br />

of <strong>leadership</strong> has been on the effectiveness of individuals in <strong>leadership</strong> positions, there is also evidence supporting the role of<br />

<strong>group</strong> member <strong>prototypicality</strong> in emergent <strong>leadership</strong> (Fielding & Hogg, 1997; van Knippenberg et al., 2000).<br />

The influence of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> is tied to <strong>group</strong> identification. It is not unreasonable to assume that most members of<br />

most <strong>group</strong>s identify with the <strong>group</strong> at least to a modest degree. <strong>Leader</strong> <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> can thus be expected to be an influence<br />

in most if not all <strong>group</strong>s. Even so, the influence of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> will be stronger the more <strong>group</strong> members identify with<br />

the <strong>group</strong> <strong>and</strong> social identity is salient (Hogg, 2001). 1 Put differently, while all other things being equal leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong><br />

may have a main effect on <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness, the relationship bet<strong>we</strong>en leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness<br />

is also moderated by social identification: the higher the identification, the stronger the relationship bet<strong>we</strong>en <strong>prototypicality</strong> <strong>and</strong> effectiveness.<br />

This proposition too is <strong>we</strong>ll-supported (Cicero, Bonaiuto, Pierro & van Knippenberg, 2008; Fielding & Hogg, 1997; Giessner<br />

& van Knippenberg, 2008; Hains et al., 1997; Hirst, van Dick, & van Knippenberg, 2009; Hogg, Fielding, Johnson, Masser, Russell, &<br />

Svensson, 2006; Hogg et al., 1998; Pierro, Cicero, Bonaiuto, van Knippenberg, & Kruglanski, 2007; Platow & van Knippenberg, 2001;<br />

Ullrich, Christ, & van Dick, 2009; van Dijke & De Cremer, 2008, 2010).<br />

2.2. Group versus <strong>leadership</strong> prototypes<br />

The social identity theory proposes that <strong>group</strong> prototypes form a “benchmark” against which leaders <strong>are</strong> implicitly judged (i.e.,<br />

leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> is leader similarity to the <strong>group</strong> prototype). <strong>Leader</strong>ship research has also documented how (potential)<br />

leaders <strong>are</strong> judged against a <strong>leadership</strong> prototype — a mental representation of (good) <strong>leadership</strong> (i.e., <strong>leadership</strong> categorization<br />

theory; Lord, Foti, & DeVader, 1984; Lord & Maher, 1991; cf. Eagly & Karau, 2002). Given that these two perspectives <strong>are</strong><br />

rooted in the same cognitive-psychological research in categorization <strong>and</strong> prototypes (Rosch, 1978), a question that naturally<br />

emerged is how the two <strong>are</strong> related. The ans<strong>we</strong>r advanced by Hogg (2001) is that <strong>leadership</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>group</strong> prototypes <strong>are</strong> not mutually<br />

exclusive as benchmarks for responses to <strong>leadership</strong>, but that the relative importance of each is contingent on social identification<br />

(<strong>and</strong> social identity salience). The higher the identification, the more perceptions, attitudes, <strong>and</strong> actions <strong>are</strong> governed by<br />

social identity, the greater the importance of <strong>group</strong> prototypes relative to <strong>leadership</strong> prototypes in responses to <strong>leadership</strong>. This<br />

proposition is consistently supported in a series of studies (Fielding & Hogg, 1997; Hains et al., 1997; Hogg et al., 1998; Platow &<br />

van Knippenberg, 2001) to the app<strong>are</strong>nt satisfaction of researchers in both <strong>leadership</strong> categorization (Lord & Hall, 2003) <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> (van Knippenberg & Hogg, 2003) that have both embraced the proposition.<br />

<strong>Leader</strong>ship categorization theory also helps explain biases against certain demographic <strong>group</strong>s in assessments of <strong>leadership</strong><br />

qualities (cf. the “glass ceiling”). Eagly <strong>and</strong> Karau (2002) <strong>and</strong> Eagly <strong>and</strong> Carli (2007) outline how stereotypical beliefs about<br />

good <strong>leadership</strong> (cf. <strong>leadership</strong> prototypes) tend to emphasize stereotypically male characteristics much more than stereotypically<br />

female characteristics. Accordingly, in the “benchmarking” process described by <strong>leadership</strong> categorization theory there is a tendency<br />

to perceive greater <strong>leadership</strong> qualities in male than in female (potential) leaders. In an interesting <strong>and</strong> potentially<br />

important twist to this analysis, Hogg et al. (2006) propose that <strong>group</strong> (or organizational) prototypes may also be gendered <strong>and</strong><br />

thus inform responses to male <strong>and</strong> female leaders (cf. Gartzia, 2010). In an experiment, Hogg et al. comp<strong>are</strong>d perceptions of male<br />

<strong>and</strong> female leaders as a function of whether <strong>group</strong> norms (cf. prototypes) emphasized stereotypically masculine (“instrumental”)<br />

or stereotypically feminine (“expressive”) qualities. They argued that the match bet<strong>we</strong>en the gendered <strong>group</strong> norm <strong>and</strong> leader gender<br />

would render either male or female leaders more <strong>group</strong> prototypical <strong>and</strong> thus influence their perceived effectiveness accordingly.<br />

This is exactly what they observed contingent on two other factors they introduced into the design: social identity salience (i.e.,<br />

which would render <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> a more important influence in <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness) <strong>and</strong> the extent to which participants<br />

held traditional gender attitudes. The rationale for this latter influence is that gendered norms would only influence gender-<br />

1 The fact that one identifies with a <strong>group</strong> does not mean that this social identity is always salient (i.e., cognitively activated; Turner et al., 1987). Strictly speaking<br />

then, social identity salience is what triggers the influence of social identification: while identification is the more enduring factor, it is the more contextual<br />

influence of salience that activates the influence of identification (e.g., van Knippenberg, 2000b). The social identity analysis thus points to the role of social identity<br />

salience as <strong>we</strong>ll as of social identification (i.e., assuming at least a modest level of identification, social identity salience would moderate leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong><br />

effects), <strong>and</strong> this moderating role of salience has been supported in experimental studies (Hains et al., 1997; Hogg et al., 2006). Even so, the emphasis in<br />

research in the social identity analysis at large has been on identification rather than salience, <strong>and</strong> I likewise prioritize it here. Moreover, for the present purposes I<br />

discuss evidence pertaining to identification <strong>and</strong> to salience as interchangeable as they concern the same point: the influence of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> is<br />

rooted in social identity <strong>and</strong> thus obtains more strongly the more follo<strong>we</strong>rs respond to <strong>leadership</strong> on the basis of their social identity.


D. van Knippenberg / The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly 22 (2011) 1078–1091<br />

1081<br />

based perceptions of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> to the extent that people hold traditional gender attitudes that would associate men<br />

<strong>and</strong> women with these stereotypical traits. Results supported these predictions, showing that gendered <strong>group</strong> norms may<br />

render leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> contingent on leader gender.<br />

Because most organizations <strong>are</strong> male-dominated environments, stereotypically male characteristics may be more likely to be<br />

seen as <strong>group</strong> prototypical (Gartzia, 2010). Accordingly, in many organizations <strong>leadership</strong> prototypes <strong>and</strong> <strong>group</strong> prototypes may<br />

converge to introduce a bias in favor of male <strong>leadership</strong> (i.e., given the gendered nature of <strong>leadership</strong> prototypes; Eagly & Karau,<br />

2002). That is, it may not only be more general <strong>leadership</strong> prototypes that introduce biases against female <strong>leadership</strong>, but also more<br />

“local” <strong>group</strong> <strong>and</strong> organizational prototypes. Accordingly, addressing the glass ceiling may revolve as much around addressing local<br />

<strong>group</strong> prototypes as around addressing more societal stereotypes. This analysis begs the question of whether similar conclusions<br />

would hold for the difficulties facing other <strong>group</strong>s that <strong>are</strong> underrepresented in <strong>leadership</strong> positions such as cultural minorities. Do<br />

the prototypical representations of organizations also favor characteristics that <strong>are</strong> stereotypically associated more with the cultural<br />

majority <strong>group</strong> than with cultural minority <strong>group</strong>s? Further development of the social identity analysis along these lines may advance<br />

our underst<strong>and</strong>ing of the glass ceiling more generally.<br />

2.3. <strong>Leader</strong> <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>group</strong>-oriented behavior<br />

A core element in the str<strong>and</strong> of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> research championed by van Knippenberg <strong>and</strong> colleagues is that leader<br />

<strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> in part derives its effectiveness from the perception that <strong>group</strong> prototypical leaders can be trusted to pursue the<br />

<strong>group</strong>'s best interest (Giessner & van Knippenberg, 2008; Giessner, van Knippenberg, & Sleebos, 2009; van Knippenberg & Hogg,<br />

2003; van Knippenberg & van Knippenberg, 2005a). The implication of this is that to be effective nonprototypical leaders have to<br />

build follo<strong>we</strong>r trust in their <strong>group</strong>-oriented motives (cf. benevolence; Mayer et al., 1995) through their actions (i.e., engage in<br />

<strong>group</strong>-oriented behavior), whereas <strong>group</strong> prototypical leaders <strong>are</strong> effective whether they engage in <strong>group</strong>-oriented behavior or<br />

not. That is, leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> <strong>and</strong> leader <strong>group</strong>-oriented behavior interact to affect <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness.<br />

This prediction has been supported for a variety of instantiations of leader <strong>group</strong>-oriented behavior such as leader self-sacrifice on<br />

behalf of the <strong>group</strong> (van Knippenberg & van Knippenberg, 2005a; cf.Conger & Kanungo, 1987; Shamir et al., 1993), <strong>group</strong>-serving<br />

allocation decisions (Platow & van Knippenberg, 2001), <strong>and</strong> appeals to the collective interest rather than to follo<strong>we</strong>r self-interest<br />

(Platow, van Knippenberg, Haslam, van Knippenberg, & Spears, 2006; cf.Bass, 1985; House, 1971). Supporting the notion that<br />

these effects <strong>are</strong> rooted in social identity, Platow <strong>and</strong> van Knippenberg (2001) also confirmed that the interactive effect of leader<br />

<strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> <strong>and</strong> leader <strong>group</strong>-oriented behavior is stronger for follo<strong>we</strong>rs that identify more with the <strong>group</strong>.<br />

Extending this analysis, the social identity theory thus also posits that leader <strong>group</strong>-oriented behavior feeds into <strong>leadership</strong><br />

effectiveness, <strong>and</strong> more so for follo<strong>we</strong>rs identifying more strongly with the <strong>group</strong> (van Knippenberg & Hogg, 2003). These predictions<br />

too have been <strong>we</strong>ll-supported (e.g., Haslam & Platow, 2001; Platow, Hoar, Reid, Harley, & Morrison, 1997). This emphasis on<br />

<strong>group</strong>-oriented behavior also provides a clear linkage with theories of charismatic <strong>leadership</strong> that point to such behaviors as leader selfsacrifice<br />

as an aspect of charismatic <strong>leadership</strong> (Choi & Mai-Dalton, 1998; Conger & Kanungo, 1987; Shamir et al., 1993). Indeed, leader<br />

<strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> has been shown to affect perceptions of charisma (Platow et al., 2006; van Knippenberg & van Knippenberg,<br />

2005a), <strong>and</strong> as van Knippenberg <strong>and</strong> Hogg (2003) argue, here lies a clear opportunity for a more integrative treatment of the social<br />

identity analysis of <strong>leadership</strong> <strong>and</strong> analyses of charismatic <strong>leadership</strong> that accord a clear role to social identity even when they do<br />

not include a role for leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> (Shamir et al., 1993; cf.Conger, Kanungo, & Menon, 2000). Van Knippenberg <strong>and</strong><br />

Hogg (2003) proposed the outlines of such a theoretical integration, but future research will have to address these issues empirically<br />

to establish the potential of this integration.<br />

2.4. Evaluating the evidence<br />

Ever since its earliest tests in the 1990s, the social identity analysis of <strong>leadership</strong> has yielded remarkably robust evidence for<br />

the role of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> in <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness. In short, I am not aw<strong>are</strong> of a single study that does not report a<br />

positive influence of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> on <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness nor of a study that includes a negative influence.<br />

These findings hold across methods <strong>and</strong> populations, indicators of <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness, <strong>and</strong> countries.<br />

Evidence for the key propositions of the social identity theory of <strong>leadership</strong> as <strong>we</strong>ll as its ne<strong>we</strong>r developments derives from<br />

laboratory experiments, scenario experiments (i.e., assessing responses to hypothetical situations), <strong>and</strong> surveys in the field.<br />

Indeed, several studies tested the same hypotheses across methodologies, thus establishing that findings cannot be attributed<br />

to any specific aspect of sample or methodology <strong>and</strong> establishing that the relationships may be observed for <strong>leadership</strong> in organizations<br />

as <strong>we</strong>ll as establishing causality in these relationships through experimental manipulations (De Cremer, van Dijke, &<br />

Mayer, 2010; Giessner & van Knippenberg, 2008; Giessner et al., 2009; Pierro, Cicero, & Higgins, 2009; Ullrich, Christ, & van<br />

Dick, 2009; van Knippenberg & van Knippenberg, 2005a).<br />

Noteworthy in this respect is that most of the field studies in leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> relied on the measure of leader<br />

<strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> advanced by van Knippenberg <strong>and</strong> van Knippenberg (2005a) while much of the experimental research<br />

used items of this measure as manipulation check (e.g., Giessner & van Knippenberg, 2008; Ullrich et al., 2009; van Knippenberg<br />

& van Knippenberg, 2005a). This use of the same measurement across methods bolsters the confidence that experimental <strong>and</strong><br />

survey findings revolve around the same relationships. As an aside, it also establishes the van Knippenberg <strong>and</strong> van Knippenberg<br />

measure as the most validated measure of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong>, in that it has been shown to be affected by experimental<br />

manipulations designed to target leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> specifically, has consistently been associated with the outcomes


1082 D. van Knippenberg / The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly 22 (2011) 1078–1091<br />

proposed by the social identity theory of <strong>leadership</strong>, <strong>and</strong> has been shown to be empirically distinct from other <strong>leadership</strong> variables<br />

studied in conjunction. De Cremer et al. (2010) for instance reported that the van Knippenberg <strong>and</strong> van Knippenberg scale is empirically<br />

distinct from transformational <strong>leadership</strong> (Bass & Avolio, 1995), leader–member exchange (Sc<strong>and</strong>ura & Graen, 1984), <strong>and</strong> leader–team<br />

similarity (cf. Ensher & Murphy, 1997). Even so, construct validation should be a continuous <strong>and</strong> ongoing process (Schriesheim &<br />

Cogliser, 2009) <strong>and</strong> future studies would ideally provide further evidence of the validity of the van Knippenberg <strong>and</strong> van Knippenberg<br />

measure (as <strong>we</strong>ll as potentially introduce further refinements of the measure).<br />

The evidence in favor of the effects of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> also derives from a variety of indicators of <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness:<br />

performance (van Knippenberg & van Knippenberg, 2005a,b), creativity (Hirst et al., 2009), perceived <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness<br />

(e.g., Giessner & van Knippenberg, 2008; Hains et al., 1997; van Knippenberg & van Knippenberg, 2005a), <strong>leadership</strong><br />

endorsement (Platow & van Knippenberg, 2001; Ullrich et al., 2009), job satisfaction (e.g., Cicero, Pierro & van Knippenberg,<br />

2010; Cicero et al., 2008; Pierro et al., 2005), turnover intentions (Cicero et al., 2010; Pierro et al., 2005), self-reported work effort<br />

(Cicero et al., 2008; Moriano León, Topa Cantisano, & Lévy Mangin, 2009; Pierro et al., 2005), support for organizational change<br />

(Pierro et al., 2007; van Knippenberg & van Knippenberg, 2005a; van Knippenberg, van Knippenberg, & Bobbio, 2008), <strong>and</strong> cooperation<br />

intentions <strong>and</strong> organizational citizenship behavior (De Cremer et al., 2010).<br />

In this respect, it is also important to note that this support for the conclusion that leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> has a positive<br />

effect on <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness is not limited to subjective perceptions of <strong>leadership</strong> <strong>and</strong> other self-rated outcomes that need to<br />

be treated with some suspicion when it comes to judging <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness (cf. Lord & Maher, 1991; van Knippenberg,<br />

2012). This evidence also includes multi-source relationships with follo<strong>we</strong>r <strong>and</strong> organizational performance (arguably the<br />

most important indicator of <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness; Kaiser, Hogan, & Craig, 2008), creativity, <strong>and</strong> organizational citizenship<br />

behavior.<br />

Evidence also derives from a range of countries spanning several continents: Australia (e.g., Fielding & Hogg, 1997; Hains et al.,<br />

1997; Hogg et al., 1998), El Salvador (Moriano León et al., 2009), Finl<strong>and</strong> (Lipponen, Koivista & Olkkonen, 2005), Germany (Giessner<br />

et al., 2009; Janson, Levy, Sitkin, & Lind, 2008; Ullrich et al., 2009), India (Janson et al., 2008), Italy (e.g., Cicero et al., 2007; Pierro et al.,<br />

2005, 2007), New Zeal<strong>and</strong> (Janson et al., 2008), The Netherl<strong>and</strong>s (e.g., Giessner & van Knippenberg, 2008; van Dijke & De Cremer,<br />

2008; van Knippenberg & van Knippenberg, 2005a), Nicaragua (Moriano León et al., 2009), S<strong>we</strong>den (Hirst et al., 2009), the UK<br />

(Hirst et al., 2009), <strong>and</strong> the US (e.g., De Cremer et al., 2010; Hirst et al., 2009; Janson et al., 2008). Those able to read Japanese may<br />

find that leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> also predicts <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness in Japan (Sakata, Fujimoto, & Kohguchi, 2005).<br />

Moreover, a couple of these studies included multinational samples <strong>and</strong> could thus establish that there <strong>we</strong>re no country influences<br />

(Hirst et al., 2009; Janson et al., 2008; Moriano León et al., 2009) or could to the same effect replicate findings across samples<br />

from different countries (De Cremer et al., 2010; Giessner & van Knippenberg, 2008; Janson et al., 2008). The current<br />

evidence would thus suggest that the role of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> in <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness is quite universal. Even so,<br />

this may not hold for all the nuances of the <strong>prototypicality</strong>–effectiveness relationship. Thus, even though the available evidence<br />

points to a universal influence (i.e., within the limits of the countries covered by empirical research), <strong>we</strong> should be open to new<br />

theory <strong>and</strong> evidence suggesting cultural variations in the ways leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> may affect <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness.<br />

Based on the accumulated evidence, then, it seems a safe conclusion that leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> has a positive effect on<br />

<strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness. This influence is moderated by follo<strong>we</strong>r social identification <strong>and</strong> leader <strong>group</strong>-oriented behavior <strong>and</strong> obtains<br />

across methodologies, indicators of <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness, <strong>and</strong> countries. There thus is solid evidence for the basic propositions<br />

of the social identity theory of <strong>leadership</strong>. In the following section, I review more recent extensions <strong>and</strong> emerging<br />

developments of the analysis of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong>.<br />

3. Extensions <strong>and</strong> emerging developments<br />

3.1. Uncertainty reduction<br />

Group prototypes as a source of information about social reality may also fulfill an important uncertainty-reducing function. Hogg<br />

(2000, 2007) outlines how uncertainty is an aversive psychological state that many people desire to reduce, <strong>and</strong> how social identity is<br />

important in this respect. By representing a sh<strong>are</strong>d social reality, social identities offer <strong>group</strong> members a means to reduce uncertainty<br />

by relying on their <strong>group</strong> membership. What this also means is that the greater people's desire to reduce uncertainty, the more they<br />

will fall back on their <strong>group</strong> memberships (i.e., provided they identify with the <strong>group</strong>). Extending this to the analysis of <strong>leadership</strong><br />

effectiveness, a desire to reduce uncertainty would render follo<strong>we</strong>rs more sensitive to leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong>, as the quality<br />

of embodying a sh<strong>are</strong>d social reality <strong>and</strong> <strong>group</strong> norms would be more important to follo<strong>we</strong>rs the more they desire to reduce uncertainty.<br />

A series of studies by Pierro <strong>and</strong> colleagues consistently support this proposition.<br />

In a first study, Pierro et al. (2005) focused on dispositional differences in the need for cognitive closure (Kruglanski & Webster,<br />

1996; Webster & Kruglanski, 1994). Need for closure reflects intolerance for uncertainty <strong>and</strong> ambiguity <strong>and</strong> a desire to reduce uncertainty<br />

<strong>and</strong> reach closure on judgments, decisions, <strong>and</strong> actions. As an individual difference variable, it thus perfectly captures individual<br />

differences in the desire to reduce uncertainty that <strong>are</strong> proposed to moderate the influence of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong>. This was<br />

exactly what Pierro et al. found. In a second study, Pierro et al. (2007) sho<strong>we</strong>d that the moderating effect of need for closure was<br />

further moderated by identification, thus more firmly establishing the social identity basis of the reliance on leader <strong>prototypicality</strong><br />

inspired by need for closure. The desire to reduce uncertainty as captured by need for closure can also be situationally induced.<br />

Thus, extending the need for closure analysis Cicero et al. (2007, 2010) sho<strong>we</strong>d that more situational influences that can be


D. van Knippenberg / The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly 22 (2011) 1078–1091<br />

1083<br />

understood to invite a desire for uncertainty-reduction (stress <strong>and</strong> role ambiguity) also moderate the leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong>–<br />

<strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness relationship.<br />

From the perspective of <strong>leadership</strong> in organizations, organizational change is probably one of the major situational influences<br />

on uncertainty, <strong>and</strong> here in particular would one expect leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> to be important. Van Knippenberg et al.<br />

(2008) discuss initial evidence that this indeed is the case. They sho<strong>we</strong>d that more prototypical leaders invite more willingness<br />

to actively contribute to successful organizational change, especially when the change is associated with greater uncertainty<br />

about its implications for the organization. Van Knippenberg et al. link this influence in particular to the perceived threat to<br />

the continuity of the organizational identity. As embodiment of the sh<strong>are</strong>d identity, <strong>group</strong> prototypical leaders <strong>are</strong> trusted<br />

more to act as an agent of continuity of identity in times of change, <strong>and</strong> therefore more effective in eliciting support for the change<br />

especially with greater uncertainty about continuity of identity.<br />

This suggests (albeit tentatively given the current scarcity of supportive evidence) that <strong>group</strong> prototypical leaders may not<br />

only be able to cater to uncertainty-reduction desires that may arise from reasons external to the leader (e.g., dispositional<br />

need for closure), but may also be able to elicit greater tolerance for inherently uncertain enterprises leaders themselves may<br />

engender like organizational change. Put differently, leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> may elicit the necessary trust to take follo<strong>we</strong>rs<br />

on more challenging <strong>and</strong> uncertain endeavors, <strong>and</strong> may thus be an important influence in successful change <strong>and</strong> innovation (also<br />

see below). In short, while the moderating role of the desire to reduce uncertainty seems relatively <strong>we</strong>ll-established, the analysis<br />

in terms of the uncertainty-reduction motive also has less explored but potentially important implications that warrant further<br />

research efforts.<br />

3.2. <strong>Leader</strong> fairness<br />

<strong>Leader</strong>s <strong>are</strong> important sources of outcomes to follo<strong>we</strong>rs. <strong>Leader</strong>s make decisions regarding promotions, tenure, educational<br />

opportunities, job assignments, resources, <strong>and</strong> so forth. Inevitably not all these decisions can yield outcomes favorable to all follo<strong>we</strong>rs,<br />

<strong>and</strong> the perceived fairness of the leader in coming to these decisions can be a salient consideration to follo<strong>we</strong>rs <strong>and</strong> an<br />

influence on <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness. Accordingly, following justice theory (Colquitt, Conlon, Wesson, Porter, & Ng, 2001; Lind<br />

& Tyler, 1988; Rupp, 2011; Thibaut & Walker, 1975), <strong>leadership</strong> research has documented how leader fairness may positively contribute<br />

to <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness (van Knippenberg, De Cremer & van Knippenberg, 2007a). More recently, research also focused<br />

on the interactive influence of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> <strong>and</strong> leader fairness.<br />

Lind (2001) outlines how fair treatment by authorities has an important function in reducing uncertainty about <strong>and</strong> instilling<br />

trust in the authority's good intentions. This provides a clear link with the role of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> in reducing uncertainty<br />

<strong>and</strong> engendering trust, <strong>and</strong> a couple of studies have advanced <strong>and</strong> tested the hypothesis that leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> leader fairness interact such that the one make follo<strong>we</strong>rs less concerned with the other — leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> renders<br />

leader effectiveness less contingent on leader fairness, or conversely leader fairness renders <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness less<br />

contingent on leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong>.<br />

Janson et al. (2008) tested this prediction for both leader procedural fairness (the fairness of the procedures used to arrive at outcomes;<br />

Lind & Tyler, 1988; Thibaut & Walker, 1975) <strong>and</strong> leader interactional fairness (the fairness of interpersonal treatment as evident<br />

in the respect <strong>and</strong> dignity with which one is treated; Bies & Moag, 1986) <strong>and</strong> found support for the proposed <strong>prototypicality</strong> by<br />

fairness interactions. van Knippenberg, van Knippenberg, <strong>and</strong> De Cremer (2005) found support for the <strong>prototypicality</strong> by procedural<br />

fairness interaction across an experiment (including effects on a behavioral measure of cooperation), a scenario-experiment, <strong>and</strong> a<br />

survey, but did not find support for a similar interaction with interactional fairness. Ullrich et al. (2009) extended these findings<br />

for procedural fairness by showing that the interaction was further moderated by follo<strong>we</strong>r social identification.<br />

The underlying logic here is that both leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> <strong>and</strong> leader procedural fairness increase trust in the leader's<br />

good intentions vis-à-vis the follo<strong>we</strong>rs. While the linkage bet<strong>we</strong>en <strong>prototypicality</strong> <strong>and</strong> trust was already established independent<br />

of fairness (van Knippenberg & van Knippenberg, 2005a), extending this logic van Dijke <strong>and</strong> De Cremer (2008, 2010) predicted<br />

<strong>and</strong> found that leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> would also raise perceptions of the leader's procedural fairness following from this<br />

trust, especially for follo<strong>we</strong>rs that identify more strongly with the collective. Consistent with the mutually attenuating influence<br />

of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> <strong>and</strong> leader fairness, ho<strong>we</strong>ver, one could then also raise the question of whether leader fairness<br />

would raise the perception of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong>, <strong>and</strong> Kalshoven <strong>and</strong> Den Hartog (2009) present evidence to that effect<br />

(cf. Janson et al.'s, 2008, notion of <strong>leadership</strong> heuristics). Extending this logic <strong>and</strong> consistent with the social identity analysis more<br />

broadly, <strong>we</strong> may then also expect that leader <strong>group</strong>-oriented behavior <strong>and</strong> leader fairness interact to similar effect, <strong>and</strong> this<br />

indeed has been shown to be the case (De Cremer & van Knippenberg, 2002; Janson et al., 2008).<br />

Other evidence suggests, ho<strong>we</strong>ver, that the issue is more complex where the interactive influence of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> leader fairness is concerned. Lipponen et al. (2005) found that leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> enhances rather than attenuates the<br />

relationship bet<strong>we</strong>en leader fairness <strong>and</strong> follo<strong>we</strong>rs' self-perceived social st<strong>and</strong>ing (i.e., pride <strong>and</strong> respect; Tyler & Blader, 2000). The<br />

issue here might be that the dependent variable is social self-evaluation <strong>and</strong> not <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness. Procedural fairness not only<br />

conveys the authority's trustworthiness (Lind, 2001). The treatment the authority chooses to give to the recipient also conveys a<br />

social evaluation: more fair treatment conveys a more positive social evaluation. As a result, fairness of treatment has a positive effect<br />

on self-evaluations of the recipient of the treatment (Koper, van Knippenberg, Bouhuijs, Vermunt, & Wilke, 1993). Whereas leader<br />

<strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> may render <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness less contingent on leader fairness by addressing concerns with leader<br />

trustworthiness that would otherwise be contingent on leader fairness, leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> may enhance the impact of leader<br />

fairness on social self-evaluations by increasing the value attached to the social evaluation by the leader.


1084 D. van Knippenberg / The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly 22 (2011) 1078–1091<br />

Probably related to this issue <strong>are</strong> findings by De Cremer et al. (2010) <strong>who</strong> studied the combined effect of leader <strong>group</strong><br />

<strong>prototypicality</strong>, leader procedural fairness toward the individual, <strong>and</strong> leader procedural fairness toward others on cooperative intentions<br />

<strong>and</strong> organizational citizenship behavior. They found that when others <strong>we</strong>re treated fairly, leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> enhanced<br />

the effect of leader fairness to self. One reading of these findings which seem at odds with the Janson et al. (2008), Ullrich et al. (2009),<br />

<strong>and</strong> van Knippenberg et al. (2005) findings, is that the comparison to others' treatment rendered the social evaluation implications of<br />

leader fairness more salient. Unfair treatment of self in the face of fair treatment to others can be interpreted as being discriminated<br />

against, which presumably makes social evaluation a particularly salient concern. Consistent with the current reading of the Lipponen<br />

et al. (2005) findings, this in turn would invite an enhancing rather than an attenuating effect of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong>. Consistent<br />

with this reading (i.e., rather than a trustworthiness reading) is the fact that it was not the inconsistency in treatment that<br />

seemed to fuel stronger responses to treatment of self (i.e., which would also imply more negative reactions to fair treatment of<br />

self when others <strong>we</strong>re treated unfairly), but the fact that others <strong>we</strong>re treated fairly (i.e., which would single self out for discrimination<br />

when treated unfairly).<br />

What this research in combination puts on the agenda then, is the finding that leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> may attenuate as<br />

<strong>we</strong>ll as enhance the effects of leader (procedural) fairness. I propose that the underlying issue here is whether concerns with<br />

leader trustworthiness (attenuating effect) or with social evaluations by the leader (enhancing effect) <strong>are</strong> more salient in<br />

follo<strong>we</strong>rs' mind, but clearly this proposition currently awaits a direct test. Such a test could include different dependent variables<br />

(e.g., follo<strong>we</strong>r performance vs. follo<strong>we</strong>r self-evaluation) as <strong>we</strong>ll as contingencies of the relative concern with leader trustworthiness<br />

versus social st<strong>and</strong>ing.<br />

3.3. The romance of <strong>leadership</strong><br />

Seminal work by Lord (e.g., Lord, Binning, Rush, & Thomas, 1978) <strong>and</strong> Meindl (e.g., Meindl, Ehrlich, & Dukerich, 1985) established<br />

that people have a tendency to overattribute collective performance (i.e., of the team or organization) to <strong>leadership</strong>, especially<br />

performance that st<strong>and</strong>s out as being a clear success or failure. People have a tendency to see successful team or organizational<br />

performance as a sign of good <strong>leadership</strong> <strong>and</strong> poor performance as indicative of bad <strong>leadership</strong> <strong>and</strong> more so than would be justified<br />

on the basis of the evidence (e.g., bad performance could also be due to circumstances; under some conditions the best <strong>leadership</strong><br />

could hope to achieve is mediocre performance, etc.). Meindl et al. (1985) dubbed the underlying issue here the “romance of <strong>leadership</strong>”,<br />

arguing that people <strong>are</strong> more ready than is objectively justified to attribute events to <strong>leadership</strong> (i.e., as opposed to other influences such<br />

as the situation).<br />

Giessner <strong>and</strong> colleagues (Giessner & van Knippenberg, 2008; Giessner et al., 2009) noted that the research on <strong>leadership</strong> perceptions<br />

as a function of leader or collective performance revolved around uninvolved “observers” <strong>and</strong>raisedthequestionof<br />

how people would respond to <strong>leadership</strong> after success or failure if it concerned their leader <strong>and</strong> the success or failure of their<br />

team or organization. They argued that leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> would be an important influence here, because leader<br />

<strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> would in a sense give leaders a “license to fail”: trust in their <strong>leadership</strong> would color perceptions of<br />

<strong>group</strong> prototypical leaders' performance such that poor performance would reflect less on the evaluations of their <strong>leadership</strong><br />

than for less prototypical leaders. Across different methodologies, Giessner et al. (2009) found support for this prediction,<br />

<strong>and</strong> sho<strong>we</strong>d that trust in <strong>leadership</strong> mediated the more positive responses to prototypical as comp<strong>are</strong>d with nonprototypical<br />

leaders following failure.<br />

Giessner <strong>and</strong> van Knippenberg (2008) further sho<strong>we</strong>d that this influence of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> is contingent on the<br />

nature of the performance goal. Some goals <strong>are</strong> associated with a categorical difference bet<strong>we</strong>en success <strong>and</strong> failure whereas<br />

others <strong>are</strong> associated with more gradual differences (Brendl & Higgins, 1996). The latter goals leave more room for relatively<br />

favorable interpretations of failure to reach goals than the former, <strong>and</strong> accordingly leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> was expected<br />

<strong>and</strong> found to only moderate the effect of failure (vs. success) for the latter type of goal. Giessner <strong>and</strong> van Knippenberg also established<br />

the moderating role of social identification in this effect, thus more firmly establishing its roots in social identity.<br />

Findings such as these regarding leaders' ability to maintain support for their <strong>leadership</strong> after poor performance <strong>are</strong> potentially<br />

important because they speak to leaders' ability to pursue endeavors that inherently <strong>are</strong> associated with a higher risk of failure<br />

such as radical innovations. They potentially also speak to a “darker” side of <strong>leadership</strong> endorsement, as they also point to situations<br />

in which leaders continue to be supported even when a change in <strong>leadership</strong> might be more advisable. These, ho<strong>we</strong>ver, <strong>are</strong> issues for<br />

future research to address.<br />

Vie<strong>we</strong>d from a slightly different angle, these findings for responses to <strong>leadership</strong> after failure also add to our underst<strong>and</strong>ing of<br />

the role of trust in <strong>group</strong> prototypical leaders more generally. Originally, trust in <strong>group</strong> prototypical leaders was advanced as a<br />

mechanism underlying the interactive effects of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> <strong>and</strong> leader <strong>group</strong>-oriented behavior – <strong>and</strong> by implication<br />

the main effect of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> – on <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness (van Knippenberg & Hogg, 2003). The more<br />

recent evidence revie<strong>we</strong>d here thus suggest that this role of trust extends to interactive effects of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> leader procedural fairness (i.e., provided these revolve around concerns with trustworthiness <strong>and</strong> not with social evaluation)<br />

<strong>and</strong> leaders' association with failure. Trust in <strong>group</strong> prototypical <strong>leadership</strong> thus also seems to extend to trust in leader procedural<br />

fairness (cf. van Dijke & De Cremer, 2008) <strong>and</strong> trust in leaders' ability to ensure performance outcomes important to the collective.<br />

Interestingly, these distinctions seem to map unto the different aspects of trustworthiness distinguished in trust theory<br />

(Mayer et al., 1995): benevolence (cf. trust in leader <strong>group</strong>-orientedness), integrity (cf. trust in leader fairness), <strong>and</strong> ability (cf.<br />

trust in leaders after failure) — an issue I revisit later.


D. van Knippenberg / The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly 22 (2011) 1078–1091<br />

1085<br />

3.4. <strong>Leader</strong>–follo<strong>we</strong>r relationships<br />

The social identity theory of <strong>leadership</strong> clearly champions an underst<strong>and</strong>ing of <strong>leadership</strong> as a <strong>group</strong>-based process. While this is a<br />

perspective that resonates with analyses of charismatic <strong>and</strong> transformational <strong>leadership</strong> (Shamir et al., 1993; van Knippenberg et al.,<br />

2004;cf.van Knippenberg & Hogg, 2003), there is also a perspective on <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness that emphasizes an underst<strong>and</strong>ing of<br />

<strong>leadership</strong> as a dyadic process unfolding in the interpersonal relationship bet<strong>we</strong>en leader <strong>and</strong> follo<strong>we</strong>r. This leader–member<br />

exchange (LMX) perspective puts the social exchange relationship bet<strong>we</strong>en leader <strong>and</strong> follo<strong>we</strong>r center-stage <strong>and</strong> sees <strong>leadership</strong><br />

effectiveness as following from the quality of this exchange relationship (Graen & Uhl-Bien, 1995; Schriesheim, Castro, & Cogliser,<br />

1999; van Gils, van Quaquebeke, & van Knippenberg, 2010). In the social exchange perspective, relationship quality is understood<br />

as a combination of the value to each party of the “goods” exchanged in the relationship (e.g., effort, loyalty, friendship) <strong>and</strong> the<br />

reciprocity in the exchange — benefits received should be repaid in kind (Blau, 1964; Gouldner, 1960; Rhoades & Eisenberger, 2002).<br />

There is good support for LMX theory's basic proposition that the quality of the social exchange relationships bet<strong>we</strong>en leader<br />

<strong>and</strong> follo<strong>we</strong>r is associated with outcomes that can be seen as indicative of <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness (Gerstner & Day, 1997). The<br />

question thus arises if <strong>and</strong> how these different perspectives on <strong>leadership</strong> may be aligned as an emphasis on sh<strong>are</strong>d identity as<br />

comp<strong>are</strong>d with an emphasis on reciprocity in social exchange represent quite different psychological experiences of the leader–<br />

follo<strong>we</strong>r relationship (cf. van Knippenberg & Sleebos, 2006). Addressing this question, Hogg, Martin, Epitropaki, Mankad, Svensson,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Weeden (2005) pointed to social identification (or social identity salience) as the key moderating variable here. They sho<strong>we</strong>d<br />

that whether <strong>leadership</strong> with an interpersonal focus (cf. LMX) or <strong>leadership</strong> with a <strong>group</strong>-based focus (cf. the social identity analysis)<br />

was more strongly associated with <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness was contingent on identification. The higher follo<strong>we</strong>r social identification,<br />

the more effective <strong>group</strong>-based <strong>leadership</strong> relative to interpersonal <strong>leadership</strong>.<br />

Bringing leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> into the equation here, in a conceptual analysis Sluss <strong>and</strong> Ashforth (2008) advanced the<br />

proposition that leader <strong>group</strong> (i.e., organizational) <strong>prototypicality</strong> would moderate the extent to which the quality of the leader–<br />

follo<strong>we</strong>r relationship would transfer to the follo<strong>we</strong>r's relationship with the organization (e.g., in terms of organizational identification;<br />

cf. Sluss & Ashforth, 2007). Their basic argument is that the more the leader is seen to embody the organizational identity,<br />

the more one's relationship with the leader is seen as indicative of one's relationship with the organization (cf. van Knippenberg,<br />

van Dick & Tav<strong>are</strong>s, 2007b). In an empirical follow-up Sluss, Ployhart, <strong>and</strong> Cobb (2010) provide support for this proposition by<br />

showing that the relationship bet<strong>we</strong>en follo<strong>we</strong>r identification with the leader <strong>and</strong> with the organization is contingent on leader<br />

<strong>group</strong> (organizational) <strong>prototypicality</strong>. Although not framed as such, a study by Eisenberger et al. (2010) provides another test of<br />

Sluss <strong>and</strong> Ashforth's (2008) proposition that leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> moderates the relationship bet<strong>we</strong>en the psychological<br />

experience of the leader–follo<strong>we</strong>r relationship <strong>and</strong> psychological attachment to the organization. 2 Eisenberger et al. focused on<br />

the relationship bet<strong>we</strong>en LMX <strong>and</strong> organizational commitment, where positive leader–follo<strong>we</strong>r relationships can be expected<br />

to be predictive of a positive attitude toward (i.e., commitment to) the organization. Consistent with Sluss <strong>and</strong> Ashforth's analysis,<br />

they proposed <strong>and</strong> found that the positive relationship bet<strong>we</strong>en LMX <strong>and</strong> commitment is stronger as the follo<strong>we</strong>r perceives the<br />

leader to be more <strong>group</strong> prototypical.<br />

Findings like these <strong>are</strong> particularly interesting because they lay the groundworks for an integrative reading of dyadic <strong>and</strong> <strong>group</strong>-based<br />

perspectives on <strong>leadership</strong> in at least two ways. First, they suggest that social identification may be the key contingency determining the<br />

extent to which <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness is contingent on social identity processes or social exchange processes. Second, they hint at a<br />

temporal dimension in the <strong>leadership</strong> process in which interpersonal leader–follo<strong>we</strong>r relationships may develop into more <strong>group</strong>based<br />

<strong>leadership</strong> processes. Over time, <strong>leadership</strong> may build follo<strong>we</strong>r identification with the collective in part through their interpersonal<br />

relationships with follo<strong>we</strong>rs (cf. Sluss & Ashforth, 2007, 2008) <strong>and</strong> thus set the stage for a shift in <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness from dyadic<br />

<strong>leadership</strong> to <strong>group</strong>-based <strong>leadership</strong>, <strong>and</strong> more <strong>group</strong> prototypical leaders may be better positioned to establish this shift. This temporal<br />

dimension is underexplored in the social identity analysis of <strong>leadership</strong>, ho<strong>we</strong>ver, <strong>and</strong> here lies a clear challenge for future research.<br />

3.5. <strong>Leader</strong> self-perceived <strong>prototypicality</strong><br />

Given the consistent evidence for the positive effects of follo<strong>we</strong>r perceptions of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> on <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness,<br />

the question arises how leaders' self-perceived <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> affects their actions. Theory <strong>and</strong> evidence support the<br />

conclusion that more <strong>group</strong> prototypical leaders <strong>are</strong> more effective because they <strong>are</strong> seen to represent a sh<strong>are</strong>d social reality <strong>and</strong><br />

trusted to pursue the <strong>group</strong>'s best interest. Does this also imply that self-perceived <strong>prototypicality</strong> motivates representing the<br />

<strong>group</strong> in word <strong>and</strong> deed? Put differently, does a leader's perception that he or she is prototypical of the <strong>group</strong> (team, organization)<br />

motivate the leader to think <strong>and</strong> act as follo<strong>we</strong>rs expect the leader to do?<br />

The limited work in <strong>leadership</strong> emergence from a social identity perspective provides circumstantial evidence that <strong>group</strong><br />

<strong>prototypicality</strong> may feed into <strong>leadership</strong> in that the positive relationship bet<strong>we</strong>en <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> <strong>and</strong> emergent <strong>leadership</strong><br />

(Fielding & Hogg, 1997; van Knippenberg et al., 2000) presumably reflects an influence of <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> on the<br />

individual's actions, but it does not speak to the question how leader self-perceived <strong>prototypicality</strong> more specifically impacts<br />

2 App<strong>are</strong>ntly unaw<strong>are</strong> of the social identity analysis of <strong>leadership</strong> <strong>and</strong> the concept of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong>, Eisenberger et al. (2010) proposed the label<br />

of “supervisor organizational embodiment” for their measure of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong>. As the concept of supervisor organizational embodiment is conceptually<br />

redundant with the concept of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong>, <strong>and</strong> <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> not only has a longer history in <strong>leadership</strong> research (including strong<br />

connections with the use of the concept of <strong>prototypicality</strong> in <strong>leadership</strong> categorization theory) but also a strong basis outside of <strong>leadership</strong> research (cf. Rosch,<br />

1978; Turner et al., 1987), I suggest that <strong>we</strong> stick to leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong>.


1086 D. van Knippenberg / The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly 22 (2011) 1078–1091<br />

<strong>leadership</strong>. <strong>Leader</strong>ship research has a long history in the study of the determinants of leader behavior or behavioral styles, but<br />

this research almost exclusively emphasizes personality <strong>and</strong> individual differences as determinants of <strong>leadership</strong> (e.g., Judge,<br />

Piccolo, & Kosalka, 2009; van Knippenberg, 2012). The study of the role of leader self-definition in shaping leader actions<br />

is only just emerging (Day, Harrison, & Halpin, 2009; Lord & Hall, 2005; Rus, van Knippenberg, & Wisse, 2010; van Dick, Hirst,<br />

Grojean, & Wieseke, 2007) <strong>and</strong> so far only two studies addressed the effects of leader self-perceived <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong>.<br />

Giessner <strong>and</strong> van Knippenberg (2007, 2009) argued that self-perceived <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> intrinsically motivates <strong>group</strong>oriented<br />

behavior, because self-definition as <strong>group</strong> prototypical implies ascribing <strong>group</strong> norms <strong>and</strong> interests to self. That is,<br />

self-perceptions complement the <strong>leadership</strong> perceptions highlighted in the social identity theory of <strong>leadership</strong>. In support of<br />

their analysis, they sho<strong>we</strong>d that <strong>group</strong> prototypical leaders <strong>we</strong>re relatively <strong>group</strong>-oriented in allocation decisions (as opposed<br />

to self-serving) irrespective of whether they <strong>we</strong>re held accountable for their actions or not, whereas nonprototypical leaders<br />

<strong>we</strong>re more <strong>group</strong>-oriented when they <strong>we</strong>re accountable than when they <strong>we</strong>re not (i.e., accountability provided an extrinsic reason<br />

to refrain from self-serving behavior). This suggests that <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> indeed intrinsically motivates <strong>group</strong>-oriented behavior.<br />

In addition, Giessner <strong>and</strong> van Knippenberg sho<strong>we</strong>d that <strong>group</strong> prototypical leaders' allocation decisions <strong>we</strong>re more contingent on<br />

<strong>group</strong> norms than those of nonprototypical leaders. This latter finding is consistent with the notion that self-perceived <strong>prototypicality</strong><br />

leads to an internalization of <strong>group</strong> norms that guide behavior.<br />

These findings thus mirror <strong>and</strong> complement the main body of the social identity theory of <strong>leadership</strong> by showing that selfperceived<br />

<strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> motivates the kind of attitudes <strong>and</strong> actions follo<strong>we</strong>rs expect <strong>group</strong> prototypical leaders to display.<br />

In a sense then, these findings can be said to show that follo<strong>we</strong>rs <strong>are</strong> correct to trust <strong>group</strong>-prototypical leaders to represent<br />

<strong>group</strong> norms <strong>and</strong> interest — at least provided that the leaders also see themselves as prototypical. In that sense, then, they add<br />

to the evidence supporting the conclusion that <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> is a <strong>leadership</strong> quality to seek in leaders. From the perspective<br />

of <strong>leadership</strong> selection <strong>and</strong> development, the combined evidence from research in the social identity theory of <strong>leadership</strong> would thus<br />

suggest searching for identity fit bet<strong>we</strong>en leader <strong>and</strong> <strong>group</strong>, team, or organization (cf. Edwards, 2008), <strong>and</strong> cultivating leaders' selfperception<br />

as a prototypical <strong>group</strong> member (e.g., through socialization processes <strong>and</strong> <strong>leadership</strong> development programs). Clearly,<br />

future research is needed to validate these implications, but given the modest (if not disappointing) achievements of the dominant<br />

personality perspective in the determinants of <strong>leadership</strong> (Judge et al., 2009; van Knippenberg, 2012), it would seem it may pay<br />

off to be open to alternative perspectives.<br />

3.6. Creativity <strong>and</strong> innovation<br />

Creativity <strong>and</strong> innovation <strong>are</strong> of crucial importance to a sustainable competitive advantage for organizations (Zhou & Shalley,<br />

2008) <strong>and</strong> an important <strong>leadership</strong> role therefore is to foster employee creativity <strong>and</strong> innovation (Bass & Riggio, 2006). Group<br />

<strong>prototypicality</strong> as a factor in <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness raises an interesting issue in this respect, because at first blush there may<br />

be an intuitive tension bet<strong>we</strong>en the notion of <strong>leadership</strong> embodying <strong>group</strong> norms <strong>and</strong> creativity <strong>and</strong> innovation. This intuition<br />

seems based on the misconception that social identities, <strong>and</strong> <strong>group</strong> norms embedded in those identities, <strong>are</strong> static <strong>and</strong> unchanging,<br />

<strong>and</strong> that therefore <strong>group</strong> norms enforce the status quo <strong>and</strong> “business as usual”. The end result then would be that <strong>group</strong> norms<br />

<strong>and</strong> an emphasis on social identity would discourage creativity, innovation, <strong>and</strong> change. This is a misconception for two reasons.<br />

Addressing these helps explain why leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> may actually be conducive to creativity <strong>and</strong> innovation, <strong>and</strong> to the<br />

related issue of collective (e.g., organizational) change (i.e., collective change arguably is a specific form of innovation — innovation<br />

of the collective itself).<br />

First, social identity <strong>and</strong> the <strong>group</strong> norms embedded in the identity may actually embrace <strong>and</strong> emphasize creativity <strong>and</strong> innovation.<br />

Indeed, some teams <strong>and</strong> organizations <strong>are</strong> in a sense defined by their focus on creativity <strong>and</strong> innovation, such as those active<br />

in Research & Development. To the extent that a focus on creativity <strong>and</strong> innovation is seen as part of collective identity, <strong>group</strong><br />

norms would in fact encourage its pursuit <strong>and</strong> <strong>group</strong> prototypical <strong>leadership</strong> would be <strong>we</strong>ll-positioned to motivate creativity <strong>and</strong><br />

innovation (Hirst et al., 2009). Second, social identities <strong>are</strong> not static <strong>and</strong> unchanging, but develop <strong>and</strong> change over time just like<br />

individual identities. What people identifying with a <strong>group</strong> or organization seek is not unchanging identity, but continuity of<br />

identity — the sense of a clear connection bet<strong>we</strong>en past, present, <strong>and</strong> future identity (Rousseau, 1998; Sani, 2008; cf. Shamir<br />

et al., 1993). Where it concerns innovation as change to the collective, people can be quite accepting <strong>and</strong> supportive of the pursuit<br />

of such change/innovation as long as they have a clear sense of continuity of identity. <strong>Leader</strong>ship of change may thus overcome<br />

resistance to change by instilling a sense of continuity of identity, <strong>and</strong> <strong>group</strong> prototypical leaders as embodiment of the sh<strong>are</strong>d<br />

identity <strong>are</strong> particularly <strong>we</strong>ll-positioned to do so (Abrams, R<strong>and</strong>sley de Moura, Marques, & Hutchison, 2008; van Knippenberg<br />

et al., 2008).<br />

The intrinsic motivation to pursue creativity <strong>and</strong> innovation is an important ingredient in realizing creative <strong>and</strong> innovative<br />

outcomes (Amabile, 1988). <strong>Leader</strong>ship that is able to intrinsically motivate the pursuit of such creative outcomes thus is off to<br />

a good start, <strong>and</strong> <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> may help leaders achieve this. There is more to <strong>leadership</strong> of creativity <strong>and</strong> innovation,<br />

ho<strong>we</strong>ver. The pursuit of creative <strong>and</strong> innovative outcomes inherently is a relatively uncertain <strong>and</strong> risky endeavor in the sense<br />

that it more or less by definition is a nonroutine activity <strong>and</strong> holds the risk of failure. This renders people's efficacy in pursuit<br />

of such creative goals (i.e., the belief in the capability to achieve these goals) a salient influence in the motivation to pursue creative<br />

outcomes (Tierney & Farmer, 2002) <strong>and</strong> <strong>leadership</strong> that is able to build such (creative) efficacy is more successful in motivating<br />

creativity (Shin & Zhou, 2007) <strong>and</strong> presumably innovation. Approaching this issue from a social identity perspective, Hirst<br />

et al. (2009) argued that, contingent on follo<strong>we</strong>r social identification, the creativity of members of R&D teams would be positively<br />

influenced by inspirational motivation, an aspect of transformational <strong>leadership</strong> that is associated with building follo<strong>we</strong>r efficacy


D. van Knippenberg / The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly 22 (2011) 1078–1091<br />

1087<br />

(Bass, 1985; cf. Shamir et al., 1993). Moreover, they expected <strong>and</strong> found that this influence is stronger with higher leader <strong>group</strong><br />

<strong>prototypicality</strong>, because <strong>prototypicality</strong> would add to the persuasiveness of leader inspirational motivation (i.e., lend credibility to<br />

the leaders' ambitions for the collective; cf. van Knippenberg et al., 1994).<br />

With only a single study addressing the relationship bet<strong>we</strong>en leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> <strong>and</strong> follo<strong>we</strong>r creativity, it clearly<br />

would be premature to jump to bold conclusions. Even so, it would seem important to note that both theory <strong>and</strong> the available<br />

evidence (corroborated by evidence for the positive relationship bet<strong>we</strong>en leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> <strong>and</strong> support for organizational<br />

change; Pierro et al., 2007; van Knippenberg & van Knippenberg, 2005a; van Knippenberg et al., 2008) point to a positive<br />

role for leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> in creativity <strong>and</strong> innovation. Perhaps contrary to some people's intuition, then, it would seem<br />

worthwhile to further develop the social identity analysis of <strong>leadership</strong> of creativity <strong>and</strong> innovation. An interesting lead in this<br />

respect may be recent research by Pierro et al. (2009) suggesting that promotion focus, a concern with achieving positive outcomes<br />

(Higgins, 1997), renders follo<strong>we</strong>rs more sensitive to leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong>. Promotion focus is also positively related<br />

to creativity (Friedman & Förster, 2001) <strong>and</strong> may thus be a particularly interesting factor to explore in a social identity analysis of<br />

<strong>leadership</strong> of creativity <strong>and</strong> innovation (cf. Kark & Van Dijk, 2007; Stam, van Knippenberg, & Wisse, 2010).<br />

4. Conclusions <strong>and</strong> future directions<br />

In reviewing the evidence for the social identity theory of <strong>leadership</strong> a first <strong>and</strong> obvious conclusion is that the theory's basic<br />

propositions receive strong <strong>and</strong> consistent support across methodologies, indicators of <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness, <strong>and</strong> countries.<br />

It does not seem overstated to conclude that especially through the test of hypotheses across experimental <strong>and</strong> survey methods<br />

support for the theory is as strong as for any theory in the field <strong>and</strong> stronger <strong>and</strong> more consistent than for most, even when the<br />

number of studies on leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> is much more modest than on in particular transformational <strong>leadership</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

LMX. In that sense, while the social identity theory of <strong>leadership</strong> may have been somewhat of a “new kid on the block” in <strong>leadership</strong><br />

research at the time of its 2003 statement (albeit with strong <strong>and</strong> deep roots in the study of social influence; Abrams & Hogg, 1990;<br />

Turner et al., 1987; van Knippenberg, 2000a), the 2011 state of the science would seem to establish it as a mature <strong>and</strong> <strong>we</strong>ll-supported<br />

theoretical perspective.<br />

A key theme in the social identity analysis of <strong>leadership</strong> is follo<strong>we</strong>r trust in the leader. While this was already present in the<br />

2003 statement of the theory, more recent research in <strong>prototypicality</strong> <strong>and</strong> fairness <strong>and</strong> in <strong>prototypicality</strong> <strong>and</strong> the romance of <strong>leadership</strong><br />

suggest that it may be highly worthwhile to develop this analysis further as these different research foci seem to emphasize<br />

different aspects of trust in <strong>leadership</strong>. In an integrative review of the trust literature Mayer et al. (1995) distinguished three aspects<br />

of perceived trustworthiness as precursors to trust in the relationship bet<strong>we</strong>en two parties (e.g., leader <strong>and</strong> follo<strong>we</strong>rs): benevolence<br />

(good intentions toward the other party), integrity (adherence to a set of (moral) principles acceptable to the other<br />

party), <strong>and</strong> ability (the perception that the other party is able to realize its intentions). Looking at the role of trust in analyses of<br />

leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness, <strong>we</strong> may note that originally the emphasis was on benevolence — trust<br />

in the leader's <strong>group</strong>-orientedness. More recently, ho<strong>we</strong>ver, these analysis seem to have exp<strong>and</strong>ed the consideration to issues of<br />

integrity <strong>and</strong> ability too. Group <strong>prototypicality</strong> as a factor engendering trust in leader fairness might be understood in terms of benevolence,<br />

but arguably also in terms of integrity, <strong>and</strong> <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong>-induced trust as a moderator of the romance of <strong>leadership</strong><br />

seems to have clear linkages with ability.<br />

The linkage with benevolence would follow most directly from the social identity analysis, which underst<strong>and</strong>s trust in leader<br />

<strong>group</strong>-orientedness to reflect the perception that a <strong>group</strong> prototypical leader also embodies (the pursuit of) <strong>group</strong> interests. Integrity<br />

could ho<strong>we</strong>ver also be argued to imply here to the extent that sh<strong>are</strong>d social identity incorporates sh<strong>are</strong>d (moral) principles <strong>and</strong> values<br />

— as the <strong>group</strong>-value model of fairness suggests it would (Lind & Tyler, 1988). The linkage with trustworthiness in terms of ability<br />

would in that sense probably be most contingent on the nature of the <strong>group</strong> — specifically, on the extent that ability is part of the<br />

<strong>group</strong> prototype. In many organizational contexts, this is arguably the case in that the <strong>group</strong> identity would somehow incorporate<br />

what the <strong>group</strong> does <strong>and</strong> presumably also its ability at doing this, but especially for non-organizational <strong>group</strong>s it need not be. Exploring<br />

these issues in future research through more specific measurement of the three aspects of trustworthiness may yield more insights<br />

in this issue. It may for instance help us determine under which conditions leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> would engender<br />

perceptions of trustworthiness-ability <strong>and</strong> whether this aspect of trustworthiness is the one that underlies the moderating effect<br />

on the romance of <strong>leadership</strong>.<br />

Setting aside the dimensionality of trustworthiness, <strong>we</strong> may note that key themes in the further development of the social<br />

identity analysis of <strong>leadership</strong> have been trust <strong>and</strong> uncertainty. <strong>Leader</strong> <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> is linked to trust in <strong>leadership</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

the ability to reduce uncertainty by representing sh<strong>are</strong>d social reality, <strong>and</strong> this expresses itself in a number of distinct but related<br />

developments in the social identity analysis of <strong>leadership</strong>. These revolve around dispositional <strong>and</strong> situational influences on the<br />

desire to reduce uncertainty that function as contingencies of the effects of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong>, around the role of leader<br />

<strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> in <strong>leadership</strong> of change, the romance of <strong>leadership</strong>, <strong>and</strong> leader fairness, <strong>and</strong> around the influence of leader<br />

self-perceived <strong>prototypicality</strong> on leader actions. In combination, these findings suggest that <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> not only elicits<br />

trust in leaders' good intentions <strong>and</strong> motivation to represent <strong>group</strong> norms, but also motivates such intentions <strong>and</strong> actions in<br />

leaders. Moreover, these findings provide initial support for the proposition that <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> positions leaders to<br />

more effectively lead people in more uncertain enterprises with inherently higher risk of failure such an organizational change<br />

<strong>and</strong> innovation.<br />

Research in leader fairness also adds a complication here, ho<strong>we</strong>ver, in suggesting that leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> may not<br />

only speak to the issues of trustworthiness <strong>and</strong> uncertainty that <strong>are</strong> tied in with leader fairness (Lind, 2001), but also to concern


1088 D. van Knippenberg / The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly 22 (2011) 1078–1091<br />

with social st<strong>and</strong>ing that <strong>are</strong> also tied in with leader fairness (Tyler & Blader, 2000). The current state of the science thus holds a<br />

clear invitation to more fully investigate this presumed role of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> in influencing follo<strong>we</strong>rs' perceptions<br />

of their social st<strong>and</strong>ing with the leader <strong>and</strong> within the <strong>group</strong>. Other developments <strong>are</strong> in a more embryonic state <strong>and</strong> concern the<br />

role of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> in the (effects of the) interpersonal relationship bet<strong>we</strong>en leader <strong>and</strong> follo<strong>we</strong>r, in <strong>leadership</strong> of<br />

creativity <strong>and</strong> innovation, <strong>and</strong> as contingent on promotion focus. Developments in these <strong>are</strong>as ask for more research much more<br />

than for bold conclusions.<br />

In taking stock of the state of the science in a field, one may also note what has not been investigated — what <strong>are</strong> perhaps surprising<br />

omissions. In this respect, I would highlight two issues: leaders' active role in conveying an image of <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong>, <strong>and</strong> explicit<br />

attention to the role of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> in team <strong>leadership</strong>.<br />

While research in leader self-perceived <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> shows that the positive effects of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong><br />

<strong>are</strong> not exclusively contingent on follo<strong>we</strong>r perceptions, clearly follo<strong>we</strong>rs' perception of the leader as embodying the sh<strong>are</strong>d social<br />

identity <strong>are</strong> a crucial element in the effects of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong>. <strong>Leader</strong>s may thus benefit from clearly conveying <strong>and</strong><br />

establishing their <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> (van Knippenberg & Hogg, 2003), <strong>and</strong> qualitative analyses by Reicher <strong>and</strong> Hopkins (2003)<br />

suggest that highly successful (political) leaders can be quite skilled in achieving just that. By the nature of their analysis, Reicher<br />

<strong>and</strong> Hopkins only describe the leader actions involved in their claims to <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> in an illustrative, case-based manner, <strong>and</strong><br />

systematic quantitative research on these processes is lacking. The po<strong>we</strong>rful message of Reicher <strong>and</strong> Hopkins's analysis is, ho<strong>we</strong>ver,<br />

that <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> is not just a “passive” quality of leaders but something that leaders can actively claim <strong>and</strong> construe to the<br />

benefit of their effectiveness as a leader. To complement the current evidence in support of the social identity theory of <strong>leadership</strong>,<br />

then, it would seem highly worthwhile to more systematically investigate how leaders may achieve this active construal of their<br />

image as <strong>group</strong> prototypical.<br />

While the social identity analysis of <strong>leadership</strong> recognizes the <strong>group</strong>-based nature of <strong>leadership</strong> probably more than any other<br />

analysis of <strong>leadership</strong> where it concerns the psychological (i.e., self-definitional) implications of <strong>leadership</strong>, it is largely agnostic<br />

when it comes to the interactive processes required of work <strong>group</strong>s <strong>and</strong> teams to produce joint outcomes. This in fact is true for<br />

<strong>leadership</strong> research far more generally <strong>and</strong> has sparked a growing attention for what is explicitly team <strong>leadership</strong> — <strong>leadership</strong><br />

targeted at fostering the interactive team processes required for the successful delivery of high-quality team products (Day,<br />

Gronn, & Salas, 2004). While <strong>leadership</strong> research at large seems to prioritize the role of <strong>leadership</strong> in motivating <strong>and</strong> inspiring<br />

(collective) performance, research in team <strong>leadership</strong> is concerned much more specifically with the interactive team processes<br />

required for collaborative work <strong>and</strong> with the team cognition driving these processes (cf. Hackman & Wageman, 2005; Hannah,<br />

Lord, & Pearce, 2011). Work <strong>group</strong>s <strong>and</strong> teams typically <strong>are</strong> the primary unit of identification in organizations (van Knippenberg &<br />

van Schie, 2000) <strong>and</strong> it would thus st<strong>and</strong> to reason that leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> would also be a factor in successfully fulfilling<br />

this team <strong>leadership</strong> role. As always, ho<strong>we</strong>ver, the proof of the pudding is in the eating, <strong>and</strong> to more fully speak to the <strong>group</strong>-based<br />

nature of <strong>leadership</strong> it would seem important that the social identity analysis of <strong>leadership</strong> also addresses the role of social identity<br />

processes in team <strong>leadership</strong> as more narrowly defined in the team <strong>leadership</strong> literature.<br />

In conclusion then, the social identity theory of <strong>leadership</strong> provides a <strong>we</strong>ll-supported basis for the study of <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness<br />

in a variety of <strong>are</strong>as, <strong>and</strong> has the clear potential to advance a more integrative underst<strong>and</strong>ing of <strong>leadership</strong> that also<br />

includes cross-linkages with other <strong>leadership</strong> perspectives (e.g., charismatic <strong>and</strong> transformational <strong>leadership</strong>, leader fairness,<br />

LMX). The combination of this strong conceptual <strong>and</strong> empirical basis <strong>and</strong> this integrative potential make further development<br />

of the social identity analysis of <strong>leadership</strong> a particularly promising direction for <strong>leadership</strong> research. The present review hopes<br />

to have provided <strong>and</strong> structured the building blocks for these further developments.<br />

References<br />

Abrams, D., & Hogg, M. A. (1990). Social identification, self-categorization <strong>and</strong> social influence. European Review of Social Psychology, 1, 195–228.<br />

Abrams, D., R<strong>and</strong>sley de Moura, G., Marques, J. M., & Hutchison, P. (2008). Innovation credit: When can leaders oppose their <strong>group</strong>'s norms? Journal of Personality<br />

<strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 95, 662–678.<br />

Amabile, T. M. (1988). A model of creativity <strong>and</strong> innovation in organizations. Research in Organizational Behavior, 10, 123–167.<br />

Ashforth, B. E., & Mael, F. (1989). Social identity theory <strong>and</strong> the organization. Academy of Management Review, 14, 20–39.<br />

Bass, B. M. (1985). <strong>Leader</strong>ship <strong>and</strong> performance beyond expectations. New York: Free Press.<br />

Bass, B. M., & Avolio, B. J. (1995). MLQ Multifactor <strong>Leader</strong>ship Questionnaire. Redwood City, CA: Mind Garden.<br />

Bass, B. M., & Riggio, R. E. (2006). Transformational <strong>leadership</strong>. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Bies, R. J., & Moag, J. S. (1986). Interactional justice communication criteria of fairness. In R. Lewicki, B. H. Sheppard, & M. H. Bazerman (Eds.), Research on negotiation in<br />

organizations. Greenwich, CN: JAI Press.<br />

Blau, P. M. (1964). Exchange <strong>and</strong> po<strong>we</strong>r in social life. New York: Wiley.<br />

Brendl, C. M., & Higgins, E. T. (1996). Principles of judging valence: What makes events positive or negative? In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social<br />

psychology, Vol. 28. (pp. 95–160)San Diego, CA: Academic Press.<br />

Choi, Y., & Mai-Dalton, R. R. (1998). On the <strong>leadership</strong> function of self-sacrifice. The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly, 9, 475–501.<br />

Cicero, L., Pierro, A., & van Knippenberg, D. (2007). <strong>Leader</strong> <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> <strong>and</strong> job satisfaction: The moderating role of job stress <strong>and</strong> team identification.<br />

Group Dynamics, 11, 165–175.<br />

Cicero, L., Bonaiuto, M., Pierro, A., & van Knippenberg, D. (2008). Employees work effort as a function of leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong>: The moderating role of team<br />

identification. Revue Européenne de Psychologie Appliquée, 58, 117–124.<br />

Cicero, L., Pierro, A., & van Knippenberg, D. (2010). <strong>Leader</strong> <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness: The moderating role of role ambiguity. British Journal of<br />

Management, 21, 411–421.<br />

Colquitt, J. A., Conlon, D. E., Wesson, M. J., Porter, C. O. L. H., & Ng, K. Y. (2001). Justice at the millennium: A meta-analytic review of 25 years of organizational<br />

justice research. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 425–445.<br />

Conger, J. A., & Kanungo, R. N. (1987). Towards a behavioral theory of charismatic <strong>leadership</strong> in organizational settings. Academy of Management Review, 12,<br />

637–647.


D. van Knippenberg / The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly 22 (2011) 1078–1091<br />

1089<br />

Conger, J. A., Kanungo, R. N., & Menon, S. T. (2000). Charismatic <strong>leadership</strong> <strong>and</strong> follo<strong>we</strong>r effects. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 21, 747–767.<br />

Day, D. V., Gronn, P., & Salas, E. (2004). <strong>Leader</strong>ship capacity in teams. The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly, 15, 857–880.<br />

Day, D. V., Harrison, M. M., & Halpin, S. M. (2009). An integrative approach to leader development. New York, NY: Routledge.<br />

De Cremer, D., & van Knippenberg, D. (2002). How do leaders promote cooperation? The effects of charisma <strong>and</strong> procedural fairness. The Journal of Applied Psychology,<br />

87, 858–866.<br />

De Cremer, D., van Dijke, M., & Mayer, D. M. (2010). Cooperating when “you” <strong>and</strong> “I” <strong>are</strong> treated fairly: The moderating role of leader <strong>prototypicality</strong>. The Journal of<br />

Applied Psychology, 95, 1121–1133.<br />

Dirks, K. T., & Ferrin, D. L. (2002). Trust in <strong>leadership</strong>: Meta-analytic findings <strong>and</strong> implications for research <strong>and</strong> practice. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 87,<br />

611–628.<br />

Eagly, A. H., & Carli, L. L. (2007). Through the labyrinth: The truth about how women become leaders. Boston: Harvard Business School Press.<br />

Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders. Psychological Review, 109, 573–598.<br />

Edwards, J. R. (2008). Person-environment fit in organizations: An assessment of theoretical progress. Academy of Management Annals, 2, 167–230.<br />

Eisenberger, R., Karagonlar, G., Stinglhamber, F., Neves, P., Becker, T. E., Gonzales-Morales, M. G., et al. (2010). <strong>Leader</strong>-member exchange <strong>and</strong> affective organizational<br />

commitment: The contribution of supervisor's organizational embodiment. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 1085–1103.<br />

Ensher, E. A., & Murphy, S. E. (1997). Effects of race, gender, perceived similarity, <strong>and</strong> contact on mentor relationships. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 50, 460–481.<br />

Fielding, K. S., & Hogg, M. A. (1997). Social identity, self-categorization, <strong>and</strong> <strong>leadership</strong>: A field study of small interactive <strong>group</strong>s. Group Dynamics, 1, 39–51.<br />

Friedman, R. S., & Förster, J. (2001). The effects of promotion <strong>and</strong> prevention cues on creativity. Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 81, 1001–1013.<br />

Gartzia, L. (2010). From “think male” to “think <strong>and</strong>rogynous”. Implications for gender equality <strong>and</strong> organizational functioning in XXI century organizations. UPV/EHU:<br />

Editorial Service.<br />

Gerstner, C. R., & Day, D. V. (1997). Meta-analytic review of leader–member exchange theory: Correlates <strong>and</strong> construct issues. The Journal of Applied Psychology,<br />

82, 827–844.<br />

Giessner, S. R., & van Knippenberg, D. (2007, April). Leading FOR the team: Situational determinants of team-oriented leader behavior. Paper presented at the 2007<br />

Annual Meeting of the Society of Industrial <strong>and</strong> Organizational Psychology, New York.<br />

Giessner, S. R., & van Knippenberg, D. (2008). “License to fail”: Goal definition, leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong>, <strong>and</strong> perceptions of <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness after leader failure.<br />

Organizational Behavior <strong>and</strong> Human Decision Processes, 105, 14–35.<br />

Giessner, S. R., & van Knippenberg, D. (2009, June). When does a leader show fair behavior? Influences of <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> <strong>and</strong> the social context. Paper presented at<br />

the Erasmus <strong>Leader</strong>ship Conference, Rotterdam, The Netherl<strong>and</strong>s.<br />

Giessner, S. R., van Knippenberg, D., & Sleebos, E. (2009). License to fail?: How leader <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> moderates the effects of leader performance on perceptions<br />

of <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness. The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly, 20, 434–451.<br />

Gouldner, A. W. (1960). The norm of reciprocity: A preliminary statement. American Sociological Review, 25, 161–178.<br />

Graen, G. B., & Uhl-Bien, M. (1995). Relationship-based approach to <strong>leadership</strong>: development of leader–member exchange (LMX) theory of <strong>leadership</strong> over 25<br />

years: Applying a multi-level multi-domain perspective. The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly, 6, 219–247.<br />

Hackman, J. R., & Wageman, R. (2005). When <strong>and</strong> how team leaders matter. Research in Organizational Behavior, 26, 37–74.<br />

Hains, S. C., Hogg, M. A., & Duck, J. M. (1997). Self-categorization <strong>and</strong> <strong>leadership</strong>: Effects of <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> <strong>and</strong> leader stereotypicality. Personality <strong>and</strong> Social<br />

Psychology Bulletin, 23, 1087–1100.<br />

Halevy, N., Chou, E. Y., & Galinsky, A. D. (2011). A functional model of hierarchy: Why, how, <strong>and</strong> when vertical differentiation enhances <strong>group</strong> performance. Organizational<br />

Psychology Review, 1, 32–53.<br />

Hannah, S. T., Lord, R. G., & Pearce, C. L. (2011). <strong>Leader</strong>ship <strong>and</strong> collective requisite complexity. Organizational Psychology Review, 1, 104–127.<br />

Haslam, S. A., & Platow, M. J. (2001). The link bet<strong>we</strong>en <strong>leadership</strong> <strong>and</strong> follo<strong>we</strong>rship: How affirming social identity translates vision into action. Personality <strong>and</strong><br />

Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1469–1479.<br />

Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure <strong>and</strong> pain. The American Psychologist, 52, 1280–1300.<br />

Hirst, G., van Dick, R., & van Knippenberg, D. (2009). A social identity perspective on <strong>leadership</strong> <strong>and</strong> employee creativity. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 30,<br />

963–982.<br />

Hogg, M. A. (2000). Subjective uncertainty reduction through self-categorization: A motivational theory of social identity processes. European Review of Social Psychology,<br />

11, 223–255.<br />

Hogg, M. A. (2001). A social identity theory of <strong>leadership</strong>. Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology Review, 5, 184–200.<br />

Hogg, M. A. (2003). Social identity. In M. R. Leary, & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book of self <strong>and</strong> identity (pp. 462–479). New York: Guilford.<br />

Hogg, M. A. (2007). Uncertainty–identity theory. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, Vol. 39. (pp. 69–126)San Diego, CA: Academic<br />

Press.<br />

Hogg, M. A., & van Knippenberg, D. (2003). Social identity <strong>and</strong> <strong>leadership</strong> processes in <strong>group</strong>s. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 1–52.<br />

Hogg, M. A., Hains, S. C., & Mason, I. (1998). Identification <strong>and</strong> <strong>leadership</strong> in small <strong>group</strong>s: Salience, frame of reference, <strong>and</strong> leader stereotypicality effects on leader<br />

evaluations. Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 75, 1248–1263.<br />

Hogg, M. A., Martin, R., Epitropaki, O., Mankad, A., Svensson, A., & Weeden, K. (2005). Effective <strong>leadership</strong> in salient <strong>group</strong>s: Revisiting leader–member exchange<br />

theory from the perspective of the social identity theory of <strong>leadership</strong>. Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 991–1004.<br />

Hogg, M. A., Fielding, K. S., Johnson, D., Masser, B., Russell, E., & Svensson, A. (2006). Demographic category membership <strong>and</strong> <strong>leadership</strong> in small <strong>group</strong>s: A social<br />

identity analysis. The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly, 17, 335–350.<br />

Holl<strong>and</strong>er, E. P. (1958). Conformity, status, <strong>and</strong> idiosyncracy credit. Psychological Review, 65, 117–127.<br />

House, R. J. (1971). A path-goal theory of leader effectiveness. Administrative Science Quarterly, 16, 321–339.<br />

Janson, A., Levy, L., Sitkin, S. B., & Lind, E. A. (2008). Fairness <strong>and</strong> other <strong>leadership</strong> heuristics: A four-nation study. European Journal of Work <strong>and</strong> Organizational<br />

Psychology, 17, 251–272.<br />

Judge, T. A., Bono, J. E., Ilies, R., & Gerhardt, M. (2002). Personality <strong>and</strong> <strong>leadership</strong>: A qualitative <strong>and</strong> quantitative review. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 87,<br />

765–780.<br />

Judge, T. A., Piccolo, R. F., & Ilies, R. (2004). The forgotten ones?: A re-examination of consideration, initiating structure, <strong>and</strong> <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness. The Journal of<br />

Applied Psychology, 89, 36–51.<br />

Judge, T. A., Piccolo, R. F., & Kosalka, T. (2009). The bright <strong>and</strong> dark side of leader traits: A review <strong>and</strong> theoretical extension of the leader trait paradigm. The <strong>Leader</strong>ship<br />

Quarterly, 20, 855–875.<br />

Kaiser, R. B., Hogan, R., & Craig, S. B. (2008). <strong>Leader</strong>ship <strong>and</strong> the fate of organizations. The American Psychologist, 63, 96–110.<br />

Kalshoven, K., & Den Hartog, D. N. (2009). Ethical leader behavior <strong>and</strong> leader effectiveness: The role of <strong>prototypicality</strong> <strong>and</strong> trust. International Journal of <strong>Leader</strong>ship<br />

Studies, 5, 102–120.<br />

Kark, R., & Van Dijk, D. (2007). Motivation to lead, motivation to follow: The role of self-regulatory focus in <strong>leadership</strong> processes. Academy of Management Review,<br />

32, 500–528.<br />

Koper, G., van Knippenberg, D., Bouhuijs, F., Vermunt, R., & Wilke, H. (1993). Procedural fairness <strong>and</strong> self-esteem. European Journal of Social Psychology, 23,<br />

313–325.<br />

Kruglanski, A. W., & Webster, D. M. (1996). Motivated closing of the mind: “Seizing” <strong>and</strong> “freezing”. Psychological Review, 103, 263–283.<br />

Lind, E. A. (2001). Fairness heuristic theory: Justice judgments as pivotal cognitions in organizational relations. In J. Greenberg, & R. Cropanzano (Eds.), Advances in<br />

organizational behavior. Lexington, MA: New Lexington.<br />

Lind, E. A., & Tyler, T. R. (1988). The social psychology of procedural justice. New York: Plenum.<br />

Lipponen, J., Koivista, S., & Olkkonen, M. -E. (2005). Procedural justice <strong>and</strong> status judgements: The moderating role of leader in<strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong>. The <strong>Leader</strong>ship<br />

Quarterly, 16, 517–528.<br />

Lord, R. G., & Brown, D. J. (2004). <strong>Leader</strong>ship processes <strong>and</strong> follo<strong>we</strong>r identity. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.


1090 D. van Knippenberg / The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly 22 (2011) 1078–1091<br />

Lord, R., & Hall, R. (2003). Identity, <strong>leadership</strong> categorization, <strong>and</strong> <strong>leadership</strong> schema. In D. van Knippenberg, & M. A. Hogg (Eds.), <strong>Leader</strong>ship <strong>and</strong> po<strong>we</strong>r: Identity<br />

processes in <strong>group</strong>s <strong>and</strong> organizations (pp. 48–64). London: Sage.<br />

Lord, R. G., & Hall, R. J. (2005). Identity, deep structure <strong>and</strong> the development of <strong>leadership</strong> skill. The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly, 16, 591–615.<br />

Lord, R. G., & Maher, K. J. (1991). <strong>Leader</strong>ship <strong>and</strong> information processing: Linking perceptions <strong>and</strong> performance. Boston, MA: Unwin Hyman.<br />

Lord, R. G., Binning, J. F., Rush, M. C., & Thomas, J. C. (1978). The effect of performance cues <strong>and</strong> leader behavior on questionnaire ratings of <strong>leadership</strong> behavior.<br />

Organizational Behavior <strong>and</strong> Human Performance, 21, 27–39.<br />

Lord, R. G., Foti, R. J., & DeVader, C. L. (1984). A test of <strong>leadership</strong> categorization theory: Internal structure, information processing, <strong>and</strong> <strong>leadership</strong> perceptions.<br />

Organizational Behavior <strong>and</strong> Human Performance, 34, 343–378.<br />

Mayer, R. C., Davis, J. H., & Schoorman, F. D. (1995). An integrative model of organizational trust. Academy of Management Review, 20, 709–734.<br />

Meindl, J. R., Ehrlich, S. B., & Dukerich, J. M. (1985). The romance of <strong>leadership</strong>. Administrative Science Quarterly, 30, 78–102.<br />

Moriano León, J. A., Topa Cantisano, G., & Lévy Mangin, J. -P. (2009). <strong>Leader</strong>ship in nonprofit organizations of Nicaragua <strong>and</strong> El Salvador: A study from the social<br />

identity theory. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 12, 667–676.<br />

Pierro, A., Cicero, L., Bonaiuto, M., van Knippenberg, D., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2005). <strong>Leader</strong> <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness: The moderating role<br />

of need for cognitive closure. The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly, 16, 503–516.<br />

Pierro, A., Cicero, L., Bonaiuto, M., van Knippenberg, D., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2007). <strong>Leader</strong> <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> <strong>and</strong> resistance to organizational change: The<br />

moderating role of need for closure <strong>and</strong> team identification. Testing, Psychometrics, Methodology in Applied Psychology, 14, 27–40.<br />

Pierro, A., Cicero, L., & Higgins, E. T. (2009). Follo<strong>we</strong>rs' satisfaction from working with <strong>group</strong>-prototypic leaders: Promotion focus as moderator. Journal of Experimental<br />

Social Psychology, 45, 1105–1110.<br />

Platow, M. J., Hoar, S., Reid, S. A., Harley, K., & Morrison, D. (1997). Endorsement of distributively fair <strong>and</strong> unfair leaders in interpersonal <strong>and</strong> inter<strong>group</strong> situations.<br />

European Journal of Social Psychology, 27, 465–494.<br />

Platow, M. J., & van Knippenberg, D. (2001). A social identity analysis of <strong>leadership</strong> endorsement: The effects of leader in<strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> <strong>and</strong> distributive<br />

inter<strong>group</strong> fairness. Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1508–1519.<br />

Platow, M. J., Haslam, S. A., Foddy, M., & Grace, D. M. (2003). <strong>Leader</strong>ship as the outcome of self-categorization processes. In D. van Knippenberg, & M. A. Hogg<br />

(Eds.), <strong>Leader</strong>ship <strong>and</strong> po<strong>we</strong>r: Identity processes in <strong>group</strong>s <strong>and</strong> organizations (pp. 34–47). London: Sage.<br />

Platow, M. J., van Knippenberg, D., Haslam, S. A., van Knippenberg, B., & Spears, R. (2006). A special gift <strong>we</strong> bestow on you for being representative of us: Considering<br />

leader charisma from a self-categorization perspective. The British Journal of Social Psychology, 45, 303–320.<br />

Reicher, S., & Hopkins, N. (2003). On the science of the art of <strong>leadership</strong>. In D. van Knippenberg, & M. A. Hogg (Eds.), <strong>Leader</strong>ship <strong>and</strong> po<strong>we</strong>r: Identity processes in<br />

<strong>group</strong>s <strong>and</strong> organizations (pp. 197–209). London: Sage.<br />

Reicher, S. D., Haslam, S. A., & Hopkins, N. (2005). Social identity <strong>and</strong> the dynamics of <strong>leadership</strong>: <strong>Leader</strong>s <strong>and</strong> follo<strong>we</strong>rs as collaborative agents in the transformation of<br />

social reality. The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly, 16, 547–568.<br />

Rhoades, L., & Eisenberger, R. (2002). Perceived organizational support: A review of the literature. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 698–714.<br />

Ridgeway, C. L. (2003). Status characteristics <strong>and</strong> <strong>leadership</strong>. In D. van Knippenberg, & M. A. Hogg (Eds.), <strong>Leader</strong>ship <strong>and</strong> po<strong>we</strong>r: Identity processes in <strong>group</strong>s <strong>and</strong><br />

organizations (pp. 65–78). London: Sage.<br />

Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch, & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition <strong>and</strong> categorization (pp. 27–48). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Rousseau, D. M. (1998). Why workers still identify with organizations. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 19, 217–233.<br />

Rupp, D. E. (2011). An employee-centered model of organizational justice <strong>and</strong> social responsibility. Organizational Psychology Review, 1, 72–94.<br />

Rus, D., van Knippenberg, D., & Wisse, B. (2010). <strong>Leader</strong> self-definition <strong>and</strong> leader self-serving behavior. The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly, 21, 509–529.<br />

Sakata, K., Fujimoto, K., & Kohguchi, H. (2005). <strong>Leader</strong>ship <strong>and</strong> <strong>group</strong> membership: Effects of <strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> on influences of a leader. The Japanese Journal<br />

of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 109–121.<br />

Sani, F. (2008). Self-continuity: Individual <strong>and</strong> collective perspectives. New York: Psychology Press.<br />

Sc<strong>and</strong>ura, T., & Graen, G. B. (1984). Moderating effects of initial leader–member exchange status on the effects of <strong>leadership</strong> interventions. The Journal of Applied<br />

Psychology, 69, 428–436.<br />

Schriesheim, C. A., & Cogliser, C. C. (2009). Construct validation in <strong>leadership</strong> research: Explication <strong>and</strong> illustration. The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly, 20, 725–736.<br />

Schriesheim, C. A., Castro, S. L., & Cogliser, C. C. (1999). <strong>Leader</strong>–member exchange (LMX) research: A comprehensive review of theory, measurement <strong>and</strong> data-analytic<br />

practices. The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly, 10, 63–113.<br />

Shamir, B., House, R., & Arthur, M. B. (1993). The motivational effects of charismatic <strong>leadership</strong>: A self-concept based theory. Organization Science, 4, 577–594.<br />

Shin, J. S., & Zhou, J. (2007). When is educational specialization heterogeneity related to creativity in research <strong>and</strong> development teams? Transformational <strong>leadership</strong> as a<br />

moderator. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 1709–1721.<br />

Sluss, D. M., & Ashforth, B. E. (2007). Relational identity <strong>and</strong> identification: Defining ourselves through work relationships. Academy of Management Review, 32, 9–32.<br />

Sluss, D. M., & Ashforth, B. E. (2008). How relational <strong>and</strong> organizational identification converge: Processes <strong>and</strong> conditions. Organization Science, 19, 807–823.<br />

Sluss, D. M., Ployhart, R. E., & Cobb, M. G. (2010, August). Converging newcomer's relational <strong>and</strong> collective identification: Prototypicality as moderator. Paper presented at<br />

the Annual Meeting of the Academy of Management, Montreal.<br />

Stam, D., van Knippenberg, D., & Wisse, B. (2010). Focusing on follo<strong>we</strong>rs: The role of regulatory focus <strong>and</strong> possible selves in explaining the effectiveness of vision<br />

statements. The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly, 21, 457–468.<br />

Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of inter<strong>group</strong> behavior. In S. Worchel, & W. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of inter<strong>group</strong> relations (pp. 7–24).<br />

Chicago: Nelson-Hall.<br />

Thibaut, J., & Walker, L. (1975). Procedural justice: A psychological analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Tierney, P., & Farmer, S. M. (2002). Creative self-efficacy: Potential antecedents <strong>and</strong> relationship to creative performance. Academy of Management Journal, 45,<br />

1137–1148.<br />

Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. D., & Wetherell, M. S. (1987). Rediscovering the social <strong>group</strong>: A self-categorization theory. Oxford, UK: Black<strong>we</strong>ll.<br />

Tyler, T. R., & Blader, S. (2000). Cooperation in <strong>group</strong>s: Procedural justice, social identity, <strong>and</strong> behavioral engagement. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.<br />

Ullrich, J., Christ, O., & van Dick, R. (2009). Substitutes for procedural fairness: Prototypical leaders <strong>are</strong> endorsed whether they <strong>are</strong> fair or not. The Journal of Applied<br />

Psychology, 94, 235–244.<br />

van Dick, R., Hirst, G., Grojean, M. W., & Wieseke, J. (2007). Relationships bet<strong>we</strong>en leader <strong>and</strong> follo<strong>we</strong>r organizational identification <strong>and</strong> implications for follo<strong>we</strong>r<br />

attitudes <strong>and</strong> behaviour. Journal of Occupational <strong>and</strong> Organizational Psychology, 80, 133–150.<br />

van Dijke, M., & De Cremer, D. (2008). How leader <strong>prototypicality</strong> affects follo<strong>we</strong>rs' status: The role of procedural fairness. European Journal of Work <strong>and</strong> Organizational<br />

Psychology, 17, 226–250.<br />

van Dijke, M., & De Cremer, D. (2010). Procedural fairness <strong>and</strong> endorsement of prototypical leaders: <strong>Leader</strong> benevolence or follo<strong>we</strong>r control? Journal of Experimental<br />

Social Psychology, 46, 85–96.<br />

van Gils, S., van Quaquebeke, N., & van Knippenberg, D. (2010). The X-factor: On the relevance of implicit <strong>leadership</strong> <strong>and</strong> follo<strong>we</strong>rship theories for leader–member<br />

exchange agreement. European Journal of Work <strong>and</strong> Organizational Psychology, 19, 333–363.<br />

van Knippenberg, D. (2000a). Group norms, <strong>prototypicality</strong>, <strong>and</strong> persuasion. In D. J. Terry, & M. A. Hogg (Eds.), Attitudes, behavior, <strong>and</strong> social context: The role of<br />

norms <strong>and</strong> <strong>group</strong> membership (pp. 157–170). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

van Knippenberg, D. (2000b). Work motivation <strong>and</strong> performance: A social identity perspective. Applied Psychology, 49, 357–371.<br />

van Knippenberg, D. (2012). <strong>Leader</strong>ship: A person-in-situation perspective. In K. Deaux, & M. Snyder (Eds.), Oxford h<strong>and</strong>book of personality <strong>and</strong> social psychology<br />

(pp. 673–700). New York: Oxford University Press.<br />

van Knippenberg, D., & Hogg, M. A. (2003). A social identity model of <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness in organizations. In R. M. Kramer, & B. M. Staw (Eds.), Research in<br />

organizational behavior, Vol. 25. (pp. 243–295)Amsterdam: Elsevier.<br />

van Knippenberg, D., & Sleebos, E. (2006). Organizational identification versus organizational commitment: Self-definition, social exchange, <strong>and</strong> job attitudes.<br />

Journal of Organizational Behavior, 27, 571–584.


D. van Knippenberg / The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly 22 (2011) 1078–1091<br />

1091<br />

van Knippenberg, B., & van Knippenberg, D. (2005). <strong>Leader</strong> self-sacrifice <strong>and</strong> <strong>leadership</strong> effectiveness: The moderating role of leader <strong>prototypicality</strong>. The Journal of<br />

Applied Psychology, 90, 25–37.<br />

van Knippenberg, B., & van Knippenberg, D. (2005, May). The effects of leader <strong>prototypicality</strong> on organizational-level performance measures. Paper presented at<br />

the XII European Congress of Work <strong>and</strong> Organizational Psychology, Istanbul.<br />

van Knippenberg, D., & van Schie, E. C. M. (2000). Foci <strong>and</strong> correlates of organizational identification. Journal of Occupational <strong>and</strong> Organizational Psychology, 73,<br />

137–147.<br />

van Knippenberg, D., Lossie, N., & Wilke, H. (1994). In-<strong>group</strong> <strong>prototypicality</strong> <strong>and</strong> persuasion: Determinants of heuristic <strong>and</strong> systematic message processing. The<br />

British Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 289–300.<br />

van Knippenberg, D., van Knippenberg, B., & van Dijk, E. (2000). Who takes the lead in risky decision making? Effects of <strong>group</strong> members' individual riskiness <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>prototypicality</strong>. Organizational Behavior <strong>and</strong> Human Decision Processes, 83, 213–234.<br />

van Knippenberg, D., van Knippenberg, B., De Cremer, D., & Hogg, M. A. (2004). <strong>Leader</strong>ship, self, <strong>and</strong> identity: A review <strong>and</strong> research agenda. The <strong>Leader</strong>ship Quarterly, 15,<br />

825–856.<br />

van Knippenberg, D., van Knippenberg, B., & De Cremer, D. (2005, May). Extending the social identity model of <strong>leadership</strong>: The interactive effects of leader <strong>prototypicality</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> leader procedural fairness. Paper presented at the XII European Congress of Work <strong>and</strong> Organizational Psychology, Istanbul.<br />

van Knippenberg, D., De Cremer, D., & van Knippenberg, B. (2007). <strong>Leader</strong>ship <strong>and</strong> fairness: The state of the art. European Journal of Work <strong>and</strong> Organizational Psychology,<br />

16, 113–140.<br />

van Knippenberg, D., van Dick, R., & Tav<strong>are</strong>s, S. (2007). Social identity <strong>and</strong> social exchange: Identification, support, <strong>and</strong> withdrawal from the job. Journal of Applied<br />

Social Psychology, 37, 457–477.<br />

van Knippenberg, D., van Knippenberg, B., & Bobbio, A. (2008). <strong>Leader</strong>s as agents of continuity: Self continuity <strong>and</strong> resistance to collective change. In F. Sani (Ed.),<br />

Self-continuity: Individual <strong>and</strong> collective perspectives (pp. 175–186). New York: Psychology Press.<br />

Webster, D. M., & Kruglanski, A. W. (1994). Individual differences in the need for cognitive closure. Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 67, 1049–1062.<br />

Zhou, J., & Shalley, C. E. (2008). H<strong>and</strong>book of organizational creativity. New York: Erlbaum.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!