30.01.2015 Views

Tagging and Graffiti - Victoria University of Wellington

Tagging and Graffiti - Victoria University of Wellington

Tagging and Graffiti - Victoria University of Wellington

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>:<br />

attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Joanne Cox, Fiona Hutton, Mike Rowe<br />

Institute <strong>of</strong> Criminology, <strong>Victoria</strong> <strong>University</strong> <strong>Wellington</strong><br />

Report prepared for Ministry <strong>of</strong> Justice<br />

December 2009<br />

1


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Contents<br />

List <strong>of</strong> tables <strong>and</strong> figures 3<br />

Glossary 4<br />

Executive summary 5<br />

Introduction 10<br />

Aims <strong>and</strong> objectives <strong>of</strong> the study 10<br />

Methodology 11<br />

Overview <strong>of</strong> survey 12<br />

Overview <strong>of</strong> focus groups 13<br />

Limitations 15<br />

Literature review <strong>and</strong> overview <strong>of</strong> policy 16<br />

Policy developments 21<br />

Findings 24<br />

General attitudes toward graffiti 24<br />

The commissioning <strong>of</strong> graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging 28<br />

Desistance from graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging 39<br />

Discussion <strong>and</strong> conclusion 48<br />

References 53<br />

Appendices<br />

On-line survey 55<br />

Schedule for focus group discussion 61<br />

Pr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>of</strong> focus groups 62<br />

2


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

List <strong>of</strong> Tables<br />

Table 1: Pr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>of</strong> respondents<br />

Table 2: Attitudes toward graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging<br />

Table 3: Do you think that graffiti is a bad thing (by gender, age,<br />

ethnicity <strong>and</strong> participation)<br />

Table 4: Positive <strong>and</strong> negative attributes <strong>of</strong> graffiti<br />

Table 5: Have you ever written graffiti<br />

Table 6: Property selected for graffiti<br />

Table 7: Type <strong>of</strong> graffiti<br />

Table 8: Reasons for writing graffiti<br />

Table 9: Future graffiti<br />

Table 10: Future participation in graffiti, by age<br />

Table 11: Ranking <strong>of</strong> Reasons for <strong>Graffiti</strong><br />

Table 12: Should more effort be made to stop graffiti, by gender, age, ethnicity<br />

<strong>and</strong> participation<br />

Table 13: Reasons to stop doing graffiti<br />

Table 14: Reasons to stop graffiti, by age<br />

Table 15: Average rating <strong>of</strong> reasons to stop doing graffiti<br />

Table 16: Average score for strategies to stop graffiti<br />

List <strong>of</strong> Figures<br />

Figure 1: Reasons people write graffiti, by personal involvement<br />

Figure 2: <strong>Graffiti</strong> is more exciting because it is illegal, by (non)involvement in writing<br />

Figure 3: Reason to stop graffiti, by form <strong>of</strong> graffiti<br />

Figure 4: Strategies to prevent graffiti, by participation<br />

3


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Glossary<br />

Bomb/bombing ‡ – To bomb or hit is to paint many surfaces in an area. Bombers <strong>of</strong>ten<br />

choose throw–ups or tags over complex pieces, as they can be executed more quickly<br />

Buff ‡ – To remove painted graffiti with chemicals <strong>and</strong> other instruments, or to paint over it<br />

with a flat colour<br />

Crew* – a group <strong>of</strong> friends with similar interests<br />

Getting up* – putting in a lot <strong>of</strong> writing<br />

<strong>Graffiti</strong>* – markings on a surface<br />

King* – a master <strong>of</strong> styles<br />

Masterpiece – see piece<br />

Piece/masterpiece* – a fully filled, multi-coloured (sometimes complex) large stylised<br />

signature<br />

Roller ‡ – same as a stomper<br />

Sketch book* – a book <strong>of</strong> sketches or drawings which writers will carry with them<br />

Stomper (or stompie) † – using large rollers to paint block letters, usually outlined in can<br />

Style* – a particular kind, sort or type, as with reference to form, appearance or character<br />

Tag* – a simple, stylised self–elected name or signature written in public<br />

Throw up* – a scarcely filled or quick bubble derived style<br />

Toy* – a new person to the scene or a less talented writer<br />

Wackie (or Wacky) † – Joint letter tags that are joined up, ‘all wackied out’<br />

Wild style † – <strong>Graffiti</strong> with text so stylized as to be difficult to read, <strong>of</strong>ten with interlocking,<br />

three–dimensional type<br />

Writer* – a person who is a participator in the art form <strong>of</strong> writing<br />

Writing* – the name for the underground movement consisting <strong>of</strong> ‘getting up <strong>and</strong> getting<br />

seen’ with your name (or alias). The act <strong>of</strong> writing graffiti using marks, paint pens or<br />

spray paint.<br />

*<br />

Words <strong>of</strong> art (2009, March 2009). Tearaway, March 2009, 5.<br />

‡<br />

Wikipedia, http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/<strong>Graffiti</strong>_terminology<br />

†Manurewa Focus group participant explanation, May 8, 2009, Auckl<strong>and</strong>, New Zeal<strong>and</strong>.<br />

4


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Executive summary<br />

The central aim <strong>of</strong> the study was to:<br />

• develop underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> <strong>of</strong>fender motivation <strong>and</strong> attitudes <strong>of</strong> young people more<br />

generally to tagging <strong>and</strong> graffiti.<br />

More specific questions form the central objectives <strong>of</strong> the study:<br />

• to develop knowledge <strong>of</strong> <strong>of</strong>fenders’ attitudes toward graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging;<br />

• to underst<strong>and</strong> the extent (if any) to which tagging <strong>and</strong> graffiti <strong>of</strong>fending is associated<br />

with other criminal activity;<br />

• to identify factors that shape ‘involvement decisions’, ‘event decisions’ <strong>and</strong><br />

‘desistance decisions’ (Cornish <strong>and</strong> Clarke, 2006);<br />

• to examine the extent to which the transgressive nature <strong>of</strong> tagging <strong>and</strong> graffiti are<br />

central to <strong>of</strong>fender motivation;<br />

• to establish the relationships between tagging, graffiti <strong>and</strong> other aspects <strong>of</strong> youth<br />

subculture.<br />

The research was conducted via an online survey <strong>and</strong> a series <strong>of</strong> focus groups. Findings from<br />

the survey <strong>and</strong> focus groups are presented in relation to three broad themes: general<br />

attitudes toward graffiti, the commissioning <strong>and</strong> writing <strong>of</strong> graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging, <strong>and</strong><br />

desistance from graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging.<br />

Four themes <strong>of</strong> general significance emerge from the data:<br />

• graffiti writers do not form a group clearly or wholly distinct from non-graffiti writers;<br />

• graffiti is a meaningful cultural <strong>and</strong> social practice for writers <strong>and</strong> only indirectly<br />

‘appreciated’ for its illegality;<br />

• graffiti writing is associated with a desire for local celebrity;<br />

• graffiti writers’ perspectives on desistance suggest highly bounded rationality about<br />

prevention strategies.<br />

General attitudes toward graffiti<br />

One <strong>of</strong> the main themes was that there is a distinction between ‘tagging’ <strong>and</strong> graffiti. The<br />

former is acknowledged as a “simple, stylised self-elected name or signature written in<br />

public” while graffiti is referred to as “markings on a surface” (Tearaway, 2009). A majority <strong>of</strong><br />

respondents argued that the circumstances determined whether graffiti is a negative thing:<br />

• 81.9 per cent ‘agreed’ or ‘strongly agreed’ that graffiti should be tolerated in some<br />

circumstances;<br />

• those who reported that they had written graffiti were most likely to report that it is<br />

never a ‘bad thing’ (23.4 per cent);<br />

5


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

• only a small minority (12.2 per cent) felt that graffiti was a ‘bad thing’ in all cases.<br />

Focus group participants also expressed the overlapping status <strong>of</strong> graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging:<br />

To other people it is v<strong>and</strong>alism but to us it is art.<br />

If it is graffiti then it is art, if it is tagging then it is v<strong>and</strong>alism.<br />

The commissioning <strong>of</strong> graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging<br />

A slight majority <strong>of</strong> respondents reported that they had not written graffiti. The survey asked<br />

respondents to state whether they write graffiti, have ever written graffiti or have not written<br />

graffiti. A large minority reported that they do write graffiti or that they had done at some<br />

point. The data indicates a substantial difference between male <strong>and</strong> female involvement in<br />

graffiti: a slight majority <strong>of</strong> male respondents agreeing that they had written graffiti at some<br />

time; while a clear majority <strong>of</strong> females reported that they had not written graffiti.<br />

Age <strong>and</strong> participation in graffiti<br />

There does not appear to be any clear direction in the relationship between age <strong>and</strong> reported<br />

writing. More than half <strong>of</strong> those aged under-14 reported that they had, at least on occasion,<br />

written graffiti, but the highest participation group was those aged 22-23 years.<br />

Property selected for graffiti<br />

The classification <strong>of</strong> property in terms <strong>of</strong> ownership status does not exert a significant<br />

influence on ‘event decisions’. Focus group extracts illustrate that the legal status <strong>of</strong> property<br />

was less significant than other factors:<br />

You only do it where people can see it.(Focus group participant)<br />

The best place is on the concrete ‘cos then they don’t remove it. (Focus group<br />

participant)<br />

Fences, big walls, plain white walls lures them to it. (Focus group participant)<br />

Gender <strong>and</strong> personal participation in graffiti<br />

Males reported that they engaged in different types <strong>of</strong> graffiti at a greater rate than females.<br />

<strong>Graffiti</strong> art was the biggest category selected by those <strong>of</strong> both genders who participated.<br />

6


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Future graffiti participation<br />

Those who had reported that they wrote graffiti were more likely to report that they would<br />

participate in future graffiti activity.<br />

Reasons for graffiti participation<br />

All respondents, including those who write graffiti <strong>and</strong> those who do not, were asked their<br />

opinion on why people write graffiti. Those who participated in graffiti-writing were less likely<br />

to refer to ‘breaking the rules’ as a general motivation for those who write. Similarly,<br />

participants were more likely to stress creativity as a general motivation.<br />

Illegal nature <strong>of</strong> graffiti writing<br />

A majority <strong>of</strong> respondents tended to agree that the illicit nature <strong>of</strong> graffiti made it more<br />

exciting. However, agreement was strongest among those ‘relatively heavily’ involved in<br />

graffiti activity. A large minority <strong>of</strong> respondents, in all three categories, ‘agreed’ that the<br />

illegality <strong>of</strong> graffiti writing made it more exciting.<br />

Desistance from graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging<br />

Youngest <strong>and</strong> oldest groups more strongly favoured greater effort to stop graffiti. Those who<br />

had not participated were more likely to agree that more should be done than those who<br />

had, <strong>and</strong> those who occasionally participated wanted more effort to stop graffiti than those<br />

who might be considered more involved in writing.<br />

Males were less likely to favour greater effort to stop graffiti than females <strong>and</strong> Māori <strong>and</strong><br />

European respondents were less likely to favour this than other ethnic groups.<br />

Reasons to stop personally doing graffiti<br />

Those who engaged in graffiti were asked a series <strong>of</strong> questions about factors that might lead<br />

them to stop writing in general terms. The most significant reason to stop, across all groups,<br />

was apprehension by police. The quick removal <strong>of</strong> graffiti was rated very lowly as a reason<br />

for graffiti writers to stop. Family disapproval was less likely to be a factor. The most common<br />

response, across most forms, was that none <strong>of</strong> these strategies would lead respondents to<br />

desist from graffiti.<br />

7


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Age <strong>and</strong> desistance<br />

Peer pressure had less influence on desistance decisions as respondents got older, while the<br />

threat <strong>of</strong> being apprehended tended to become more notable as an influence among older<br />

age cohorts. Responses showed that moving through life course stages was seen as an<br />

influence on desistance.<br />

Reasons for desistence in general<br />

All respondents were asked to rate reasons why people in general might stop writing graffiti.<br />

Each option was rated between 5 (strongly agree) <strong>and</strong> 1 (strongly disagree); the average<br />

rating for each option being:<br />

• providing legitimate sites (e.g. graffiti walls): 4.1<br />

• making <strong>of</strong>fenders clean-up graffiti: 3.4<br />

• custodial sentences: 3.1<br />

• better education on victim impact: 3.1.<br />

• ‘naming <strong>and</strong> shaming’ <strong>of</strong>fenders: 2.9<br />

Pr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>of</strong> respondents<br />

A total <strong>of</strong> 272 youth were involved in 21 focus groups around New Zeal<strong>and</strong> between April <strong>and</strong><br />

May 2009. No demographic details <strong>of</strong> these participants were recorded. The on-line survey<br />

was completed by 773 respondents: 372 were males (48.1 per cent) <strong>and</strong> 319 (41.3 per cent)<br />

female, with 82 (10.6 per cent) not specifying their gender. The ethnic pr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>of</strong> respondents<br />

by percentage:<br />

% %<br />

NZ European 42.8 Asian 4.7<br />

Māori 18.5 European 9.1<br />

Pacific 6.2 Other ‘Mixed’ 17.6<br />

The age pr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>of</strong> respondents (per cent) was:<br />

% % %<br />


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Conclusion<br />

The research findings provide little evidence to suggest that graffiti writers are a distinct subgroup<br />

among young people with identifiable perceptions <strong>of</strong> graffiti, <strong>and</strong> the most appropriate<br />

responses to it, that differ from the wider population. The implications <strong>of</strong> this survey <strong>and</strong><br />

focus group research for policy responses to graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging are many <strong>and</strong> various. Key<br />

among these is that young people expressed a wide-range <strong>of</strong> perspectives on graffiti <strong>and</strong><br />

tagging, <strong>and</strong> that those who admitted some degree <strong>of</strong> participation did not always differ<br />

greatly from those who do not participate. Young people had highly nuanced views such that<br />

some forms <strong>of</strong> writing are valued for their artistic content <strong>and</strong> the risks overcome <strong>and</strong> skill<br />

required in producing pieces. Only a small minority <strong>of</strong> young people suggested that v<strong>and</strong>alism<br />

<strong>and</strong> criminal damage were primary motivations. Even those who admitted participating in<br />

graffiti <strong>and</strong> v<strong>and</strong>alism maintained that there were areas <strong>and</strong> types <strong>of</strong> property that were not<br />

acceptable sites for writing <strong>and</strong> that these should not be subject to the damage <strong>and</strong><br />

‘disrespect’ sometimes associated with such activity.<br />

9


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Introduction<br />

A range <strong>of</strong> evidence suggests that problems relating to graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging in New Zeal<strong>and</strong><br />

have had a high pr<strong>of</strong>ile in recent years. Attention has been focused on cases where owners <strong>of</strong><br />

property that has been tagged have responded in ways that have raised concerns about the<br />

use <strong>of</strong> force <strong>and</strong> the extent to which private citizens are entitled to directly intervene to<br />

protect their premises. Perceptions that graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging are growing problems are<br />

reinforced by steps taken by courts, police, local government <strong>and</strong> Parliament to introduce<br />

more effective intervention strategies. In 2008 the court in Napier sentenced several teenage<br />

taggers to terms in jail, partly, according to press reports <strong>of</strong> judges’ comments in court, in<br />

order to deter others from contributing to a growing problem in the Hawke’s Bay region. In<br />

the same period, it was reported that police in <strong>Wellington</strong> had adopted a high visibility<br />

response to tagging whereby <strong>of</strong>fenders were required to wear pink vests while deployed to<br />

clean up local graffiti. Local government expenditure on cleaning up graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging<br />

represents a significant commitment from scarce resources, as is reviewed below. Nationally,<br />

legal controls on the sale <strong>of</strong> spray paint were tightened by the Summary Offences (<strong>Tagging</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong> V<strong>and</strong>alism) Amendment Act 2008 <strong>and</strong> changes to the 1961 Crimes Act<br />

established a maximum term <strong>of</strong> seven years imprisonment for such intentional property<br />

damage (New Zeal<strong>and</strong> Government, 1961). A range <strong>of</strong> other responses are included in the<br />

STOP (Stop <strong>Tagging</strong> Our Place) strategy that was introduced in 2008. With the strategy <strong>and</strong><br />

the general context <strong>of</strong> concern about the extent <strong>and</strong> impact <strong>of</strong> graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging, the<br />

current study was developed <strong>and</strong> conducted in the first half <strong>of</strong> 2009.<br />

It should be noted that this research project was not an evaluation <strong>of</strong> the existing STOP<br />

strategy, nor <strong>of</strong> the various ways that policy makers have tried to encourage graffiti writers to<br />

desist from their behaviour e.g. legal graffiti walls. The primary aims <strong>of</strong> this study were to<br />

develop an underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> what motivates graffiti artists <strong>and</strong> to examine the attitudes <strong>of</strong> a<br />

broad range <strong>of</strong> young people towards graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging.<br />

Aim <strong>and</strong> objectives <strong>of</strong> the study<br />

While many initiatives have been introduced to tackle problems associated with graffiti <strong>and</strong><br />

tagging, it continues to be the case that little is known about the perspectives <strong>of</strong> young<br />

people in general <strong>and</strong> participants in particular. To that end, the central aim <strong>of</strong> the study was:<br />

• to develop underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> <strong>of</strong>fender motivation <strong>and</strong> attitudes <strong>of</strong> young people more<br />

generally to tagging <strong>and</strong> graffiti.<br />

10


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

More specific questions form the central objectives <strong>of</strong> the study:<br />

• to develop knowledge <strong>of</strong> <strong>of</strong>fenders’ attitudes toward graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging;<br />

• to underst<strong>and</strong> the extent (if any) to which tagging <strong>and</strong> graffiti <strong>of</strong>fending is associated<br />

with other criminal activity;<br />

• to identify factors that shape ‘involvement decisions’, ‘event decisions’ <strong>and</strong><br />

‘desistance decisions’ (Cornish <strong>and</strong> Clarke, 2006);<br />

• to examine the extent to which the transgressive nature <strong>of</strong> tagging <strong>and</strong> graffiti are<br />

central to <strong>of</strong>fender motivation;<br />

• to establish the relation between tagging, graffiti <strong>and</strong> other aspects <strong>of</strong> youth<br />

subculture.<br />

Methodology<br />

Researching those who are involved in criminal or deviant activities has many pitfalls, a key<br />

issue being that <strong>of</strong> recruiting respondents. Young people as a group are also notoriously<br />

difficult to engage in research projects, especially those who are engaged in activities such as<br />

graffiti writing. The research project discussed here is based on a mixed methods approach<br />

towards underst<strong>and</strong>ing a specific issue: to develop underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> <strong>of</strong>fender motivation <strong>and</strong><br />

attitudes <strong>of</strong> young people more generally to tagging <strong>and</strong> graffiti. The project takes a<br />

pragmatic approach to mixed methods research <strong>and</strong> aims to view graffiti from more than one<br />

viewpoint to gain better knowledge about the issues surrounding this behaviour.<br />

Mixed methods research refers to that which uses two or more methods in a single research<br />

project (Gilbert 2008), although social scientists disagree about what counts as mixing<br />

methods. Academics such as Bryman (2004), <strong>and</strong> Creswell <strong>and</strong> Plano Clark (2007) use the<br />

term in relation to projects that only combine qualitative <strong>and</strong> quantitative methods. Gilbert<br />

(2008) <strong>and</strong> Alex<strong>and</strong>er et al. (2008, cited in Gilbert 2008: 127) argue that this is a narrow<br />

definition <strong>and</strong> that mixed methods should also refer to research that brings together two or<br />

more qualitative or quantitative methods. Mixed methods research was used in this project to<br />

combine both qualitative <strong>and</strong> quantitative methods; an online survey <strong>and</strong> focus groups. The<br />

survey was designed to get a ‘broad brush’ view <strong>of</strong> graffiti <strong>and</strong> to elicit the responses <strong>of</strong><br />

participants towards deterrence-based strategies to stop graffiti in public places. The focus<br />

groups were employed to examine the issues related to graffiti such as its diversity <strong>and</strong><br />

complexity in more detail, with several different groups <strong>of</strong> respondents making up the focus<br />

group discussions.<br />

The question <strong>of</strong> conducting qualitative <strong>and</strong> mixed methods research on criminological topics<br />

has been raised by many researchers (for example see Noakes <strong>and</strong> Wincup, 2004). In the<br />

11


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

context <strong>of</strong> studying graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging there are a number <strong>of</strong> reasons in choosing a mixed<br />

methods approach that includes qualitative methods. First, this approach allows some insight<br />

into the ‘dark figure <strong>of</strong> crime’ defined as, ‘the figure for unrecorded crime or undetected<br />

<strong>of</strong>fenders, that is to say those not included in <strong>of</strong>ficial statistics’ (Coleman <strong>and</strong> Moynihan,<br />

1996; 146). Using both quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative methods <strong>and</strong> combining different ways <strong>of</strong><br />

collecting data <strong>of</strong>fers the opportunity for a more complete underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> the nature <strong>and</strong><br />

extent <strong>of</strong> graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging. The use <strong>of</strong> qualitative methods such as focus groups <strong>of</strong>fers the<br />

opportunity to examine the contexts in which graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging take place as well as the<br />

meanings attached to these behaviours by graffiti writers. Second, the term ‘appreciative<br />

studies’ was first used by Matza (1969) in referring to studies <strong>of</strong> deviant subcultures.<br />

Appreciative criminology refers to ‘an approach that seeks to underst<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> appreciate the<br />

social world from the point <strong>of</strong> view <strong>of</strong> the individual or category <strong>of</strong> individual, with particular<br />

reference to crime <strong>and</strong> deviance’ (Jupp, 2001; 12). Developing an appreciation <strong>of</strong> the<br />

meanings <strong>and</strong> motivations <strong>of</strong> graffiti writers is important in developing effective strategies to<br />

prevent graffiti v<strong>and</strong>alism.<br />

Overview <strong>of</strong> survey<br />

An online survey was established on the Ministry <strong>of</strong> Youth Development (MYD) website, using<br />

the Survey Monkey web-tool. The survey comprised <strong>of</strong> 23 questions relating to general<br />

perceptions <strong>of</strong> tagging <strong>and</strong> graffiti, the deviant <strong>and</strong> criminal status <strong>of</strong> tagging <strong>and</strong> graffiti, <strong>and</strong><br />

factors that might influence desistance. Demographic information (age, gender <strong>and</strong> ethnicity)<br />

was collected. A copy <strong>of</strong> the survey is included in Appendix 1. The survey was hosted on the<br />

MYD website in May <strong>and</strong> June 2009. Participants in the survey were self-selected <strong>and</strong><br />

although a relatively large number completed returns it is not possible to ascertain the extent<br />

to which they are representative <strong>of</strong> the broader youth population <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>.<br />

The on-line survey was completed by 773 respondents: 372 were males (48.1 per cent) <strong>and</strong><br />

319 (41.3 per cent) female, with 82 (10.6 per cent) not specifying their gender. Table 1<br />

shows the ethnic pr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>of</strong> respondents <strong>and</strong> age breakdown <strong>of</strong> those who completed the<br />

survey.<br />

12


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Table 1: Pr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>of</strong> respondents (number <strong>and</strong> percentages)<br />

253040 Total<br />

% n % n % n % n % n % n % n % n % n % n<br />

NZ<br />

30.0 3 34.0 54 44.0 102 40.4 21 46.4 13 62.5 15 53.8 21 43.4 23 55.2 16 42.8 268<br />

European<br />

Māori 20.0 2 22.6 36 18.5 43 15.4 8 10.7 3 0.0 0 7.7 3 30.2 16 17.2 5 18.5 116<br />

Pacific 10.0 1 6.3 10 5.2 12 11.5 6 7.1 2 4.2 1 7.7 3 3.8 2 6.9 2 6.2 39<br />

Asian 10.0 1 8.8 14 4.3 10 5.8 3 14.3 4 4.2 1 5.1 2 1.9 1 0.0 0 5.8 36<br />

European 20.0 2 4.4 7 9.1 21 19.2 10 10.7 3 12.5 3 12.8 5 5.7 3 10.3 3 9.1 57<br />

Other 10.0 1 23.9 38 19.0 44 7.7 4 10.7 3 16.7 4 12.8 5 15.1 8 10.3 3 17.6 110<br />

ethnic<br />

Total 100 10 100 159 100 232 100 52 100 28 100 24 100 39 100 53 100 29 100 626<br />

In keeping with established practice respondents were able to select more than one ethnic<br />

affiliation. Respondents who did so (17.6 per cent <strong>of</strong> all respondents) are categorised as<br />

’other’ in this analysis.<br />

Overview <strong>of</strong> focus groups<br />

The other method <strong>of</strong> collecting data was via a series <strong>of</strong> focus groups, conducted in various<br />

locations around New Zeal<strong>and</strong> in April <strong>and</strong> May 2009. Thirteen <strong>of</strong> these were facilitated with<br />

the help <strong>of</strong> MYD staff. Some groups incorporated young people engaged in alternative<br />

education, mentoring based programmes <strong>and</strong> structured activity based programmes. Further<br />

youth were recruited through graffiti cleanup <strong>and</strong> community groups; others were members<br />

<strong>of</strong> youth panels that inform policy development across a range <strong>of</strong> government sectors.<br />

Participants were aged between 14 <strong>and</strong> 24 years <strong>of</strong> age; no demographic data was recorded<br />

for these participants. A copy <strong>of</strong> the interview schedule used in the focus group discussions is<br />

in Appendix 2 <strong>and</strong> a list <strong>of</strong> the focus groups is included in Appendix 3. These focus groups<br />

were intended to provide qualitative data from a cross-section <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong> youth. Another<br />

series <strong>of</strong> seven groups were conducted in various locations with young people engaged to<br />

various degrees in tagging <strong>and</strong> graffiti behaviour. Some <strong>of</strong> these groups were coordinated via<br />

graffiti cleanup groups, others through alternative education <strong>and</strong> youth projects working with<br />

young people who might not be accessible through more mainstream contacts with education<br />

<strong>and</strong> youth services.<br />

An immediate problem this research project encountered was the recruitment <strong>of</strong> graffiti<br />

‘<strong>of</strong>fenders’. Marginalised young people feature prominently among ‘hard to reach groups’ that<br />

criminological research <strong>of</strong>ten seeks to study (Pain <strong>and</strong> Francis, 2003). The original intention<br />

was to secure access to those who had been arrested <strong>and</strong> convicted through agencies such<br />

as the NZ Police <strong>and</strong> Department <strong>of</strong> Corrections. However, given the complex ethical<br />

considerations such an approach would entail <strong>and</strong> time constraints on the project, this was<br />

13


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

not possible. An additional problem with this way <strong>of</strong> recruiting <strong>of</strong>fenders was how to<br />

disentangle their graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging from other <strong>of</strong>fending behaviour. It was considered<br />

unlikely that those who had been formally processed by the criminal justice system (CJS)<br />

would have been convicted for graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging alone. <strong>Graffiti</strong> <strong>and</strong> tagging for this group<br />

may be intertwined with other <strong>of</strong>fending behaviours that had no relation to graffiti writing.<br />

Therefore a different approach to recruiting participants was adopted. Utilising the extensive<br />

contacts <strong>of</strong> the MYD 1 a variety <strong>of</strong> groups <strong>of</strong> young people were accessed for focus group<br />

discussions. In conducting the focus groups problems became apparent in distinguishing<br />

those who had been involved in graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging <strong>and</strong> those who had not. In discussing<br />

illegal behaviour with young people a direct approach focussing on their own behaviour was<br />

not deemed appropriate as this would cause respondents to ‘shut down’ <strong>and</strong> little data would<br />

be forthcoming.<br />

The approach taken was to ask for the opinions <strong>of</strong> focus group respondents around a series<br />

<strong>of</strong> issues relating to graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging. Seven <strong>of</strong> the focus groups contained respondents<br />

who were directly involved in graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging. 2 The other 13 groups contained<br />

respondents who may have been involved in graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging (some respondents from<br />

these groups referred to their own illegal activities), <strong>and</strong> those who were not directly involved<br />

in graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging. Researching this issue is particularly problematic because it is<br />

embedded in youth culture through hip hop music <strong>and</strong> style. Consequently the majority <strong>of</strong> the<br />

respondents had detailed knowledge about graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging <strong>and</strong> used slang terms such as<br />

‘bomb’ <strong>and</strong> ‘wackie’. They also <strong>of</strong>ten referred to knowledge <strong>of</strong> peer group behaviour as well<br />

as their own graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging behaviour.<br />

The research team also conducted a number <strong>of</strong> informal interviews with key informants, such<br />

as police staff, community workers <strong>and</strong> local government <strong>of</strong>ficers with a particular interest in<br />

graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging. Many <strong>of</strong> these respondents were identified by the Ministry <strong>of</strong> Justice <strong>and</strong><br />

are engaged in the STOP strategy. These interviews were not recorded but have helped to<br />

shape underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> the issues.<br />

The research was conducted with the approval <strong>of</strong> <strong>Victoria</strong> <strong>University</strong> <strong>Wellington</strong>’s ethics<br />

committee (reference # 16374). All those who participated in the focus groups completed<br />

consent forms, signed by parents or guardians for those aged under 16 years. The online<br />

survey asked for no information that would enable respondents to be identified <strong>and</strong><br />

participation was taken as indication <strong>of</strong> consent.<br />

1 The help <strong>of</strong> Sen Thong <strong>and</strong> Lorraine Gittings has been invaluable in this part <strong>of</strong> the research project.<br />

2 The respondents from these groups were identified by police <strong>and</strong> community agencies as having been<br />

involved in graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging. Others were involved in graffiti writing through their attachment to<br />

the hip hop community.<br />

14


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Limitations<br />

The survey did not draw a representative sample <strong>and</strong> so it is not possible to generalise these<br />

findings to the whole population. There were several reasons why a representative sample<br />

could not be selected. Online respondents might be more likely to be involved in or interested<br />

in graffiti <strong>and</strong> this might predispose them to participate. As graffiti is an under researched<br />

topic in New Zeal<strong>and</strong> the research team did not have enough information about graffiti<br />

writers as a population to undertake representative sampling techniques. As stated previously<br />

researching those involved in criminal or deviant activities has many challenges not least the<br />

recruiting <strong>of</strong> respondents. The survey respondents were a self selecting, open sample. The<br />

sample was ‘open’ as its purpose was not to represent a particular population (given the<br />

difficulties this would entail) but to keep the collection <strong>of</strong> data as unconstrained as possible.<br />

This approach was considered necessary due to the difficulty <strong>of</strong> accessing respondents for<br />

this kind <strong>of</strong> research <strong>and</strong> the uncertainty as to how successful this online approach to<br />

gathering data would be. Therefore the survey was designed to access as large a sample as<br />

possible in order to gain information in a short space <strong>of</strong> time.<br />

An additional problem in using the survey was that occasionally the responses contradicted<br />

those from the focus groups. For example, a common theme in the focus groups discussions<br />

was that graffiti writers were motivated by a desire to ‘get their name up’, to be recognised,<br />

to be a ‘celebrity’. In the survey only 24 out <strong>of</strong> 309 graffiti writers chose the category ‘to get<br />

may name up <strong>and</strong> feel like a celebrity’ in answer to the question ‘what would you say was the<br />

MAIN reason why you did it’ However the question does ask what the main reason is for<br />

engaging in graffiti. Therefore the responses from the survey suggest that ‘to get may name<br />

up <strong>and</strong> feel like a celebrity’ is not the most significant reason for engaging in graffiti writing<br />

(the survey respondents could only choose one response).<br />

As no respondents were excluded from the survey <strong>and</strong> the sample was self selecting, some <strong>of</strong><br />

the respondents fell outside the category <strong>of</strong> ‘youth’. The survey did not specify when or how<br />

recently graffiti writing was carried out so it was unclear whether graffiti writing was<br />

contemporary behaviour or something no longer engaged in. This may account for the fact<br />

that 61 out <strong>of</strong> 773 respondents were over 25. The older age group are also likely to be those<br />

who have desisted from graffiti so their views are important when considering policy<br />

interventions.<br />

15


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Literature review <strong>and</strong> overview <strong>of</strong> policy<br />

Problems <strong>of</strong> incivilities, <strong>of</strong>fensiveness <strong>and</strong> disorder have a long history <strong>and</strong> are increasingly a<br />

concern in contemporary society (Stanko, 2000). One reason for this might be that crimes <strong>of</strong><br />

a disorderly or ‘anti-social’ nature are argued to contribute strongly to people’s fear <strong>of</strong> crime<br />

(Herbert, 1993; Doran <strong>and</strong> Lees, 2005). Offences that might be minor in legal <strong>and</strong> criminal<br />

justice terms (such as littering, graffiti <strong>and</strong> public nuisance) are referred to as ‘incivilities’ <strong>and</strong><br />

are said to make people feel unsafe. The broken windows thesis (Wilson <strong>and</strong> Kelling, 1982)<br />

stated that crimes such as graffiti encouraged further criminal activities <strong>and</strong> that disorder <strong>and</strong><br />

crime are usually linked – one broken window in a building that is left unfixed is a sign that<br />

nobody cares <strong>and</strong> further acts <strong>of</strong> v<strong>and</strong>alism will follow. This way <strong>of</strong> thinking about crime has<br />

been heavily criticised as it is argued that it was wrong in assuming an automatic escalation<br />

in crime because <strong>of</strong> disorder (Bottoms, 2006). In addition, it was found that people’s<br />

perceptions <strong>of</strong> crime in areas where incivilities occurred did not necessarily correlate with the<br />

actual crime rate. People <strong>of</strong>ten perceived that crime rates were higher than they actually<br />

were. However, the concept <strong>of</strong> signal crimes as put forward by Innes (2004), in which certain<br />

crimes act as warnings to specific populations about risk, emphasises the importance <strong>of</strong><br />

disorder in discussions <strong>of</strong> crime. Research has shown that the top ‘signals’ in relation to<br />

perceived risk in the United Kingdom (UK) are ‘disorderly events occurring in public space’<br />

(Innes, 2004: 158).<br />

Despite criticisms, the ideas contained within the broken windows thesis have continued to<br />

influence crime prevention policy. Situational crime prevention (SCP) <strong>and</strong> Crime Prevention<br />

Through Environmental Design (CPTED) programmes are based on altering the environment<br />

or situation to make it less likely that crime would occur. Many <strong>of</strong> these kinds <strong>of</strong> programmes<br />

are focussed on ‘incivilities’ such as graffiti <strong>and</strong> v<strong>and</strong>alism. Situational crime control<br />

programmes are also influenced by rational choice theory (Cornish <strong>and</strong> Clarke, 1986) in that<br />

the ‘criminal’ is assumed to make a rational choice about whether to commit crime based on<br />

assessment <strong>of</strong> risks <strong>and</strong> benefits. Logically, following this line <strong>of</strong> reasoning suggests that<br />

harsher punishments or increased police presence (risk <strong>of</strong> being caught) should act as a<br />

deterrent to potential <strong>of</strong>fenders. However, when examining the motivations <strong>of</strong> graffiti writers<br />

in New Zeal<strong>and</strong> (<strong>and</strong> globally) a different picture is constructed in which it is clear that<br />

deterrence based strategies will not always be the most effective in tackling the issues<br />

surrounding graffiti.<br />

In this context, the purpose <strong>of</strong> this literature review is to investigate young peoples’ attitudes<br />

towards graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging in New Zeal<strong>and</strong>. <strong>Graffiti</strong> has become a topic <strong>of</strong> concern in New<br />

Zeal<strong>and</strong> in recent years (Ministry <strong>of</strong> Justice, 2006; Ministry <strong>of</strong> Justice, 2008), but even a<br />

cursory review <strong>of</strong> these debates quickly reveals that it is important to clarify the terms<br />

16


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

‘graffiti’ <strong>and</strong> ‘tagging’. Although the two are <strong>of</strong>ten combined, they are not the same nor<br />

interchangeable (Halsey <strong>and</strong> Young, 2002). Indeed, the discussion below reveals a host <strong>of</strong><br />

other terms used for different forms <strong>of</strong> graffiti writing. A tag is described as a “simple,<br />

stylised self-elected name or signature written in public” while graffiti is referred to as<br />

“markings on a surface” (Tearaway, 2009). For the purpose <strong>of</strong> this report the terms ‘tagging’<br />

<strong>and</strong> ‘graffiti’ are used synonymously, although it is recognised that writers themselves <strong>of</strong>ten<br />

attach considerable significance to different forms.<br />

The legal framework surrounding graffiti is complex in New Zeal<strong>and</strong>. The Summary Offences<br />

Act (1981) defines graffiti in the following terms:<br />

• damages or defaces any building, structure, road, tree, property, or other thing by<br />

writing, drawing, painting, spraying, or etching on it, or otherwise marking it;<br />

• without lawful authority; <strong>and</strong><br />

• without the consent <strong>of</strong> the occupier or owner or other person in lawful control.<br />

New Zeal<strong>and</strong> Government (1981)<br />

The Local Government Act (2002) covers various local authority bylaws to deal with tagging<br />

<strong>and</strong> graffiti as part <strong>of</strong> a wider scope <strong>of</strong> dealing with community concerns (Ministry <strong>of</strong> Justice,<br />

2008). Auckl<strong>and</strong>’s Manukau City Council had previous legislation to deal with graffiti under<br />

the Manukau City Council (Control <strong>of</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>) Act 2008. This law was repealed under the<br />

amendment to the Summary Offences Act (1981) with the passing <strong>of</strong> the Summary Offences<br />

(<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong> V<strong>and</strong>alism) Amendment Act (2008). <strong>Graffiti</strong> is also incorporated in the<br />

Crimes Act (1961) s.269(2) under intentional damage, which refers to:<br />

• Intentional damage – intentionally damaging property, with a penalty <strong>of</strong> a prison<br />

term <strong>of</strong> up to seven years.<br />

The Summary Offences Act (1981) was amended in 2008 to introduce new legislation<br />

restricting the sale <strong>of</strong> spray cans <strong>and</strong> other paraphernalia related to graffiti to those under the<br />

age <strong>of</strong> 18. Under the new law spray cans must be stored in areas the general public do not<br />

have direct access to <strong>and</strong> must ask sales staff for assistance to purchase said cans (New<br />

Zeal<strong>and</strong> Government, 2008).<br />

A search <strong>of</strong> the literature reveals a scarcity <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong> research; the available material<br />

also shows a focus on crime prevention rather than the graffiti writer’s viewpoint (Halsey <strong>and</strong><br />

Young, 2002: 166). <strong>Graffiti</strong> is an issue that costs local <strong>and</strong> state governments in New Zeal<strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> internationally (Ferrell, 1995; Halsey <strong>and</strong> Young, 2002; Ministry <strong>of</strong> Justice, 2006). Halsey<br />

<strong>and</strong> Young (2002) described the financial <strong>and</strong> time intensive costs for graffiti removal in<br />

recent Australian research. Although hundreds <strong>of</strong> graffiti crimes are processed annually, a<br />

large number <strong>of</strong> graffitists elude capture <strong>and</strong> punishment (Halsey <strong>and</strong> Young 2002). Local<br />

17


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

government in New Zeal<strong>and</strong> spends considerable sums on removing it: in 2006 Auckl<strong>and</strong>,<br />

New Zeal<strong>and</strong>, spent approximately $5 million on graffiti clean-up. In 2007 it is estimated that<br />

Manukau City Council spent $1 million on its <strong>Graffiti</strong> Management Strategies (Police News,<br />

2008). Rapid removal <strong>of</strong> graffiti can lead to the illusion that the problem has abated, as the<br />

general public do not see evidence <strong>of</strong> <strong>of</strong>fensive tags or graffiti (Craw, Lel<strong>and</strong> et al., 2006).<br />

Despite these efforts, as targeted sites are cleaned up by councils, they are <strong>of</strong>ten quickly retargeted<br />

(Craw, Lel<strong>and</strong> et al., 2006).<br />

Much <strong>of</strong> the general media, political <strong>and</strong> policy debate surrounding graffiti underst<strong>and</strong> the<br />

phenomenon almost solely in terms <strong>of</strong> damaging v<strong>and</strong>alism that has a negative impact on<br />

crime <strong>and</strong> quality <strong>of</strong> life issues (Ministry <strong>of</strong> Justice <strong>and</strong> Local Government New Zeal<strong>and</strong>, 2006;<br />

Ministry <strong>of</strong> Justice, 2008). On the other h<strong>and</strong>, many youth see it as creative expression, a<br />

way to show society how they feel (Ferrell, 1995; White, 2001). There is a tension between<br />

those organisations attempting to remove <strong>and</strong> prevent graffiti, <strong>and</strong> youth wanting to break<br />

free <strong>of</strong> social constraint (White, 2001). <strong>Graffiti</strong> writers <strong>and</strong> authorities are juxtaposed (Ferrell,<br />

1995; White, 2001). <strong>Graffiti</strong>sts see a blank canvas to be brightened up with colour <strong>and</strong><br />

design; the need to beautify a plain wall (Ferrell, 1995; White, 2001).<br />

In contrast, local government sees the damage caused by graffiti <strong>and</strong> the high cost <strong>of</strong><br />

cleaning it up (Ferrell, 1995; Halsey <strong>and</strong> Young, 2002; Halsey <strong>and</strong> Young, 2006). Several<br />

authors see the need to examine the reasons why young people involve themselves in this<br />

illicit activity (White, 2001; Halsey <strong>and</strong> Young, 2002; Craw, Lel<strong>and</strong> et al., 2006; Halsey <strong>and</strong><br />

Young, 2006). Halsey <strong>and</strong> Young (2006) interviewed graffiti writers to ascertain their<br />

motivations for immersion in the culture. Key among those is that writers gain pleasure <strong>and</strong><br />

enjoyment from writing. Other reasons for graffiti writing included a sense <strong>of</strong> belonging to<br />

the culture, fame <strong>and</strong> the affective response to the writing process; taking hold <strong>of</strong> the can,<br />

seeing their work finished (Halsey <strong>and</strong> Young 2006). These themes form an important part <strong>of</strong><br />

this study <strong>and</strong> are reviewed in more detail below.<br />

Relevant to this discussion is graffiti’s centrality to cultural criminology through its association<br />

with the expression <strong>of</strong> youth culture (Goldson, 2007). Cultural criminology emerged in the<br />

1990’s from sociological <strong>and</strong> critical criminological thinking emphasising youth identity,<br />

activity <strong>and</strong> location (Goldson, 2007). While not all graffiti writers are young, it is seen as a<br />

youth ‘problem’ as a number <strong>of</strong> teenagers engage in graffiti writing (Halsey <strong>and</strong> Young,<br />

2002). Halsey <strong>and</strong> Young (2006) question the perception that graffiti writers are bored <strong>and</strong><br />

cause damage to property due to a lack <strong>of</strong> respect for it. In contrast, graffiti writers feel a<br />

sense <strong>of</strong> belonging to a community, a comradeship with other young people immersed in<br />

graffiti culture. The feeling <strong>of</strong> belonging is supported by Ferrell’s (1995) work in Colorado in<br />

the US, which noted social cohesion between disaffected youth who connect with each other<br />

through the activity <strong>of</strong> writing. Seeing another youth’s tag <strong>and</strong> recognising it supports the<br />

18


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

cohesion to the group (Ferrell, 1995; Halsey <strong>and</strong> Young, 2006). Halsey <strong>and</strong> Young (2006)<br />

criticise academics for presuming that graffiti writers are oppressed or disenfranchised, <strong>and</strong><br />

for ignoring the familial support <strong>and</strong> recognition felt within their community. Furthermore,<br />

Halsey <strong>and</strong> Young (2006) argue against focusing on graffiti writing as meaningless <strong>and</strong><br />

excessive behaviour, as this contradicts the reality <strong>of</strong> preparation <strong>and</strong> practice that goes into<br />

graffitists’ work. Cultural criminology is a theoretical perspective that focuses on the intense<br />

meanings invested in activities such as graffiti <strong>and</strong> the centrality <strong>of</strong> such activities to<br />

participants’ lifestyles. Crime <strong>and</strong> deviance are <strong>of</strong>ten analysed by cultural criminologists as<br />

<strong>of</strong>fering thrills, pleasure <strong>and</strong> the ‘seductions <strong>of</strong> crime’ (Katz, 1988 cited in Carrabine, Iganski,<br />

Lee <strong>and</strong> Plummer, 2004; 84). Cultural criminology also sought to critique traditional<br />

motivational accounts <strong>of</strong> crime which <strong>of</strong>ten saw crime as being in some way pathological.<br />

Although cultural criminology is critical <strong>of</strong> rational choice theories <strong>of</strong> crime, arguing that some<br />

kinds <strong>of</strong> crime such as graffiti are motivated by excitement <strong>and</strong> thrill seeking, this does not<br />

mean that there are not important meanings or motivations behind activities such as graffiti.<br />

Another motivation for graffiti writers comes from their alternative view <strong>of</strong> the urban<br />

l<strong>and</strong>scape (Halsey <strong>and</strong> Young, 2006). <strong>Graffiti</strong>sts see surfaces as marked by weather <strong>and</strong> rust,<br />

damaged by atmospheric conditions <strong>and</strong> so on. As such, no area is pristine <strong>and</strong> consequently<br />

can be altered by anyone. Likewise graffitists look at blank walls as an opportunity to liven up<br />

the environment. All structures <strong>and</strong> surfaces are a ready canvas to explore; to decorate a<br />

negative space <strong>and</strong> bring it to life (Halsey <strong>and</strong> Young, 2006). Ferrell (1993) describes the<br />

alternative way young writers communicate via their art form; for some it is the only way<br />

they have to express themselves publicly. Writing is also a way <strong>of</strong> connecting to the city, with<br />

pride taken in the recognition gained in their alternative community (Halsey <strong>and</strong> Young,<br />

2006). Additionally, White (2001) explains that there are as many different types <strong>of</strong> graffiti<br />

writer as there are forms <strong>of</strong> graffiti; from political messages to art pieces. In order to address<br />

graffiti v<strong>and</strong>alism, policy makers must underst<strong>and</strong> the graffiti writers’ perspective on their<br />

activities (Halsey <strong>and</strong> Young, 2006).<br />

It is also important to note that graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging, although not exclusively carried out by<br />

young people (those aged between 14 <strong>and</strong> 25), are <strong>of</strong>ten linked to ‘youth’, <strong>and</strong> it is the case<br />

that many <strong>of</strong> those involved in graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging can be classed as young people. <strong>Graffiti</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> tagging therefore need to be considered in the wider context <strong>of</strong> youth crime. There is<br />

<strong>of</strong>ten the perception that youth crime is out <strong>of</strong> control, although according to the Ministry <strong>of</strong><br />

Youth Justice in 2000, young <strong>of</strong>fenders cannot be seen as a category <strong>of</strong> ‘criminals’ that is<br />

homogeneous. For example, <strong>of</strong> all the young men who commit an <strong>of</strong>fence, as a quarter <strong>of</strong> all<br />

young men in New Zeal<strong>and</strong> do, the vast majority will settle down to become law abiding<br />

citizens in their twenties: only 15-20 per cent <strong>of</strong> the ‘ever <strong>of</strong>fended’ category go on to commit<br />

more <strong>of</strong>fences later in life. This follows international patterns with countries like Australia, UK<br />

<strong>and</strong> United States (USA) reporting similar patterns in youth <strong>of</strong>fending. The vast majority (75<br />

19


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

per cent) <strong>of</strong> young people in New Zeal<strong>and</strong> never <strong>of</strong>fend. Of the 25 per cent <strong>of</strong> young men<br />

who do, 80 per cent <strong>of</strong>fend only once <strong>and</strong> the remaining 20 per cent tend to commit a large<br />

number <strong>of</strong> crimes over a period <strong>of</strong> years (M<strong>of</strong>fit 1993). These statistics highlight two<br />

categories that are commonly used to refer to young <strong>of</strong>fenders; persisters <strong>and</strong> desisters<br />

(M<strong>of</strong>fit, 1996). Desisters commit at least one crime but tend to start later in adolescence,<br />

after the age <strong>of</strong> 13. They then stop <strong>of</strong>fending by ages 24-28. Desisters show only two risk<br />

factors – substance use <strong>and</strong> mixing with anti-social peers. 3 Persisters, in contrast, start<br />

<strong>of</strong>fending early, before the age <strong>of</strong> 14 <strong>and</strong> as early as age 10, <strong>of</strong>fend at a high rate, <strong>and</strong><br />

persist until well into adulthood. Additionally they exhibit a wider range <strong>of</strong> risk factors than<br />

desisters. M<strong>of</strong>fit (1996) argued that ‘desisters’ could also be termed ‘adolescent limited’<br />

<strong>of</strong>fenders who start <strong>and</strong> end their criminal careers relatively abruptly. They may also behave<br />

antisocially in some environments but not in all: for example, with friends but not in school.<br />

For this reason there may be disagreement between parents, teachers <strong>and</strong> the young people<br />

themselves about the extent <strong>of</strong> their criminal behaviour. For most youth <strong>of</strong>fenders, growing<br />

up is the single most important factor that affects their move towards desistance. However,<br />

in terms <strong>of</strong> graffiti, the wide age range <strong>of</strong> respondents (from ‘under 14’ years to ‘over 40’<br />

years) suggests that this ‘<strong>of</strong>fence’ for some graffiti writers is not adolescent limited. In<br />

addition, the commitment <strong>of</strong> many graffiti writers to this form <strong>of</strong> creative expression (as<br />

noted by Ferrell, 1993) suggests that they will not desist as they get older.<br />

3 There are many risk factors associated with youth <strong>of</strong>fending; inadequate parenting, child<br />

abuse/maltreatment, family disruption, poor parental supervision, parent <strong>and</strong> sibling criminality,<br />

teenage parents, unstable living conditions, effects <strong>of</strong> economic disadvantage (Arthur, 2006; 9).<br />

20


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Policy developments<br />

<strong>Graffiti</strong> v<strong>and</strong>alism has come to the fore in recent years in New Zeal<strong>and</strong> as a result <strong>of</strong> several<br />

high pr<strong>of</strong>ile events (Dominion Post, 2007; City Independent, 2009). Auckl<strong>and</strong> businessman<br />

Bruce Emery’s stabbing to death <strong>of</strong> 15 year old tagger Pihema Cameron polarised attention<br />

on the issue <strong>of</strong> graffiti (Koubaridis, Vass et al., 2008). Other media attention has highlighted<br />

the work <strong>of</strong> Judge Tony Adeane in jailing a series <strong>of</strong> recidivist taggers in Napier in recent<br />

years (Dominion Post, 2007). The Ministry <strong>of</strong> Justice implemented Stop <strong>Tagging</strong> Our Place<br />

(STOP) strategy in 2007 to help local government, non-Government agencies (NGOs) <strong>and</strong><br />

regional authorities deal with graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging.<br />

STOP (Ministry <strong>of</strong> Justice, 2008b) aims to prevent, manage <strong>and</strong> enforce action to reduce<br />

graffiti via community engagement <strong>and</strong> ownership <strong>of</strong> the problem. The strategy guides local<br />

government to facilitate reduction measures <strong>and</strong> actively enforce new <strong>and</strong> existing legal<br />

frameworks to address graffiti. STOP (Ministry <strong>of</strong> Justice, 2008b) supports the use <strong>of</strong> CPTED<br />

principles. CPTED draws on Jeffery’s (1971) work on the prevention <strong>of</strong> criminal activity via<br />

means <strong>of</strong> urban design, for example open plan housing developments with well lit walkways<br />

<strong>and</strong> paths, plus the removal <strong>of</strong> isolated or obscured areas where crime could occur (Jeffery,<br />

1971; Wortley <strong>and</strong> Mazerolle, 2008). In order to assist with implementing the<br />

recommendations in STOP, a funding grant programme was established (Ministry <strong>of</strong> Justice,<br />

2008). The <strong>Graffiti</strong> v<strong>and</strong>alism prevention grant fund application guidelines 2008/09 has a one<strong>of</strong>f<br />

grant <strong>of</strong> $5,000 to $30,000 with the same dollar value being available for longer term<br />

projects running for up to two years (Ministry <strong>of</strong> Justice, 2008a).<br />

A strong emphasis in policy efforts towards the long term reduction in graffiti appears to be<br />

community engagement <strong>and</strong> civic pride (Ministry <strong>of</strong> Justice <strong>and</strong> Local Government New<br />

Zeal<strong>and</strong>, 2006). This has been seen in Hastings District Council (HDC) <strong>and</strong> Keep Hastings<br />

Beautiful Trust’s (KHBT) work with residents in the Hastings suburb <strong>of</strong> Camberley (J. Barnes,<br />

personal communication, May 11, 2009). By involving residents in a community cleanup, they<br />

begin to take ownership <strong>of</strong> the problem <strong>and</strong> thus help to prevent graffiti reoccurring.<br />

Camberley’s shops had been a regular target <strong>of</strong> taggers but with the help <strong>of</strong> a local artist the<br />

residents cleaned up the area <strong>and</strong> painted a mural. Several local schools have also seen a<br />

clean-up <strong>and</strong> mural programme being effective in reducing graffiti.<br />

Other initiatives in Hastings include the Chesterhope bridge’s ‘paint out’ day, where police<br />

<strong>and</strong> KHBT supervised a group <strong>of</strong> serious graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging <strong>of</strong>fenders to tidy up the area.<br />

Then each person was allowed to paint one support wall <strong>of</strong> the bridge. Since that event in<br />

September 2008 there has only been one minor incident <strong>of</strong> tagging in what was previously a<br />

regular target for graffiti (J. Barnes, personal communication, May 11, 2009).<br />

21


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Another response to the problem <strong>of</strong> graffiti has been establishing a 24 hour time frame for<br />

removal <strong>of</strong> painting (Ministry <strong>of</strong> Justice, 2008b; Timaru District Council, 2009). Various New<br />

Zeal<strong>and</strong> local government strategies have implemented rapid removal plans for graffiti (Craw,<br />

Lel<strong>and</strong> et al., 2006; Hastings District Council, 2008). One recent example is HDC’s<br />

development <strong>of</strong> a graffiti strategy in 2008, based on CPTED principles. In partnership with<br />

Keep Hastings Beautiful Trust (KHBT), HDC established steps to reduce <strong>and</strong> prevent graffiti<br />

v<strong>and</strong>alism (Hastings District Council, 2008). A website with information <strong>and</strong> a free phone<br />

number were established in 2008 so that members <strong>of</strong> the public could report graffiti. HDC<br />

engaged the Department <strong>of</strong> Correction’s Community Work scheme to clean up council<br />

property <strong>and</strong> businesses, <strong>of</strong>ten using young <strong>of</strong>fenders prosecuted for tagging. The council<br />

also has a full time clean up team to respond to reported incidents. Since the strategy’s 2008<br />

introduction, graffiti reports have reduced from 200-300 cases per month to approximately<br />

100 cases (J. Barnes, personal communication, May 11, 2009). Similarly, Timaru District<br />

Council (TDC) has had success with graffiti eradication through funding from Ministry <strong>of</strong><br />

Justice (J. Cullimore, personal communication, May 5, 2009). TDC work with police <strong>and</strong><br />

Department <strong>of</strong> Corrections to record <strong>and</strong> clean up graffiti. Anecdotal evidence shows that this<br />

is having a visible impact on graffiti levels in parks <strong>and</strong> reserves in the area.<br />

Recent New Zeal<strong>and</strong> research has linked the use <strong>of</strong> mural walls to a reduction in graffiti<br />

(Craw, Lel<strong>and</strong> et al., 2006). The study by Craw et al (2006) found only eight graffiti attacks in<br />

20 weeks following the painting <strong>of</strong> a mural on a previously well targeted concrete city wall.<br />

This is in contrast with 14 attacks on the same wall in the two week pre-trial period. Reasons<br />

for the reduction included a high pr<strong>of</strong>ile location in the inner city, the mural’s colour made<br />

graffiti difficult to read <strong>and</strong> taggers were thought to have respect for the mural artist (Craw,<br />

Lel<strong>and</strong> et al., 2006). This supports HDC’s mural <strong>and</strong> paint out community projects where<br />

residents have cleaned up a school or shopping centre then painted a mural (J. Barnes,<br />

personal communication, May 11, 2009). As a result <strong>of</strong> programmes, seen in several Hastings<br />

primary schools <strong>and</strong> Camberley shops, the mural areas have seen a reduction in the amount<br />

<strong>of</strong> graffiti (ibid).<br />

Education is a further reduction strategy which is seen to be important in discouraging young<br />

people from graffiti, especially teenagers, who are seen as the more prolific age group<br />

involved in the activity (Halsey <strong>and</strong> Young, 2002; Constable T. Gommans, personal<br />

communication, May 26, 2009). <strong>Wellington</strong> community police <strong>of</strong>ficer, Constable Gommans,<br />

has taken an education programme into schools in Eastern <strong>Wellington</strong> for the past few years.<br />

Part <strong>of</strong> the programme includes explaining that those caught tagging must clean up wearing<br />

a fluorescent pink vest with ‘TAGGER’ printed on the reverse. Prior to the pink vest initiative,<br />

Gommans had worked hard for several years <strong>and</strong> to reduce the amount <strong>of</strong> graffiti by 50 to 60<br />

per cent (T. Gommans, personal communication, May 26 2009). In 2007, <strong>Wellington</strong> City<br />

Council financially supported an education programme for Year 8 students. As a result <strong>of</strong> the<br />

22


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

school talks with the vest, graffiti in Eastern <strong>Wellington</strong> dropped away to negligible figures (T.<br />

Gommans, personal communication, May 26 2009). However, an unintended consequence <strong>of</strong><br />

this kind <strong>of</strong> crime prevention initiative is that it moves the problem around to other suburbs 4 .<br />

The western <strong>and</strong> northern suburbs <strong>of</strong> <strong>Wellington</strong> have apparently reported an increase in<br />

graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging since the introduction <strong>of</strong> the ‘pink vest’ initiative in eastern parts <strong>of</strong> the<br />

city (City Life Independent Herald, March 2009). In addition as Gommans notes, crime<br />

prevention initiatives might not lead to sustained <strong>and</strong> continuing reductions in <strong>of</strong>fending<br />

behaviour.<br />

For over six months I had hardly anything. It was great because I have other things I<br />

have to do but slowly it started to come back. A lot <strong>of</strong> them now know about the pink<br />

vest <strong>and</strong> they put down the spray can when they know the consequences.<br />

(Constable T. Gommans, May 26, 2009)<br />

A second education programme, run by Manukau Beautification Charitable Trust (MBCT),<br />

shows Year 5 to 8 students the impact on their community <strong>of</strong> graffiti (B. Carney, personal<br />

communication, June 2, 2009). Run in conjunction with community police <strong>of</strong>ficers, students<br />

discuss the issues around the problem then help to ‘paint out’ a targeted wall or space in<br />

local parks, reserves or walkways. MBCT’s Barbara Carney believes that undertaking the<br />

clean-up gives students pride in the work. MBCT also gives presentations on the topic to high<br />

school students. A further MBCT initiative, ENVIRO ARTS, works with at-risk youth in a<br />

programme teaching new skills <strong>and</strong> attempting to build their self esteem. Carney explained<br />

that this gives them employment skills for a trade, as the practical experience <strong>of</strong> building park<br />

benches, retaining walls <strong>and</strong> similar can facilitate entry into further training or labouring work<br />

(B. Carney, personal communication, May 8, 2009).<br />

It is clear that in New Zeal<strong>and</strong> currently the issue <strong>of</strong> graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging is a source <strong>of</strong><br />

concern for communities <strong>and</strong> politicians alike. It is also clear that there are a number <strong>of</strong><br />

diverse initiatives that are focussing on dealing with the issues that graffiti raises. Those that<br />

have been successful are those that are most closely aligned to the attitudes <strong>of</strong> the young<br />

people <strong>and</strong> graffiti writers studied in this research project. For example, those that clear up<br />

‘tagging’ <strong>and</strong> replace them with murals or graffiti art are successful because the ‘rules’<br />

relating to graffiti strongly prohibit tagging on respected ‘pieces’.<br />

4 This is also apparent as a consequence <strong>of</strong> alcohol bans which have simply moved the problem <strong>of</strong><br />

street drinkers from Glover Park to Cuba Street to Aro Valley, <strong>and</strong> is a form <strong>of</strong> displacement <strong>of</strong>ten<br />

associated with this kind <strong>of</strong> crime prevention initiative.<br />

23


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Findings<br />

The survey results <strong>and</strong> data from the focus groups is presented below in relation to three<br />

broad themes: general attitudes toward graffiti, the commissioning <strong>of</strong> graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging,<br />

<strong>and</strong> desistance from graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging.<br />

General attitudes toward graffiti<br />

The on-line questionnaire asked some introductory questions about the status <strong>and</strong> nature <strong>of</strong><br />

graffiti. Table 2 shows the results <strong>of</strong> these questions <strong>and</strong> indicates a broad distribution <strong>of</strong><br />

views in relation to the status <strong>of</strong> graffiti as v<strong>and</strong>alism that should not be tolerated, with<br />

almost equal proportions <strong>of</strong> respondents in each category <strong>of</strong> responses. The survey showed a<br />

high-level <strong>of</strong> agreement that graffiti should be tolerated in some circumstances: 39.0 per cent<br />

‘agreed’ or ‘strongly agreed’ with the statement in the question. Both responses suggest that<br />

no distinct public perspective was articulated about the nature <strong>of</strong> graffiti in general terms.<br />

Table 2: Attitudes toward graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging (number <strong>and</strong> percentages)<br />

<strong>Graffiti</strong> is<br />

v<strong>and</strong>alism <strong>and</strong><br />

should never be<br />

tolerated<br />

<strong>Graffiti</strong> is an urban<br />

art-form that<br />

should be valued<br />

in some<br />

circumstances<br />

Strongly<br />

agree<br />

Agree<br />

Neither<br />

agree nor<br />

disagree<br />

Disagree<br />

Strongly<br />

disagree<br />

Total<br />

% n % n % n % n % n % n<br />

17.3 124 21.7 155 21.8 156 21.7 155 17.5 125 100 715<br />

41.8 312 40.1 299 9.4 70 5.2 39 3.5 26 100 746<br />

That respondent attitudes toward graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging were nuanced is further evident from<br />

the results presented in Table 3, which shows that only a small minority (12.2 per cent) felt<br />

that graffiti was a ‘bad thing’ in all cases. By far the majority <strong>of</strong> respondents argued that the<br />

circumstances determined whether graffiti is a negative thing. Male respondents were more<br />

likely than female to respond that graffiti was never a bad thing <strong>and</strong> slightly more likely to<br />

say it was always a bad thing, <strong>and</strong> considerable differences were found between those less<br />

than 30 <strong>and</strong> older respondents: while 11.4 per cent <strong>of</strong> 22-23 year olds, for example,<br />

answered that graffiti is always a bad thing, this proportion was much higher among those<br />

who were 30+ (19.0 per cent) <strong>and</strong> the 40+ age cohort (38.7 per cent). Those who reported<br />

that they had written graffiti were most likely to report that it is never a ‘bad thing’ (23.4 per<br />

cent).<br />

24


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Table 3: Do you think that graffiti is a bad thing ((number <strong>and</strong> percentages)), by<br />

gender, age, ethnicity <strong>and</strong> participation<br />

Yes, always Sometimes No, never<br />

% n % n % n<br />

All 12.2 94 76.7 593 11.1 86<br />

Males 13.2 49 72.0 268 14.8 55<br />

Females 11.9 38 80.9 258 7.2 23<br />

Under 14 9.1 1 81.8 9 9.1 1<br />

14-16 12.3 21 71.3 122 16.4 28<br />

17-19 11.3 29 79.0 203 9.7 25<br />

20-21 7.3 4 85.5 47 7.3 4<br />

22-23 11.4 4 74.3 26 14.3 5<br />

24-25 3.4 1 86.2 25 10.3 3<br />

25+ 9.1 4 79.5 35 11.4 5<br />

30+ 19.0 11 70.7 41 10.3 6<br />

40+ 38.7 12 58.1 18 3.2 1<br />

NZ European 17.9 48 77.2 207 4.9 13<br />

Māori 9.5 11 69.0 80 21.6 25<br />

Pacific peoples 15.4 6 71.8 28 12.8 5<br />

Asian 16.7 6 80.6 29 2.8 1<br />

European 3.5 2 77.2 44 19.3 11<br />

Other ethnicity 9.1 10 77.3 85 13.6 15<br />

Have not written 17.5 78 78.0 347 4.5 20<br />

graffiti<br />

Have sometimes 13.4 9 79.1 53 7.5 5<br />

written<br />

Have written 2.7 7 73.9 193 23.4 61<br />

The diverse nature <strong>of</strong> graffiti writing was highlighted in comments made by the online survey<br />

participants:<br />

There is a line that defines graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging. <strong>Graffiti</strong> art is different to <strong>Tagging</strong>.<br />

That needs to be established.<br />

There are two types <strong>of</strong> graffiti. One v<strong>and</strong>alism <strong>and</strong> two, the creative artistic side.<br />

Understnd this.<br />

I strongly disagree with tagging, I’m sure you underst<strong>and</strong> this but taggers are also a<br />

problem for <strong>Graffiti</strong> artists. They Tag our artistic work also making it go from<br />

respectable art to a hideous mess. 5<br />

These kinds <strong>of</strong> distinctions <strong>and</strong> definitions influenced the responses presented in Table 3<br />

above. As noted by graffiti researchers Ferrell (1993) <strong>and</strong> Halsey <strong>and</strong> Young (2002), defining<br />

‘graffiti’ can be problematic, which is supported in the opinions <strong>of</strong> the respondents in this<br />

study. Focus group participants also expressed the overlapping status <strong>of</strong> graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging<br />

as art <strong>and</strong> as v<strong>and</strong>alism:<br />

5 Throughout the report quotes from respondents are unchanged <strong>and</strong> spelling/grammar has not been<br />

corrected.<br />

25


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Art. It is just expressing yourself.<br />

If you are pr<strong>of</strong>essional it is art.<br />

To other people it is v<strong>and</strong>alism but to us it is art.<br />

V<strong>and</strong>alism is when it is on someone’s property.<br />

If it is graffiti then it is art, if it is tagging then it is v<strong>and</strong>alism.<br />

Participants were asked to respond to a series <strong>of</strong> statements relating to potentially<br />

problematic aspects <strong>of</strong> graffiti <strong>and</strong> a similar list <strong>of</strong> positive attributes. Table 4 ranks these<br />

items according to the percentage <strong>of</strong> respondents who ‘strongly agreed’ or ‘agreed’ with the<br />

list <strong>of</strong> problems <strong>and</strong> benefits. These results reinforce the complex <strong>and</strong> contradictory<br />

perceptions <strong>of</strong> graffiti <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong> the impact that it has on individuals <strong>and</strong> society. While a clear<br />

majority agreed to some extent that graffiti is an art form there was also a strong agreement<br />

that graffiti damages property <strong>and</strong> some agreement that it impacts on perceptions <strong>of</strong><br />

neighbourhoods.<br />

Table 4: Positive <strong>and</strong> negative attributes <strong>of</strong> graffiti (number <strong>and</strong> percentages)<br />

Positive attributes<br />

Strongly agree/<br />

Agree Negative attributes<br />

Strongly agree/<br />

Agree<br />

% n % n<br />

<strong>Graffiti</strong> is a form <strong>of</strong> art 84.7 648 <strong>Graffiti</strong> damages 80.9 621<br />

people’s property<br />

<strong>Graffiti</strong> is a way for people 84.0 639 <strong>Graffiti</strong> affects how 62.1 465<br />

to express themselves<br />

people feel about the<br />

area they live<br />

<strong>Graffiti</strong> requires creative 75.2 569 <strong>Graffiti</strong> is illegal 51.5 378<br />

skills<br />

<strong>Graffiti</strong> is a way for people 52.0 390 <strong>Graffiti</strong> looks messy 45.5 345<br />

to gain status <strong>and</strong><br />

respect<br />

<strong>Graffiti</strong> is associated 40.3 303<br />

with other dangerous<br />

activities<br />

<strong>Graffiti</strong> makes places<br />

feel unsafe<br />

38.3 289<br />

Contributions to the focus group discussions tended to reinforce these findings. In particular,<br />

distinctions were frequently drawn between graffiti that is legitimate <strong>and</strong> that which is<br />

categorised as v<strong>and</strong>alism. One focus group participant reflected the ambiguity <strong>of</strong><br />

distinguishing graffiti art from v<strong>and</strong>alism, noting that graffiti is “v<strong>and</strong>alism. Well, I reckon it’s<br />

art, but in the law it is v<strong>and</strong>alism”. When asked whether graffiti is art or v<strong>and</strong>alism typical<br />

responses included:<br />

Some <strong>of</strong> it’s art; some <strong>of</strong> it’s v<strong>and</strong>alism. (Focus group participant)<br />

26


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

If you are doing a bombing that is art, but if you just draw lines, that is v<strong>and</strong>alism.”<br />

(Focus group participant).<br />

The distinction between art <strong>and</strong> v<strong>and</strong>alism was sometimes cast in narrow legal terms (“if you<br />

are allowed to do it [it is art]”) but the difference between the two designations was more<br />

commonly thought through in more general terms that related to the content <strong>and</strong> style <strong>of</strong><br />

what was written. Some reflected on the impact that graffiti has on audiences, <strong>and</strong> suggested<br />

that this shaped whether it should be considered as art or v<strong>and</strong>alism. Participants were asked<br />

what makes some forms <strong>of</strong> graffiti art, <strong>and</strong> common reflections included:<br />

When you do a piece [as opposed to less skilful forms] (Focus group participant)<br />

When it becomes legal. (Focus group participant)<br />

If your letters are st<strong>and</strong>ing up, people are looking at it like it is art. (Focus group<br />

participant)<br />

When it looks good. (Focus group participant)<br />

If it is an expression <strong>of</strong> who you are that is art. But it is a message, something<br />

written on a wall just to say I wrote something on a wall, that is v<strong>and</strong>alism.<br />

(Focus group participant)<br />

Some people have a real passion for it, to express their feelings <strong>and</strong> also for<br />

the story. (Focus group participant)<br />

It is an outlet <strong>of</strong> your emotions so depending on how you feel at the time.<br />

(Focus group)<br />

I reckon it is just mindless. There’s a good spot so let’s just hit it. (Focus<br />

group)<br />

Well, yeah but they do it for turf as well. Try to think that they are out there<br />

but it is just writing on the wall. (Focus group)<br />

“<strong>Graffiti</strong> is an element <strong>of</strong> hip hop so it is not anything negative” (Focus group<br />

participant).<br />

Much media <strong>and</strong> political comment regards graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging as gateway <strong>of</strong>fending that<br />

becomes associated with other illegal <strong>and</strong> problematic behaviour. Again focus group<br />

participants provided mixed perspectives on the extent to which tagging <strong>and</strong> graffiti is linked<br />

to problematic illegal behaviour. While some respondents noted that it is associated with<br />

other deviant youth activity, others suggested that more dedicated writers avoided alcohol<br />

27


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

<strong>and</strong> drugs in order to focus on graffiti, or even that writing is an alternative to these<br />

activities:<br />

Alcohol <strong>and</strong> drugs sort <strong>of</strong> put you in that…mood. (Focus group participant)<br />

No, I know heaps <strong>of</strong> people who do it just for fun. (Focus group participant)<br />

Instead <strong>of</strong> taking drugs or getting drunk they tag. (Focus group participant).<br />

Some survey respondents also highlighted that ‘to damage something’ or ‘to destroy<br />

something’ was an unpopular reason for doing graffiti <strong>and</strong> were highly critical <strong>of</strong> the<br />

researchers for including it as a category. More on the relation between illegality <strong>and</strong> graffiti<br />

follows further in the discussion section <strong>of</strong> this report.<br />

The commissioning <strong>of</strong> graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging<br />

A slight majority (57.6 per cent) <strong>of</strong> respondents reported that they had not written graffiti.<br />

The survey asked respondents to state either simply ‘yes’ that they do write graffiti or that<br />

they ‘sometimes’ did: 33.8 per cent fell into the former category <strong>and</strong> 8.7 per cent the latter.<br />

In some <strong>of</strong> the discussion that follows distinction is drawn between ‘committed’, ‘occasional’<br />

<strong>and</strong> ‘non’ graffiti writers, <strong>and</strong> this is based on this self-categorisation. Table 5 shows how<br />

responses to this question varied by age, gender <strong>and</strong> ethnicity.<br />

Table 5: Have you ever written graffiti (number <strong>and</strong> percentages)<br />

Yes Sometimes No<br />

% n % n % n<br />

All 33.8 232 8.7 52 57.6 407<br />

Males 47.6 177 7.8 29 44.6 166<br />

Females 17.2 55 7.2 23 75.5 241<br />

Under 14 36.4 4 18.2 2 45.5 5<br />

14-16 34.5 59 6.4 11 59.1 101<br />

17-19 35.4 91 7.4 19 57.2 147<br />

20-21 34.5 19 10.9 6 54.5 30<br />

22-23 45.7 16 8.6 3 45.7 16<br />

24-25 37.9 11 6.9 2 55.2 16<br />

25+ 43.2 19 11.4 5 45.5 20<br />

30+ 20.7 12 5.2 3 74.1 43<br />

40+ 3.2 1 3.2 1 93.5 29<br />

NZ European 22.0 59 4.5 12 73.5 197<br />

Māori 50.9 59 11.2 13 37.9 44<br />

Pacific peoples 41.0 16 12.8 5 46.2 18<br />

Asian 8.3 3 5.6 2 86.1 31<br />

European 56.1 32 12.3 7 31.6 18<br />

Other ethnicity 32.7 36 9.1 10 58.2 64<br />

Clearly the data indicates a substantial difference between male <strong>and</strong> female involvement in<br />

graffiti: with 55.4 per cent <strong>of</strong> male respondents agreeing that they had written graffiti, at<br />

28


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

least occasionally. In contrast, 75.5 per cent <strong>of</strong> females reported that they had not written<br />

graffiti. Levels <strong>of</strong> self-reported involvement in writing graffiti also changed with the age <strong>of</strong><br />

respondents; although there does not appear to be any clear pattern in the relationship<br />

between age <strong>and</strong> reported writing. More than half <strong>of</strong> those aged under-14 reported that they<br />

had, at least on occasion, written graffiti. The group with the highest participation was those<br />

aged 22-23 years (45.7 per cent reported that ‘yes’ they wrote graffiti <strong>and</strong> 8.6 per cent<br />

admitted that they ‘sometimes’ participated). The various age groups under 30 years all<br />

reported that they participated at a rate between 34.5 <strong>and</strong> 45.7 per cent. It was only in<br />

relation to the two oldest cohorts that reported participation became notably lower. It is<br />

important to note that the data does not relate to the age at which respondents first wrote<br />

graffiti: respondents were specifically asked whether they had ever participated so the lower<br />

participation rate among older respondents probably does not reflect that they have desisted<br />

from writing graffiti; nor does it necessarily reveal anything about the ‘peak age’ <strong>of</strong> <strong>of</strong>fending.<br />

Those who reported that they had written graffiti were asked about the property that had<br />

been targeted. Table 6 shows that a broad range <strong>of</strong> properties were selected, <strong>and</strong> that ‘other’<br />

was also chosen by many respondents (more than one category could be chosen). This<br />

suggests that the classification <strong>of</strong> property in terms <strong>of</strong> ownership status <strong>and</strong> so on does not<br />

exert a strong influence on ‘event decisions’: focus group discussion suggested that the<br />

preference for public space might be related to the desire that graffiti be seen as widely as<br />

possible.<br />

Table 6: Property selected for graffiti (number <strong>and</strong> percentages)<br />

% n<br />

Public space (e.g. alleyway, bus shelter, power box) 68.8 212<br />

Other 57.1 176<br />

Public building (e.g. school, hospital) 42.2 130<br />

Private residence (e.g. wall, fence or garage) 40.6 125<br />

Waste-ground 36.0 111<br />

Private business (e.g. shop) 35.7 110<br />

Focus group discussions reinforced these findings; a very wide range <strong>of</strong> locations were<br />

identified as likely venues for graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging. Decisions about location were related to<br />

accessibility <strong>and</strong> visibility as well as the suitability <strong>of</strong> the surface <strong>of</strong>fered. Typical comments<br />

included:<br />

You only do it where people can see it (Focus group participant)<br />

The best place is on the concrete ‘cos then they don’t remove it (Focus group<br />

participant)<br />

29


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Fences, big walls, plain white walls lures them to it (Focus group participant)<br />

The train station; that is just hit on top <strong>of</strong> hit on top <strong>of</strong> hit (Focus group participant)<br />

In the city … alleyways, fences, shops. On my walls (laughter) (Focus group<br />

participant)<br />

Cars, buses, government property (Focus group participant).<br />

Table 7 shows that respondents’ participation in different forms <strong>of</strong> graffiti varied by gender,<br />

age, ethnicity <strong>and</strong> whether they were involved in graffiti <strong>of</strong> any form. Although there was<br />

variation in the extent <strong>of</strong> participation, respondents who engaged in some form <strong>of</strong> graffiti<br />

were most likely to identify their activity as ‘graffiti art’. Males reported that they engaged in<br />

different types <strong>of</strong> graffiti at a greater rate than females, although graffiti art was the biggest<br />

category selected by those who participated <strong>of</strong> both genders. Of those who have written<br />

graffiti 34.9 per cent reported that they had done ‘graffiti art’; those who had ‘sometimes’<br />

participated also selected this category above the others, as 33.3 per cent chose it.<br />

Exceptions to this trend were the youngest age cohort (under 14 years) who were more likely<br />

to have tagged (50.0 per cent) or done stylised writing (25.1 per cent) (although the very<br />

small numbers in these groups means considerable caution is required), Māori (who were<br />

more likely to have tagged, at 41.5 per cent, than have done graffiti art) <strong>and</strong> Pacific peoples<br />

(who were more likely to have done stylised writing). These latter findings reflect the<br />

apparent differentials in involvement in tagging <strong>and</strong> graffiti across ethnic groups that were<br />

indicated in Table 5.<br />

Table 7: Type <strong>of</strong> graffiti (number <strong>and</strong> percentages)<br />

Stylised<br />

writing<br />

V<strong>and</strong>alism <strong>Graffiti</strong> art <strong>Tagging</strong> Political<br />

expression<br />

None <strong>of</strong><br />

these<br />

N % N % N % N % N % N %<br />

All 25 14.9 8 4.8 73 43.5 49 29.2 13 7.7 20 11.9<br />

Males 19 16.1 6 5.1 46 39.0 37 31.4 10 8.5 14 11.9<br />

Females 6 12.0 2 4.0 27 54.0 12 24.0 3 6.0 6 12.0<br />

Under 14 1 25.0 0 0.0 0 0.0 2 50.0 0 0.0 1 25.0<br />

14-16 5 8.9 2 3.6 27 48.2 21 37.5 1 1.8 4 7.1<br />

17-19 10 16.1 3 4.8 25 40.3 20 32.3 4 6.5 9 14.5<br />

20-21 2 22.2 0 0.0 4 44.4 2 22.2 1 11.1 0 0.0<br />

22-23 1 9.1 2 18.2 5 45.5 1 9.1 2 18.2 1 9.1<br />

24-25 1 14.3 0 0.0 2 28.6 1 14.3 2 28.6 1 14.3<br />

25+ 4 30.8 0 0.0 5 38.5 1 7.7 1 7.7 2 15.4<br />

30+ 1 9.1 1 9.1 4 36.4 1 9.1 2 18.2 2 18.2<br />

40+ 0 0.0 0 0.0 1 100.0 0 0.0 0 0.0 0 0.0<br />

30


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

NZ European 8 15.1 2 3.8 19 35.8 9 17.0 6 11.3 9 17.0<br />

Maori 6 9.2 2 3.1 25 38.5 27 41.5 2 3.1 3 4.6<br />

Pacific<br />

5 35.7 1 7.1 4 28.6 2 14.3 0 0.0 2 14.3<br />

peoples<br />

Asian 1 33.3 0 0.0 1 33.3 0 0.0 0 0.0 1 33.3<br />

European 2 40.0 0 0.0 1 20.0 0 0.0 1 20.0 1 20.0<br />

Other<br />

ethnicity<br />

2 5.7 2 5.7 18 51.4 8 22.9 3 8.6 2 5.7<br />

Have written 24 14.5 7 4.2 65 39.4 42 25.5 13 7.9 14 8.5<br />

Have<br />

sometimes<br />

written<br />

6 15.4 1 2.6 13 33.3 8 20.5 1 2.6 10 25.6<br />

Focus group discussion provided further elaboration on the range <strong>of</strong> different types <strong>and</strong><br />

writing that young people identify. Much <strong>of</strong> the discussion <strong>and</strong> evaluation <strong>of</strong> the legitimacy <strong>of</strong><br />

writing was related to the type <strong>and</strong> style. Fine gradations existed between, for example,<br />

scribbles, tags <strong>and</strong> bombs, which were forms <strong>of</strong> relatively unsophisticated writing that require<br />

no particular skill <strong>and</strong> were <strong>of</strong>ten associated in group discussions with v<strong>and</strong>alism. Indeed<br />

pieces <strong>of</strong> writing that were more sophisticated <strong>and</strong> valued by young people were, they noted,<br />

sometimes subject to degradation by having tags written over them: in that way graffiti itself<br />

was sometimes v<strong>and</strong>alised. One focus group participant expressed the distinctions between<br />

different forms in the following terms:<br />

There is a difference between tagging <strong>and</strong> bombing. We have that in south Taranaki.<br />

A lot <strong>of</strong> areas where the community youth get together <strong>and</strong> done a cool bomb on the<br />

wall. And yet when it is done like that <strong>and</strong> the youth are proud <strong>of</strong> something they<br />

have done, they don’t tag it, like we don’t see tagging as much. We get the youth<br />

involved in making bombing. It is still a problem but it is not as bad as it used to be.<br />

(Focus group participant)<br />

As Table 8 indicates, ‘creative expression’ was the most significant reason why respondents<br />

wrote graffiti. The second biggest category was ‘none <strong>of</strong> these’. Only very small proportions<br />

reported that they were motivated by a ‘sense <strong>of</strong> danger’ or the desire to ‘damage<br />

something’. Gender differences were marginal in response to this question: females were<br />

more likely than males to report that they participated in graffiti as ‘part <strong>of</strong> a group or<br />

31


Table 8: reasons for writing graffiti (number <strong>and</strong> percentages)<br />

Sense <strong>of</strong><br />

danger<br />

Creative<br />

expression To protest<br />

As part <strong>of</strong> a<br />

group culture<br />

To damage<br />

something<br />

To get my<br />

name up<br />

For a sense <strong>of</strong><br />

accomplishment Excitement<br />

Because it<br />

breaks the<br />

rules None <strong>of</strong> these<br />

% n % n % n % n % n % n % n % n % n % n<br />

All 1.6 5 47.2 146 7.1 22 7.4 23 1.9 6 7.8 24 2.9 9 3.9 12 5.5 17<br />

14.6<br />

45<br />

Males 1.9 4 46.1 94 7.3 15 5.3 11 1.5 3 9.7 19 3.9 8 3.9 8 4.9 10<br />

15.5<br />

32<br />

Females 0.0 0 51.3 40 7.7 6 10.3 8 2.6 2 3.8 3 0.0 0 5.1 4 5.1 4 14.1 11<br />

Under 14 0.0 0 50.0 3 0.0 0 16.7 1 16.7 1 0.0 0 0.0 0 .0 0 .0 0 16.7 1<br />

14-16 0.0 0 37.1 26 5.7 4 5.7 4 1.4 1 15.7 11 2.9 2 5.7 4 8.6 6 17.1 12<br />

17-19 2.7 3 51.8 57 7.3 8 4.5 5 1.8 2 5.5 6 2.7 3 3.6 4 3.6 4 16.4 18<br />

20-21 4.0 1 60.0 15 4.0 1 8.0 22 0.0 0 4.0 1 4.0 1 .0 0 8.0 2 8.0 2<br />

22-23 0.0 0 47.4 9 5.3 1 15.8 3 0.0 0 0.0 0 5.3 1 .0 0 5.3 1 21.1 4<br />

24-25 0.0 0 61.5 8 0.0 0 7.7 1 0.0 0 15.4 2 7.7 1 .0 0 .0 0 7.7 1<br />

25+ 0.0 0 41.7 10 12.5 3 8.3 2 0.0 0 12.5 3 0.0 0 8.3 2 4.2 3 12.5 3<br />

30+ 0.0 0 46.7 7 20.0 3 6.7 1 6.7 1 0.0 0 0.0 0 13.3 2 .0 0 6.7 1<br />

40+ 0.0 0 0.0 0 50.0 1 .0 0 0.0 0 0.0 0 0.0 0 .0 0 .0 0 50.0 1<br />

NZ European 0.0 0 49.3 35 14.1 10 1.4 1 1.4 1 5.6 4 1.4 1 5.6 4 4.2 3 16.9 12<br />

Māori 0.0 0 38.9 28 5.6 4 11.1 8 2.8 2 9.7 7 0.0 0 4.2 3 8.3 6 19.4 14<br />

Pacific<br />

peoples 4.8 1 47.6 10 4.8 1 19.0 4 0.0 0 9.5 2 4.8 1 4.8 1 4.8 1 0.0 0<br />

Asian 0.0 0 60.0 3 20.0 1 0.0 0 0.0 0 0.0 0 0.0 0 0.0 0 0.0 0 20.0 1<br />

European 2.6 1 53.8 21 2.6 1 0.0 0 0.0 0 2.6 1 10.3 4 5.1 2 5.1 2 17.9 7<br />

Other<br />

ethnicity 4.3 2 52.2 24 4.3 2 13.0 2 4.3 2 4.3 2 2.2 1 4.3 2 2.2 1 8.7 4<br />

32


culture’, <strong>and</strong> males more likely to report that they wanted ‘to get their name up’. Table 8<br />

shows that the youngest cohort were more likely than others to report that they were<br />

motivated by group culture <strong>and</strong> a desire to damage something <strong>and</strong> older cohorts, who<br />

answered in small numbers, were more likely than others to suggest that their participation<br />

was related to protest <strong>of</strong> some kind. Very few respondents reported that they were attracted<br />

by a sense <strong>of</strong> danger, excitement, or by a desire for celebrity (‘to get my name up’), although<br />

these themes did emerge from focus group discussion with graffiti-writers. Similarly,<br />

comments included in survey responses sometimes reflected the range <strong>of</strong> motivations for<br />

writing graffiti. The comment below illustrates something <strong>of</strong> this, as well as the distinctions<br />

drawn between different types <strong>of</strong> writing:<br />

There are two types <strong>of</strong> graffiti artist, the bad type <strong>and</strong> the good type. The good type<br />

just want to express there art, the thrill <strong>of</strong> getting ur piece up for a couple <strong>of</strong> people<br />

to admir [sic] before it is removed it addictive. But there are the bad ones that ruin it<br />

for the good one. They tag because <strong>of</strong> the group <strong>of</strong> people the hang out with <strong>and</strong> the<br />

competition they have with each other. The art they do doesn’t express any meaning.<br />

(Online survey respondent)<br />

It was clear that many respondents had complex reasons for engaging in graffiti which has<br />

implications for deterrence based strategies such as fines <strong>and</strong> prison sentences. Online<br />

survey respondents felt that graffiti was part <strong>of</strong> their lifestyle <strong>and</strong> it was incredibly important<br />

to them to be able to express themselves.<br />

I do it because I like it, why I like it is complicated but mostly has to do with being<br />

able to take control <strong>of</strong> my environment. (Online survey respondent)<br />

Stopping me from doing it That would be impossible. <strong>Graffiti</strong> is already part <strong>of</strong> my<br />

lifestyle <strong>and</strong> doing it is like therapy for me. (Online survey respondent)<br />

An older respondent who used to graffiti but had desisted stated that:<br />

Young people will stop tagging when they feel they matter <strong>and</strong> have purpose <strong>and</strong><br />

hope. <strong>Tagging</strong> is not the issue, the society <strong>and</strong> families that they come from are.<br />

(Online survey respondent)<br />

As noted by Ferrell (1995) <strong>and</strong> White (2001) graffiti writers <strong>of</strong>ten see parts <strong>of</strong> the city as<br />

spaces that need brightening up <strong>and</strong> this view <strong>of</strong> graffiti writing was endorsed by some <strong>of</strong> the<br />

survey <strong>and</strong> focus group respondents:<br />

I don’t like tagging but love murals <strong>and</strong> big pieces <strong>of</strong> aerosol art in public spaces,<br />

legal or not! It certainly brightens the dull <strong>and</strong> lifeless cement <strong>and</strong> is more stimulating<br />

to look at than advertisements as big as 10 story buildings! ….And is your city pretty<br />

anyway (Online survey respondent)<br />

33


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Without graffiti there would be no glittering bright spots in our decaying trash riddin<br />

inner cities. The colours can inspire a little happiness to the darkest <strong>of</strong> places.(Online<br />

survey respondent)<br />

Cos they think the city is ugly. They want to make it beautiful by writing their<br />

name all over it. Colourful. (Focus group participant)<br />

Creative expression, expressing sentiments, <strong>and</strong> sharing opinions were reasons highlighted by<br />

the online survey respondents for doing graffiti writing..<br />

I find it an easy way to express how I am feeling about current economic time <strong>and</strong><br />

the current government. (Online survey respondent)<br />

Why stop expressing myself I do it to share an opinion or art, not to destroy.<br />

(Online survey respondent)<br />

Comments like those above also clearly highlight the divisions between different types <strong>of</strong><br />

graffiti <strong>and</strong> those who engage in these kinds <strong>of</strong> activities. As with many criminological issues,<br />

graffiti writers are a diverse population <strong>and</strong> there is no simple ‘one size fits all’ solution to the<br />

perceived problems surrounding graffiti.<br />

While Māori <strong>and</strong> Pacific respondents were also more likely to report ‘creative expression’ as<br />

the main reason for their participation in graffiti, they were more likely than other ethnic<br />

groups to report that their involvement was attributable to being ‘part <strong>of</strong> a group culture’ <strong>and</strong><br />

the desire to ‘get my name up’.<br />

Respondents also referred to the informal rules surrounding the location <strong>of</strong> graffiti writing <strong>and</strong><br />

noted that some venues were strictly prohibited for ‘genuine’ graffiti writers. Those who<br />

broke these informal rules were seen as engaging in v<strong>and</strong>alism <strong>and</strong> highly disapproved <strong>of</strong>.<br />

One focus group participant summarised the informal criteria surrounding the location <strong>of</strong><br />

graffiti thus:<br />

There are rules, you know, even the graffiti guys have rules – so when someone<br />

breaks them it’s the pits. You know, the OG writers don’t tag churches or private<br />

property, schools. But people break those rules, most <strong>of</strong> the time it’s just hooligans.<br />

Those who were more engaged in graffiti were more likely to report that they would<br />

participate in future graffiti activity. Table 9 shows that more than two-thirds <strong>of</strong> that group<br />

would participate in future, compared to 26.9 per cent <strong>of</strong> those who reported that they had<br />

‘sometimes’ written graffiti. That past performance might be a guide to future activity is<br />

further suggested by the finding that 44.8 per cent <strong>of</strong> those who had sometimes participated<br />

in graffiti answered that they might write again in future.<br />

34


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Table 9: Future graffiti (number <strong>and</strong> percentages)<br />

Would you graffiti again<br />

Have you written graffiti<br />

Yes<br />

Sometimes<br />

% n % n<br />

Yes 67.8 177 26.9 18<br />

No 12.3 32 16.4 11<br />

Maybe 15.7 41 44.8 30<br />

No response 4.2 11 11.9 8<br />

Those over 30 years were much less likely to report that they would be involved in graffiti in<br />

the future than younger age cohorts. As Table 10 shows, the group most likely to indicate<br />

that they would write graffiti in the future were those aged 22-23 years: 42.9 per cent <strong>of</strong><br />

whom said that they would participate, <strong>and</strong> 8.6 per cent that they might do so. These<br />

responses emphasise that graffiti is <strong>of</strong>ten part <strong>of</strong> youth cultures <strong>and</strong> lifestyles.<br />

Table 10: Future participation in graffiti, by age (number <strong>and</strong> percentages)<br />

Yes No Maybe<br />

% n % n % n<br />

Under 14 36.4 4 0.0 0 18.2 2<br />

14-16 26.9 46 2.3 4 11.7 20<br />

17-19 24.9 64 6.6 17 11.3 29<br />

20-21 25.5 14 9.1 5 10.9 6<br />

22-23 42.9 15 2.9 1 8.6 3<br />

24-25 37.9 11 3.4 1 3.4 1<br />

25+ 38.6 17 13.6 6 2.3 1<br />

30+ 10.3 6 8.6 5 6.9 4<br />

40+ 0.0 0 6.5 2 0.0 0<br />

Survey respondents who stated that they wrote graffiti were asked to rank potential<br />

motivating factors, scoring them between 1 (‘most important reason’) <strong>and</strong> 6 (‘least important<br />

reason’). Table 11 shows the average rating for the six potential reasons: the lower the score<br />

the more importance respondents attached to that reason. The creativity <strong>of</strong> graffiti again<br />

emerges as an important factor <strong>and</strong> the desire to cause damage as the least significant<br />

factor. This is also supported by the online survey respondents who stated that expressing<br />

themselves creatively was an important reason for graffiti writing. This form <strong>of</strong> expression<br />

was even referred to as ‘therapy’ by one respondent.<br />

35


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Table 11: Ranking <strong>of</strong> reasons for graffiti<br />

Reasons for graffiti<br />

Score<br />

Because it is a form <strong>of</strong> being creative 2.53 Most significant<br />

Because it is fun 2.90<br />

Because I enjoy it 3.15<br />

Because it is exciting 3.18<br />

Because it is part <strong>of</strong> a group that I hang out with 3.91<br />

Because I want to damage things 5.08 Least significant<br />

All survey respondents, including those who write graffiti <strong>and</strong> those who do not, were asked<br />

for their opinion on why people write graffiti. Figure 1 shows that those who reported that<br />

they do not participate were more likely to suggest that graffiti was prompted by a desire to<br />

‘break the rules’: 7.2 per cent <strong>of</strong> non-participants selected this option compared to 2.7 per<br />

cent <strong>of</strong> participants. Similarly, non-participants were more likely to associate graffiti with the<br />

desire for celebrity (‘because they want to get their name up’) or with peer group activity<br />

(‘because it is part <strong>of</strong> a group that they hang out with’) than were those who participated.<br />

Conversely, those who participated in graffiti were more likely than non-participants to<br />

suggest that creativity is the primary motivation <strong>of</strong> graffiti writers: 21.5 per cent <strong>of</strong> writers<br />

suggested this was the key motivating factor, the most cited factor among this cohort.<br />

Enjoyment <strong>and</strong> excitement were also more likely to be cited by participants in graffiti writing<br />

(15.7 <strong>and</strong> 5.7 per cent <strong>of</strong> graffiti writers respectively). Focus group participants noted that<br />

graffiti writers in general <strong>of</strong>ten participated for pleasure, enjoyment <strong>and</strong> to achieve a measure<br />

<strong>of</strong> local celebrity:<br />

Busting out, mixing different colours. Doing different styles (Focus group<br />

participant)<br />

They wanna get famous (Focus group participant)<br />

No, they do it cos it’s a fact that you get noticed by other people (Focus<br />

group participant)<br />

For some people it is the only thing they are good at (Focus group<br />

participant)<br />

If you are in a gang you gotta get your name out there (Focus group<br />

participant).<br />

36


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Figure 1: Reasons people write graffiti, per cent, by personal involvement<br />

'Yes' graffiti writer<br />

21.5<br />

15.7 15.7<br />

5.7<br />

2.3 2.7<br />

3.8<br />

2.3<br />

8.8<br />

0.8<br />

7.3<br />

2.3<br />

11.1<br />

Sometime graffiti writers<br />

19.4%<br />

22.4%<br />

11.9% 11.9%<br />

11.9%<br />

9.0%<br />

6.0%<br />

1.5%<br />

.0%<br />

3.0% 3.0%<br />

.0%<br />

.0%<br />

Non-graffiti writers<br />

19.6%<br />

15.3%<br />

12.4%<br />

2.0%<br />

5.4%<br />

7.2%<br />

4.9% 4.3%<br />

9.0%<br />

2.2%<br />

8.5%<br />

.7%<br />

8.5%<br />

Because it is<br />

exciting<br />

For a sense <strong>of</strong><br />

accomplishment<br />

Because it<br />

breaks the rules<br />

None <strong>of</strong> the<br />

above<br />

Because <strong>of</strong> the<br />

sense <strong>of</strong> risk or<br />

danger<br />

Because they<br />

enjoy it<br />

Because they<br />

want to get<br />

their name up<br />

Because it is<br />

part <strong>of</strong> a group<br />

that they hang<br />

Because it is a<br />

form <strong>of</strong> being<br />

creative<br />

Because they<br />

want to damage<br />

things<br />

Because they<br />

feel like they<br />

don't have a<br />

Because they<br />

are angry or<br />

frustrated<br />

Non-response<br />

Because it is<br />

exciting<br />

For a sense <strong>of</strong><br />

accomplishment<br />

Because it<br />

breaks the rules<br />

None <strong>of</strong> the<br />

above<br />

Because <strong>of</strong> the<br />

sense <strong>of</strong> risk or<br />

danger<br />

Because they<br />

enjoy it<br />

Because they<br />

want to get<br />

their name up<br />

Because it is<br />

part <strong>of</strong> a group<br />

that they hang<br />

Because it is a<br />

form <strong>of</strong> being<br />

creative<br />

Because they<br />

want to damage<br />

things<br />

Because they<br />

feel like they<br />

don't have a<br />

Because they<br />

are angry or<br />

frustrated<br />

Non-response<br />

Because it is<br />

exciting<br />

For a sense <strong>of</strong><br />

accomplishment<br />

Because it<br />

breaks the rules<br />

None <strong>of</strong> the<br />

above<br />

Because <strong>of</strong> the<br />

sense <strong>of</strong> risk or<br />

danger<br />

Because they<br />

enjoy it<br />

Because they<br />

want to get<br />

their name up<br />

Because it is<br />

part <strong>of</strong> a group<br />

that they hang<br />

Because it is a<br />

form <strong>of</strong> being<br />

creative<br />

Because they<br />

want to damage<br />

things<br />

Because they<br />

feel like they<br />

don't have a<br />

Because they<br />

are angry or<br />

frustrated<br />

Non-response<br />

37


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

All survey respondents, whether they reported that they wrote graffiti or not, were asked<br />

whether they felt the illegal nature <strong>of</strong> writing made it more attractive. Figure 2 shows that a<br />

majority <strong>of</strong> respondents, whether they wrote or not, tended to agree that the illicit nature <strong>of</strong><br />

graffiti made it more exciting. Analysis <strong>of</strong> the survey results showed no differences in<br />

response on this issue in terms <strong>of</strong> age, gender or ethnicity. Agreement was strongest among<br />

those relatively involved in graffiti activity with 22.9 per cent ‘strongly agreeing’ that illegality<br />

made it more attractive. Online survey respondents cited the ‘thrill’ <strong>of</strong> illicit activity by stating<br />

that they stopped because <strong>of</strong>:<br />

Not feeling the buzz to hit up anymore (Online survey respondent)<br />

Focus group participants noted different views <strong>of</strong> the excitement associated with writing<br />

graffiti:<br />

Yeah, you get a buzz but not because it is exciting (Focus group participant)<br />

It’s exciting. If you see a cop <strong>and</strong> you haven’t finished your piece you got to get<br />

away (Focus group participant)<br />

Just do it because we do it. Nothing else to do in the ‘hood (Focus group participant)<br />

Figure 2: <strong>Graffiti</strong> is more exciting because it is illegal, by (non)involvement in<br />

writing<br />

Strongly agree<br />

11.5%<br />

15.6%<br />

22.9%<br />

Agree<br />

38.1%<br />

44.2%<br />

45.2%<br />

Neither agree nor<br />

disagree<br />

25.1%<br />

28.8%<br />

25.2%<br />

Disagree<br />

8.7%<br />

11.5%<br />

8.6%<br />

Strongly disagree<br />

5.2%<br />

3.8%<br />

5.4%<br />

No Sometimes Yes<br />

The attraction <strong>of</strong> the illicit nature <strong>of</strong> graffiti was apparent in focus group discussion. Many<br />

respondents spoke about the adrenalin rush they associated with graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging activity<br />

<strong>and</strong> that this would be muted if it were legalised. Others reported that being caught would<br />

change attitudes toward potential future <strong>of</strong>fending. The following excerpts reflect these<br />

views:<br />

38


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Depends on whether you’ve been caught or not. Before you get caught it is<br />

exciting but after you have been caught then you just don’t wanna get caught<br />

(Focus group participant)<br />

You don’t get a rush from something you are allowed to do (Focus group<br />

participant)<br />

It would still be exciting if it was legal but it would be way reduced (Focus<br />

group participant).<br />

Desistance from graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging<br />

All survey respondents were asked if they felt that ‘more effort should be made to stop<br />

graffiti’. Table 12 indicates views were near equally divided among all respondents but that<br />

the youngest age cohort <strong>and</strong> older groups (those over 30 years) strongly favoured more<br />

effort to stop graffiti. Differences between ethnic groups were also apparent, although not<br />

Table 12: Should more effort be made to stop graffiti, by gender, age, ethnicity<br />

<strong>and</strong> participation (number <strong>and</strong> percentages)<br />

Yes<br />

No<br />

% n % n<br />

All 49.8 344 50.2 347<br />

Male 41.1 153 58.9 219<br />

Female 59.9 191 40.1 128<br />

Under 14 72.7 8 27.3 3<br />

14-16 55.0 94 45.0 77<br />

17-19 50.2 129 49.8 128<br />

20-21 38.2 21 61.8 34<br />

22-23 48.6 17 51.4 18<br />

24-25 34.5 10 65.5 19<br />

25+ 29.5 13 70.5 31<br />

30+ 58.6 34 41.4 24<br />

40+ 58.1 18 41.9 13<br />

NZ European 57.8 155 42.2 113<br />

Māori 38.8 45 61.2 71<br />

Pacific peoples 64.1 25 35.9 14<br />

Asian 69.4 25 30.6 11<br />

European 19.3 11 80.7 46<br />

Other ethnicity 48.2 53 51.8 57<br />

Have not written 65.6 267 34.4 140<br />

Have written 24.6 57 75.4 175<br />

Have written ‘sometimes’ 38.5 20 61.5 32<br />

39


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

clear-cut: Māori <strong>and</strong> European respondents were more strongly against greater preventative<br />

efforts than other ethnic groups. Those who had not participated were more likely to agree<br />

that more should be done than those who had, <strong>and</strong> those who had participated only<br />

‘sometimes’ were more likely to want more effort to stop graffiti than those who might be<br />

considered more involved.<br />

Those who engaged in graffiti were asked a series <strong>of</strong> questions about factors that might lead<br />

them to stop writing in general terms. Table 13 shows that the quick removal <strong>of</strong> graffiti, a key<br />

plank <strong>of</strong> much local government policy, was rated very lowly as a reason for graffiti writers to<br />

stop: only 3.4 per cent <strong>of</strong> writers <strong>and</strong> 3.0 per cent <strong>of</strong> ‘sometime’ writers stated that this would<br />

be the main reason why they would desist. The online survey respondents also highlighted<br />

that graffiti removal presents a challenge to writers but in some cases provides a better<br />

surface for doing graffiti on:<br />

…In actual fact your anti tag paint creates us an easier surface to paint on. Bare<br />

brick walls <strong>and</strong> bare wooden fences eat spray paint <strong>and</strong> makes it hard to paint, buff<br />

paint makes a nice solid surface to paint on. (Online survey respondent).<br />

The most significant reason to stop, across all groups, was apprehension by police. Factors<br />

relating to ‘peer pressure’ or family disapproval were not cited as the strongest reasons to<br />

desist. Those who reported that they write graffiti were more likely to report that none <strong>of</strong> the<br />

potential factors listed would stop them from participating. This proportion was smaller<br />

among those who reported that they had written graffiti ‘sometimes’.<br />

The survey <strong>of</strong>fered respondents the opportunity to include ‘free text’ comments about<br />

reasons why they might stop graffiti. More than 100 comments were added, many <strong>of</strong> which<br />

noted, in various terms, that nothing would stop them from writing. Common sentiments<br />

included:<br />

Nothing would stop me (Online Survey respondent)<br />

If my h<strong>and</strong>s were cut <strong>of</strong>f (Online Survey respondent)<br />

Nothing. I'm in it for life (Online Survey respondent)<br />

Cut out my h<strong>and</strong>s (Online Survey respondent)<br />

I will never stop (Online Survey respondent).<br />

40


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Table 13: Reasons to stop doing graffiti (number <strong>and</strong> percentages)<br />

<strong>Graffiti</strong> writers Have written<br />

‘sometimes’<br />

% n % n<br />

Being caught 6.9 18 9.0 6<br />

Being apprehended by police 20.3 53 23.9 16<br />

‘If my friends didn’t like what I was doing’ 6.9 18 6.0 4<br />

If the graffiti was removed soon after I did it 3.4 9 3.0 2<br />

If my family disapproved 6.5 17 9.0 6<br />

None <strong>of</strong> these 51.0 133 37.3 25<br />

Comments from the survey showed that many respondents recognised that they might stop<br />

participating in graffiti for ‘life course’ reasons which is widely held as important in desistance<br />

from youth crime more generally (Sampson <strong>and</strong> Laub, 1993). Respondents cited<br />

employment, changing religious practices <strong>and</strong> parenthood as factors that might deter them<br />

from continuing to write graffiti. Typical comments included:<br />

Having a proper job <strong>and</strong> working full-time (Online Survey respondent)<br />

Growing up. Finding other forms <strong>of</strong> expression … (Online Survey respondent)<br />

Growing up <strong>and</strong> getting over that stage in your life!!! (Online Survey respondent)<br />

Figure 3 shows that the prospect <strong>of</strong> being caught or being apprehended by the police was<br />

stated as the reason to stop graffiti by over one third (37.5 per cent) <strong>of</strong> those who reported<br />

that they were engaged in v<strong>and</strong>alism. Interpreting these findings is difficult: it could be the<br />

case that those involved in graffiti or tagging are in general conforming <strong>and</strong> law abiding, <strong>and</strong><br />

therefore not involved in other criminal activities apart from graffiti writing. This is supported<br />

to some extent by the focus group data, as there was little reporting <strong>of</strong> other anti-social<br />

behaviour or criminal activities. Those that were alluded to were ‘status <strong>of</strong>fences’ such as<br />

underage drinking. It should be noted that the majority <strong>of</strong> focus group respondents stated<br />

drugs <strong>and</strong> alcohol adversely affected their ability to graffiti <strong>and</strong> that these substances were<br />

avoided while graffiti writing. The speedy removal <strong>of</strong> graffiti was a greater deterrence for<br />

those who categorised their graffiti as tagging or as ‘graffiti art’: almost no respondents in<br />

other categories reported that this would be reason for them to stop. These variations<br />

notwithst<strong>and</strong>ing, the most common response across all forms was that none <strong>of</strong> these<br />

strategies would lead respondents to desist from graffiti. This response is to be expected if<br />

the reasons for desistence are linked to desistence from youth crime in general, such as<br />

growing up. The strategies or reasons referred to in this question are not linked to these<br />

desistence factors.<br />

41


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Figure 3: Reason to stop graffiti, by form <strong>of</strong> graffiti (percentage)<br />

Stylised w riting<br />

<strong>Tagging</strong><br />

53.3%<br />

46.9%<br />

10.0%<br />

23.3%<br />

13.3%<br />

.0% .0%<br />

10.2%<br />

14.3%<br />

6.1%<br />

4.1%<br />

18.4%<br />

Being caught<br />

Being<br />

apprehended<br />

by police<br />

If my friends<br />

did not like<br />

what I was<br />

doing<br />

If the graffiti<br />

was removed<br />

soon after I<br />

did it<br />

if my family<br />

disapproved<br />

None <strong>of</strong><br />

these<br />

V<strong>and</strong>alism<br />

50.0%<br />

25.0%<br />

12.5% 12.5%<br />

.0% .0%<br />

Being caught<br />

Being<br />

apprehended<br />

by police<br />

If my friends<br />

did not like<br />

what I was<br />

doing<br />

If the graffiti<br />

was removed<br />

soon after I<br />

did it<br />

if my family<br />

disapproved<br />

None <strong>of</strong><br />

these<br />

<strong>Graffiti</strong> art<br />

50.6%<br />

19.5%<br />

3.9%<br />

10.4% 10.4%<br />

5.2%<br />

Being caught<br />

Being<br />

apprehended<br />

by police<br />

If my friends<br />

did not like<br />

what I was<br />

doing<br />

If the graffiti<br />

was removed<br />

soon after I<br />

did it<br />

if my family<br />

disapproved<br />

None <strong>of</strong><br />

these<br />

Being caught<br />

Being<br />

apprehended<br />

by police<br />

If my friends<br />

did not like<br />

what I was<br />

doing<br />

If the graffiti<br />

was removed<br />

soon after I<br />

did it<br />

if my family<br />

disapproved<br />

None <strong>of</strong><br />

these<br />

Political expression<br />

64.3%<br />

.0%<br />

14.3% 14.3%<br />

.0%<br />

7.1%<br />

Being caught<br />

Being<br />

apprehended<br />

by police<br />

If my friends<br />

did not like<br />

what I was<br />

doing<br />

If the graffiti<br />

was removed<br />

soon after I<br />

did it<br />

if my family<br />

disapproved<br />

None <strong>of</strong><br />

these<br />

None <strong>of</strong> these<br />

62.5%<br />

8.3%<br />

12.5% 12.5%<br />

.0%<br />

4.2%<br />

Being caught<br />

Being<br />

apprehended<br />

by police<br />

If my friends<br />

did not like<br />

what I was<br />

doing<br />

If the graffiti<br />

was removed<br />

soon after I<br />

did it<br />

if my family<br />

disapproved<br />

None <strong>of</strong><br />

these<br />

Table 14 shows that possible reasons for stopping graffiti varied according to age. The<br />

findings indicate that peer <strong>and</strong> family pressure may become less <strong>of</strong> an influence on<br />

desistance decisions as respondents got older, although the small number <strong>of</strong> older<br />

respondents means that this must be regarded as highly tentative. For all groups, the most<br />

common response was to select ‘none <strong>of</strong> these’, although younger participants chose this to a<br />

lesser extent than their older counterparts: those aged 22-23 years were most likely to report<br />

that none <strong>of</strong> the factors listed would cause them to stop writing graffiti. The data suggested<br />

42


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

no clear pattern in terms <strong>of</strong> gender or ethnic differences in attitudes toward factors that<br />

might lead respondents to stop writing graffiti.<br />

Table 14: Reasons to stop graffiti, by age (number <strong>and</strong> percentages)<br />

Being caught Being Friends’ Family Removal None <strong>of</strong> these<br />

apprehended<br />

by police<br />

disapproval disapproval<br />

% n % n % n % n % n % n<br />

All<br />

respondents<br />

7.8 24 22.5 69 7.2 22 7.5 23 3.6 11 51.5 158<br />

Under 14 0.0 0 16.7 1 16.7 1 16.7 1 0.0 0 50.0 3<br />

14-16 8.7 6 15.9 11 14.5 10 8.7 6 4.3 3 47.8 33<br />

17-19 7.3 8 29.4 32 5.5 6 10.1 11 3.7 4 44.0 48<br />

20-21 20.0 5 16.0 4 0.0 0 8.0 2 0.0 0 56.0 14<br />

22-23 0.0 0 21.1 4 5.3 1 0.0 0 0.0 0 73.7 14<br />

24-25 7.7 1 30.8 4 0.0 0 0.0 0 7.7 1 53.8 7<br />

Over 25 0.0 0 29.2 7 4.2 1 4.2 1 4.2 1 58.3 14<br />

Over 30 6.7 1 26.7 4 6.7 1 6.7 1 0.0 0 53.3 8<br />

Over 40 0.0 0 50.0 1 0.0 0 0.0 0 0.0 0 50.0 1<br />

Have written 7.3 18 21.4 53 7.3 18 6.9 17 3.6 9 53.6 133<br />

Have<br />

‘sometimes’<br />

written<br />

10.2 6 27.1 16 6.8 4 10.2 6 3.4 2 42.4 25<br />

Table 14 also indicates that those who have written graffiti were less likely to agree that<br />

being apprehended would stop people writing graffiti, whereas those who had ‘sometimes’<br />

written graffiti were more likely to suggest that this would cause people to desist.<br />

All respondents, whether they wrote graffiti or not, were asked to rank a similar list <strong>of</strong><br />

reasons why those who participate might stop doing graffiti on a scale <strong>of</strong> 1 to 5 (where 1 is<br />

the most effective <strong>and</strong> 5 the least effective). Table 15 shows the average rating from all<br />

respondents: the lower the average rating the more effective the item was rated. As was<br />

found in relation to perceived reasons for personal desistance, the quick removal <strong>of</strong> graffiti<br />

was rated as a relatively ineffective means to stop people writing.<br />

43


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Table 15: Average rating <strong>of</strong> reasons to stop doing graffiti<br />

Reasons to stop doing graffiti Rating<br />

Being apprehended by police 2.34 Most effective<br />

If my friends didn’t like what I was doing 2.56<br />

Being caught 2.70<br />

If my family disapproved 3.46<br />

If the graffiti was removed soon after I did it 3.80 Least effective<br />

Others stated that having legal graffiti walls or ‘tolerance zones’ would stop them from doing<br />

graffiti illegally.<br />

Having areas where <strong>Graffiti</strong>-Art was legal would stop me from expressing my art in<br />

public places that are considered illegal (Online survey respondent)<br />

If there were public places where one could demonstrate their art which was allowed<br />

then that would help. If you look into Venice Beach or a place called homel<strong>and</strong>, they<br />

have areas <strong>and</strong> competitions where you have a piece <strong>of</strong> wall <strong>and</strong> are free to<br />

demonstrate your work. Please look into this (Online survey respondent).<br />

Other ways to deter people from tagging were as highlighted as:<br />

No one will paint on plants. So build a fence or framing against the wall <strong>and</strong> grow a<br />

vine over it. Sure it costs money but you only need to do it once on that wall rather<br />

than using your so called ‘ant-tag’ paint weekly (Online survey respondent)<br />

There should be a big building with a competition with inside walls. Make them<br />

practice at home <strong>and</strong> then have prizes <strong>of</strong> the best wall. It would be like an art gallery<br />

(Focus group participant)<br />

All respondents were asked to rate five approaches to stopping young people writing graffiti,<br />

all <strong>of</strong> which are practiced or have been proposed in recent debates about tagging <strong>and</strong> graffiti<br />

in New Zeal<strong>and</strong>. Respondents were asked to ‘strongly agree’, ‘agree’, ‘neither agree nor<br />

disagree’, ‘disagree or strongly disagree’; each category was scored, from 5 for strongly<br />

agree down to 1 for strongly disagree. In aggregate terms, Table 16 shows the average score<br />

for each option:<br />

44


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Table 16: Average score for strategies to stop graffiti<br />

Strategies to stop graffiti<br />

Average<br />

score<br />

Providing legitimate sites (e.g. graffiti walls) 4.1 Strongest agreement<br />

Making <strong>of</strong>fenders clean-up graffiti 3.4<br />

Better education on victim impact 3.1<br />

Custodial sentences 3.1<br />

‘Naming <strong>and</strong> shaming’ <strong>of</strong>fenders 2.9 Weakest agreement<br />

Focus group discussion also reflected a preference for the provision <strong>of</strong> legitimate sites at<br />

which graffiti could be written. Requiring taggers to participate in ‘clean up’ programmes was<br />

regarded by many as counter-productive <strong>and</strong> that ‘naming <strong>and</strong> shaming’ activities such as the<br />

<strong>Wellington</strong> ‘pink vest’ solution might also have unintended consequences. The following<br />

responses reflected these discussions:<br />

By making a place where they can tag [graffiti will be reduced] (Focus group<br />

participant)<br />

Places that people can just go <strong>and</strong> hit up. Graff spots (Focus group participant)<br />

Youth workers to connect with them (Focus group participant)<br />

[Making taggers clean up] You’ll make the taggers angry, <strong>and</strong> that is why it<br />

goes up (Focus group participant)<br />

The pink vest – that is shaming eh (Focus group participant)<br />

We just have a laugh about it (Focus group participant)<br />

It is going to make them tag more (Focus group participant)<br />

It will piss some people <strong>of</strong>f, having to clean up other people’s work, unless<br />

they are tight with those people (Focus group participant).<br />

Cos if you have one <strong>of</strong> those jackets <strong>and</strong> you are scrubbing <strong>of</strong>f a ‘mean as’ piece that<br />

is fame (Focus group participant)<br />

It would make me go out more (Focus group participant)<br />

Figure 4 shows that support for the various measures that might prevent graffiti varied<br />

between those who participated, sometimes participated or were non-participants. The figure<br />

shows the proportion <strong>of</strong> respondents who ‘strongly agreed’ or ‘agreed’ with the various<br />

strategies. While there was broad support for the provision <strong>of</strong> legitimate venues in which<br />

45


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

graffiti could be written (so-called ‘zones <strong>of</strong> tolerance’) – to which around 80 per cent <strong>of</strong> all<br />

three groups ‘strongly agreed’ or ‘agreed’ – those who wrote graffiti (whether relatively <strong>of</strong>ten<br />

or only occasionally) reported much less agreement for measures than did those who were<br />

non-graffiti writers. The comments provided by more than 100 respondents provide some<br />

insight into the impact that the provision <strong>of</strong> legal sites for graffiti had:<br />

… I love to walk around the city <strong>and</strong> see little doodles in imaginative places. I think<br />

that maybe there should be areas which are okay to graffiti, but have these areas<br />

monitored to ensure there’s no gang tagging. (Online survey respondent).<br />

Figure 4: Strategies to prevent graffiti, by participation (per cent agree/strongly<br />

agree)<br />

82.4%<br />

78.8% 78.2%<br />

68.6%<br />

52.9%<br />

42.3%<br />

41.3%<br />

47.8%<br />

45.1%<br />

54.0%<br />

50.4%<br />

33.5%<br />

33.8%<br />

23.8%<br />

25.4%<br />

Legitimate venues Offender clean-up Name <strong>and</strong> shame'<br />

<strong>of</strong>fenders<br />

Custodial sentences<br />

Education on victim<br />

impact<br />

Writers Sometime writers Non-writers<br />

As noted in the literature review, several community projects involving painting murals have<br />

had success in deterring graffiti v<strong>and</strong>alism or tagging. <strong>Graffiti</strong> writers from both the survey<br />

<strong>and</strong> the focus groups also stated that they would not tag respected murals, <strong>and</strong> agreed that<br />

this would be an effective strategy to deter graffiti v<strong>and</strong>alism or tagging.<br />

For the respect <strong>of</strong> ‘<strong>Graffiti</strong> Art’ I would not go round taggin on a piece <strong>of</strong> artwork<br />

(Online survey respondent)<br />

….. or if there was an art work/mural on the surface [I wouldn’t graffiti or tag it]<br />

(Online survey respondent)<br />

46


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

If you put a bombing on a wall it will stop people just putting gang signs <strong>and</strong> stuff on<br />

it. So that is why you gotta put a mural on it (Focus group participant)<br />

If they did something cool there [on a subway that is regularly hit with graffiti] it<br />

wouldn’t get tagged (Focus group participant).<br />

Participants in graffiti were less likely to agree that <strong>of</strong>fender clean-up programmes, ‘naming<br />

<strong>and</strong> shaming’ <strong>of</strong> <strong>of</strong>fenders, custodial sentences or educational initiatives would prove effective<br />

in stopping young people from writing graffiti. The views <strong>of</strong> ‘sometime participants’ tended to<br />

more closely relate to those who were ‘non-participants’; they tended to agree in greater<br />

numbers that educational initiatives would prove effective desistance strategies. Focus group<br />

discussion considered the impact that various responses to graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging might have<br />

<strong>and</strong> tended to suggest that tougher criminal justice sanctions might not directly deter<br />

<strong>of</strong>fenders:<br />

[If given community service] You would probably do 50 hours, half <strong>of</strong> that then<br />

get back <strong>and</strong> do some more tagging (Focus group participant)<br />

I think the way <strong>of</strong> putting in harsher punishments for young people isn’t going<br />

to do anything because young people don’t have much <strong>of</strong> an outlet in the first<br />

place. So doing this sort <strong>of</strong> stuff <strong>and</strong> punishing them for it is just going to<br />

make them even more rebellious. If you actually give them an outlet to do it<br />

that is what will solve the problem. Rather than punishing them for expressing<br />

themselves (Focus group participant).<br />

47


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Discussion <strong>and</strong> conclusion<br />

The results presented above have implications for policy makers <strong>and</strong> the wide range <strong>of</strong><br />

agencies involved in crime prevention initiatives. Four themes <strong>of</strong> general significance emerge<br />

from the data:<br />

• it is clear that graffiti writers do not form a group clearly or wholly distinct from nongraffiti<br />

writers;<br />

• graffiti is a meaningful cultural <strong>and</strong> social practice for writers <strong>and</strong> only indirectly<br />

appreciated for its illegality;<br />

• for some (but not all), graffiti writing is associated with a desire for local celebrity,<br />

<strong>and</strong>;<br />

• graffiti writers’ perspectives on desistance suggest highly bounded rationality about<br />

prevention strategies such that consequences <strong>and</strong> implications were <strong>of</strong>ten poorly<br />

understood <strong>and</strong> not considered. Each <strong>of</strong> these is discussed in turn, <strong>and</strong> policy<br />

implications <strong>of</strong> each are highlighted.<br />

The research findings provide little evidence to suggest that graffiti writers are a distinct subgroup<br />

among young people with identifiable perceptions <strong>of</strong> graffiti, <strong>and</strong> the most appropriate<br />

responses to it, that differ from the wider population. Although graffiti writers were less likely<br />

to report that graffiti was ‘always a bad thing’, the survey data show that participants <strong>and</strong><br />

non-participants tended to share a contextual view <strong>of</strong> graffiti that recognises it has negative<br />

consequences in some circumstances. Similarly, all respondents to the survey tend to ‘agree’<br />

or ‘strongly agree’ that graffiti is an art form, <strong>and</strong> a form <strong>of</strong> personal expression that required<br />

creativity. They also tended to agree that it had negative attributes in terms <strong>of</strong> property<br />

damage <strong>and</strong> affect on residents’ perceptions <strong>of</strong> their neighbourhood. Attitudes toward graffiti<br />

were shaped by fine judgements relating to the perceived legitimacy <strong>of</strong> the venue, the<br />

physical challenges overcome to write on that location, <strong>and</strong> the aesthetic value <strong>of</strong> the graffiti<br />

itself. Even those who engaged in graffiti writing <strong>and</strong> appreciated it as an artistically<br />

worthwhile activity tended to report that some venues, such as schools or marae, were not<br />

legitimate sites for graffiti. As cited earlier, one focus group participant summarised the<br />

informal criteria surrounding the location <strong>of</strong> graffiti thus: ‘there are rules, you know, even the<br />

graffiti guys have rules – so when someone breaks them it’s the pits. You know, the OG<br />

writers don’t tag churches or private property, schools. But people break those rules, most <strong>of</strong><br />

the time it’s just hooligans’. Responses to graffiti writing that cast participants as antisocial<br />

criminals are unlikely to coincide with young people’s perceptions <strong>and</strong> attitudes toward graffiti<br />

<strong>and</strong> tagging, which are bound up with conceptions <strong>of</strong> the value <strong>of</strong> private property <strong>and</strong> public<br />

venues that are not legitimate sites for writing.<br />

48


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

A second key finding from the study is that participants underst<strong>and</strong> graffiti writing as an<br />

activity that is meaningful in cultural <strong>and</strong> aesthetic terms. Cultural criminologists argue young<br />

people invest intensely in activities such as graffiti that have a myriad <strong>of</strong> complex meanings<br />

to them. Those who are already involved in graffiti writing see it as meaningful in some way<br />

<strong>and</strong> intend to carry on writing. The informal codes that deem some sites as valid for graffiti<br />

while defining others as unacceptable are themselves mediated by aesthetic judgements<br />

about the content <strong>of</strong> graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging. As was noted in Table 2, more than 80 per cent <strong>of</strong><br />

respondents ‘agreed’ or ‘strongly agreed’ that graffiti is an art form <strong>and</strong> it was very apparent<br />

from focus group discussions that it is an activity imbued with cultural meaning that can be<br />

greatly valued by participants. Artistic meaning is evidenced by the careful distinctions made<br />

about different forms <strong>of</strong> writing <strong>and</strong> the relative merit <strong>of</strong> highly crafted conceptualised<br />

‘pieces’, which are ranked as more significant than ‘bombs’ (stylised writing) compared to<br />

‘tags’. Focus group discussion showed that graffiti is understood by writers as an engaging<br />

active event, something that happens <strong>and</strong> is corporeal (as Halsey <strong>and</strong> Young (2000) put it, it<br />

connects the writer to the urban l<strong>and</strong>scape) but for non-writers, willing or unwilling,<br />

consumers, viewers or spectators, it is a product: a material outcome that signifies criminal<br />

damage or trespass <strong>of</strong> some kind that is not judged in aesthetic terms. Writers have very<br />

different relations with graffiti <strong>and</strong> value the skill, imagination <strong>and</strong> audacity – <strong>and</strong> perhaps the<br />

collaboration – that has been invested in the production <strong>of</strong> a piece. This contrasts vividly with<br />

the relationship <strong>of</strong> the passive viewer to the finished product. The pleasure <strong>and</strong> enjoyment<br />

that participants derive from writing graffiti is associated not with the damage to property<br />

that preoccupies owners – which graffiti writers recognise as they report that they would be<br />

unhappy were their own property targeted – but with the physical challenge <strong>of</strong> producing<br />

complex artistic forms in difficult circumstances. The illegality <strong>of</strong> graffiti is an important<br />

dimension <strong>of</strong> the excitement (<strong>of</strong>ten described in focus groups <strong>and</strong> the online survey as the<br />

‘adrenalin rush’) associated with writing, <strong>and</strong> many respondents spoke <strong>of</strong> the fun associated<br />

with evading police <strong>and</strong> other authorities. The importance <strong>of</strong> illegality in underst<strong>and</strong>ing graffiti<br />

is further complicated since it is also apparent that graffiti is not always easily equated with<br />

criminal damage or v<strong>and</strong>alism although it <strong>of</strong>ten incorporates damage to property (Craw,<br />

Lel<strong>and</strong> et al., 2006). It must be stated that graffiti artists do not necessarily engage in other<br />

forms <strong>of</strong> v<strong>and</strong>alism, for example, damaging train seats <strong>and</strong> windows, or in criminal activity<br />

related to gang activity or drug <strong>and</strong> alcohol use (Halsey <strong>and</strong> Young 2006).<br />

The findings clearly show that graffiti writing is imbued with meaning derived from a<br />

combination <strong>of</strong> an appreciation <strong>of</strong> the aesthetic merits <strong>of</strong> a mural <strong>and</strong> an evaluation <strong>of</strong><br />

respect for the perceived effort <strong>and</strong> risk involved in its production. Focus group respondents<br />

<strong>of</strong>ten spoke about graffiti as a craft that requires dedication <strong>and</strong> learning, as practitioners<br />

develop from informal scribbles <strong>and</strong> tagging to pieces that require greater technical skill <strong>and</strong><br />

an appreciation <strong>of</strong> artistic <strong>and</strong> cultural reference points associated with the art form. Some <strong>of</strong><br />

49


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

these techniques <strong>and</strong> conventions were learnt <strong>and</strong> developed through association with more<br />

established practitioners in ways not wholly unlike more conventional apprenticeships. Not all<br />

those who do ‘throw ups’, ‘bombs’ or ‘stompies’ will develop the necessary talent <strong>and</strong><br />

application to progress to more aesthetic pieces worthy <strong>of</strong> greater respect. In these terms,<br />

young people discussed different styles <strong>and</strong> levels <strong>of</strong> graffiti in ways that might <strong>of</strong>ten be<br />

associated with the practice <strong>of</strong> legitimate art forms <strong>and</strong> skills.<br />

Crime prevention <strong>and</strong> policy responses need to underst<strong>and</strong> the cultural meaning <strong>and</strong> value<br />

attached to many forms <strong>of</strong> graffiti <strong>and</strong> to move beyond simplistic dichotomies between art<br />

<strong>and</strong> criminal damage. One <strong>of</strong> the key findings <strong>of</strong> this research is that young people tend to<br />

associate both with graffiti writing but that the artistic merit associated with complex<br />

sophisticated pieces is held to outweigh problems <strong>of</strong> crime <strong>and</strong> property damage. This<br />

perspective also raises questions about eradication programmes based on the speedy<br />

removal <strong>of</strong> graffiti. Largely based on an epidemiological approach that assumes the presence<br />

<strong>of</strong> graffiti encourages imitative behaviour, eradication programmes effectively contest control<br />

over territory in ways that exacerbate the risks <strong>and</strong> challenges that attract writers in the first<br />

place (Ferrell, 1995).<br />

A third key feature <strong>of</strong> the research findings relates to the importance <strong>of</strong> celebrity <strong>and</strong> local<br />

fame associated with graffiti writing. While the gaining <strong>and</strong> sustaining <strong>of</strong> respect through<br />

graffiti might seem invalid or relatively hollow to those not involved, it was clearly an<br />

important motivator for young people, <strong>and</strong> even those who did not participate in writing<br />

spoke about reading local tags <strong>and</strong> graffiti in these terms. Pursuing peer group credibility<br />

might take other forms among other sections <strong>of</strong> society but it is not an activity pursued only<br />

by youth subcultures (Halsey <strong>and</strong> Young, 2006: 281). Indeed graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging might be<br />

considered a form <strong>of</strong> micro-celebrity not wholly divorced from other forms <strong>of</strong> media in<br />

contemporary society. The proliferation <strong>of</strong> websites <strong>and</strong> social media that celebrate graffiti<br />

tend to reinforce the idea that this form <strong>of</strong> recognition can be considered on a spectrum with<br />

other more apparently legitimate avenues to secure respect through recognition. The<br />

increasing salience <strong>of</strong> hip hop culture, from which much graffiti culture emerged in the United<br />

States in the late 1970s <strong>and</strong> 1980s within mainstream TV, music, film, internet <strong>and</strong> gaming<br />

media, underpins the blurred boundaries between celebrity status that can be gained from<br />

illegal graffiti <strong>and</strong> legitimate cultural forms (Ferrell, 1993; Halsey <strong>and</strong> Young, 2006). While<br />

gaining respect was an important facet <strong>of</strong> writing for many respondents to this study, this<br />

was sometimes tempered by the need to protect identity in order to avoid detection. An<br />

attractive part <strong>of</strong> the subterfuge associated with writing is the distinction between securing<br />

recognition for tags or particular graffiti style <strong>and</strong> the maintenance <strong>of</strong> individual anonymity.<br />

This might have consequences in terms <strong>of</strong> crime prevention <strong>and</strong> detection since efforts to<br />

collate databases <strong>of</strong> prolific local taggers serve to reinforce recognition <strong>and</strong> cement writers’<br />

reputations for renowned high-pr<strong>of</strong>ile work. Some research respondents reflected on this in<br />

50


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

focus group discussion. One survey respondent argued that media coverage in general<br />

reinforces the attraction <strong>of</strong> writing, arguing that an important strategy to stop graffiti <strong>and</strong><br />

tagging would be to:<br />

QUIT the media coverage!!! If you show tags on tv, Its Tags you will get!! show<br />

some good stuff for once .. at least that way you will have people doing decent work<br />

in effort to get on tv instead <strong>of</strong> this scribble rubbish the media promotes. Scribble<br />

Tags are not what graffiti art is about. That’s just what the no namer kids do to get<br />

famous. (Online survey respondent)<br />

The fourth main finding from the study relates more directly to crime prevention initiatives as<br />

it relates to research participants’ perspectives on desistance from graffiti. Many <strong>of</strong> those who<br />

were ‘occasional’ or ‘frequent’ writers <strong>of</strong> graffiti reported that none <strong>of</strong> the preventative<br />

measures identified in the questionnaire would stop them from participating. Almost 70 per<br />

cent <strong>of</strong> writers said that they would graffiti again <strong>and</strong> 16 per cent said that they might do so.<br />

Nearly 45 per cent <strong>of</strong> ‘sometime’ graffiti writers said maybe they would do so in the future. A<br />

majority <strong>of</strong> both writers <strong>and</strong> ‘sometime’ writers disagreed that more should be done to tackle<br />

graffiti; for both groups the prospect <strong>of</strong> being apprehended by the police <strong>of</strong>fered the<br />

strongest reason for them personally to desist. In more general terms, relating to all<br />

participation in graffiti, survey respondents strongly favoured the provision <strong>of</strong> legitimate<br />

venues as a strategy to prevent graffiti. Clearly more rigorous evaluation <strong>of</strong> such initiatives is<br />

needed <strong>and</strong> it seems unlikely that legitimate walls will eradicate illegal graffiti, even if it<br />

reduces its incidence. While respondents reacted more or less favourably to potential<br />

interventions to stop or divert graffiti, it was <strong>of</strong>ten apparent that respondents thought little <strong>of</strong><br />

wider implications or long-term consequences <strong>of</strong> writing, either in terms <strong>of</strong> comeback on<br />

themselves or more generally. One focus group participant observed that ‘some taggers<br />

though don’t even register the severity <strong>of</strong> what they are doing or the punishment they could<br />

get’. Moreover, since writers are motivated, in part, by the thrill <strong>and</strong> adrenaline associated<br />

with their activity, the prospect <strong>of</strong> stronger sanctions was seen by many as adding to risk <strong>and</strong><br />

attraction <strong>of</strong> graffiti writing. In addition, tougher sanctions may elevate the respect that can<br />

be secured through tagging <strong>and</strong> graffiti. Many comments in the focus group discussion<br />

reflected the perceived limitations <strong>of</strong> introducing stronger penalties for tagging <strong>and</strong> graffiti:<br />

Tougher penalties means bigger thrills (Focus group participant)<br />

If you succeed <strong>and</strong> go to jail you will be seen as a hero among that group or among<br />

your friends or whatever (Focus group participant)<br />

The implications <strong>of</strong> this survey <strong>and</strong> focus group research for policy responses to graffiti <strong>and</strong><br />

tagging are many <strong>and</strong> various. Key among these are that young people expressed a widerange<br />

<strong>of</strong> perspectives on graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging, <strong>and</strong> that those who admitted some degree <strong>of</strong><br />

participation did not always differ greatly from those who do not participate. Young people<br />

51


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

had highly nuanced views such that some forms <strong>of</strong> writing are valued for their artistic content<br />

<strong>and</strong> the risks override the skill required in producing pieces. Only a small minority <strong>of</strong> young<br />

people suggested that v<strong>and</strong>alism <strong>and</strong> criminal damage were primary motivations. Even those<br />

who admitted participating in graffiti <strong>and</strong> v<strong>and</strong>alism maintained that there were areas <strong>and</strong><br />

types <strong>of</strong> property that were not acceptable sites for writing <strong>and</strong> that these should not be<br />

subject to the damage <strong>and</strong> ‘disrespect’ sometimes associated with such activity.<br />

52


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

References<br />

Arthur, R. (2006) Family Life <strong>and</strong> Youth Offending: Home Is Where the Hurt Is, New York:<br />

Routledge.<br />

Bottoms, A. (2006) ‘Incivilities, Offence <strong>and</strong> Social Order in Residential Communities’, in: von<br />

Hirsch, A. <strong>and</strong> Simester, A.P. (eds) Incivilities: Regulating Offensive Behaviour, Oxford,<br />

UK: Hart Publishing, pp. 239–280.<br />

Bryman, A. (2004) Social Science Research Methods, Oxford: Oxford <strong>University</strong> Press..<br />

City Life Independent Herald (2009) ‘<strong>Tagging</strong> escalates after spray can theft’, March 11th.<br />

Coleman, C. <strong>and</strong> Moynihan, J. (1996) Underst<strong>and</strong>ing Crime Data, Buckingham: Open<br />

<strong>University</strong> Press.<br />

Cornish, D.B. <strong>and</strong> Clarke, R.V. (eds) (1986) Reasoning Criminal - Rational Choice Perspectives<br />

on Offending, New Jersey: Springer-Verlag.<br />

Craw, P.J., Lel<strong>and</strong>, Jr., L.S., et al. (2006) ‘The Mural as <strong>Graffiti</strong> Deterrence’, Environment <strong>and</strong><br />

Behavior, 38(3), pp. 422-434.<br />

Creswell, J. W., & Plano Clark, V. L. (2006) Designing <strong>and</strong> Conducting Mixed Methods<br />

Research, Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: Sage.<br />

Dominion Post (2008) 'Taggers in pink while cleaning up their work’ 4th June.<br />

Doran, B. <strong>and</strong> Lees, B. (2005) ‘Investigating the Spatiotemporal Links Between Disorder,<br />

Crime <strong>and</strong> the Fear <strong>of</strong> Crime’, The Pr<strong>of</strong>essional Geographer, 57(1) pp.1-12.<br />

Ferrell, J. (1993) Crimes <strong>of</strong> Style: Urban <strong>Graffiti</strong> <strong>and</strong> the Politics <strong>of</strong> Criminality, New York:<br />

Garl<strong>and</strong>.<br />

Ferrell, J. (1995) 'Urban <strong>Graffiti</strong> - Crime, Control <strong>and</strong> Resistance', Youth <strong>and</strong> Society, 27(1),<br />

pp. 73-92.<br />

Gilbert, N. (ed) (2008) Researching Social Life, London: Sage.<br />

Goldson, B., (ed) (2007) The Dictionary <strong>of</strong> Youth Justice, Willan: Cullompton..<br />

Halsey, M. <strong>and</strong> Young, A. (2002) 'The Meaning <strong>of</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong> <strong>and</strong> Municipal Administration', The<br />

Australian <strong>and</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong> Journal <strong>of</strong> Criminology, 35(2), pp. 165-186.<br />

Halsey, M. <strong>and</strong> Young, A. (2006) ''Our Desires are Ungovernable': Writing <strong>Graffiti</strong> in Urban<br />

space', Theoretical Criminology, 10(3), pp. 275-306.<br />

Hastings District Council (2008) <strong>Graffiti</strong> V<strong>and</strong>alism Strategy 2008, Hastings: Hastings District<br />

Council.<br />

Herbert, D. (1993) ‘Neighbourhood Incivilities <strong>and</strong> the Study <strong>of</strong> Crime in Place’, Area, 25, pp.<br />

45-54.<br />

Innes, M. (2004) ‘Reinventing Tradition Reassurance, Neighbourhood Security <strong>and</strong> Policing’,<br />

Criminal Justice, 4 (2), pp. 151-171.<br />

Jeffery, C. R. (1971) Crime Prevention Through Environmental Design, Beverly Hills <strong>and</strong><br />

London: Sage.<br />

Jupp, V. (2001) 'Appreciative Criminology', in McLauglin, E. <strong>and</strong> Muncie, J. (eds) The Sage<br />

Dictionary <strong>of</strong> Criminology, London: Sage, pp. 12-13.<br />

Jupp.V, Davies. P, & Francis, P. (eds) (2000) Doing Criminological Research, Sage, London.<br />

Katz, J. (1988) Seductions <strong>of</strong> Crime: Moral <strong>and</strong> Sensual Attractions in Doing Evil, New York:<br />

Basic Books.<br />

Koubaridis, A., Vass, B., et al. (2008) ‘The Day Bruce Emery Saw Red’, Auckl<strong>and</strong>: APN News<br />

& Media.<br />

Matza, D. (1969) Becoming Deviant, New Jersey: Prentice Hall.<br />

Ministry <strong>of</strong> Justice (2006) How to STOP <strong>Graffiti</strong> Guide, <strong>Wellington</strong>: Ministry <strong>of</strong> Justice.<br />

Ministry <strong>of</strong> Justice (2006) KnowHow Beat <strong>Graffiti</strong> Guide, <strong>Wellington</strong>: Ministry <strong>of</strong> Justice.<br />

53


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Ministry <strong>of</strong> Justice (2008) <strong>Graffiti</strong> V<strong>and</strong>alism Prevention Grant Fund Application Guidelines<br />

2008/09, <strong>Wellington</strong>: Ministry <strong>of</strong> Justice.<br />

Ministry <strong>of</strong> Justice (2008) STOP Strategy, Crime Prevention Unit, <strong>Wellington</strong>: Ministry <strong>of</strong><br />

Justice.<br />

M<strong>of</strong>fitt, T.E. (1993) ‘Adolescence-Limited <strong>and</strong> Life Course Persistent Antisocial Behaviour: A<br />

Developmental Taxonomy’, Psychological Review, 100, pp. 674-701.<br />

New Zeal<strong>and</strong> Government (2008) Summary Offences (<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong> V<strong>and</strong>alism)<br />

Amendment Act 2008, <strong>Wellington</strong>: New Zeal<strong>and</strong> Government.<br />

Noakes, L. <strong>and</strong> Wincup, E. (2004) Criminological Research: Underst<strong>and</strong>ing Qualitative<br />

Methods, London: Sage.<br />

Pain, R. <strong>and</strong> Francis, P. (2003) ‘Reflections on Participatory Research’, Area, 35(1), pp. 46-<br />

54.<br />

Police News (2008) ‘<strong>Tagging</strong>: the Scourge <strong>of</strong> Our Streets’, January/February, pp. 12-16.<br />

Stanko, E (2000) ‘Victims R Us: the Life History <strong>of</strong> ‘Fear <strong>of</strong> Crime’ <strong>and</strong> the Politicisation <strong>of</strong><br />

Violence’, in Hope, T. <strong>and</strong> Sparks, R. (eds) Crime, Risk <strong>and</strong> Insecurity, London:<br />

Routledge..<br />

White, R. (2001) ‘Grafffiti, Crime Prevention <strong>and</strong> Cultural Space’, Current Issues in Criminal<br />

Justice, 12(3) pp. 253-68.<br />

Wilson, J.Q. <strong>and</strong> Kelling, G. (1982) ‘The Police <strong>and</strong> Neighbourhood Safety’, Atlantic Monthly,<br />

March, pp. 28-38.<br />

Wortley, R. <strong>and</strong> Mazerolle, L. (eds) (2008) Environmental Criminology <strong>and</strong> Crime Analysis,<br />

Cullompton: Willan Publishing.<br />

54


Appendix one: on-line survey<br />

55


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

56


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

57


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

58


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

59


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

60


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Appendix two: schedule for focus group discussion<br />

The aims <strong>of</strong> this research project are<br />

• To find out why people do graffiti <strong>and</strong> or tagging<br />

• To find out how young people feel about graffiti <strong>and</strong> those who do it<br />

• To find out if there are any ways that graffiti can be prevented<br />

The focus groups will ask participants for their opinions <strong>and</strong> to share their<br />

knowledge about graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging.<br />

Preamble:<br />

As you might know, there has been a lot <strong>of</strong> discussion about the issue <strong>of</strong> tagging<br />

<strong>and</strong> graffiti in New Zeal<strong>and</strong> in recent months. We want to discuss these topics<br />

with you today, so that we can underst<strong>and</strong> young people’s perspectives on these<br />

important issues.<br />

The discussion will last for around 1 hour <strong>and</strong> is being recorded. Your personal<br />

details will remain confidential <strong>and</strong> you will not be identified in any <strong>of</strong> the reports<br />

based on this discussion.<br />

Note for facilitator: the key questions are listed below, with supplementary<br />

suggestions beneath each point. It is important that each key question is<br />

covered. If significant additional points are raised during the discussion please<br />

note them but move on <strong>and</strong> return to them at the end, should time permit.<br />

1. Have you ever discussed tagging <strong>and</strong> graffiti with your friends<br />

• do you think that tagging <strong>and</strong> graffiti are serious problems in your area<br />

• where are you most likely to see tagging <strong>and</strong> graffiti in your area<br />

2. How do you feel when you see tagging <strong>and</strong> graffiti<br />

• Do your feelings change depending on where the tagging <strong>and</strong> graffiti is<br />

3. Do you think that graffiti is art or is it v<strong>and</strong>alism<br />

• Is tagging <strong>and</strong> graffiti linked to problem behaviour, such as underage<br />

drinking, drug-taking, v<strong>and</strong>alism, <strong>and</strong> so on<br />

4. Does anyone that you know do tagging or graffiti<br />

• Would they boast about it to their friends<br />

5. Should tagging <strong>and</strong> graffiti be allowed in some places<br />

• What places would be ok<br />

• Do you feel differently about graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging depending on where it is<br />

A school compared to waste-ground, for example<br />

6. Would tougher penalties stop people from tagging<br />

• If not, what other measures might stop graffiti <strong>and</strong> tagging<br />

61


<strong>Tagging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Graffiti</strong>: attitudes <strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>of</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong>ers<br />

Appendix three: pr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>of</strong> focus groups<br />

Focus groups run by VUW researchers<br />

Location <strong>and</strong> sponsoring organisation Date Number <strong>of</strong> participants<br />

NZ Institute <strong>of</strong> Sport, <strong>Wellington</strong> 30 April 2009 17 (in two groups)<br />

Hutt Valley High School, Alternative<br />

Education Centre<br />

<strong>Wellington</strong> City Mission, Alternative<br />

Education Centre<br />

4 May 2009 14<br />

6 May 2009 11<br />

New Lynn, Tag Out Trust 8 May 2009 7<br />

Manurewa, Manukau Beautification<br />

Charitable Trust<br />

Rangatahi, Wainuiomata, Alternative<br />

Education Centre<br />

8 May 2009 7<br />

13 May 2009 3<br />

59 total<br />

Focus groups run by Ministry <strong>of</strong> Youth Development staff<br />

Location <strong>and</strong> sponsoring organisation Date Number <strong>of</strong> participants<br />

Auckl<strong>and</strong> City Council <strong>Graffiti</strong> Free<br />

Project<br />

15 April 2009 15<br />

Christchurch, YMCA 8 April 2009 12<br />

Highbury Community <strong>and</strong> Whānau<br />

Centre, Palmerston North<br />

20 April 2009 15<br />

Otaki, House <strong>of</strong> Hope 20 April 2009 10<br />

Mangere Bridge, Tamatiaro Trust,<br />

Youth Training Programme<br />

14 April 2009 30 (in two groups)<br />

Napier, Napier Youth Council 16 April 2009 10<br />

Whangarei, Bream Bay Community<br />

Centre<br />

7 April 2009 6<br />

Stratford, Youth Council 21 April 2009 10<br />

Rotorua, Te Arawa Lakes Trust 22 April 2009 20 (in two groups)<br />

Whangarei, People Potential 6 April 2009 9<br />

Christchurch, Project Legit 8 April 2009 8<br />

145 total<br />

62

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!