20.01.2015 Views

Human Agency in Social Cognitive Theory - American Psychological ...

Human Agency in Social Cognitive Theory - American Psychological ...

Human Agency in Social Cognitive Theory - American Psychological ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

to them (Locke, Frederick, Lee, & Bobko, 1984; Taylor,<br />

Locke, Lee, & Gist, 1984; Wood & Bandura, <strong>in</strong> press).<br />

As I will show later, challeng<strong>in</strong>g goals raise the level of<br />

motivation and performance atta<strong>in</strong>ments (Locke, Shaw,<br />

Saari, & Latham, 1981; Mento, Steel, & Karren, 1987).<br />

A major function of thought is to enable people to<br />

predict the occurrence of events and to create the means<br />

for exercis<strong>in</strong>g control over those that affect their daily<br />

lives. Many activities <strong>in</strong>volve <strong>in</strong>ferential judgments about<br />

conditional relations between events <strong>in</strong> probabilistic environments.<br />

Discernment of predictive rules requires<br />

cognitive process<strong>in</strong>g of multidimensional <strong>in</strong>formation<br />

that conta<strong>in</strong>s many ambiguities and uncerta<strong>in</strong>ties. In ferret<strong>in</strong>g<br />

out predictive rules, people must draw on their<br />

state of knowledge to generate hypotheses about predictive<br />

factors, to weight and <strong>in</strong>tegrate them <strong>in</strong>to composite rules,<br />

to test their judgments aga<strong>in</strong>st outcome <strong>in</strong>formation, and<br />

to remember which notions they had tested and how well<br />

they had worked. It requires a strong sense of efficacy to<br />

rema<strong>in</strong> task oriented <strong>in</strong> the face of judgmental failures.<br />

Indeed, people who believe strongly <strong>in</strong> their problemsolv<strong>in</strong>g<br />

capabilities rema<strong>in</strong> highly efficient <strong>in</strong> their analytic<br />

th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> complex decision-mak<strong>in</strong>g situations, whereas<br />

those who are plagued by self-doubts are erratic <strong>in</strong> their<br />

analytic th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g (Bandura & Wood, 1989; Wood & Bandura,<br />

1989). Quality of analytic th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong> turn, affects<br />

performance accomplishments.<br />

People's perceptions of their efficacy <strong>in</strong>fluence the<br />

types of anticipatory scenarios they construct and reiterate.<br />

Those who have a high sense of efficacy visualize<br />

success scenarios that provide positive guides for performance.<br />

Those who judge themselves as <strong>in</strong>efficacious are<br />

more <strong>in</strong>cl<strong>in</strong>ed to visualize failure scenarios that underm<strong>in</strong>e<br />

performance by dwell<strong>in</strong>g on how th<strong>in</strong>gs will go<br />

wrong. <strong>Cognitive</strong> simulations <strong>in</strong> which <strong>in</strong>dividuals visualize<br />

themselves execut<strong>in</strong>g activities skillfully enhance<br />

subsequent performance (Bandura, 1986; Corb<strong>in</strong>, 1972;<br />

Feltz & Landers, 1983; Kazd<strong>in</strong>, 1978; Markus, Cross, &<br />

Wurf, <strong>in</strong> press). Perceived self-efficacy and cognitive simulation<br />

affect each other bidirectionally. A high sense of<br />

efficacy fosters cognitive constructions of effective actions,<br />

and cognitive reiteration of efficacious courses of action<br />

strengthens self-perceptions of efficacy (Bandura &<br />

Adams, 1977; Kazd<strong>in</strong>, 1979),<br />

Self-efficacy beliefs usually affect cognitive function<strong>in</strong>g<br />

through the jo<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong>fluence of motivational and<br />

<strong>in</strong>formation-process<strong>in</strong>g operations. This dual <strong>in</strong>fluence<br />

is illustrated <strong>in</strong> studies of different sources of variation<br />

<strong>in</strong> memory performance. The stronger people's beliefs <strong>in</strong><br />

their memory capacities, the more effort they devote to<br />

cognitive process<strong>in</strong>g of memory tasks, which, <strong>in</strong> turn,<br />

enhances their memory performances (Berry, 1987).<br />

Preparation of this article was facilitated by Public Health Research<br />

Grant No. MH-5162-25 from the National Institute for Mental Health.<br />

This article was presented as an <strong>in</strong>vited address at the XXIV International<br />

Congress of Psychology, Sydney, Australia, August 1988.<br />

Correspondence concern<strong>in</strong>g this article should be addressed to Albert<br />

Bandura, Build<strong>in</strong>g 420, Jordan Hall, Stanford University, Stanford,<br />

CA 94305.<br />

Motivational Processes<br />

People's self-efficacy beliefs determ<strong>in</strong>e their level of motivation,<br />

as reflected <strong>in</strong> how much effort they will exert<br />

<strong>in</strong> an endeavor and how long they will persevere <strong>in</strong> the<br />

face of obstacles. The stronger the belief <strong>in</strong> their capabilities,<br />

the greater and more persistent are their efforts<br />

(Bandura, 1988a). When faced with difficulties, people<br />

who are beset by self-doubts about their capabilities<br />

slacken their efforts or abort their attempts prematurely<br />

and quickly settle for mediocre solutions, whereas those<br />

who have a strong belief <strong>in</strong> their capabilities exert greater<br />

effort to master the challenge (Bandura & Cervone, 1983,<br />

1986; Cervone & Peake, 1986; Jacobs, Prentice-Dunn,<br />

& Rogers, 1984; We<strong>in</strong>berg, Gould, & Jackson, 1979).<br />

Strong perseverance usually pays off <strong>in</strong> performance accomplishments.<br />

There is a grow<strong>in</strong>g body of evidence that human<br />

atta<strong>in</strong>ments and positive well-be<strong>in</strong>g require an optimistic<br />

sense of personal efficacy (Bandura, 1986). This is because<br />

ord<strong>in</strong>ary social realities are strewn with difficulties. They<br />

are full of impediments, failures, adversities, setbacks,<br />

frustrations, and <strong>in</strong>equities. People must have a robust<br />

sense of personal efficacy to susta<strong>in</strong> the perseverant effort<br />

needed to succeed. Self-doubts can set <strong>in</strong> quickly after<br />

some failures or reverses. The important matter is not<br />

that difficulties arouse self-doubt, which is a natural immediate<br />

reaction, but the speed of recovery of perceived<br />

self-efficacy from difficulties. Some people quickly recover<br />

their self-assurance; others lose faith <strong>in</strong> their capabilities.<br />

Because the acquisition of knowledge and competencies<br />

usually requires susta<strong>in</strong>ed effort <strong>in</strong> the face of difficulties<br />

and setbacks, it is resiliency of self-belief that counts.<br />

In his reveal<strong>in</strong>g book, titled Rejection, John White<br />

(1982) provides vivid testimony that the strik<strong>in</strong>g characteristic<br />

of people who have achieved em<strong>in</strong>ence <strong>in</strong> their<br />

fields is an <strong>in</strong>ext<strong>in</strong>guishable sense of efficacy and a firm<br />

belief <strong>in</strong> the worth of what they are do<strong>in</strong>g. This resilient<br />

self-belief system enabled them to override repeated early<br />

rejections of their work. A robust sense of personal efficacy<br />

provides the needed stay<strong>in</strong>g power.<br />

Many of our literary classics brought their authors<br />

repeated rejections. The novelist, Saroyan, accumulated<br />

several thousand rejections before he had his first literary<br />

piece published. Gertrude Ste<strong>in</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>ued to submit<br />

poems to editors for about 20 years before one was f<strong>in</strong>ally<br />

accepted. Now that is <strong>in</strong>v<strong>in</strong>cible self-efficacy. Such extraord<strong>in</strong>ary<br />

persistence <strong>in</strong> the face of massive un<strong>in</strong>terrupted<br />

rejection defies explanation <strong>in</strong> terms of either re<strong>in</strong>forcement<br />

theory or utility theory. James Joyce's book,<br />

the Dubl<strong>in</strong>ers, was rejected by 22 publishers. Over a dozen<br />

publishers rejected a manuscript by e. e. cumm<strong>in</strong>gs. When<br />

his mother f<strong>in</strong>ally published it, the dedication, pr<strong>in</strong>ted <strong>in</strong><br />

upper case, read: "With no thanks to..." followed by<br />

the long list of publishers who had rejected his offer<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Early rejection is the rule, rather than the exception,<br />

<strong>in</strong> other creative endeavors. The Impressionists had to<br />

arrange their own art exhibitions because their works were<br />

rout<strong>in</strong>ely rejected by the Paris Salon. Van Gogh sold only<br />

one pa<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g dur<strong>in</strong>g his lifetime. Rod<strong>in</strong> was repeatedly<br />

1176 September 1989 • <strong>American</strong> Psychologist

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!