12.01.2015 Views

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

494 CONTENT ANALYSIS AND GROUNDED THEORY<br />

and in which the major modifications reduce as<br />

underlying uniformities and properties are discovered<br />

and in which theoretical saturation takes<br />

place. The final stage – of writing theory – occurs<br />

when the researcher has gathered and generated<br />

coded data, memos, and a theory, and this is then<br />

written in full.<br />

By going through the previous sections of data,<br />

particularly the search for confirming, negative<br />

and discrepant cases, the researcher is able to keep<br />

a‘runningtotal’ofthesecasesforaparticular<br />

theory. The researcher also generates alternative<br />

theories for the phenomena under investigation<br />

and performs the same count of confirming,<br />

negative and discrepant cases. Lincoln and Guba<br />

(1985: 253) argue that the theory with the greatest<br />

incidence of confirming cases and the lowest<br />

incidence of negative and discrepant cases is the<br />

most robust.<br />

Constant comparison, LeCompte and Preissle<br />

(1993: 256) suggest, combines the elements of<br />

inductive category coding (discussed above) with<br />

simultaneously comparing these with the other<br />

events and social incidents that have been<br />

observed and coded over time and location.<br />

This enables social phenomena to be compared<br />

across categories, where necessary giving rise<br />

to new dimensions, codes and categories. Glaser<br />

(1978) indicates that constant comparison can<br />

proceed from the moment of starting to collect<br />

data, to seeking key issues and categories, to<br />

discovering recurrent events or activities in<br />

the data that become categories of focus, to<br />

expanding the range of categories. This process<br />

can continue during the writing-up process,<br />

which should be ongoing, so that a model or<br />

explanation of the phenomena can emerge that<br />

accounts for fundamental social processes and<br />

relationships.<br />

The core variables and saturation<br />

Through the use of constant comparison a<br />

core variable is identified: that variable which<br />

accounts for most of the data and to which<br />

as much as possible is related; that variable<br />

around which most data are focused. As Flick<br />

et al.(2004:19)suggest:‘thesuccessiveintegration<br />

of concepts leads to one or more key categories<br />

and thereby to the core of the emerging<br />

theory’.<br />

Saturation is reached when no new insights,<br />

codes or categories are produced even when<br />

new data are added, and when all of the data<br />

are accounted for in the core categories and<br />

subcategories (Glaser and Strauss 1967: 61).<br />

As Ezzy (2002: 93) remarks: ‘saturation is achieved<br />

when the coding that has already been completed<br />

adequately supports and fills out the emerging<br />

theory’. Of course one can never know for certain<br />

that the categories are saturated, as there are<br />

limits to induction, i.e. fresh data may come<br />

along that refute the existing theory. The partner<br />

of saturation is theoretical completeness, when<br />

the theory is able to explain the data fully and<br />

satisfactorily.<br />

Developing grounded theory<br />

As a consequence of theoretical sampling, coding,<br />

constant comparison, the identification of the core<br />

variable, and the saturation of data, categories<br />

and codes, the grounded theory (of whatever<br />

is being theorized) emerges from the data in<br />

an unforced manner, accounting for all of<br />

the data. How adequate the derived theory is<br />

can be evaluated against several criteria. Glaser<br />

and Strauss (1967: 237) suggest four main<br />

criteria:<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

The closeness of the fit between the theory and<br />

the data.<br />

How readily understandable the theory is by the<br />

lay persons working in the field, i.e. that it<br />

makes sense to them.<br />

The ability of the theory to be general to a<br />

‘multitude of diverse daily situations within<br />

the substantive area, not just to a specific type<br />

of situation’.<br />

The theory must allow ‘partial control over the<br />

structure and process of daily situations as they<br />

change through time’, i.e. it must ‘enable the<br />

person who uses it to have enough control<br />

in everyday situations to make its application<br />

worth trying’ (Glaser and Strauss 1967: 245).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!