RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

12.01.2015 Views

HOW DOES CONTENT ANALYSIS WORK 477 Step 2: Define the population from which units of text are to be sampled The population here refers not only to people but also, and mainly, to text – the domains of the analysis. For example, is it to be newspapers, programmes, interview transcripts, textbooks, conversations, public domain documents, examination scripts, emails, online conversations and so on involved; who was present; where the documents come from; how the material was recorded and/or edited; whether the person was willing to, able to, and did tell the truth; whether the data are accurately reported (Robson 1993: 273); whether the data are corroborated; the authenticity and credibility of the documents; the context of the generation of the document; the selection and evaluation of the evidence contained in the document. Chapter 23 Step 3: Define the sample to be included Here the rules for sampling people can apply equally well to documents. One has to decide whether to opt for a probability or non-probability sample of documents, a stratified sample (and, if so, the kind of strata to be used), random sampling, convenience sampling, domain sampling, cluster sampling, purposive, systematic, time sampling, snowball and so on (see Chapter 4). Robson (1993: 275–9) indicates the careful delineation of the sampling strategy here, for example, suchand-such a set of documents, such-and-such a time frame (e.g. of newspapers), such-and-such anumberoftelevisionprogrammesorinterviews. The key issues of sampling apply to the sampling of texts: representativeness, access, size of the sample and generalizability of the results. Krippendorp (2004: 145) indicates that there may be ‘nested recording units’, where one unit is nested within another, for example, with regard to newspapers that have been sampled it may be thus: the issues of a newspaper sampled; the articles in an issue of a newspaper sampled; the paragraphs in an article in an issue of a newspaper sampled; the propositions constituting a paragraph in an article in an issue of a newspaper sampled. (Krippendorp 2004: 145) This is the equivalent of stage sampling, discussed in Chapter 4. Step 4: Define the context of the generation of the document This will examine, for example: how the material was generated (Flick 1998: 193); who was Step 5: Define the units of analysis This can be at very many levels, for example, aword,phrase,sentence,paragraph,wholetext, people and themes. Robson (1993: 276) includes here, for newspaper analysis, the number of stories on a topic, column inches, size of headline, number of stories on a page, position of stories within a newspaper, the number and type of pictures. His suggestions indicate the careful thought that needs to go into the selection of the units of analysis. Different levels of analysis will raise different issues of reliability, and these are discussed later. It is assumed that the units of analysis will be classifiable into the same category text with the same or similar meaning in the context of the text itself (semantic validity) (Krippendorp 2004: 296), although this can be problematic (discussed later). The description of units of analysis will also include the units of measurement and enumeration. The coding unit defines the smallest element of material that can be analysed, while the contextual unit defines the largest textual unit that may appear in a single category. Krippendorp (2004: 99–101) distinguishes three kinds of units. Sampling units are those units that are included in, or excluded from, an analysis; they are units of selection. Recording/coding units are units that are contained within sampling units and are smaller than sampling units, thereby avoiding the complexity that characterises sampling units; they are units of description. Context units are ‘units of textual matter that set limits on the information to be considered in the description of recording units’; they are units that ‘delineate the scope of information that coders

478 CONTENT ANALYSIS AND GROUNDED THEORY need to consult in characterising the recording units’ (Krippendorp 2004: 101, 103). Krippendorp (2004) continues by suggesting a further five kinds of sampling units: physical (e.g. time, place, size); syntactical (words, grammar, sentences, paragraphs, chapters, series etc.); categorical (members of a category have something in common); propositional (delineating particular constructions or propositions); and thematic (putting texts into themes and combinations of categories). The issue of categories signals the next step. The criterion here is that each unit of analysis (category – conceptual, actual, classification element, cluster, issue) should be as discrete as possible while retaining fidelity to the integrity of the whole, i.e. that each unit must be a fair rather than a distorted representation of the context and other data. The creation of units of analysis can be done by ascribing codes to the data (Miles and Huberman 1984). This is akin to the process of ‘unitizing’ (Lincoln and Guba 1985: 203). Step 6: Decide the codes to be used in the analysis Codes can be at different levels of specificity and generality when defining content and concepts. There may be some codes which subsume others, thereby creating a hierarchy of subsumption – subordination and superordination – in effect creating a tree diagram of codes. Some codes are very general; others are more specific. Codes are astringent, pulling together a wealth of material into some order and structure. They keep words as words; they maintain context specificity. Codes may be descriptive and might include (Bogdan and Biklen 1992: 167–72): situation codes; perspectives held by subjects; ways of thinking about people and objects; process codes; activity codes; event codes; strategy codes; relationship and social structure codes; methods codes. However, to be faithful to the data, the codes themselves derive from the data responsively rather than being created pre-ordinately. Hence the researcher will go through the data ascribing codes to each piece of datum. A code is a word or abbreviation sufficiently close to that which it is describing for the researcher to see at a glance what it means (in this respect it is unlike a number). For example, the code ‘trust’ might refer to a person’s trustworthiness; the code ‘ power’ might refer to the status or power of the person in the group. Miles and Huberman (1984) advise that codes should be kept as discrete as possible and that coding should start earlier rather than later as late coding enfeebles the analysis, although there is a risk that early coding might influence too strongly any later codes. It is possible, they suggest, for as many as ninety codes to be held in the working memory while going through data, although clearly, there is a process of iteration and reiteration whereby some codes that are used in the early stages of coding might be modified subsequently and vice versa, necessitating the researcher to go through a data set more than once to ensure consistency, refinement, modification and exhaustiveness of coding (some codes might become redundant, others might need to be broken down into finer codes). By coding up the data the researcher is able to detect frequencies (which codes are occurring most commonly) and patterns (which codes occur together). Hammersley and Atkinson (1983: 177–8) propose that the first activity here is to read and reread the data to become thoroughly familiar with them, noting also any interesting patterns, any surprising, puzzling or unexpected features, any apparent inconsistencies or contradictions (e.g. between groups, within and between individuals and groups, between what people say and what they do). Step 7: Construct the categories for analysis Categories are the main groupings of constructs or key features of the text, showing links between units of analysis. For example, a text concerning teacher stress could have groupings such as ‘causes of teacher stress’, ‘the nature of teacher stress’, ‘ways of coping with stress’ and ‘the effects of stress’. The researcher will have to decide whether to have mutually exclusive categories (preferable

HOW DOES CONTENT ANALYSIS WORK 477<br />

Step 2: Define the population from which<br />

units of text are to be sampled<br />

The population here refers not only to<br />

people but also, and mainly, to text – the<br />

domains of the analysis. For example, is<br />

it to be newspapers, programmes, interview<br />

transcripts, textbo<strong>ok</strong>s, conversations, public<br />

domain documents, examination scripts, emails,<br />

online conversations and so on<br />

involved; who was present; where the documents<br />

come from; how the material was recorded and/or<br />

edited; whether the person was willing to, able<br />

to, and did tell the truth; whether the data are<br />

accurately reported (Robson 1993: 273); whether<br />

the data are corroborated; the authenticity and<br />

credibility of the documents; the context of the<br />

generation of the document; the selection and<br />

evaluation of the evidence contained in the<br />

document.<br />

Chapter 23<br />

Step 3: Define the sample to be included<br />

Here the rules for sampling people can apply<br />

equally well to documents. One has to decide<br />

whether to opt for a probability or non-probability<br />

sample of documents, a stratified sample (and, if<br />

so, the kind of strata to be used), random sampling,<br />

convenience sampling, domain sampling, cluster<br />

sampling, purposive, systematic, time sampling,<br />

snowball and so on (see Chapter 4). Robson<br />

(1993: 275–9) indicates the careful delineation<br />

of the sampling strategy here, for example, suchand-such<br />

a set of documents, such-and-such a<br />

time frame (e.g. of newspapers), such-and-such<br />

anumberoftelevisionprogrammesorinterviews.<br />

The key issues of sampling apply to the sampling of<br />

texts: representativeness, access, size of the sample<br />

and generalizability of the results.<br />

Krippendorp (2004: 145) indicates that there<br />

may be ‘nested recording units’, where one unit is<br />

nested within another, for example, with regard to<br />

newspapers that have been sampled it may be thus:<br />

the issues of a newspaper sampled; the articles in<br />

an issue of a newspaper sampled; the paragraphs in<br />

an article in an issue of a newspaper sampled; the<br />

propositions constituting a paragraph in an article in<br />

an issue of a newspaper sampled.<br />

(Krippendorp 2004: 145)<br />

This is the equivalent of stage sampling, discussed<br />

in Chapter 4.<br />

Step 4: Define the context of the<br />

generation of the document<br />

This will examine, for example: how the material<br />

was generated (Flick 1998: 193); who was<br />

Step 5: Define the units of analysis<br />

This can be at very many levels, for example,<br />

aword,phrase,sentence,paragraph,wholetext,<br />

people and themes. Robson (1993: 276) includes<br />

here, for newspaper analysis, the number of stories<br />

on a topic, column inches, size of headline, number<br />

of stories on a page, position of stories within a<br />

newspaper, the number and type of pictures. His<br />

suggestions indicate the careful thought that needs<br />

to go into the selection of the units of analysis.<br />

Different levels of analysis will raise different<br />

issues of reliability, and these are discussed later.<br />

It is assumed that the units of analysis will be<br />

classifiable into the same category text with the<br />

same or similar meaning in the context of the text<br />

itself (semantic validity) (Krippendorp 2004: 296),<br />

although this can be problematic (discussed later).<br />

The description of units of analysis will also include<br />

the units of measurement and enumeration.<br />

The coding unit defines the smallest element of<br />

material that can be analysed, while the contextual<br />

unit defines the largest textual unit that may appear<br />

in a single category.<br />

Krippendorp (2004: 99–101) distinguishes<br />

three kinds of units. Sampling units are those units<br />

that are included in, or excluded from, an analysis;<br />

they are units of selection. Recording/coding units<br />

are units that are contained within sampling<br />

units and are smaller than sampling units,<br />

thereby avoiding the complexity that characterises<br />

sampling units; they are units of description.<br />

Context units are ‘units of textual matter that set<br />

limits on the information to be considered in the<br />

description of recording units’; they are units that<br />

‘delineate the scope of information that coders

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!