12.01.2015 Views

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

426 TESTS<br />

0.85), negative correlations between items 9 and<br />

10 and all the other items (e.g. −0.79), and a very<br />

low intercorrelation between items 6 and 8 and all<br />

the others (e.g. 0.26). Item-to-total correlations<br />

(one kind of item analysis in which the item in<br />

question is correlated with the sum of the other<br />

items) vary here. What do you do You can retain<br />

items 1–5. For items 9 and 10 you can reverse the<br />

scoring (as these items lo<strong>ok</strong>ed at positive rather<br />

than negative aspects), and for items 6 and 8 you<br />

can consider excluding them from the test, as they<br />

appear to be measuring something else. Such item<br />

analysis is designed to include items that measure<br />

the same construct and to exclude items that do<br />

not. We refer readers to Howitt and Cramer (2005:<br />

Ch. 12) for further discussion of this.<br />

An alternative approach to deciding which<br />

items to retain or exclude from the list of ten items<br />

above is to use factor analysis (see Chapter 25), a<br />

method facilitated greatly by SPSS. Factor analysis<br />

will group together a cluster of similar items<br />

and keep that cluster separate from clusters of<br />

other items. So, for our example above, the factor<br />

analysis could have found, by way of illustration,<br />

three factors:<br />

positive feelings (items 9 and 10)<br />

negative psychological states (items 2, 3, 4,<br />

5, 7)<br />

physical, behavioural changes (items 1, 6, 8).<br />

By lo<strong>ok</strong>ing at the factor loadings (see Chapter 25)<br />

the researcher would have to decide which<br />

were the most appropriate factors to retain, and,<br />

thereby, which items to include and exclude. As<br />

a general rule, items with low factor loadings<br />

(e.g. ≤ 0.3) should be considered for exclusion, as<br />

they do not contribute sufficiently to the factor.<br />

Factor analysis will indicate, also, whether the<br />

construct is unidimensional or multidimensional<br />

(if there is only one factor it is probably<br />

unidimensional).<br />

Consider the form of the test<br />

Much of the discussion in this chapter<br />

assumes that the test is of the pen-andpaper<br />

variety. Clearly this need not be the<br />

case; for example, tests can be written, oral,<br />

practical, interactive, computer-based, dramatic,<br />

diagrammatic, pictorial, photographic, involve the<br />

use of audio and video material, presentational and<br />

role-play, simulations. Oral tests, for example, can<br />

be conducted if the researcher feels that reading<br />

and writing will obstruct the true purpose of<br />

the test (i.e. it becomes a reading and writing<br />

test rather than, say, a test of mathematics).<br />

This does not negate the issues discussed in this<br />

chapter, for the form of the test will still need<br />

to consider, for example, reliability and validity,<br />

difficulty, discriminability, marking and grading,<br />

item analysis, timing. Indeed several of these<br />

factors take on an added significance in nonwritten<br />

forms of testing; for example, reliability is a<br />

major issue in judging live musical performance or<br />

the performance of a gymnastics routine – where a<br />

‘one-off’ event is likely. Furthermore, reliability<br />

and validity are significant issues in group<br />

performance or group exercises – where group<br />

dynamics may prevent a testee’s true abilities<br />

from being demonstrated. Clearly the researcher<br />

will need to consider whether the test will be<br />

undertaken individually, or in a group, and what<br />

form it will take.<br />

Write the test item<br />

The test will need to address the intended and<br />

unintended clues and cues that might be provided<br />

in it, for example (Morris et al.1987):<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

The number of blanks might indicate the<br />

number of words required.<br />

The number of dots might indicate the number<br />

of letters required.<br />

The length of blanks might indicate the length<br />

of response required.<br />

The space left for completion will give cues<br />

about how much to write.<br />

Blanks in different parts of a sentence will<br />

be assisted by the reader having read the<br />

other parts of the sentence (anaphoric and<br />

cataphoric reading cues).<br />

Hanna (1993: 139–41) and Cunningham<br />

(1998) provide several guidelines for constructing

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!