12.01.2015 Views

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

NATURALISTIC AND PARTICIPANT OBSERVATION 405<br />

observations, comments, behaviour, events and<br />

activities and the views of all participants in a situation.<br />

Participant observation is often combined<br />

with other forms of data collection that, together,<br />

elicit the participants’ definitions of the situation<br />

and their organizing constructs in accounting for<br />

situations and behaviour. By staying in a situation<br />

over a long period the researcher is also<br />

able to see how events evolve over time, catching<br />

the dynamics of situations, the people, personalities,<br />

contexts, resources, roles etc. Morrison (1993:<br />

88) argues that by ‘being immersed in a particular<br />

context over time not only will the salient<br />

features of the situation emerge and present themselves<br />

but a more holistic view will be gathered of<br />

the interrelationships of factors’. Such immersion<br />

facilitates the generation of ‘thick descriptions’,<br />

particularly of social processes and interaction,<br />

which lend themselves to accurate explanation<br />

and interpretation of events rather than relying<br />

on the researcher’s own inferences. The data derived<br />

from participant observation are ‘strong on<br />

reality’.<br />

Components of ‘thick descriptions’ involve<br />

recording (Carspecken 1996: 47), for example:<br />

speech acts; non-verbal communication; descriptions<br />

in low-inference vocabulary; careful and<br />

frequent recording of the time and timing of<br />

events; the observer’s comments that are placed<br />

into categories; detailed contextual data.<br />

Observations are recorded in field notes; these<br />

can be written at several levels (see http://<br />

www.routledge.com/textbo<strong>ok</strong>s/9780415368780 –<br />

Chapter 18, file 18.8.ppt). At the level of description<br />

(Spradley 1980; Bogdan and Biklen 1992:<br />

120–1; LeCompte and Preissle 1993: 224), observations<br />

can take the following forms:<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

quick, fragmentary jottings of key words/<br />

symbols<br />

transcriptions and more detailed observations<br />

written out fully<br />

descriptions that, when assembled and written<br />

out, form a comprehensive and comprehensible<br />

account of what has happened<br />

pen portraits of participants<br />

reconstructions of conversations<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

descriptions of the physical settings of events<br />

descriptions of events, behaviour and activities<br />

adescriptionoftheresearcher’sactivitiesand<br />

behaviour.<br />

Lincoln and Guba (1985: 273) suggest a variety<br />

of elements or types of observations that include:<br />

ongoing notes, either verbatim or categorized<br />

in situ<br />

logs or diaries of field experiences (similar to<br />

field notes though usually written some time<br />

after the observations have been made)<br />

notes that are made on specific, predetermined<br />

themes (e.g. that have arisen from grounded<br />

theory)<br />

‘chronologs’, where each separate behavioural<br />

episode is noted, together with the time at<br />

which it occurred, or recording an observation<br />

at regular time intervals, e.g. every two or three<br />

minutes<br />

context maps: maps, sketches, diagrams or<br />

some graphic display of the context (usually<br />

physical) within which the observation takes<br />

place, such graphics enabling movements to be<br />

charted<br />

entries on predetermined schedules (including<br />

rating scales, checklists and structured observation<br />

charts), using taxonomic or categoric<br />

systems, where the categories derive from previous<br />

observational or interview data<br />

sociometric diagrams indicating social relationships,<br />

e.g. isolates (whom nobody chooses),<br />

stars (whom everyone chooses) and dyads (who<br />

choose each other)<br />

debriefing questionnaires from respondents<br />

that are devised for, and by, the observer only,<br />

to be used for reminding the observer of main<br />

types of information and events once she or he<br />

has left the scene<br />

data from debriefing sessions with other<br />

researchers, again as an aide-memoire.<br />

LeCompte and Preissle (1993: 199–200)<br />

provide a useful set of guidelines for directing<br />

observations of specific activities, events or scenes,<br />

suggesting that they should include answers to the<br />

following questions:<br />

Chapter 18

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!