12.01.2015 Views

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

332 QUESTIONNAIRES<br />

Box 15.4<br />

Potential problems in conducting research<br />

1 Gaining access to schools and teachers<br />

2 Gaining permission to conduct the research (e.g. from principals)<br />

3 Resentment by principals<br />

4 People vetting what could be used<br />

5 Finding enough willing participants for your sample<br />

6 Schools suffering from ‘too much research’ by outsiders and insiders<br />

7 Schools or people not wishing to divulge information about themselves<br />

8 Schools not wishing to be identifiable, even with protections guaranteed<br />

9 Local political factors that impinge on the school<br />

10 Teachers’ fear of being identified/traceable, even with protections guaranteed<br />

11 Fear of participation by teachers (e.g. if they say critical matters about the<br />

school or others they could lose their contracts)<br />

12 Unwillingness of teachers to be involved because of their workload<br />

13 The principal deciding on whether to involve the staff, without consultation<br />

with the staff<br />

14 Schools’ or institutions’ fear of criticism or loss of face<br />

15 The sensitivity of the research: the issues being investigated<br />

Size of the<br />

problem<br />

(1–5)<br />

How much<br />

the problem<br />

was solved<br />

(1–5)<br />

negatively and positively worded items in the same<br />

scale, they argue, compromises both validity and<br />

reliability. Indeed they suggest that respondents<br />

may not read negatively worded items as carefully<br />

as positively worded items.<br />

Contingency questions, filters and<br />

branches<br />

Contingency questions depend on responses to<br />

earlier questions, for example: ‘if your answer to<br />

question (1) was ‘‘yes’’ please go to question (4)’.<br />

The earlier question acts as a filter for the later<br />

question, and the later question is contingent<br />

on the earlier, and is a branch of the earlier<br />

question. Some questionnaires will write in words<br />

the number of the question to which to go (e.g.<br />

‘please go to question 6’); others will place an arrow<br />

to indicate the next question to be answered if your<br />

answer to the first question was such-and-such.<br />

Contingency and filter questions may be useful<br />

for the researcher, but they can be confusing for<br />

the respondent as it is not always clear how<br />

to proceed through the sequence of questions<br />

and where to go once a particular branch has<br />

been completed. Redline et al. (2002) found<br />

that respondents tend to ignore, misread and<br />

incorrectly follow branching instructions, such<br />

that item non-response occurs for follow-up<br />

questions that are applicable only to certain<br />

subsamples, and respondents skip over, and<br />

therefore fail to follow-up on those questions<br />

that they should have completed. Redline et al.<br />

(2002) found that the increased complexity of<br />

the questionnaire brought about by branching<br />

instructions negatively influenced its correct<br />

completion.<br />

Redline et al. (2002:7)reportthatthenumber<br />

of words in the question affected the respondents’<br />

ability to follow branching instructions – the<br />

greater the number of words in the question,<br />

the greater was the likelihood of the respondents<br />

overlo<strong>ok</strong>ing the branching instructions. Redline<br />

et al. (2002:19)reportthatuptosevenitems,

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!