12.01.2015 Views

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

TRIANGULATION 141<br />

student’s problem-solving ability. The researcher<br />

might observe the student working on a problem,<br />

or might talk to the student about how she is<br />

tackling the problem, or might ask the student<br />

to write down how she tackled the problem.<br />

Here the researcher has three different datacollecting<br />

instruments – observation, interview<br />

and documentation respectively. If the results<br />

all agreed – concurred – that, according to given<br />

criteria for problem-solving ability, the student<br />

demonstrated a good ability to solve a problem,<br />

then the researcher would be able to say with<br />

greater confidence (validity) that the student was<br />

good at problem-solving than if the researcher had<br />

arrived at that judgement simply from using one<br />

instrument.<br />

Concurrent validity is very similar to its<br />

partner – predictive validity – in its core concept<br />

(i.e. agreement with a second measure); what<br />

differentiates concurrent and predictive validity<br />

is the absence of a time element in the former;<br />

concurrence can be demonstrated simultaneously<br />

with another instrument.<br />

An important partner to concurrent validity,<br />

which is also a bridge into later discussions of<br />

reliability, is triangulation.<br />

Triangulation<br />

Triangulation may be defined as the use of two<br />

or more methods of data collection in the study<br />

of some aspect of human behaviour. The use of<br />

multiple methods, or the multi-method approach<br />

as it is sometimes called, contrasts with the<br />

ubiquitous but generally more vulnerable singlemethod<br />

approach that characterizes so much of<br />

research in the social sciences. In its original<br />

and literal sense, triangulation is a technique<br />

of physical measurement: maritime navigators,<br />

military strategists and surveyors, for example,<br />

use (or used to use) several locational markers<br />

in their endeavours to pinpoint a single spot<br />

or objective. By analogy, triangular techniques<br />

in the social sciences attempt to map out, or<br />

explain more fully, the richness and complexity<br />

of human behaviour by studying it from more<br />

than one standpoint and, in so doing, by making<br />

use of both quantitative and qualitative data.<br />

Triangulation is a powerful way of demonstrating<br />

concurrent validity, particularly in qualitative<br />

research (Campbell and Fiske 1959).<br />

The advantages of the multi-method approach<br />

in social research are manifold and we examine<br />

two of them. First, whereas the single observation<br />

in fields such as medicine, chemistry and<br />

physics normally yields sufficient and unambiguous<br />

information on selected phenomena, it provides<br />

only a limited view of the complexity of human<br />

behaviour and of situations in which human<br />

beings interact. It has been observed that as<br />

research methods act as filters through which<br />

the environment is selectively experienced, they<br />

are never atheoretical or neutral in representing<br />

the world of experience (Smith 1975). Exclusive<br />

reliance on one method, therefore, may bias or<br />

distort the researcher’s picture of the particular<br />

slice of reality being investigated. The researcher<br />

needs to be confident that the data generated<br />

are not simply artefacts of one specific method<br />

of collection (Lin 1976). Such confidence can<br />

be achieved, as far as nomothetic research<br />

is concerned, when different methods of data<br />

collection yield substantially the same results.<br />

(Where triangulation is used in interpretive<br />

research to investigate different actors’ viewpoints,<br />

the same method, e.g. accounts, will naturally<br />

produce different sets of data.)<br />

Further, the more the methods contrast with<br />

each other, the greater the researcher’s confidence.<br />

If, for example, the outcomes of a questionnaire<br />

survey correspond to those of an observational<br />

study of the same phenomena, the more the<br />

researcher will be confident about the findings.<br />

Or, more extreme, where the results of a rigorous<br />

experimental investigation are replicated in,<br />

say, a role-playing exercise, the researcher will<br />

experience even greater assurance. If findings are<br />

artefacts of method, then the use of contrasting<br />

methods considerably reduces the chances of<br />

any consistent findings being attributable to<br />

similarities of method (Lin 1976).<br />

We come now to a second advantage: some<br />

theorists have been sharply critical of the limited<br />

use to which existing methods of inquiry in the<br />

Chapter 6

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!