27.12.2014 Views

Chapter 8 - Understanding Individual Differences in Adaptation in ...

Chapter 8 - Understanding Individual Differences in Adaptation in ...

Chapter 8 - Understanding Individual Differences in Adaptation in ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Provided for non-commercial research and educational use only.<br />

Not for reproduction, distribution or commercial use.<br />

This chapter was orig<strong>in</strong>ally published <strong>in</strong> the book International Review of Research <strong>in</strong><br />

Mental Retardation, Vol. 36, published by Elsevier, and the attached copy is provided<br />

by Elsevier for the author's benefit and for the benefit of the author's <strong>in</strong>stitution, for<br />

non-commercial research and educational use <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g without limitation use <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>struction at your <strong>in</strong>stitution, send<strong>in</strong>g it to specific colleagues who know you, and<br />

provid<strong>in</strong>g a copy to your <strong>in</strong>stitution’s adm<strong>in</strong>istrator.<br />

All other uses, reproduction and distribution, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g without limitation commercial<br />

repr<strong>in</strong>ts, sell<strong>in</strong>g or licens<strong>in</strong>g copies or access, or post<strong>in</strong>g on open <strong>in</strong>ternet sites, your<br />

personal or <strong>in</strong>stitution’s website or repository, are prohibited. For exceptions,<br />

permission may be sought for such use through Elsevier's permissions site at:<br />

http://www.elsevier.com/locate/permissionusematerial<br />

From: Mal<strong>in</strong> B. Olsson, <strong>Understand<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>Individual</strong> <strong>Differences</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Adaptation</strong> <strong>in</strong><br />

Parents of Children with Intellectual Disabilities: A Risk and Resilience Perspective.<br />

In Lara<strong>in</strong>e Masters Glidden, editor: International Review of Research <strong>in</strong> Mental<br />

Retardation, Vol. 36, Burl<strong>in</strong>gton: Academic Press, 2008, pp. 281-315.<br />

ISBN: 978-0-12-374476-0<br />

© Copyright 2008 Elsevier Inc.<br />

Academic Press.


Author's personal copy<br />

CHAPTER<br />

EIGHT<br />

<strong>Understand<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>Individual</strong> <strong>Differences</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong> <strong>Adaptation</strong> <strong>in</strong> Parents of Children<br />

with Intellectual Disabilities: A Risk<br />

and Resilience Perspective<br />

Mal<strong>in</strong> B. Olsson*<br />

Contents<br />

1. Introduction 282<br />

2. Risk and Protective Factors <strong>in</strong> the Field of Families with<br />

a Child with ID 285<br />

2.1. Critical evaluation of Wallander and Varni’s risk<br />

and resistance model 286<br />

2.2. Three broad areas of risk and protective factors <strong>in</strong> families<br />

of children with ID 286<br />

3. Risk Factors 287<br />

3.1. Child and disability risk factors 287<br />

3.2. Socio-ecological risk factors 290<br />

3.3. Intrapersonal risk factors 292<br />

3.4. Summary: Risk factors 292<br />

4. Protective Factors 293<br />

4.1. Child and disability protective factors 293<br />

4.2. Socio-ecological protective factors 294<br />

4.3. Intrapersonal protective factors 298<br />

4.4. Summary: Protective factors 300<br />

5. Outcomes 301<br />

5.1. Depression <strong>in</strong> parents of children with ID 301<br />

5.2. Family quality of life 302<br />

6. Discussion 303<br />

6.1. Design improvements 304<br />

6.2. Why study resilience 306<br />

7. Conclusion 307<br />

References 307<br />

* Department of Psychology, Göteborg University, Göteborg, Sweden<br />

International Review of Research <strong>in</strong> Mental Retardation, Volume 36<br />

ISSN 0074-7750, DOI: 10.1016/S0074-7750(08)00008-6<br />

# 2008 Elsevier Inc.<br />

All rights reserved.<br />

281


Author's personal copy<br />

282 Mal<strong>in</strong> B. Olsson<br />

Abstract<br />

In this chapter, I focus on the theoretical perspective of risk and resilience as a<br />

framework for study<strong>in</strong>g the processes lead<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>in</strong>dividual variation <strong>in</strong> adaptation<br />

of parents with children with <strong>in</strong>tellectual disabilities (ID). Relevant literature<br />

related to parental adaptation to a child with ID is reviewed, organized, and<br />

discussed. The challenge of conceptualiz<strong>in</strong>g risk and protective factors as well<br />

as resilience is exam<strong>in</strong>ed, and designs to improve future research are suggested.<br />

I argue that to understand the processes lead<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>in</strong>dividual variation<br />

<strong>in</strong> well-be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> parents of children with ID, we need to do more with<strong>in</strong>-group<br />

analyses and address the complex <strong>in</strong>terplay of cumulative risk and protective<br />

factors.<br />

1. Introduction<br />

Two different views of parents of children with an <strong>in</strong>tellectual disability<br />

are extant <strong>in</strong> research and <strong>in</strong> society at large. These parents are either<br />

pitied or seen as exceptionally strong and competent. In contrast, Mary Jo<br />

Herbert, a mother of a child with disabilities, expressed the importance of a<br />

balanced view of parents of children with disabilities <strong>in</strong> her essay <strong>in</strong> the<br />

American Association on Mental Retardation News & Notes <strong>in</strong> 2001.<br />

She wrote:<br />

Parents of children with disabilities have a serious image problem ... The<br />

image that I am referr<strong>in</strong>g to is that of the Exceptional Parent. But the idea<br />

that parents of children who are developmentally disabled are exceptional is<br />

a myth. The hard truth is that parents of kids with disabilities br<strong>in</strong>g no<br />

<strong>in</strong>herent exceptional qualities to their role. What they br<strong>in</strong>g is the same<br />

amount of patience, <strong>in</strong>tuition, energy, and enthusiasm as any other parent<br />

gets...If there is anyth<strong>in</strong>g exceptional about our lives it is the exceptional<br />

conditions under which we live them. We are ord<strong>in</strong>ary parents try<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

live ord<strong>in</strong>ary lives under extraord<strong>in</strong>ary circumstances...(pp. 7–8).<br />

The focus of this chapter is on understand<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>dividual differences<br />

<strong>in</strong> adaptation to these extraord<strong>in</strong>ary circumstances. That is, what happens<br />

when parents with their <strong>in</strong>dividually different lives and experiences meet the<br />

extraord<strong>in</strong>ary circumstances <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> car<strong>in</strong>g for a child with <strong>in</strong>tellectual<br />

disabilities (ID)<br />

Parents of children with disabilities often raise their children with<strong>in</strong> the<br />

context of a powerful societal discourse that devalues disabilities and they<br />

are therefore expected to feel emotionally burdened (Green, 2007). Parents<br />

with positive attitudes toward rais<strong>in</strong>g a child with a disability are often<br />

pathologized as be<strong>in</strong>g unrealistic, <strong>in</strong> denial, and fail<strong>in</strong>g to accept their<br />

‘‘tragic’’ circumstances s<strong>in</strong>ce professionals often view acceptance of the


Author's personal copy<br />

Risk and Resilience 283<br />

child’s disability as the first step <strong>in</strong> a heal<strong>in</strong>g process (Green). But simple<br />

terms such as acceptance and denial do not capture parents’ experiences of<br />

rais<strong>in</strong>g a child with disabilities (Larson, 1998). Be<strong>in</strong>g a parent of a child with<br />

a disability does not only <strong>in</strong>clude a loss, as there is also the joy of hav<strong>in</strong>g<br />

become a parent. Parents may be able to f<strong>in</strong>d benefits <strong>in</strong> hav<strong>in</strong>g a child with<br />

a disability, and family, friends, and professionals who discourage them from<br />

do<strong>in</strong>g so may be imped<strong>in</strong>g the development of the positive aspects of car<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Larson argues that acceptance as borrowed from the conceptualization of<br />

the stages of griev<strong>in</strong>g is not accurate <strong>in</strong> this <strong>in</strong>stance, where <strong>in</strong>stead of a<br />

certa<strong>in</strong> future, parents face an <strong>in</strong>determ<strong>in</strong>ate future for their child with a<br />

disability. She found that when mothers were asked what would improve<br />

their well-be<strong>in</strong>g, all of them wished for miraculous cures—that the child<br />

would walk and talk, that the child would be normal. Yet at the same time,<br />

they expressed a deep love and affection for their children just as they were.<br />

While embrac<strong>in</strong>g their child despite the disability, mothers simultaneously<br />

rejected the limitations of the disability, cont<strong>in</strong>u<strong>in</strong>g to aspire to more typical<br />

mother<strong>in</strong>g. There is no doubt that complex emotions and experiences are<br />

set <strong>in</strong> motion when a child with disabilities is born, but parents do not<br />

describe their child, per se, as the reason for <strong>in</strong>creased stress or depression.<br />

Rather, it is societal expectations and the exceptional life situation that can<br />

lead to these experiences.<br />

Much research has focused on negative outcomes such as parental stress<br />

and depression <strong>in</strong> parents (mostly mothers) of children with disabilities. In a<br />

meta-analysis, S<strong>in</strong>ger (2006) found consistent evidence of a 10% <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong><br />

the prevalence of depression <strong>in</strong> mothers of children with ID compared to<br />

control mothers. The greater risk of depression has traditionally been<br />

attributed to the emotional burdens of hav<strong>in</strong>g a child with ID, rather than<br />

on the burdens imposed by negative public attitudes toward disability and<br />

<strong>in</strong>adequate support for the expensive and time-consum<strong>in</strong>g task of car<strong>in</strong>g for<br />

a child with special needs (Green, 2007). Even though there is an <strong>in</strong>creased<br />

risk for a lower level of well-be<strong>in</strong>g, most parents adapt well to the challenge<br />

of rais<strong>in</strong>g a child with ID (Emerson, Hatton, Llewllyn, & Graham, 2006;<br />

Olsson & Hwang, <strong>in</strong> press; Hassall & Rose, 2005). A child with ID does not<br />

<strong>in</strong>evitably cause stress and depression <strong>in</strong> parents; the <strong>in</strong>dividual variation <strong>in</strong><br />

parental well-be<strong>in</strong>g is great and depends upon a number of factors. Rather<br />

than focus<strong>in</strong>g on the depressed or the competent parent it is important to<br />

learn more about the processes lead<strong>in</strong>g to different outcomes.<br />

Several researchers <strong>in</strong> the field of parent<strong>in</strong>g a child with disabilities have<br />

asked for a greater focus on the positive aspects of car<strong>in</strong>g for a child with ID<br />

(Hassall & Rose, 2005; Ylvén, Bjröck-Åkesson, & Grandlund, 2006).<br />

Positive psychology is a fairly new trend <strong>in</strong> psychology (Gillham, 2000;<br />

Snyder & Lopez, 2002) and has <strong>in</strong>fluenced recent work <strong>in</strong> the study<br />

of parents of children with ID (Green, 2007; Scorgie & Sobsey, 2000;


Author's personal copy<br />

284 Mal<strong>in</strong> B. Olsson<br />

Sta<strong>in</strong>ton & Besser, 1998; Ylvén et al., 2006). A greater focus on positive<br />

aspects of car<strong>in</strong>g for a child with ID is, of course, welcome, but the positive<br />

and negative aspects and outcomes of car<strong>in</strong>g for a child with ID should<br />

preferably be addressed simultaneously. Hav<strong>in</strong>g a child with a disability<br />

<strong>in</strong>volves both joy and challenges, both rewards and stress [as <strong>in</strong> any other<br />

parenthood (Deater-Deckard, & Scarr, 1996)] and no benefit can be ga<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

from neglect<strong>in</strong>g either the positive or the negative impact that the parent<strong>in</strong>g<br />

experience can have on the well-be<strong>in</strong>g of parents. <strong>Individual</strong> variation <strong>in</strong><br />

the experience of positive and negative aspects is evident as well as <strong>in</strong>dividual<br />

variation <strong>in</strong> outcome (e.g., well-be<strong>in</strong>g), but we know very little about<br />

the processes lead<strong>in</strong>g to these differences. It is clear that some people extract<br />

positive psychological growth from difficult life experiences, not as a result<br />

of magic but as a result of determ<strong>in</strong>ation to cope with stress and adversity as<br />

well as possible (Lazarus, 2003). Why psychological growth and good<br />

adaptation takes place <strong>in</strong> some parents of children with ID while others<br />

are more troubled is a complex question.<br />

A relevant theoretical construct to understand <strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong><br />

reaction to stressful life conditions is resilience. Resilience means that some<br />

<strong>in</strong>dividuals have a relatively good psychological outcome despite suffer<strong>in</strong>g<br />

risk experiences that might be expected to br<strong>in</strong>g about poorer outcomes.<br />

Resilience is an <strong>in</strong>teractive concept that refers to a relative resistance to<br />

environmental risk experiences, or the overcom<strong>in</strong>g of stress or adversity.<br />

It differs from traditional concepts of risk and protection <strong>in</strong> its focus on<br />

<strong>in</strong>dividual variation <strong>in</strong> response to comparable experiences (Rutter, 2006).<br />

Risk and protection both start with a focus on variables, with the implicit<br />

assumption that the impact of risk and protective factors will be comparable<br />

<strong>in</strong> everyone, and that outcomes will depend on the mix and balance<br />

between risk and protective <strong>in</strong>fluences. Resilience, by contrast, starts with<br />

the recognition of the <strong>in</strong>dividual variation <strong>in</strong> people’s response to the same<br />

experiences. The two perspectives do, however, go together s<strong>in</strong>ce there is<br />

evidence that much of the variation <strong>in</strong> psychological outcomes can be<br />

accounted for by the summative effects of risk and protective factors.<br />

Resilience can only be studied if there is a thorough measurement of risk<br />

and protective factors.<br />

The aim of the present chapter is to discuss and organize results of a<br />

review of empirical research on parental adaptation to a child with ID<br />

with<strong>in</strong> the theoretical framework of resilience and risk and protective factors.<br />

With <strong>in</strong>creased knowledge about risk and protective factors <strong>in</strong> parents of<br />

children with ID, we can do more to m<strong>in</strong>imize the risks and to strengthen<br />

the protective factors and thereby enhance the life situation of parents of<br />

children with ID and <strong>in</strong> the longer run even improve the well-be<strong>in</strong>g of the<br />

children themselves, whom we know are at markedly <strong>in</strong>creased risk for<br />

poor psychological health (Emerson, 2003).


Author's personal copy<br />

Risk and Resilience 285<br />

2. Risk and Protective Factors <strong>in</strong> the Field<br />

of Families with a Child with ID<br />

Several theoretical models have been used <strong>in</strong> research to facilitate the<br />

understand<strong>in</strong>g of how different factors contribute to parental adaptation <strong>in</strong><br />

families with children with ID. However, the concept of resilience is<br />

seldom used. There is agreement that a child with ID results <strong>in</strong> heightened<br />

stress <strong>in</strong> parents (Beckman, 1991; Cameron, Dodson, & Day,1991; Dumas,<br />

Wolf, Fisman, & Culligan, 1991; Dyson, 1991, 1993; Innocenti, Huh,<br />

& Boyce, 1992; Most, Fidler, Laforce-Booth, & Kelly, 2006; Plant &<br />

Sanders, 2007; Reddon, McDonald, & Kysela, 1992; S<strong>in</strong>ger, 2006), and<br />

I use this consistent f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g as a start<strong>in</strong>g po<strong>in</strong>t for view<strong>in</strong>g this group as be<strong>in</strong>g<br />

exposed to stress which can be expected to lead to an <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> adverse<br />

outcomes. I further use the consistent f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong><br />

adaptation (usually measured as depression) as a sign of resilience <strong>in</strong> many<br />

parents. Researchers have found that stress scores do not reliably predict the<br />

outcome (depression) for an <strong>in</strong>dividual parent. There are no objective<br />

stressors that affect everybody the same way, and whether a stressor will<br />

cause a stress reaction <strong>in</strong> the person exposed to it depends on the <strong>in</strong>terplay<br />

between the stressful stimulus condition and the vulnerability/resiliency of<br />

the <strong>in</strong>dividual (Lazarus, 1999).<br />

Wallander and Varni (1998) have formulated a model <strong>in</strong> which the risk<br />

and protective factors are specific to the life situation of parents of children<br />

with disease/disability. They proposed the follow<strong>in</strong>g risk factors for parental<br />

adjustment to childhood chronic illness/disability:<br />

a. Disability (they call it disease) parameters (type of diagnosis, severity, and<br />

prognosis) and care stra<strong>in</strong> (the burden associated with car<strong>in</strong>g for the child<br />

and extensive or frustrat<strong>in</strong>g contact with professionals)<br />

b. Psychosocial stress (factors directly or <strong>in</strong>directly related to the child’s<br />

disability socioeconomic stress, loss of career opportunity, daily life<br />

stressors)<br />

They also proposed the follow<strong>in</strong>g resistance factors:<br />

c. Parental stress-process<strong>in</strong>g abilities (i.e., cop<strong>in</strong>g strategies, cognitive<br />

appraisals, problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g ability)<br />

d. Intrapersonal factors (general cognitive and affective patterns of behavior,<br />

or personality, i.e., dispositional optimism, self-perception, hope)<br />

e. Socio-ecological factors (i.e., social support, good family relations, marital<br />

satisfaction, formal support, and aspects of the broader community)


Author's personal copy<br />

286 Mal<strong>in</strong> B. Olsson<br />

2.1. Critical evaluation of Wallander and Varni’s risk<br />

and resistance model<br />

I see at least four problems with Wallander and Varni’s model. The first<br />

problem concerns the appropriate placement of different factors. Wallander<br />

and Varni offer specification of risk and protective factors, but the research<br />

on families of children with ID arch over a larger area than def<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> their<br />

model. Socioeconomic situation and demographic factors are, for example,<br />

hard to place <strong>in</strong> the model. The second difficulty has to do with discrim<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g<br />

between stress-process<strong>in</strong>g abilities and <strong>in</strong>trapersonal factors, as those<br />

concepts are very closely l<strong>in</strong>ked. The third difficulty is that risk and protective<br />

factors are not easily separated, but are rather better understood as a<br />

cont<strong>in</strong>uum where high scores can mean protection and low scores risk.<br />

For example, SES may be a risk factor if the family does not have the<br />

tangible resources, but a resilience factor if the family is economically welloff,<br />

can buy good medical and support care, etc. Similarly, Wallander and<br />

Varni view <strong>in</strong>trapersonal factors as sources of resilience, but they can be risk<br />

factors as well. Personality is a good example of this, where dispositional<br />

optimism can be a protective factor, but neuroticism can mean risk. Rather<br />

than try<strong>in</strong>g to classify entire concepts such as socioeconomic status SES or<br />

personality as either risk or protective factors, it is important to study the<br />

level at which someth<strong>in</strong>g goes from be<strong>in</strong>g a risk to hav<strong>in</strong>g protective<br />

functions. The fourth problem has to do with outcome. Wallander and<br />

Varni (1998) simply call the outcome adaptation, but do not discuss or<br />

specify what that could be. An <strong>in</strong>dividual’s resilience or adaptation can be<br />

assessed with regard to many different outcomes as long as these are related<br />

to whether the <strong>in</strong>dividual functions <strong>in</strong> an adaptive or expected way despite<br />

hav<strong>in</strong>g been challenged by risk/stress.<br />

2.2. Three broad areas of risk and protective factors<br />

<strong>in</strong> families of children with ID<br />

The literature on parents of children with ID arches over many aspects and<br />

is not easy to summarize <strong>in</strong>to risk and protective factors. On the basis of the<br />

model of Wallander and Varni and my objections to it, I suggest that risk<br />

and protective factors for parental adaptation to a child with ID can be<br />

categorized <strong>in</strong> three broad areas (child and disability related issues, <strong>in</strong>trapersonal,<br />

and socio-ecological as depicted <strong>in</strong> Fig. 8.1), with further subcategories.<br />

I have organized the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs from the literature review <strong>in</strong> this<br />

chapter <strong>in</strong>to a section on studied risk factors with<strong>in</strong> each of the broad<br />

areas and a section on studied protective factors with<strong>in</strong> each of the<br />

broad areas.


Author's personal copy<br />

Risk and Resilience 287<br />

Child and<br />

disability related<br />

issues<br />

Intrapersonal<br />

factors<br />

Outcome<br />

Socioecological<br />

factors<br />

Figure 8.1 Risk and protective factors affect<strong>in</strong>g parental outcome to a child with<br />

<strong>in</strong>tellectual disabilities, categorized <strong>in</strong> three broad areas.<br />

3. Risk Factors<br />

3.1. Child and disability risk factors<br />

3.1.1. Child behaviors<br />

Children’s behavior problems <strong>in</strong>fluence parental stress levels more than<br />

children’s level of cognitive function<strong>in</strong>g or extent of physical disability<br />

(Baker et al., 2003; Blacher & McIntyre, 2006; Emerson, 2003;<br />

Eisenhower, Baker, & Blacher, 2005; Hast<strong>in</strong>gs, 2003; Herr<strong>in</strong>g et al.,<br />

2006; Lecavalier, Leone, & Wiltz, 2006; Lewis et al., 2006; Saloviita,<br />

Itäl<strong>in</strong>na, & Le<strong>in</strong>onen, 2003). Communication difficulties, impaired social<br />

ability, difficult temperament, repetitive behavior, demand<strong>in</strong>g patterns of<br />

behavior, and cl<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>g are all child characteristics that have been found to<br />

<strong>in</strong>crease parental stress (Hassall, Rose, & McDonald, 2005; Hodapp,<br />

Dykens, & Mas<strong>in</strong>o, 1997; Most et al., 2006; Plant & Sanders, 2007;<br />

Wheeler, Hatton, Reichardt, & Bailey, 2007). These are also symptoms<br />

found <strong>in</strong> most children with autism, which can expla<strong>in</strong> why some studies<br />

<strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g the impact of different diagnoses on stress levels have found<br />

that parents of children with autism experience more stress than parents of<br />

children with mental retardation without autism (Blacher & McIntyre,<br />

2006; Dale, Jahoda, & Knott, 2006; Eisenhower et al., 2005; Sanders &<br />

Morgan, 1997; Siklos & Kerns, 2007). Maladaptive child behavior and<br />

parent<strong>in</strong>g stress have been found to have mutually escalat<strong>in</strong>g effects on<br />

each other (Baker et al., 2003). Children’s behavior problems lead to stress<br />

<strong>in</strong> parents, and parents under stress adopt certa<strong>in</strong> parent<strong>in</strong>g behaviors that<br />

tend to re<strong>in</strong>force the child’s behavior problems (Hast<strong>in</strong>gs, 2003; Wheeler<br />

et al., 2007). The parent<strong>in</strong>g environment <strong>in</strong>teracts with the characteristics of<br />

the child (<strong>in</strong> this case, child problem behavior) and the child’s behaviors


Author's personal copy<br />

288 Mal<strong>in</strong> B. Olsson<br />

have critical impact on the parent<strong>in</strong>g environment. It has also been found<br />

that parent<strong>in</strong>g behaviors have a greater impact on children at developmental<br />

risk (Paczkowski & Baker, 2007), so that when mothers of children with ID<br />

are high <strong>in</strong> nonsupportive reactions their children have much higher levels<br />

of problem behavior than those <strong>in</strong> a nondelayed group (Paczkowski &<br />

Baker, 2007).<br />

3.1.2. Different diagnoses<br />

Both fathers and mothers rate their children with Down syndrome (DS), as<br />

compared to those with other types of ID, as hav<strong>in</strong>g more positive personality<br />

traits and fewer maladaptive behaviors (Ricci & Hodapp, 2003).<br />

Families of children with DS consistently appear to cope better than families<br />

with children with other diagnoses (Dykens & Hodapp, 2001) at least when<br />

the child is young (Most et al., 2006). This DS advantage, although not<br />

demonstrated <strong>in</strong> all samples (Cahill & Glidden, 1996; Glidden & Cahill<br />

1998), is usually expla<strong>in</strong>ed by DS be<strong>in</strong>g the most common chromosomal<br />

cause of mental retardation that is diagnosed at birth; it is a reasonably wellknown<br />

condition, with networks of parent-groups quite prevalent. Further,<br />

children with DS are often perceived to have easy temperaments and<br />

agreeable personalities and are sometimes reported as easier to br<strong>in</strong>g up<br />

than are children with other ID (Dykens & Hodapp, 2001). Other explanations<br />

for the DS advantage <strong>in</strong>volve older mothers be<strong>in</strong>g more mature and<br />

experienced and children with DS exhibit<strong>in</strong>g higher levels of adaptive<br />

behavior than children with other ID etiologies (Corrice & Glidden, 2008).<br />

Different diagnoses may <strong>in</strong>volve changes <strong>in</strong> the nature of the stress<br />

variable which, <strong>in</strong> turn, can <strong>in</strong>fluence the appraisal of stress. Lazarus suggests<br />

that familiarity, predictability, and clarity favor challenge, and unfamiliarity<br />

and long duration favor threat. This may help to understand why parents of<br />

children with DS are often found to experience less distress than parents<br />

of children with autism. DS is a well-known condition to most people, the<br />

cause is known, there is much <strong>in</strong>formation on prognosis, and homogenous<br />

parent support groups are available. Time of diagnosis and the diagnostic<br />

procedure also seem important for parental stress, where a lengthy period<br />

with uncerta<strong>in</strong>ty and a delayed diagnosis seem to make parents more stressed<br />

(Graungaard & Skov, 2006). Parents of children with autism have been<br />

found to experience higher levels of stress and depression than parents of<br />

children with other diagnoses (Blacher & McIntyre, 2006; Dale et al., 2006;<br />

Eisenhower et al., 2005; Sanders & Morgan, 1997; Siklos & Kerns, 2007;<br />

Olsson & Hwang, 2001).<br />

3.1.3. Objectively and subjectively measured burden<br />

Objective measures of car<strong>in</strong>g demands and impairment severity (e.g., test<br />

results and cl<strong>in</strong>ical judgments) are typically not related to parents’ experience<br />

of stress (Hassall et al., 2005; Lecavalier et al., 2006; Luescher, Dede,


Author's personal copy<br />

Risk and Resilience 289<br />

Gitten, Fennell, & Maria, 1999; Nooj<strong>in</strong> & Wallander, 1997). In contrast,<br />

parents’ own subjective estimates of the impact of the child’s disability on<br />

daily life are typically related to parental stress and adaptation (Beckman,<br />

1991; Fisman, Wolf, & Noh, 1989; Plant & Sanders, 2007; Wallander &<br />

Venters, 1995). The more negative impact the child’s disability is perceived<br />

to have <strong>in</strong> their life, the more stress is likely to be felt by the parents. This<br />

supports the relational stress-process<strong>in</strong>g model of Lazarus (Lazarus, 1999,<br />

2000; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). They propose that there are no objective<br />

stressors, only potential stressors and the potential stressors are mediated<br />

through an appraisal process. Appraisal connotes an evaluation of the<br />

personal significance of what is happen<strong>in</strong>g. If we make the appraisal that<br />

what is occurr<strong>in</strong>g is a condition of stress, we def<strong>in</strong>e the situation as constitut<strong>in</strong>g<br />

harm/loss, threat, or challenge. In support of this, Plant and<br />

Sanders (2007) and Hassall et al. (2005) showed that psychological variables<br />

such as parental locus of control, parent<strong>in</strong>g satisfaction, and parental cognitive<br />

appraisal of caregiv<strong>in</strong>g responsibilities had mediat<strong>in</strong>g effects on the<br />

relation between child’s level of disability and parent<strong>in</strong>g stress.<br />

3.1.4. Daily hassles, time demands, and disruption of daily life<br />

Time is described as the factor most affected by car<strong>in</strong>g for a child with ID.<br />

Time restrictions and the disruption of daily life are more frequent <strong>in</strong><br />

parents of children with ID than <strong>in</strong> control families (Bristol, Gallagher, &<br />

Scholper, 1988; Carp<strong>in</strong>iello, Piras, Pariante, & Carta, 1995; Green, 2007;<br />

Gustavsson, 1989; Herman & Thompson, 1995; Sloper, 1999), and are<br />

related to adjustment problems <strong>in</strong> parents (Harris & McHale, 1989).<br />

Families with children with ID devote more time to childcare and spend<br />

less time <strong>in</strong> social activities and enjoy less active free time than parents of<br />

developmentally typical children (Barnett & Boyce, 1995; Green, 2007;<br />

Mactavish & Schleien, 2004; Sanders & Morgan, 1997). Parents also have<br />

much more contact with professionals and contrary to the <strong>in</strong>tention, contact<br />

with professionals is often perceived as an additional source of stress if it is<br />

extensive or frustrat<strong>in</strong>g (Olsson & Hwang, 2003).<br />

3.1.5. Restrictions <strong>in</strong> life due to care demands<br />

As a result of the demands of car<strong>in</strong>g, mothers of children with ID often<br />

report role restriction and limitations <strong>in</strong> pursu<strong>in</strong>g a career (Barnett & Boyce,<br />

1995; Gustavsson, 1989; Herman & Thompson, 1995; Olsson & Hwang,<br />

2003, 2006; Reddon et al., 1992; Wallander & Venters, 1995). The more<br />

restricted the mother feels, the more likely she is to experience motherhood<br />

as frustrat<strong>in</strong>g, unhappy, and wear<strong>in</strong>g (Breslau, Staruch, & Mortimer, 1982).<br />

One major obstacle for mothers’ employment is the difficulty <strong>in</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g<br />

suitable daycare for their child with ID (Shearn & Todd, 2000) and reoccurr<strong>in</strong>g<br />

ill health and appo<strong>in</strong>tments. Hedov, Wikblad, and Annerén (2006)<br />

found that mothers of children with DS stayed home to care for their child


Author's personal copy<br />

290 Mal<strong>in</strong> B. Olsson<br />

because of illness three times more often than control parents did for their<br />

nondisabled children.<br />

3.1.6. Difference <strong>in</strong> care stra<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> mothers and fathers<br />

Many studies of parents of children with ID have been conducted with<br />

mothers as sole <strong>in</strong>formants, but among those <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g both mothers and<br />

fathers, the results are mixed. Some report that mothers and fathers experience<br />

the same amount of stress as a result of br<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>g up a child with<br />

disabilities (Dyson, 1997; Fisman et al., 1989; Hadadian, 1994; Hast<strong>in</strong>gs,<br />

2003; Reddon et al., 1992; Wyngaarden Krauss, 1993), while some f<strong>in</strong>d that<br />

mothers experience more stress than fathers (Dumas et al., 1991;<br />

Gustavsson, 1989). A reduced level of responsibility has usually expla<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

the lower stress levels <strong>in</strong> fathers. Compared to mothers, fathers are also<br />

typically less restricted by hav<strong>in</strong>g a child with disabilities, and more often<br />

cont<strong>in</strong>ue their <strong>in</strong>volvement <strong>in</strong> professional life (Bristol et al., 1988;<br />

Gustavsson, 1989; Heller, Hsiesh &Rowitz, 1997; Olsson & Hwang, 2006).<br />

3.2. Socio-ecological risk factors<br />

3.2.1. Family function<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Poorer family function<strong>in</strong>g has been found to be associated with higher levels<br />

of stress <strong>in</strong> families with children with disabilities (Dyson, 1997; Sanders &<br />

Morgan, 1997; Wyngaarden Krauss, 1993). Some studies have found parents<br />

of children with disabilities to be at risk for marital and family conflict<br />

(Bristol et al., 1988; Fisman et al., 1989), while others do not (Dyson, 1991;<br />

Reddon et al., 1992; Sanders & Morgan, 1997; Van Riper, Ryff, &<br />

Pridham, 1992). In a meta-analytic review, Risdal and S<strong>in</strong>ger (2004) concluded<br />

that there was an <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> divorce of about 6% <strong>in</strong> families of<br />

children with ID. Research <strong>in</strong> typical populations has found that when<br />

experiences of parenthood were not as positive as expected, there was a<br />

decl<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong> marital satisfaction (Harwood, McLean, & Durk<strong>in</strong>, 2007).<br />

One hypothesis about the rather small <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> divorce rate for parents<br />

of children with ID is that for some parents, with weaker relations before<br />

the birth of the child with disability, the extra burden of a child with<br />

disability is an additional risk factor that further <strong>in</strong>creases stress and marital<br />

tension and may lead to divorce. For other parents, with good relations<br />

before the birth of the child, the support the partners can give one another<br />

can serve as a protective factor that can actually br<strong>in</strong>g the couple closer<br />

together.<br />

3.2.2. Social support<br />

Receiv<strong>in</strong>g unsatisfactory social support is strongly associated with difficulties<br />

<strong>in</strong> family function<strong>in</strong>g and parental mental health (Gowen, Johnson-<br />

Mart<strong>in</strong>, Goldman, & Appelbaum, 1989; Gray & Holden, 1992; Reddon


Author's personal copy<br />

Risk and Resilience 291<br />

et al., 1992). Parents of children with ID have been found to have more<br />

limited social networks then parents of typically develop<strong>in</strong>g children. They<br />

have more contact with family members and less contact with friends than<br />

parents of developmentally typical children (Herman & Thompson, 1995).<br />

Help from grandparents for <strong>in</strong>formal childcare is valued and often necessary<br />

for mothers to be engaged <strong>in</strong> paid work outside the home (Mitchell, 2007).<br />

Participation <strong>in</strong> social and leisure activities is often restricted <strong>in</strong> parents of<br />

children with ID, <strong>in</strong> part due to lack of time and energy and accessibility<br />

(Mactavish & Schleien, 2004; Olsson & Hwang, 2003).<br />

3.2.3. Socioeconomic situation<br />

Poverty and s<strong>in</strong>gle parenthood (Beckman, 1983; Olsson & Hwang, 2001)<br />

have been found to constitute risk factors for parental well-be<strong>in</strong>g. F<strong>in</strong>ancial<br />

difficulty is sometimes caused by the additional costs of car<strong>in</strong>g for a disabled<br />

child <strong>in</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ation with decreased possibilities for both parents to take<br />

part <strong>in</strong> paid work (Beresford, 1996; Emerson, 2003; Harris & McHale,<br />

1989; Herman & Thompson, 1995; Sloper, 1999). Several studies have<br />

found poverty and parental unemployment to be related to stress <strong>in</strong> parents<br />

of children with ID (Birenbaum, 2002; Ellis, Luiselli, & Amerault, 2002;<br />

Emerson et al., 2006; Eisenhower & Blacher, 2006). Emerson et al. (2006)<br />

argued that the <strong>in</strong>creased risk for poorer well-be<strong>in</strong>g among parents of<br />

children with ID could be attributed to their <strong>in</strong>creased risk of exposure to<br />

poorer socioeconomic circumstances, such as poverty and poor hous<strong>in</strong>g<br />

quality. They suggested that failure to address socioeconomic position is<br />

likely to re<strong>in</strong>force an overly pathological orientation <strong>in</strong> which children with<br />

ID are assumed to be a ‘‘burden’’ and <strong>in</strong> themselves a cause of maternal<br />

distress when it is actually the social circumstances that constitute the risk.<br />

3.2.4. Formal support<br />

Many families perceive that they do not receive the service and support to<br />

which they are entitled. The advocacy activities, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g negotiations and<br />

paperwork necessary to provide children with the services needed, can be<br />

an additional draw on parental energy and time (Green, 2007; Olsson &<br />

Hwang, 2003). Areas of need that are often reported by parents as be<strong>in</strong>g<br />

unmet are as follows: <strong>in</strong>formation and advice about services, the child’s<br />

diagnosis and prognosis, how to help the child, f<strong>in</strong>ancial and material<br />

support with hous<strong>in</strong>g and transportation, family support and practical help<br />

with respite care (Baxter, Cumm<strong>in</strong>s, & Polak, 1995; Douma, Dekker, &<br />

Koot, 2006; Herman & Thompson 1995; Lärka Paul<strong>in</strong>, Bernehäll Claesson,<br />

& Brod<strong>in</strong>, 2001; Olauson & Hvalstedt, 2001; Sloper, 1999; Stallard &<br />

Lenton, 1992; Summers et al., 2007). Research suggests that there exist<br />

<strong>in</strong>equalities <strong>in</strong> the extent to which people with disabilities and their families<br />

get access to benefits and support. Robertson et al. (2007) found that those<br />

with mental health, emotional or behavior problems, autism or multiple


Author's personal copy<br />

292 Mal<strong>in</strong> B. Olsson<br />

health problems were less likely to receive a person-centered plan, and they<br />

were also less likely to benefit if they received a plan.<br />

3.3. Intrapersonal risk factors<br />

Lazarus (1999) suggested that the experience of stress can create a negative<br />

stress reaction, but only if the person does not have the possibility to cope<br />

successfully with the stress. The experience and consequences of stress are<br />

therefore dependent on the <strong>in</strong>terplay of the environment (the potential<br />

stressors present) and the resources available to the <strong>in</strong>dividual to cope with<br />

the stressor. Parents of children with ID with high neuroticism have been<br />

found to use more of the cop<strong>in</strong>g strategies of escape-avoidance and accept<strong>in</strong>g<br />

responsibility (which has a self-blame component). Both of these<br />

strategies are strongly related to poorer well-be<strong>in</strong>g (Glidden, Bill<strong>in</strong>gs, &<br />

Jobe, 2006). Mothers who report high levels of stress often describe a<br />

tendency to avoid problems and feel out of control of their emotions.<br />

Possibly as a consequence of their higher levels of overall negative affectivity,<br />

these mothers tend to select strategies focus<strong>in</strong>g on the regulation of<br />

emotions rather than on active problem solv<strong>in</strong>g (Nooj<strong>in</strong> & Wallander,<br />

1997). Passive, emotion-focused cop<strong>in</strong>g, such as wishful th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g, selfblame,<br />

distanc<strong>in</strong>g, and self-control, have been found to have a negative<br />

relation to adaptation (Dykens & Hodapp, 2001; Nooj<strong>in</strong> & Wallander,<br />

1997; Sloper & Turner, 1993).<br />

3.4. Summary: Risk factors<br />

Several risk factors for parental adaptation to a child with ID have been<br />

studied but often <strong>in</strong> isolation; that is, studies have looked at the impact of<br />

one or two of the risk factors on adaptation. With the exception of child<br />

behavior problems and poverty the relation between the proposed risk<br />

factors and parental adaptation is not very strong. Because risk factors tend<br />

to cluster <strong>in</strong> the same <strong>in</strong>dividuals (Sameroff & Rosenblum, 2006), focus<strong>in</strong>g<br />

on a s<strong>in</strong>gle risk factor or challenge does not address the reality of most<br />

people’s lives. One way of improv<strong>in</strong>g predictive power could be to count<br />

the number or assess the cumulative effect of risk factors <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>dividual’s<br />

life <strong>in</strong> relation to adaptation (Sameroff & Rosenblum, 2006). It is then<br />

important to consider not only risk factors associated with the disability but<br />

also other general risk factors such as socioeconomic circumstances and<br />

major life events or parental adaptation.<br />

The length of time that <strong>in</strong>dividuals have been exposed to a risk is also<br />

important s<strong>in</strong>ce stress <strong>in</strong>itiates a series of biological and psychological processes<br />

called allostatic load. Allostatic load, or the cost of wear and tear on<br />

the body produced by repeated activation of biological stress response<br />

systems, contributes to physical disease and emotional and behavioral


Author's personal copy<br />

Risk and Resilience 293<br />

disorder (Sameroff & Rosenblum, 2006). For this reason, it is important to<br />

measure not only the exposure to possible risk factors but also the duration<br />

under which parents have been experienc<strong>in</strong>g them. Therefore, child age<br />

and time s<strong>in</strong>ce diagnosis may be important for the understand<strong>in</strong>g of risk<br />

factors over time.<br />

Stress sets <strong>in</strong> motion complex biological processes that form the basis for<br />

adaptation and survival <strong>in</strong> the face of challenge. The human stress response<br />

shows high levels of specificity <strong>in</strong> reactivity to and recovery from stress.<br />

To determ<strong>in</strong>e the role of stress <strong>in</strong> processes of resilience (and vulnerability),<br />

careful attention must be given also to the type of stress that an <strong>in</strong>dividual<br />

encounters (Sameroff & Rosenblum, 2006). We need more careful work<br />

to differentiate, def<strong>in</strong>e, and operationalize the different risk factors that<br />

are common and salient <strong>in</strong> the lives of parents with children with ID. It<br />

may, for example, be that stressors related to the relationship with the child<br />

(such as behavior problems) have more adverse effect on parental health<br />

than time demands and daily hassles because they threaten more salient<br />

processes.<br />

4. Protective Factors<br />

4.1. Child and disability protective factors<br />

Beresford (1996) found that the positive aspects of car<strong>in</strong>g for a child with<br />

disabilities <strong>in</strong>volved the feel<strong>in</strong>gs of rewards and pleasures of parenthood, as<br />

well as a sense of moral responsibility. Parents who enjoyed their role and<br />

found it a satisfy<strong>in</strong>g experience viewed car<strong>in</strong>g for their child as a challenge,<br />

someth<strong>in</strong>g they enjoyed respond<strong>in</strong>g to. This strong feel<strong>in</strong>g of mean<strong>in</strong>gfulness<br />

is probably extremely positive for both the parents’ and the children’s wellbe<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

There is anecdotal evidence that the emotional bond between the<br />

parent and the child with ID is the strongest protective factor of all, and<br />

support<strong>in</strong>g moments of joyful <strong>in</strong>teraction between the parent and the child is<br />

probably a powerful way of support<strong>in</strong>g well-be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> both parents and<br />

children (Beresford, 1996). Beck, Hast<strong>in</strong>gs, and Daley (2004) found that<br />

child pro-social behavior <strong>in</strong>dependent of behavior problems predicted<br />

maternal health, <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g the possible buffer<strong>in</strong>g effect of moments of<br />

joy and connection. Most parents are able to develop a secure and reward<strong>in</strong>g<br />

relationship with their child, even when the child has extreme social<br />

atypical behaviors as is the case with some children with autism (Bir<strong>in</strong>gen,<br />

Fidler, Barrett, & Kubicek, 2005) but the risk of an <strong>in</strong>secure attachment<br />

<strong>in</strong>creases with degree of <strong>in</strong>tellectual disability and severity of autistic<br />

symptoms.


Author's personal copy<br />

294 Mal<strong>in</strong> B. Olsson<br />

4.2. Socio-ecological protective factors<br />

An essential question related to adversity and resilience is the <strong>in</strong>dividual’s<br />

development of an effective set of responses to stress. Central components of<br />

the stress response <strong>in</strong>clude the <strong>in</strong>itial appraisal of the event and its emotional<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g, the ability to sufficiently regulate one’s emotions and arousal to<br />

<strong>in</strong>itiate problem solv<strong>in</strong>g and gather more <strong>in</strong>formation, the fuller cognitiveaffective<br />

<strong>in</strong>terpretations of the event, and one’s behavioral response<br />

(Greenberg, 2006). Masten and Obradovic (2006) have noted that among<br />

the most important resilience factors are these very cognitive and emotion<br />

regulation skills.<br />

4.2.1. Cop<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Upon exposure to stress, complex cognitive, behavioral, emotional, and<br />

biological processes are set <strong>in</strong> motion that serve the purpose of adaptation<br />

through either avoid<strong>in</strong>g/withdraw<strong>in</strong>g from the source of stress, act<strong>in</strong>g on<br />

the source of stress to reduce its effects, or adjust<strong>in</strong>g to the source of stress<br />

when neither escape nor confrontations are possible (Compas, 2006).<br />

Two fundamental processes are <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> self-regulation <strong>in</strong> response to<br />

stress. First, there is a set of automatic processes that are activated <strong>in</strong> response<br />

to stress that are related to but dist<strong>in</strong>ct from cop<strong>in</strong>g. Second, <strong>in</strong>dividuals<br />

<strong>in</strong>itiate a set of controlled, volitional responses to stress. It is these voluntary<br />

responses to stress that are <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the concept of cop<strong>in</strong>g. Compas<br />

def<strong>in</strong>ed cop<strong>in</strong>g as a ‘‘conscious volitional effort to regulate emotion, cognition,<br />

behavior, physiology, and the environment <strong>in</strong> response to stressful<br />

events or circumstances.’’ These regulatory processes both draw on and are<br />

constra<strong>in</strong>ed by the biological, cognitive, social, and emotional development<br />

of the <strong>in</strong>dividual. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Compas, cop<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>cludes three primary<br />

subtypes: primary control cop<strong>in</strong>g (problem solv<strong>in</strong>g, emotional expression,<br />

emotional modulation); secondary control cop<strong>in</strong>g, or efforts to adapt to the<br />

source of stress (acceptance, distraction, cognitive restructur<strong>in</strong>g, positive<br />

th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g); and disengagement cop<strong>in</strong>g, or efforts to withdraw from the<br />

source of stress and one’s emotions (avoidance, denial, wishful th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g).<br />

Effective cop<strong>in</strong>g depends on be<strong>in</strong>g flexible (Lazarus, 1999). The best<br />

cop<strong>in</strong>g should be responsive to the requirements of the stressful situation.<br />

Some cop<strong>in</strong>g strategies are tied to personality variables, whereas others are tied<br />

to the social context. Certa<strong>in</strong> cop<strong>in</strong>g strategies show significant with<strong>in</strong>-subject<br />

consistency from encounter to encounter. For example, if positive reappraisal<br />

were employed by a person <strong>in</strong> one encounter, it is likely that the person would<br />

use it aga<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> another encounter. However, other cop<strong>in</strong>g strategies, such as<br />

seek<strong>in</strong>g social support, are used very <strong>in</strong>consistently across different encounters.<br />

When conditions of stress are appraised as changeable—that is when they are<br />

viewed as fall<strong>in</strong>g with<strong>in</strong> the person’s control—problem-focused cop<strong>in</strong>g


Author's personal copy<br />

Risk and Resilience 295<br />

predom<strong>in</strong>ates. However, when the conditions are appraised as unchangeable,<br />

emotion-focused cop<strong>in</strong>g predom<strong>in</strong>ates (Lazarus, 1999).<br />

4.2.2. Cop<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> parents of children with ID<br />

The majority of parents with children with ID employ a variety of cop<strong>in</strong>g<br />

behaviors <strong>in</strong> their efforts to deal with the demands fac<strong>in</strong>g them (Beresford,<br />

1996; Reddon et al., 1992) and a larger repertoire and more use of cop<strong>in</strong>g is<br />

related to more positive adaptation (Taanila, Syrjälä, Kokkonen, & Järvel<strong>in</strong>,<br />

2002). The use of active or problem-focused cop<strong>in</strong>g strategies which<br />

emphasize efforts to seek social support, actively solve the problem, and<br />

ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> a positive outlook on life, appear to be recorded more frequently<br />

and to have a favorable effect on parental well-be<strong>in</strong>g (Dykens & Hodapp,<br />

2001; Glidden et al., 2006; Judge, 1998, Kim, Greenberg, Seltzer, & Krauss,<br />

2003; Luescher et al., 1999). Parents of children with ID are often reported<br />

to use denial and wishful th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g and this has usually been considered a<br />

negative cop<strong>in</strong>g strategy. But s<strong>in</strong>ce the disability is an unchangeable condition,<br />

emotion-focused cop<strong>in</strong>g is theoretically likely to occur regularly.<br />

4.2.3. Personality<br />

The stress-process<strong>in</strong>g abilities (cop<strong>in</strong>g) described above have been shown to<br />

be l<strong>in</strong>ked to personality factors which, <strong>in</strong> turn, have been shown to be<br />

strongly related to levels of well-be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> parents of children with ID<br />

(Glidden & Schoolcraft, 2003; Baker, Blacher, & Olsson, 2005; Glidden<br />

et al., 2006; Hast<strong>in</strong>gs, 2002; Hassall & Rose, 2005; Oelofsen & Richardson,<br />

2006; Olsson & Hwang, 2002; Ylvén et al., 2006). The route from personality<br />

to well-be<strong>in</strong>g is thought to advance via the process of appraisal and<br />

cop<strong>in</strong>g. Parents with more optimistic outlooks, higher sense of coherence<br />

(SOC), more <strong>in</strong>ternal locus of control, and lower neuroticism may appraise a<br />

potential stressor as a challenge <strong>in</strong>stead of a threat (or reappraise the situation<br />

all together), have more confidence <strong>in</strong> their ability to alter the source of stress<br />

and therefore use more active cop<strong>in</strong>g strategies (problem solv<strong>in</strong>g and social<br />

support). Nooj<strong>in</strong> and Wallander (1997) found confidence <strong>in</strong> problemsolv<strong>in</strong>g<br />

ability to <strong>in</strong>crease the likelihood of select<strong>in</strong>g more active strategies<br />

when faced with disability-related stress. Mothers who perceived themselves<br />

as effective problem solvers generally reported less distress. Mothers who<br />

reported better psychological adjustment described themselves as hav<strong>in</strong>g<br />

high levels of confidence <strong>in</strong> their problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g abilities. The def<strong>in</strong>ition<br />

of personality is that it is a global and rather stable characteristic of the<br />

<strong>in</strong>dividual, whereas cop<strong>in</strong>g strategies, although l<strong>in</strong>ked to personality, are<br />

more variable across situations.


Author's personal copy<br />

296 Mal<strong>in</strong> B. Olsson<br />

4.2.4. Positive view of life and self<br />

Research has <strong>in</strong>dicated that despite the <strong>in</strong>creased demands of parent<strong>in</strong>g a<br />

child with a disability, many parents are able to report positive changes <strong>in</strong><br />

themselves, their family or their lives because of the child with disabilities<br />

(Erw<strong>in</strong> & Soodak, 1995; Scorgie & Sobsey, 2000; Sta<strong>in</strong>ton & Besser, 1998).<br />

Some of the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs from parents who adapt and cope effectively suggest<br />

that positive refram<strong>in</strong>g of thoughts and attitudes about their circumstances is<br />

important for adaptive parental cop<strong>in</strong>g (Hast<strong>in</strong>gs & Taunt, 2002). Personality<br />

factors <strong>in</strong>fluence this appraisal process. It has been shown that hard<strong>in</strong>ess<br />

(Weiss, 2002), parental self-efficacy (Hast<strong>in</strong>gs & Brown, 2002), and parental<br />

SOC about their lives (Hedov, Anneren & Wikblad, 2002; Olsson &<br />

Hwang, 2002) are associated with fewer parental mental health problems<br />

<strong>in</strong> parents of children with ID. Parents who manage their lives effectively<br />

seem to show evidence of such personal traits as patience, flexibility,<br />

hopefulness, positive outlook, determ<strong>in</strong>ation, and good problem solv<strong>in</strong>g<br />

skills (Beresford, 1994; Kwai-sang Yau & Li-Tsang, 1999; Nooj<strong>in</strong> &<br />

Wallander, 1997; Tunali & Power, 1993).<br />

4.2.5. Sense of coherence<br />

There has been an <strong>in</strong>creased <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> positive outcomes <strong>in</strong> parents and factors<br />

that contribute to ‘‘bonadaptation’’ (Erw<strong>in</strong> & Soodak, 1995; Scorgie &<br />

Sobsey, 2000). The salutogenic perspective of Antonovsky (1987) comb<strong>in</strong>es<br />

the <strong>in</strong>terest of personality with the <strong>in</strong>terest of positive adaptation. Antonovsky’s<br />

theory of SOC was developed to understand what makes people manage<br />

their lives despite high levels of stress, very similar to the concept of resilience<br />

but with a strong focus on <strong>in</strong>trapersonal protective factors. I will discuss<br />

the concept of SOC at some length as it is highly relevant to resilience.<br />

Antonovsky suggested that resistance resources all have one th<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong><br />

common: They contribute to make stressors understandable. The cumulative<br />

experience of stressors as understandable will, with time, create a strong<br />

SOC def<strong>in</strong>ed as ‘‘a global orientation that expresses the extent to which one<br />

has a pervasive, endur<strong>in</strong>g though dynamic feel<strong>in</strong>g of confidence that (1) the<br />

stimuli deriv<strong>in</strong>g from one’s <strong>in</strong>ternal and external environments <strong>in</strong> the course<br />

of liv<strong>in</strong>g are structured, predictable, and explicable; (2) the resources are<br />

available to meet the demands posed by these stimuli; and (3) these demands<br />

are challenges, worthy of <strong>in</strong>vestment and engagement’’ (Antonovsky,<br />

1987 ). The strength <strong>in</strong> people with a high SOC is not that they are<br />

especially successful <strong>in</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g one or the other k<strong>in</strong>d of cop<strong>in</strong>g strategy, but<br />

that they approach the world with the generalized expectation that stressors<br />

are mean<strong>in</strong>gful and comprehensible. Instead of rigidly rely<strong>in</strong>g on one<br />

cop<strong>in</strong>g strategy, people with a strong SOC are characterized by flexibility.<br />

High SOC has been shown to be related to high well-be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong><br />

both mothers and fathers of children with ID (Olsson & Hwang, 2002;


Author's personal copy<br />

Risk and Resilience 297<br />

Oelofsen & Richardson, 2006). But parents of children with ID have also<br />

been shown to have lower SOC than control parents (Olsson & Hwang;<br />

Oelofsen & Richardsson) with mothers hav<strong>in</strong>g lower SOC than fathers. A<br />

strong relation between SOC and health has been reported <strong>in</strong> many different<br />

areas, and one study found that parents of children with ID with higher SOC<br />

levels had fewer registered days of sickness absence (Hedov et al., 2006).<br />

The first of the three central components of SOC, comprehensibility,<br />

refers to an experience of the confronted stimuli as mak<strong>in</strong>g cognitive<br />

sense, as <strong>in</strong>formation that is ordered, consistent, structured, and clear,<br />

rather than chaotic, disordered, random, or accidental (Antonovsky,<br />

1987). The fact that <strong>in</strong>sufficient <strong>in</strong>formation about services, the child’s<br />

conditions and prognosis are some of the most common unmet needs<br />

mentioned by parents of children with ID (Granlund, Roll-Pettersson,<br />

Steensson, & Sund<strong>in</strong>, 1994; Sloper, 1999), suggests that the feel<strong>in</strong>g of<br />

comprehensibility is perceived as essential but often threatened <strong>in</strong> these<br />

parents. The theory can also help understand the result by Green (2004)<br />

who found that mothers with a high <strong>in</strong>ternal locus of control who also<br />

believed <strong>in</strong> chance reported less stress. In contrast, belief <strong>in</strong> chance without<br />

a strong <strong>in</strong>ternal locus of control related to <strong>in</strong>creased subjective burden, as<br />

mothers may feel overwhelmed by the uncontrollable nature of life events.<br />

The positive results of psychoeducational <strong>in</strong>terventions also suggest that<br />

<strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g the mother’s knowledge about her child’s condition and strategies<br />

to handle difficult behavior can <strong>in</strong>crease feel<strong>in</strong>gs of comprehensibility<br />

(Bristol et al., 1993; McIntyre, <strong>in</strong> press). Advice on how to handle difficult<br />

child behavior is probably also related to the second central concept of<br />

SOC namely, manageability.<br />

Manageability is the experience that one is <strong>in</strong> possession of resources<br />

adequate to meet the demands posed by the stimuli, the resources may be<br />

under one’s own control or controlled by others whom one feels one can<br />

count on. The support offered to parents <strong>in</strong> the above-mentioned studies<br />

helped parents to decrease difficult child behavior, probably with an<br />

<strong>in</strong>creased sense of manageability as a consequence. The cognitive strategies<br />

described by parents who ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed a positive outlook on life, <strong>in</strong> a study<br />

by Beresford (1996), were clearly directed at <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g their feel<strong>in</strong>g of<br />

manageability. These parents took one day at a time, did not dwell on<br />

difficulties, hoped for improvement and compared themselves to others<br />

worse off. Glidden and collaborators have demonstrated that the depression<br />

trajectories for mothers rear<strong>in</strong>g children with ID were quite different<br />

depend<strong>in</strong>g on whether the child had been born <strong>in</strong>to the family or had<br />

been know<strong>in</strong>gly adopted by it (Flaherty & Glidden, 2000). If the parents had<br />

know<strong>in</strong>gly adopted a child with disabilities they probably believed <strong>in</strong> their<br />

ability to manage the situation, and they also showed lower levels of<br />

depression <strong>in</strong>itially. The feel<strong>in</strong>g of mastery seems to be important for the<br />

feel<strong>in</strong>g of manageability. In a study by Grant (1998), parents noted that the


Author's personal copy<br />

298 Mal<strong>in</strong> B. Olsson<br />

moments they found to be most positive emerged as a result of develop<strong>in</strong>g<br />

strategies for cop<strong>in</strong>g with the challenge of rais<strong>in</strong>g their child.<br />

The third core concept of SOC, mean<strong>in</strong>gfulness refers to an experience of<br />

shap<strong>in</strong>g one’s dest<strong>in</strong>y as well as one’s daily experience. This <strong>in</strong>cludes the<br />

extent to which one feels that life makes sense emotionally, that at least some<br />

of the problems and demands posed by liv<strong>in</strong>g are worth an <strong>in</strong>vestment of<br />

energy. Not be<strong>in</strong>g able to pursue personal <strong>in</strong>terests and goals <strong>in</strong> life may<br />

severely threaten the feel<strong>in</strong>g of mean<strong>in</strong>gfulness <strong>in</strong> life. Antonovsky proposes<br />

that the feel<strong>in</strong>g of mean<strong>in</strong>gfulness is the most central aspect of a strong SOC.<br />

The issue of mean<strong>in</strong>gfulness can be illustrated by the study of Tunali and<br />

Power (1993), <strong>in</strong> which they found mothers’ abilities to redef<strong>in</strong>e their goals <strong>in</strong><br />

life to suit the actual circumstances and limitations of their daily lives had a<br />

strong relation to their well-be<strong>in</strong>g. If reach<strong>in</strong>g one’s personal goals is<br />

prevented by the life situation, then either the situation itself or one’s goals<br />

must be changed to <strong>in</strong>crease the feel<strong>in</strong>g of mean<strong>in</strong>gfulness. For many parents<br />

of children with ID, it is hard to change the life situation substantially enough<br />

to reach the personal ideals held before the birth of the child (Gustavsson,<br />

1989). The ability to undertake, and support for, chang<strong>in</strong>g personal goals, is<br />

important for parents of children with ID, but may not be easy <strong>in</strong> an<br />

<strong>in</strong>dustrialized society where personal value is often based on achievement,<br />

career <strong>in</strong>volvement, and an active leisure-time (Olsson & Hwang, 2006).<br />

4.3. Intrapersonal protective factors<br />

4.3.1. Social support<br />

Mothers of children with ID who experience support from their partner and<br />

from social networks experience less stress and depression (Gowen et al.,<br />

1989; Plant & Sanders, 2007; Reddon et al., 1992; Sharpley & Bitsika, 1997;<br />

Sh<strong>in</strong>, 2002; Wallander & Venters, 1995; Weiss, 2002). Overall, parents seem<br />

to be quite content with the size, availability, and emotional support provided<br />

by their social networks (Baxter et al., 1995; Reddon et al., 1992). Many<br />

parents engage <strong>in</strong> parent<strong>in</strong>g groups and parents report that quality of life can<br />

be enhanced through participation <strong>in</strong> social outlets that are accept<strong>in</strong>g of<br />

the child’s disability (Mactavish & Schleien, 2004; Poston & Turnbull, 2004).<br />

4.3.2. Formal support<br />

Some studies have demonstrated the positive impact of service supports on<br />

families (Cowen & Reed, 2002; Ellis et al., 2002; Romer & Richardson,<br />

2002). In a Norwegian study, parents <strong>in</strong>dicated that the formal support they<br />

received for themselves and their child was one of the most important<br />

factors for their cop<strong>in</strong>g with the situation (Kvande & Knutheim, 1995).<br />

Families appear to emphasize the <strong>in</strong>terpersonal aspects of their relationships<br />

with providers as much as they emphasize the services themselves (Summers<br />

et al., 2007).


Author's personal copy<br />

Risk and Resilience 299<br />

When parents are asked about important factors for good support they<br />

place much emphasis on flexibility, <strong>in</strong>dividuality, and partnership. It is<br />

important for parents that service providers view every family as unique<br />

and consider and respect that which the parents th<strong>in</strong>k is best for their child<br />

(Lärka Paul<strong>in</strong> et al., 2001). The more control families perceive themselves to<br />

have over the care and management of the services the more satisfied they<br />

are with the service (Blacher, Neece, & Paczkowski, 2005; Caldwell &<br />

Heller, 2003; Dunst & Dempsey, 2007; Nachshen & M<strong>in</strong>nes, 2005).<br />

Unfortunately, parents also <strong>in</strong>dicate that they have to fight for the cooperation<br />

from, and control over, support services. Stronger and more<br />

persistent parents get more resources and more control (Knox, Parmenter,<br />

Atk<strong>in</strong>son, & Yazbeck, 2000; Lärka Paul<strong>in</strong> et al., 2001), which may expla<strong>in</strong><br />

the positive effect of confrontive cop<strong>in</strong>g strategies (Glidden et al., 2006).<br />

The rates of satisfaction by parents can be <strong>in</strong>creased with improved<br />

service. Parents have, for example, evaluated centers that have models<br />

based on partnership, with recognition of the complementary knowledge<br />

and expertise of parents and professionals, as be<strong>in</strong>g very helpful and supportive<br />

(Sloper, 1999). Even if the support system could never totally<br />

elim<strong>in</strong>ate the extra care associated <strong>in</strong> car<strong>in</strong>g for a child with ID, it can<br />

facilitate the family’s situation and <strong>in</strong>crease the family’s ability to choose the<br />

best way for families to live life with their child with ID.<br />

Not all parents need or want the same k<strong>in</strong>d of services. Therefore, support<br />

programs should preferably be flexible enough to offer support that matches<br />

families’ actual needs (Granlund et al., 1994). It is also important to acknowledge<br />

that the needs change over time. Todd and Sheam (1996) suggested that<br />

support services should have two phases. The first phase should <strong>in</strong>volve<br />

support to parents through shar<strong>in</strong>g the parental workload, such as respite<br />

care. The second phase should <strong>in</strong>volve support<strong>in</strong>g the parent and his/her<br />

personal aspirations and <strong>in</strong>terests. Families who have a coord<strong>in</strong>ator who can<br />

provide an overview and coord<strong>in</strong>ate the family’s different needs, report better<br />

relationships with professionals. Those who do not have a coord<strong>in</strong>ator report<br />

significantly more unmet needs (Sloper, 1999).<br />

4.3.3. Parent<strong>in</strong>g support<br />

Support for families <strong>in</strong> the management of their children’s challeng<strong>in</strong>g<br />

behavior is imperative and should be provided before the behavior becomes<br />

so well established that <strong>in</strong>tensive <strong>in</strong>tervention from specialist service providers<br />

is required (Hudson et al., 2003; Soresi, Nota, & Ferrari, 2007). Parents<br />

who are helped to understand their children’s disability and to manage their<br />

child’s learn<strong>in</strong>g and behavior problems have been shown to have lower<br />

stress levels and better well-be<strong>in</strong>g (Hudson et al., 2003; Soresi et al., 2007).<br />

The weight of evidence suggests that cognitive behavioral treatment and<br />

<strong>in</strong>terventions which br<strong>in</strong>g parents together <strong>in</strong> groups with psychoeducational<br />

material reduce maternal distress (S<strong>in</strong>ger, 2006). These <strong>in</strong>terventions


Author's personal copy<br />

300 Mal<strong>in</strong> B. Olsson<br />

are designed to strengthen confidence <strong>in</strong> problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g, and directed at<br />

support<strong>in</strong>g adequate cop<strong>in</strong>g, and have been proven useful (Bristol et al.,<br />

1993; McIntyre & Phaneuf, 2008; Sloper, 1999). Apart from deal<strong>in</strong>g<br />

explicitly with challeng<strong>in</strong>g behavior, families may need: (a) assistance with<br />

establish<strong>in</strong>g structure <strong>in</strong> home rout<strong>in</strong>es; (b) assistance with deal<strong>in</strong>g with<br />

stress; (c) aid with provision of respite care; and (d) assistance with engag<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>in</strong> advocacy (Soresi et al., 2007; Turnbull & Ruef, 1996; Williams &<br />

Wishart, 2003). Management strategies for children’s problem behavior<br />

may be affected by parents’ explanations of that behavior and their associated<br />

emotional reactions. If the children’s behavior is thought to be a<br />

consequence of the disability, then it may not be viewed as changeable.<br />

Consequently, <strong>in</strong>terventions might aim to help parents generate alternative<br />

explanations for their child’s behavior, particularly to <strong>in</strong>clude consideration<br />

of environmental factors, and to enhance the emotional quality of parent–<br />

child relationships (Hassall & Rose, 2005). Hast<strong>in</strong>gs and Johnson (2001)<br />

showed that parents of children with autism reported lower levels of stress<br />

and pessimism when they had positive beliefs <strong>in</strong> the efficacy of an <strong>in</strong>tensive<br />

behavioral <strong>in</strong>tervention they were conduct<strong>in</strong>g. They, therefore, suggest that<br />

attention to parents’ efficacy beliefs about <strong>in</strong>terventions is important if these<br />

<strong>in</strong>terventions are to achieve their <strong>in</strong>tended results.<br />

4.4. Summary: Protective factors<br />

Personality factors, cognition, and cop<strong>in</strong>g are the concepts so far studied<br />

most <strong>in</strong> relation to positive adaptation. Positive child characteristics and the<br />

impact of positive parent–child relationships are understudied area. Most<br />

studies reviewed have used lack of negative outcome (e.g., no depressive<br />

symptoms or low stress) as measures of positive adaptation. The absence of<br />

depression is obviously not a measure of good adaptation and more studies<br />

us<strong>in</strong>g outcome measures with the possibility of detect<strong>in</strong>g variation <strong>in</strong> the<br />

positive end of the adaptation cont<strong>in</strong>uum are needed. Without an outcome<br />

measure allow<strong>in</strong>g for variation <strong>in</strong> the positive end, it is difficult to actually<br />

assess resilience or protection. An example from developmental psychology<br />

can serve to illustrate: In a study of children of mothers with major mental<br />

illness a positive l<strong>in</strong>k was found between maternal warmth and child competence<br />

(Luthar, Sawyer, & Brown, 2006). This might logically suggest that<br />

mothers’ warmth served as a protective factor. However, when the distributions<br />

of children’s scores were exam<strong>in</strong>ed, that was not what was found.<br />

The children experienc<strong>in</strong>g high maternal warmth had competence scores<br />

close to the national average; they did not show ‘‘superior’’ competence.<br />

In contrast, those experienc<strong>in</strong>g low warmth had very low competence.<br />

In this case, therefore, high warmth was not particularly promotive—rather,<br />

low maternal warmth connoted significant vulnerability. Exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the


Author's personal copy<br />

Risk and Resilience 301<br />

distribution of scores vis-à-vis norms can be critical <strong>in</strong> illum<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g whether<br />

the construct connotes exceptional well-be<strong>in</strong>g at one end, or exceptional<br />

dysfunction at the other.<br />

5. Outcomes<br />

Resilience is qu<strong>in</strong>tessentially <strong>in</strong>ferential: Judg<strong>in</strong>g the resilience of a<br />

system requires criteria for identify<strong>in</strong>g whether the system is do<strong>in</strong>g whatever<br />

it is supposed to be do<strong>in</strong>g (Masten & Obradovic, 2006). Important questions<br />

to ask <strong>in</strong> resilience research are as follows: Who decides or def<strong>in</strong>es the<br />

criteria for judg<strong>in</strong>g good adaptation Does resilience refer to positive <strong>in</strong>ternal<br />

adaptation, positive external adaptation, or both In the literature on<br />

resilience <strong>in</strong> adults, the focus is usually on how the person herself feels.<br />

Prom<strong>in</strong>ent are <strong>in</strong>dices such as subjective well-be<strong>in</strong>g, happ<strong>in</strong>ess, selfreported<br />

absence of distress, and so on. Few, if any, studies on resilience<br />

among adults have def<strong>in</strong>ed ‘‘do<strong>in</strong>g well’’ <strong>in</strong> terms of others’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of them,<br />

such as if they functioned as good spouses or parents (Luthar et al., 2006).<br />

In research on adaptation of families with children with ID the outcome<br />

variable most commonly discussed is parent<strong>in</strong>g stress. From a resilience perspective,<br />

stress is not an outcome but a prerequisite for resilience. The second<br />

most frequently measured outcome is depression. Consider<strong>in</strong>g that there is<br />

agreement that a system has experienced serious risk (stress), positive mental<br />

health can be an accetable outcome measure of resilience. Depression, however,<br />

hardly meets this criterion, s<strong>in</strong>ce there is no possibility of detect<strong>in</strong>g any<br />

function<strong>in</strong>g better than no depression. Studies of family quality of life have a<br />

focus on positive function<strong>in</strong>g that fits well with the concept of resilience. Other<br />

adaptive adult behaviors such as positive parent<strong>in</strong>g, participation <strong>in</strong> work<br />

and social life, and marital satisfaction should also be relevant as outcomes.<br />

5.1. Depression <strong>in</strong> parents of children with ID<br />

Depression is a normal response to certa<strong>in</strong> life situations. One life event that<br />

sometimes leads to parental depression is the diagnosis of a child with a<br />

disability. Initially, there appears to be a grief reaction that is often <strong>in</strong>tense at<br />

the time of diagnosis. The feel<strong>in</strong>gs of grief <strong>in</strong>itially described by parents are<br />

similar to those experienced <strong>in</strong> loss situations and can <strong>in</strong>clude shock,<br />

disbelief, and anger (Baxter et al., 1995; Cameron et al., 1991) but are not<br />

present <strong>in</strong> all parents. It was previously thought that parents went through<br />

sequential stages of sorrow after learn<strong>in</strong>g about their child’s diagnosis, f<strong>in</strong>ally<br />

reach<strong>in</strong>g a stage of acceptance (Blacher, 1984; Shapiro, 1983). Recent<br />

research suggests, however, that feel<strong>in</strong>gs of grief, disbelief, and shock<br />

associated with the diagnosis may not be f<strong>in</strong>ally resolved by all parents,


Author's personal copy<br />

302 Mal<strong>in</strong> B. Olsson<br />

but may reoccur throughout the child’s and parent’s life (Baxter et al., 1995;<br />

Pianta, Marv<strong>in</strong>, Britner, & Borowitz, 1996). In addition, some parents<br />

experience mourn<strong>in</strong>g for the loss of the expected ‘‘perfect child’’; others<br />

experience feel<strong>in</strong>gs of guilt for not be<strong>in</strong>g a good-enough parent to the child<br />

and his/her sibl<strong>in</strong>gs, anxiety over the child’s welfare and future, and/or<br />

sorrow over lost possibilities <strong>in</strong> the parent’s own personal life (Gustavsson,<br />

1989; Harris & McHale, 1989). These emotions and concerns can be found<br />

to a greater or lesser degree <strong>in</strong> all parents.<br />

Depression experienced by birth parents is not trivial and may lead to<br />

lifelong consequences. For example, two studies have reported that 20%<br />

of parents decided not to raise their child with DS, but <strong>in</strong>stead to rel<strong>in</strong>quish<br />

the <strong>in</strong>fant for adoption (Dumaret, De Vigan, Julian Reynier, Goujars, &<br />

Aymé, 1998; Sadetzki et al., 2000). Although these studies did not specifically<br />

report parental depression scores, it is likely that parents who experience<br />

negative emotional reactions to their child’s diagnosis are more likely<br />

to make this decision (Glidden & Schoolcraft, 2003; Gustavsson, 1989).<br />

Most studies <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g the mental health of parents with children<br />

with disabilities have <strong>in</strong>itially found higher scores for maternal depression<br />

compared to norms or to control groups (e.g., Blacher & Baker, 2002;<br />

Blacher & Lopez, 1997; Olsson & Hwang, 2001; Seltzer, Floyd, & H<strong>in</strong>des,<br />

2004; S<strong>in</strong>ger, 2006; White & Hast<strong>in</strong>gs, 2004; Veisson, 1999). There are,<br />

however, some studies, mostly <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g parents of children with DS, that<br />

have not found differences <strong>in</strong> depression scores (Glidden & Schoolcraft,<br />

2003; Scott, Atk<strong>in</strong>son, M<strong>in</strong>ton, & Bowman, 1997; Van Riper et al., 1992).<br />

Maternal reports of depression have usually been generalized to parental<br />

depression, and the very few studies which have <strong>in</strong>cluded fathers have<br />

usually found normal depression scores or far fewer symptoms of depression<br />

<strong>in</strong> fathers of children with disabilities than <strong>in</strong> mothers (Dumas et al., 1991;<br />

Fisman et al., 1989; Gray & Holden, 1992; Olsson & Hwang, 2001, 2006;<br />

Veisson, 1999). Studies of parents with children with disabilities suggest that<br />

30% of mothers with children with disabilities score above the cut-off for<br />

depression compared to 20% <strong>in</strong> large samples of women of child-bear<strong>in</strong>g<br />

age (Olsson & Hwang, 2001; S<strong>in</strong>ger, 2006; White & Hast<strong>in</strong>gs, 2004;<br />

Veisson, 1999). S<strong>in</strong>ce mothers of children with ID are at higher risk for<br />

depression, the exist<strong>in</strong>g social service system for children with ID should<br />

specifically target this problem (S<strong>in</strong>ger, 2006).<br />

5.2. Family quality of life<br />

Family quality of life has been proposed as an appropriate outcome for<br />

families of young children with disabilities. The construct implies a family’s<br />

(so far usually the mother’s) personal <strong>in</strong>terpretation of well-be<strong>in</strong>g, life<br />

satisfaction, and <strong>in</strong>terpersonal relationships (Summers et al., 2007). The<br />

few studies <strong>in</strong> the literature that have used family quality of life as an


Author's personal copy<br />

Risk and Resilience 303<br />

outcome have found high rat<strong>in</strong>gs, suggest<strong>in</strong>g that 39% rate their quality of<br />

life as excellent. Almost 65% of parents rate their family quality of life as<br />

moderate to very good (Summers et al., 2007). These figures actually<br />

correspond to the studies us<strong>in</strong>g depression as the outcome, which usually<br />

f<strong>in</strong>d that 65% of parents do not report elevated levels of depression.<br />

6. Discussion<br />

Several conclusions derive from this review. First, the good news is that<br />

it seems that studies us<strong>in</strong>g depression as well as studies us<strong>in</strong>g quality of life as<br />

outcomes agree that at least 65% of parents show resilience to be<strong>in</strong>g a parent<br />

of a child with ID, and 30% show adverse outcomes. Second, many<br />

different factors, concepts and measures are studied <strong>in</strong> the research on families<br />

of children with ID; and third, much of the research on parental adaptation to<br />

a child with ID lacks explicit conceptual def<strong>in</strong>itions and theory. Without a<br />

theoretical framework, it will be difficult, if not impossible, to understand and<br />

empirically test the processes lead<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong> adaptation.<br />

It seems that some factors are studied over and over aga<strong>in</strong> (e.g., the impact of<br />

behavior problems, and the importance of marital support) without an<br />

explicit theoretical foundation but more as a result of positive f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs and a<br />

match with hypotheses. But are there other important risk and resistance<br />

factors or processes lead<strong>in</strong>g to resilience that we are not study<strong>in</strong>g<br />

In a review of f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs from resilience research primarily <strong>in</strong> developmental<br />

psychology, Rutter (2006) suggests that resistance may derive from<br />

several different sources. One of these could be controlled exposure to risk<br />

(rather than the total avoidance of risk). It seems as if the key element is<br />

some form of prior successful cop<strong>in</strong>g with a similar challenge. Is this process<br />

of resilience relevant <strong>in</strong> parents of children with ID Perhaps hav<strong>in</strong>g made a<br />

successful transition to parenthood before hav<strong>in</strong>g a child with a disability<br />

could be related to resilience. Parents with a child with ID as a second or<br />

later child may be more resilient than first time parents with a child with ID.<br />

Relatedly, hav<strong>in</strong>g previous exposure to disability such as the experience of a<br />

close relative, friend or neighbor experienc<strong>in</strong>g disability could also be a<br />

resilience factor.<br />

Rutter (2006) further suggests that resistance may derive from traits or<br />

circumstances that are without major effects <strong>in</strong> the absence of the relevant<br />

risk or stress. The ability to be focused, organized, and able to work a way<br />

through the service system may be a skill that proves more useful to parents<br />

of children with ID than to their counterparts rear<strong>in</strong>g typically develop<strong>in</strong>g<br />

children. Thus, it may be a protective factor and related to better adaptation<br />

<strong>in</strong> parents who need this skill than <strong>in</strong> those who do not.


Author's personal copy<br />

304 Mal<strong>in</strong> B. Olsson<br />

Resistance may also derive from physiological or psychological cop<strong>in</strong>g<br />

processes rather than external risk or protective factors. Research needs to<br />

move away from a focus on external risks to a focus on how these external<br />

risks are dealt with by the <strong>in</strong>dividual <strong>in</strong> the form of cop<strong>in</strong>g mechanisms,<br />

mental sets, and the operation of personal agency. Studies suggest that<br />

personal agency and a concern to overcome adversity, a self-reflective<br />

style and a commitment to relationships are important features <strong>in</strong> resilient<br />

people. Parents who report positive aspects of hav<strong>in</strong>g a child with ID usually<br />

mention a positive focus on their close relationships and hav<strong>in</strong>g met new<br />

and <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g people whom they would not have met without their child<br />

with ID. Delayed resilience may also occur, deriv<strong>in</strong>g from ‘‘turn<strong>in</strong>g po<strong>in</strong>t’’<br />

experiences <strong>in</strong> life, sometimes long after the <strong>in</strong>itial risk experience. Such<br />

transformational experiences have been described <strong>in</strong> the literature on parents<br />

of children with ID by Wilgosh and Scorgie (2006).<br />

As already mentioned, judg<strong>in</strong>g the resilience of a system requires criteria<br />

for identify<strong>in</strong>g whether the system is do<strong>in</strong>g whatever it is supposed to be<br />

do<strong>in</strong>g. In research on resilience <strong>in</strong> parents and families, it is important to<br />

critically ask what a resilient outcome is. Good parental mental health and<br />

well-be<strong>in</strong>g might be a relevant measure of resilience, as can quality of life.<br />

Parent<strong>in</strong>g behaviors, parent–child attachment, or parental <strong>in</strong>volvement <strong>in</strong><br />

different life spheres might be just as relevant as would be a comb<strong>in</strong>ation of<br />

several of these. Studies on resilience beyond the traditional well-be<strong>in</strong>g<br />

paradigm are necessary. A resilience approach requires researchers to<br />

develop methods of assessment and analyses of competence, assets,<br />

resources, promotive, and protective factors, and the diagnosis of resilience<br />

<strong>in</strong> addition to depression, risk, vulnerability, and stressors.<br />

6.1. Design improvements<br />

Several research designs and statistical methods have been used <strong>in</strong> resilience<br />

research <strong>in</strong> general (see Masten & Obradovic, 2006, for a review), but only a<br />

few have dom<strong>in</strong>ated <strong>in</strong> the field of families with children with ID.<br />

The follow<strong>in</strong>g designs should be considered to improve research on resilience<br />

<strong>in</strong> parents of children with ID.<br />

Interaction f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs (adversity a moderat<strong>in</strong>g variable) are by far the<br />

most commonly used ways of study<strong>in</strong>g risk and protective factors <strong>in</strong> families<br />

of children with ID. Interaction f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs highlight the strik<strong>in</strong>g exception<br />

of <strong>in</strong>dividuals with high adversity scores whose adaptive success appeared to<br />

be ‘‘off the gradient’’ (better than predicted by their level of risk). This<br />

method is, for example, used <strong>in</strong> a study by Baker et al. (2005) where it was<br />

found that maternal dispositional optimism moderated the relationship<br />

between child behavior problems and maternal depression. Interaction<br />

effects are conceptually very <strong>in</strong>trigu<strong>in</strong>g; <strong>in</strong> contrast, search<strong>in</strong>g for them by<br />

us<strong>in</strong>g contrast<strong>in</strong>g control groups can sometimes be counter productive


Author's personal copy<br />

Risk and Resilience 305<br />

(Luthar et al., 2006). There is a need for more with<strong>in</strong>-group analyses with<br />

discrete groups of people at (the same level of ) risk. When researchers work<br />

with particular at-risk populations, they seek to illum<strong>in</strong>ate what makes a<br />

difference for these people—not whether these factors also affect comparison<br />

groups (Luthar et al., 2006). Comparative approaches that document<br />

group differences need to be complemented by with<strong>in</strong>-group analyses<br />

illum<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g the particularly critical process for <strong>in</strong>dividuals from given<br />

subgroups. Classic studies of resilience often identified a large risk group<br />

(such as parents of children with ID) and then compared a subgroup of<br />

<strong>in</strong>dividuals do<strong>in</strong>g well across multiple criteria of positive adaptation to<br />

another subgroup <strong>in</strong> the sample that shares similar high-risk levels but is<br />

do<strong>in</strong>g poorly <strong>in</strong> multiple ways. Comparisons of this nature often f<strong>in</strong>d strong<br />

similarities <strong>in</strong> the high competence groups, despite divergent adversity<br />

exposure, and strik<strong>in</strong>g differences between the resilient and maladaptive<br />

groups, despite shared risks or adversity exposure. As levels of adversity<br />

<strong>in</strong>crease, the differences <strong>in</strong> the resources of resilient versus maladaptive<br />

<strong>in</strong>dividuals can be even greater. There is also a notably ‘‘empty cell’’ effect<br />

<strong>in</strong> such studies, suggest<strong>in</strong>g that cases of low risk and poor adaptation are<br />

much less common than cases of high-risk and good adaptation (Masten &<br />

Obradovic, 2006).<br />

There are different ways of analyz<strong>in</strong>g the impact of <strong>in</strong>dividual or cumulative<br />

effect of risk factors (Klebanov & Brooks-Gunn, 2006). Samples can<br />

be divided based on s<strong>in</strong>gle risk factors known to be a strong predictor of<br />

poor adaptation, such as poverty or child behavior problems. The effects of<br />

risk can then be considered separately for poor and more affluent families<br />

because of the greater risk experienced as a result of poverty. Otherwise, the<br />

effects of the studied risk factor may be obscured.<br />

S<strong>in</strong>gle case studies are seldom used but can serve as powerful heuristic<br />

and communication tools, illustrat<strong>in</strong>g dramatic turn<strong>in</strong>g po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> a process<br />

when powerful risk factors are elim<strong>in</strong>ated or protective factors are boosted<br />

as, for example, <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terventions. If they are used spar<strong>in</strong>gly and with full<br />

recognition of their limitations they can enrich the more usual quantitative<br />

approaches.<br />

Rutter (2007) argues that theory driven research is the key for beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g<br />

to address causality. Causal graphs spell<strong>in</strong>g out the implications of the<br />

background knowledge or theory can lead to statistical model<strong>in</strong>g that can<br />

<strong>in</strong>crease or decrease the likelihood of the causal <strong>in</strong>ference be<strong>in</strong>g correct.<br />

Rutter (2007) also discusses the use of propensity scores to equate groups on<br />

the basis of likelihood of exposure to risk rather than controll<strong>in</strong>g for<br />

differences <strong>in</strong> risk. Sensitivity analyses to quantify how strong a confounder<br />

would have to be to overturn a causal <strong>in</strong>ference may also be very helpful.<br />

He further proposes the use of regression discont<strong>in</strong>uity as a quasiexperimental<br />

design that could be very useful for mak<strong>in</strong>g causal <strong>in</strong>ferences.


Author's personal copy<br />

306 Mal<strong>in</strong> B. Olsson<br />

6.2. Why study resilience<br />

There is consensus that parents (or at least mothers) of children with ID<br />

constitute a risk group <strong>in</strong> terms of depression and poor well-be<strong>in</strong>g (S<strong>in</strong>ger,<br />

2006). At the same time, there is also consensus that the majority of parents<br />

are do<strong>in</strong>g well. To understand the <strong>in</strong>dividual differences and the processes<br />

lead<strong>in</strong>g to these different outcomes, we need to better understand the<br />

factors associated with positive outcomes and resilience. This is also the<br />

route toward design<strong>in</strong>g and study<strong>in</strong>g the effect of <strong>in</strong>terventions aimed at<br />

improv<strong>in</strong>g the lives of families of children with ID.<br />

With very few exceptions, there is no such th<strong>in</strong>g as a s<strong>in</strong>gle necessary and<br />

sufficient cause (Rutter, 2007). Even though mothers of children with ID<br />

are at higher risk for depression, the birth of a child with ID does not<br />

‘‘cause’’ depression <strong>in</strong> mothers. It is not just that depression is only one of<br />

several possible outcomes for parents, but also that several different causal<br />

pathways may all lead to depression. Furthermore, almost all causal pathways<br />

<strong>in</strong>volve several different phases. For example, the pathway to depression<br />

does not beg<strong>in</strong> with a causal risk factor, but it must be preceded by the<br />

pathway lead<strong>in</strong>g to exposure to the risk factor. Thus, if the hypothesized<br />

cause is, say a child with ID <strong>in</strong> the family, the risk may lie <strong>in</strong> either genetic,<br />

societal <strong>in</strong>fluences or <strong>in</strong>dividual propensities or often a complex mixture of<br />

all. This complexity is further exacerbated because it is also the case that<br />

<strong>in</strong>fluences are often reciprocal, with the causal arrow runn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> both<br />

directions.<br />

Luthar et al. (2006) proposed four guidel<strong>in</strong>es for the selection of risk and<br />

protective processes <strong>in</strong> future resilience research. First, given a particular atrisk<br />

condition (such as a child with ID), there must be concerted attention<br />

to factors that are salient <strong>in</strong> that particular life context, those that affect a<br />

relatively large number of people <strong>in</strong> that group (e.g., daily hassles, service<br />

provision, or child behavior problems). Second, we should prioritize attention<br />

to <strong>in</strong>dices that are relatively malleable—risk modifiers amenable to<br />

change via external <strong>in</strong>terventions or ‘‘modifiable modifiers’’ (e.g., child<br />

behavior problems may be modified by parent<strong>in</strong>g practices). Third, focus<br />

should be afforded to <strong>in</strong>dices that tend to be relatively endur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the life<br />

situation (parent<strong>in</strong>g practices). F<strong>in</strong>ally, it is critical to attend to <strong>in</strong>dices that<br />

are generative of other assets, those that set <strong>in</strong>to motion ‘‘cascades’’ where<strong>in</strong><br />

they catalyze other protective processes (if behavior problems are prevented<br />

or m<strong>in</strong>imized the child will have less negative impact on the family and all<br />

family members can have more possibilities of engag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> positive <strong>in</strong>teractions<br />

and activities). Luthar et al. concluded that close supportive family<br />

relationships clearly meet all these four criteria, as do probably other aspects<br />

such as the formal support system <strong>in</strong> the lives of families of children with ID.<br />

The one-sided focus on maladjustment <strong>in</strong> literature on families of<br />

children with ID is addressed by Glidden (1993) <strong>in</strong> which she challenged


Author's personal copy<br />

Risk and Resilience 307<br />

the notion that families with a child with ID are always stra<strong>in</strong>ed. She argued<br />

that the persistence of the maladjustment hypothesis has cont<strong>in</strong>ued despite<br />

substantial methodological problems with the research on which the belief<br />

is based. Overall adjustment or maladjustment is dependent not only on the<br />

presence or absence of stress but also on the presence or absence of positive<br />

outcomes. Positive outcomes can coexist with negative outcomes but may<br />

never get measured because <strong>in</strong>vestigators are not hypothesiz<strong>in</strong>g that they are<br />

present. The discourse of disability as a tragedy has been reproduced by<br />

researchers by the focus on maladjustment. The resilience perspective offers<br />

a possibility of acknowledg<strong>in</strong>g the extra stress and stra<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> families of<br />

children with ID at the same time as it puts the focus on the factors that<br />

contribute to positive outcomes <strong>in</strong> a majority of the parents.<br />

7. Conclusion<br />

In this chapter, I have proposed a theoretical model of resilience to<br />

help <strong>in</strong>terpret past f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs and guide future research on families of children<br />

with ID. Thus, far most family research has zoomed to a narrow focus on<br />

factors important to understand the 30% of mothers with depressive symptoms.<br />

I th<strong>in</strong>k we need to zoom out to see the bigger picture. Risk and<br />

protective factors should be addressed simultaneously with robust outcome<br />

measures that allow for variation <strong>in</strong> the negative as well as <strong>in</strong> the positive end<br />

of the well-be<strong>in</strong>g spectrum, <strong>in</strong> both parents. We need to do more with<strong>in</strong><br />

subgroup analyses on parents who are do<strong>in</strong>g well and those do<strong>in</strong>g less well.<br />

To do this, we need bigger samples. We also need to <strong>in</strong>clude fathers <strong>in</strong><br />

research s<strong>in</strong>ce there is consistent evidence that the experience and outcomes<br />

are sometimes different for mothers and fathers. Also essential is more<br />

research on the importance of positive relations between the child with<br />

ID and his or her parents, and <strong>in</strong>tervention studies designed to strengthen<br />

this relationship.<br />

REFERENCES<br />

Antonovsky, A. (1987). Unravel<strong>in</strong>g the mystery of health: How people manage stress and stay well<br />

(1st ed.). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.<br />

Baker, B., Blacher, J., & Olsson, M. B. (2005). Preschool children with and without<br />

developmental delay: Behavioural problems, parents’ optimism and well-be<strong>in</strong>g. Journal<br />

of Intellectual Disability Reseach, 49, 575–590.<br />

Baker, B. L., McIntyre, L. L., Blacher, J., Crnic, K., Edelbrock, C., & Low, C. (2003).<br />

Pre-school children with and without developmental delay: Behaviour problems and<br />

parent<strong>in</strong>g stress over time. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 47, 217–230.<br />

Barnett, S., & Boyce, G. (1995). Effects of children with Down syndrome on parents’<br />

activities. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 100, 115–127.


Author's personal copy<br />

308 Mal<strong>in</strong> B. Olsson<br />

Baxter, C., Cumm<strong>in</strong>s, R., & Polak, S. (1995). A longitud<strong>in</strong>al study of parental stress and<br />

support: From diagnosis of disability to leav<strong>in</strong>g school. International Journal of Disability,<br />

Development and Education, 42, 125–136.<br />

Beck, A., Hast<strong>in</strong>gs, R., & Daley, D. (2004). Pro-social behavior and behavior problems<br />

<strong>in</strong>dependently predict maternal stress. Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Disability, 29,<br />

339–349.<br />

Beckman, P. (1983). Characteristics of handicapped <strong>in</strong>fants. A study of the relationship<br />

between child characteristics and stress as reported by mothers. American Journal of Mental<br />

Deficiency, 91, 313–320.<br />

Beckman, P. (1991). Comparison of mothers’ and fathers’ perceptions of the effect of young<br />

children with and without disabilities. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 95,<br />

585–595.<br />

Beresford, B. (1996). Cop<strong>in</strong>g with the care of a severely disabled child. Health and Social Care<br />

<strong>in</strong> the Community, 4, 30–40.<br />

Beresford, B. A. (1994). Resources and strategies: How parents cope with the care of a<br />

disabled child. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 171–209.<br />

Birenbaum, A. (2002). Poverty, welfare reform and disproportionate rates of disability<br />

among children. Mental Retardation, 40, 212–218.<br />

Bir<strong>in</strong>gen, Z., Fidler, D., Barrett, C., & Kubicek, L. (2005). Apply<strong>in</strong>g the emotional availability<br />

scales to children with disabilities. Infant Mental Health Journal, 26, 369–391.<br />

Blacher, J. (1984). Sequential stages of parental adjustment to the birth of a child with<br />

handicap: Fact or artifact. Mental Retardation, 22, 55–68.<br />

Blacher, J., & Baker, B. (2002). The best of AAMR. Families and disability. Wash<strong>in</strong>gton, DC:<br />

American Association on Mental Retardation.<br />

Blacher, J., & Lopez, S. (1997). Contributions to depression <strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>a Mothers with and<br />

without children with retardation: Implications for care-giv<strong>in</strong>g. Family Relations, 46,<br />

325–334.<br />

Blacher, J., & McIntyre, L. (2006). Syndrome specificity and behavioural disorders <strong>in</strong> young<br />

adults with <strong>in</strong>tellectual disability: Cultural differences <strong>in</strong> family impact. Journal of Intellectual<br />

Disability Research, 50, 184–198.<br />

Blacher, J., Neece, C., & Paczkowski, E. (2005). Families and <strong>in</strong>tellectual disability. Current<br />

Op<strong>in</strong>ion <strong>in</strong> Psychiatry, 18, 507–513.<br />

Breslau, N., Staruch, K. S., & Mortimer, E. A. (1982). Psychological distress <strong>in</strong> mothers of<br />

disabled children. American Journal of Diseases of Children, 136, 682–686.<br />

Bristol, M., Gallagher, J., & Holt, K. (1993). Maternal depressive symptoms <strong>in</strong> autism:<br />

Response to psychoeducational <strong>in</strong>tervention. Rehabilitation Psychology, 38, 3–10.<br />

Bristol, M., Gallagher, J., & Schopler, E. (1988). Mothers and fathers of young developmentally<br />

disabled and nondisabled boys: <strong>Adaptation</strong> and spousal support. Developmental<br />

Psychology, 24, 441–451.<br />

Cahill, B., & Glidden, L. M. (1996). Influence of child diagnosis on family and parental<br />

function<strong>in</strong>g: Down syndrome versus other disabilities. American Journal on Mental Retardation,<br />

101, 149–160.<br />

Caldwell, J., & Heller, T. (2003). Management of respite and personal assistance services <strong>in</strong> a<br />

consumer-directed family support programme. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research,<br />

47, 352–366.<br />

Cameron, S., Dodson, L., & Day, D. (1991). Stress <strong>in</strong> parents of develompentally delayed<br />

and non-delayed preschool children. Canada’s Mental Health, 39, 13–17.<br />

Carp<strong>in</strong>iello, B., Piras, A., Pariante, C., & Carta, M. (1995). Psychiatric morbidity and family<br />

burden among parents of disabled children. Psychiatric Services, 46, 940–942.<br />

Compas, B. (2006). Psychobiological processes of stress and cop<strong>in</strong>g. Implications for resilience<br />

<strong>in</strong> children and adolescents – comments on the papers of Romeo & McEwen and<br />

Fisher et al. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094, 226–234.


Author's personal copy<br />

Risk and Resilience 309<br />

Corrice, A. M., & Glidden, L. M. (2008). The Down syndrome advantage: Fact or fiction.<br />

Manuscript submitted for publication.<br />

Cowen, P. S., & Reed, D. A. (2002). Effects of respite care for children with developmental<br />

disabilities: Evaluation of an <strong>in</strong>tervention for at risk families. Public Health Nurs<strong>in</strong>g, 19,<br />

272–283.<br />

Dale, E., Jahoda, A., & Knott, F. (2006). Mothers’ attributions follow<strong>in</strong>g their child’s<br />

diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder: Explor<strong>in</strong>g l<strong>in</strong>ks with maternal levels of stress,<br />

depression and expectations about their child’s future. Autism, 10, 463–479.<br />

Deater-Deckard, K., & Scarr, S. (1996). Parent<strong>in</strong>g stress among dual earner mothers and<br />

fathers: Are there gender differences. Journal of Family Psychology, 10, 45–59.<br />

Douma, J. C. H., Dekker, M. C., & Koot, H. M. (2006). Support<strong>in</strong>g parents of youths with<br />

<strong>in</strong>tellectual disabilities and psychopathology. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 50,<br />

570–581.<br />

Dumaret, A. C., De Vigan, C., Julian-Reynier, C., Goujars, J. R. D., & Aymé, S. (1998).<br />

Adoption and foster<strong>in</strong>g of babies with Down syndrome: A cohort of 593 cases. Prenatal<br />

Diagnosis, 18, 437–445.<br />

Dumas, J., Wolf, L., Fisman, S., & Culligan, A. (1991). Parent<strong>in</strong>g stress, child behavior<br />

problems, and dysphoria <strong>in</strong> parents of children with autism, Down syndrome, behavior<br />

disorders, and normal development. Exceptionality, 2, 97–110.<br />

Dunst, C., & Dempsey, I. (2007). Family-professional partnership and parent<strong>in</strong>g competence,<br />

confidence, and enjoyment. International Journal of Disability, Development and<br />

Education, 54, 305–318.<br />

Dykens, E., & Hodapp, R. (2001). Research <strong>in</strong> mental retardation: Toward an etiological<br />

approach. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 49–71.<br />

Dyson, L. L. (1991). Families of young children with handicaps: Parental stress and family<br />

function<strong>in</strong>g. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 95, 623–629.<br />

Dyson, L. L. (1993). Response to the presence of a child with disabilities: Parental stress and<br />

family function<strong>in</strong>g over time. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 98, 207–218.<br />

Dyson, L. L. (1997). Fathers and mothers of school-age children with developmental<br />

disabilities: Parental stress, family function<strong>in</strong>g, and social support. American Journal on<br />

Mental Retardation, 102, 267–279.<br />

Eisenhower, A., Baker, B., & Blacher, J. (2005). Preschool children with <strong>in</strong>tellectual<br />

disability: Syndrome specificity, behavior problems, and maternal well-be<strong>in</strong>g. Journal of<br />

Intellectual Disability Research, 49, 657–671.<br />

Eisenhower, A., & Blacher, J. (2006). Mothers of young adults with <strong>in</strong>tellectual disability:<br />

Multiple roles, ethnicity and well-be<strong>in</strong>g. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 50,<br />

905–916.<br />

Ellis, J. T., Luiselli, J. K., & Amirault, D. (2002). Families of children with developmental<br />

disabilities: Assessment and comparison of self-reported needs <strong>in</strong> relation to situational<br />

variables. Journal of Physical and Developmental Disability, 14, 191–202.<br />

Emerson, E. (2003). Mothers of children and adolescents with <strong>in</strong>tellectual disability: Social and<br />

economic situation, mental health status, and the self assessed social and psychological impact<br />

of the child’s difficulties. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 47, 385–399.<br />

Emerson, E., Hatton, C., Llewllyn, B. J., & Graham, H. (2006). Socio-economic position,<br />

household composition, health status and <strong>in</strong>dicators of the well-be<strong>in</strong>g of mothers of<br />

children with and without <strong>in</strong>tellectual disabilities. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research,<br />

50, 862–873.<br />

Erw<strong>in</strong>, E. J., & Soodak, L. C. (1995). I never knew I could stand up to the system: Families’<br />

perspectives on pursu<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>clusive education. Journal of the Association for Persons with<br />

Severe Handicaps, 20, 136–146.<br />

Fisman, S., Wolf, L., & Noh, S. (1989). Marital Intimacy <strong>in</strong> parents of exceptional children.<br />

Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 34, 519–525.


Author's personal copy<br />

310 Mal<strong>in</strong> B. Olsson<br />

Flaherty, E. M., & Glidden, L. M. (2000). Positive adjustment <strong>in</strong> parents rear<strong>in</strong>g children<br />

with Down syndrome. Early Education and Development, 11, 407–422.<br />

Gillham, J. (2000). The science of optimism and hope: Research essays <strong>in</strong> honor of Mart<strong>in</strong> E.P<br />

Seligman. Radnor, Pennsylvania: Templeton Foundation Press.<br />

Glidden, L. M. (1993). What we do not know about families with children who have<br />

developmental disabilities: Questionnaire on Resources and Stress as a case study.<br />

American Journal on Mental Retardation, 97, 481–495.<br />

Glidden, L. M., Bill<strong>in</strong>gs, F. J., & Jobe, B. M. (2006). Personality, cop<strong>in</strong>g style and well-be<strong>in</strong>g<br />

of parents rear<strong>in</strong>g children with developmental disabilities. Journal of Intellectual Disability<br />

Research, 50, 949–962.<br />

Glidden, L. M., & Cahill, B. M. (1998). Successful adoption of children with Down<br />

syndrome and other developmental disabilities. Adoption Quarterly, 1, 27–43.<br />

Glidden, L. M., & Schoolcraft, S. A. (2003). Depression: Its trajectory and correlates <strong>in</strong><br />

mothers rear<strong>in</strong>g children with <strong>in</strong>tellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability<br />

Research, 47, 250–263.<br />

Gowen, J., Johnson-Mart<strong>in</strong>, N., Goldman, B., & Appelbaum, M. (1989). Feel<strong>in</strong>gs of<br />

depression and parent<strong>in</strong>g competence of mothers of handicapped and nonhandicapped<br />

<strong>in</strong>fants: A longitud<strong>in</strong>al study. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 94, 259–271.<br />

Granlund, M., Roll-Pettersson, L., Steensson, A., & Sund<strong>in</strong>, M. (1994). Att leva och arbeta<br />

med handikappade små barn. Stockholm: Ala, FUB:s stiftelse.<br />

Grant, G., Ramchandran, P., McGrath, M., Nolan, M., & Keady, J. (1998). Rewards and<br />

gratifications among family caregivers: Towards a ref<strong>in</strong>ed model of car<strong>in</strong>g and cop<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 42, 58–71.<br />

Gray, D., & Holden, W. (1992). Psychosocial well-be<strong>in</strong>g among the parents of children with<br />

autism. Australia and New Zealand Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 18, 83–93.<br />

Green, S. E. (2004). Attitudes toward control <strong>in</strong> uncontrollable situations: The multidimensional<br />

impact of health locus of control on the well-be<strong>in</strong>g of mothers of children with<br />

disabilities. Sociological Inquiries, 74, 20–49.<br />

Green, S. E. (2007). We’re tired not sad: Benefits and burdens of mother<strong>in</strong>g a child with a<br />

disability. Social Science and Medic<strong>in</strong>e, 64, 150–163.<br />

Greenberg, T. (2006). Promot<strong>in</strong>g resilience <strong>in</strong> children and youth. Preventive <strong>in</strong>terventions<br />

and their <strong>in</strong>terface with neuroscience. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094,<br />

139–150.<br />

Graungaard, A., & Skov, L. (2006). Why do we need a diagnosis A qualitative study of<br />

parents’ experiences, cop<strong>in</strong>g and needs, when the newborn child is severely disabled.<br />

Child: Care, Health and Development, 33, 296–307.<br />

Gustavsson, A. (1989). Samhällsideal och Föräldraansvar. Om föräldrar till förståndshandikappade<br />

barn. Stockholm: Nordstedts Förlag AB.<br />

Hadadian, A. (1994). Stress and social support <strong>in</strong> fathers and mothers of young children with<br />

and without disabilities. Early Education and Development, 5, 226–235.<br />

Harris, V. S., & McHale, S. M. (1989). Family life problems, daily caregiv<strong>in</strong>g activities, and<br />

the psychological well-be<strong>in</strong>g of mothers of mentally retarded children. American Journal on<br />

Mental Retardation, 94, 231–239.<br />

Hassall, R., & Rose, J. (2005). Parental cognitions and adaptation to the demands of car<strong>in</strong>g<br />

for a child with <strong>in</strong>tellectual disability: A review of the literature and implications for<br />

cl<strong>in</strong>ical <strong>in</strong>terventions. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 33, 71–88.<br />

Hassall, R., Rose, J., & McDonald, J. (2005). Parent<strong>in</strong>g stress <strong>in</strong> mothers of children with an<br />

<strong>in</strong>tellectual disability: The effects of parental cognitions <strong>in</strong> relation to child characteristics<br />

and family support. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 49, 405–418.<br />

Hast<strong>in</strong>gs, R., & Johnson, E. (2001). Stress <strong>in</strong> UK families conduct<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tensive home-based<br />

behavioral <strong>in</strong>tervention for their young child with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental<br />

Disorders, 31, 327–336.


Author's personal copy<br />

Risk and Resilience 311<br />

Hast<strong>in</strong>gs, R. P. (2003). Child behaviour problems and partner mental health as correlates of<br />

stress <strong>in</strong> mothers and fathers of children with autism. Journal of Intellectual Disability<br />

Research, 47, 231–237.<br />

Hast<strong>in</strong>gs, R. P., & Brown, T. (2002). Behavior problems of children with autism, parental<br />

self-efficacy, and mental health. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 107, 222–232.<br />

Hast<strong>in</strong>gs, R. P., & Taunt, H. M. (2002). Positive perceptions <strong>in</strong> families of children with<br />

developmental disabilities. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 107, 116–127.<br />

Harwood, K., McLean, N., & Durk<strong>in</strong>, K. (2007). First-time mothers’ expectations of<br />

parenthood: What happens when optimistic expectations are not matched by later<br />

experiences Developmental Psychology, 43, 1–12.<br />

Hedov, G., Anneren, G., & Wikblad, K. (2002). Swedish parents of children with Down’s<br />

syndrome. Scand<strong>in</strong>avian Journal of Car<strong>in</strong>g Science, 16, 424–430.<br />

Hedov, G., Wikblad, K., & Annerén, G. (2006). Sickness absence <strong>in</strong> Swedish parents of<br />

children with Down’s syndrome: Relation to self-perceived health, stress and sense of<br />

coherence. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 50, 546–552.<br />

Heller, T., Hsiesh, K., & Rowitz, L. (1997). Maternal and paternal caregiv<strong>in</strong>g of persons<br />

with mental retardation across the lifespan. Family Relations: Interdiscipl<strong>in</strong>ary Journal of<br />

Applied Family Studies, 46, 407–415.<br />

Herbert, M. J. (2001). The unexceptional parent. AAMR News & Notes.<br />

Herman, S., & Thompson, L. (1995). Families’ perceptions of their resources for car<strong>in</strong>g for<br />

children with developmental disabilities. Mental Retardation, 33, 73–83.<br />

Herr<strong>in</strong>g, S., Gray, K., Taffe, J., Tonge, B., Sweeney, D., & E<strong>in</strong>feld, S. (2006). Behaviour and<br />

emotional problems <strong>in</strong> toddlers with pervasive developmental disorder and developmental.<br />

Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 50, 874–882.<br />

Hodapp, R. M., Dykens, E. M., & Mas<strong>in</strong>o, L. L. (1997). Families of children with Prader-<br />

Willi syndrome: Stress-support and relations to child characteristics. Journal of Autism and<br />

Developmental Disorders, 27, 11–24.<br />

Hudson, A. M., Matthews, J. M., Gavidia-Payne, S. T., Cameron, C. A. M., Mildon, R. L.,<br />

Radler, G. A., et al. (2003). Evaluation of an <strong>in</strong>tervention system for parents of children<br />

with <strong>in</strong>tellectual disability and challeng<strong>in</strong>g behaviour. Journal of Intellectual Disability<br />

Research, 47, 238–249.<br />

Innocenti, M. S., Huh, K., & Boyce, G. C. (1992). Families of children with disabilities:<br />

Normative data and other considerations on parent<strong>in</strong>g stress. Topics <strong>in</strong> Early Childhood<br />

Special Education, 12, 403–427.<br />

Judge, S. L. (1998). Parental cop<strong>in</strong>g strategies and strengths <strong>in</strong> families of young children<br />

with disabilities. Family Relations, 47, 263–268.<br />

Kim, H. W., Greenberg, J. S., Seltzer, M. M., & Krauss, M. W. (2003). The role of cop<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong><br />

ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the psychological well-be<strong>in</strong>g of mothers of adults with <strong>in</strong>tellectual disability<br />

and mental illness. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 47, 313–327.<br />

Klebanov, P., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2006). Cumulative, human capital, and psychological risk<br />

<strong>in</strong> the context of early <strong>in</strong>tervention. L<strong>in</strong>ks with IQ at age 3,5, and 8. Annals of the<br />

New York Academy of Sciences, 1094, 63–82.<br />

Knox, M., Parmenter, T. R., Atk<strong>in</strong>son, N., & Yazbeck, M. (2000). Family control:<br />

The views of families who have a child with an <strong>in</strong>tellectual disability. Journal of Applied<br />

Research <strong>in</strong> Intellectual Disabilities, 13, 17–28.<br />

Kvande, K., & Knutheim, A. (1995). Samtaler om mestr<strong>in</strong>g. Focus På Familjen, 4, 217–224.<br />

Kwai-sang Yau, M., & Li-Tsang, C. (1999). Adjustment and adaptation <strong>in</strong> parents of<br />

children with developmental disability <strong>in</strong> two-parent families: A review of the characteristics<br />

and attributes. The British Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 45, 38–51.<br />

Lärka Paul<strong>in</strong>, S., Bernehäll Claesson, I., & Brod<strong>in</strong>, J. (2001). Hur ser stödet till familjer med<br />

barn med muskelsjukdomar ut Socialmedis<strong>in</strong>sk Tidskrift, 2, 161–171.


Author's personal copy<br />

312 Mal<strong>in</strong> B. Olsson<br />

Larson, E. (1998). Refram<strong>in</strong>g the mean<strong>in</strong>g of disability to families: The embrace of paradox.<br />

Social Science Medic<strong>in</strong>e, 47, 865–875.<br />

Lazarus, R., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal and cop<strong>in</strong>g. New York: Spr<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Lazarus, R. (1999). Stress and emotion: A new synthesis. New York: Spr<strong>in</strong>ger.<br />

Lazarus, R. (2000). Toward better research on stress and cop<strong>in</strong>g. American Psychologist, 55,<br />

665–673.<br />

Lazarus, R. (2003). The Lazarus manifesto for positive psychology and psychology <strong>in</strong><br />

general. Psychological Inquiry, 14, 173–189.<br />

Lecavalier, L., Leone, S., & Wiltz, J. (2006). The impact of behaviour problems on caregiver<br />

stress <strong>in</strong> young people with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Intellectual Disability<br />

Research, 50, 172–183.<br />

Lewis, P., Abbeduto, L., Murphy, M., Richmond, E., Giles, N., Bruno, L., et al. (2006).<br />

Psychological well-be<strong>in</strong>g of mothers of youth with fragile X syndrome: Syndrome specificity<br />

and with<strong>in</strong>-syndrome variability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 50, 894–904.<br />

Luescher, J., Dede, D., Gitten, J., Fennell, E., & Maria, B. (1999). Parental burden, cop<strong>in</strong>g<br />

and family function<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> primary caregivers of children with Joubert syndrome. Journal of<br />

Child Neurology, 14, 642–648.<br />

Luthar, S., Sawyer, J., & Brown, P. (2006). Conceptual issues <strong>in</strong> studies of resilience. Past,<br />

present, and future research. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094, 105–115.<br />

Mactavish, J. B., & Schleien, S. J. (2004). Re-<strong>in</strong>ject<strong>in</strong>g spontaneity and balance <strong>in</strong> family life:<br />

Parents’ perspectives on recreation <strong>in</strong> families that <strong>in</strong>clude children with developmental<br />

disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 48, 123–141.<br />

Masten, A., & Obradovic, J. (2006). Competence and resilience <strong>in</strong> development. Annals of<br />

the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094, 13–27.<br />

McIntyre, L., & Phaneuf, L. (2008). A three-tier model of parent education <strong>in</strong> early<br />

childhood: Apply<strong>in</strong>g a problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g model. Topics <strong>in</strong> Early Childhood Special Education,<br />

27, 214–222.<br />

Mitchell, W. (2007). Research review: The role of grandparents <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>tergenerational support<br />

for families with disabled children: A review of the literature. Child and Family Social<br />

Work, 12, 94–101.<br />

Most, D. E., Fidler, D. J., Laforce-Booth, C., & Kelly, J. (2006). Stress trajectories <strong>in</strong><br />

mothers of young children with Down syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research,<br />

50, 501–514.<br />

Nachshen, J. S., & M<strong>in</strong>nes, P. (2005). Empowerment <strong>in</strong> parents of school-aged children<br />

with and without developmental disabilities. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 49,<br />

889–904.<br />

Nooj<strong>in</strong>, A. B., & Wallander, J. L. (1997). Perceived problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g ability, stress, and<br />

cop<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> mothers of children with physical disabilities: Potential cognitive <strong>in</strong>fluences on<br />

adjustment. International Journal of Behavioral Medic<strong>in</strong>e, 4, 415–432.<br />

Oelofsen, N., & Richardson, P. (2006). Sense of coherence and parent<strong>in</strong>g stress <strong>in</strong> mothers<br />

and fathers of preschool children with developmental disability. Journal of Intellectual &<br />

Developmental Disability, 31, 1–12.<br />

Olauson, A., & Hvalstedt, C. (2001). Ågrenska: Idé -och startplats för familjeaktiviteter på<br />

1980-talet - idag vidareutbildn<strong>in</strong>gs- och nationellt kompetenscentrum. Socialmedic<strong>in</strong>sk<br />

Tidskrift, 2, 111–116.<br />

Olsson, M. B., & Hwang, C. P. (2001). Depression <strong>in</strong> mothers and fathers of children with<br />

<strong>in</strong>tellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 45, 535–543.<br />

Olsson, M. B., & Hwang, C. P. (2002). Sense of coherence <strong>in</strong> parents of children with different<br />

developmental disabilities. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 46, 548–559.<br />

Olsson, M. B., & Hwang, C. P. (2003). Influence of macrostructure of society on the life<br />

situation of families with a child with <strong>in</strong>tellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability<br />

Research, 47, 328–341.


Author's personal copy<br />

Risk and Resilience 313<br />

Olsson, M. B., & Hwang, C. P. (2006). Well-be<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong>volvement <strong>in</strong> paid work and division<br />

of child-care <strong>in</strong> parents of children with <strong>in</strong>tellectual disabilities <strong>in</strong> Sweden. Journal of<br />

Intellectual Disability Research, 50, 963–969.<br />

Olsson, M. B. Hwang, C. P. (<strong>in</strong> press). Socioeconomic and psychological variables as risk<br />

and protective factors for parental well-be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> families of children with ID Journal of<br />

Intellectual Disability Research.<br />

Paczkowski, E., & Baker, B. L. (2007). Parent<strong>in</strong>g children with and without developmental<br />

delay: The role of self-mastery. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 51, 435–446.<br />

Phaneuf, L., & McIntyre, L. L. (2007). Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40, 37–741.<br />

Pianta, R., Marv<strong>in</strong>, R., Britner, P., & Borowitz, K. (1996). Mothers’ resolution of their<br />

children’s diagnosis: Organized patterns of caregiv<strong>in</strong>g representations. Infant Mental<br />

Health Journal, 17, 239–256.<br />

Plant, K. M., & Sanders, M. R. (2007). Predictors of care-giver stress <strong>in</strong> families of<br />

preschool-aged children with developmental disabilities. Journal of Intellectual Disability<br />

Research, 51, 109–124.<br />

Poston, D. J., & Turnbull, A. P. (2004). Role of spirituality and religion <strong>in</strong> family quality of<br />

life for families of children with disabilities. Education and Tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Developmental<br />

Disabilities, 39, 95–108.<br />

Reddon, J., McDonald, L., & Kysela, G. (1992). Parental cop<strong>in</strong>g and family stress I:<br />

Resources for and function<strong>in</strong>g of families with a preschool child hav<strong>in</strong>g a developmental<br />

disability. Early Child Development and Care, 83, 1–26.<br />

Ricci, L. A., & Hodapp, R. M. (2003). Fathers of children with Down’s syndrome versus<br />

other types of <strong>in</strong>tellectual disability: Perceptions, stress and <strong>in</strong>volvement. Journal of<br />

Intellectual Disability Research, 47, 273–284S.<br />

Risdal, D., & S<strong>in</strong>ger, G. H. S. (2004). Marital adjustment <strong>in</strong> parents of children with<br />

disabilities: A historical review and meta-analysis. Research and Practice for Persons with<br />

Severe Disabilities. Special Family and Disability, 29, 95–103.<br />

Robertson, J., Emerson, E., Hatton, C., Elliot, J., McIntosh, B., Swift, P., et al. (2007).<br />

Person-centered plann<strong>in</strong>g: Factors associated with successful outcomes for people with<br />

<strong>in</strong>tellectual disabilities. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 51, 232–243.<br />

Romer, L. T., & Richardson, M. L. N. D. (2002). Provid<strong>in</strong>g family support through<br />

community guides. Mental Retardation, 40, 191–200.<br />

Rutter, M. (2006). Implications of resilience concepts for scientific understand<strong>in</strong>g. Annals of<br />

the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094, 1–12.<br />

Rutter, M. (2007). Proceed<strong>in</strong>g from observed correlation to causal <strong>in</strong>ference. The use of<br />

natural experiments. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 377–395.<br />

Sadetzki, S., Chetrit, A., Akste<strong>in</strong>, E., Ke<strong>in</strong>an, L., Luxenburg, O., & Modan, B. (2000).<br />

Rel<strong>in</strong>quishment of <strong>in</strong>fants with Down syndrome <strong>in</strong> Israel: Trends by time. American<br />

Journal on Mental Retardation, 105, 480–485.<br />

Saloviita, T., Itäl<strong>in</strong>na, M., & Le<strong>in</strong>onen, E. (2003). Expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the parental stress of fathers<br />

and mothers car<strong>in</strong>g for a child with <strong>in</strong>tellectual disability: A double ABCX Model. Journal<br />

of Intellectual Disability Research, 47, 300–312.<br />

Sameroff, A., & Rosenblum, K. (2006). Psychosocial constra<strong>in</strong>ts on the development of<br />

resilience. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094, 116–124.<br />

Sanders, J. L., & Morgan, S. B. (1997). Family stress and adjustment as perceived by parents<br />

of children with autism or Down syndrome: Implications for <strong>in</strong>tervention. Child and<br />

Family Behavior Therapy, 19, 15–32.<br />

Scorgie, K., & Sobsey, D. (2000). Transformational outcomes associated with parent<strong>in</strong>g<br />

children who have disabilities. Mental Retardation, 38, 195–206.<br />

Seltzer, M. M., Floyd, F. J., & H<strong>in</strong>des, A. R. (2004). Research methods <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>tellectual<br />

disabilities: The family context. In E. Emerson, C. Hatton, T. Thompson, &<br />

T. Parmenter (Eds.), International Handbook of Applied Research Methods <strong>in</strong> Intellectual Disabilities<br />

(pp. 237–260). New York: Guilford Press.


Author's personal copy<br />

314 Mal<strong>in</strong> B. Olsson<br />

Shapiro, J. (1983). Family reactions and cop<strong>in</strong>g strategies <strong>in</strong> response to the physically ill or<br />

handicapped child: A review. Social Science and Medic<strong>in</strong>e, 17, 913–931.<br />

Sharpley, C., & Bitsika, V. (1997). Influence of gender, parental health, and perceived<br />

expertise of assistance upon stress, anxiety, and depression among parents of children<br />

with autism. Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Disability, 22, 19–29.<br />

Shearn, J., & Todd, S. (2000). Maternal employment and family responsibilities: The<br />

perspective of mothers of children with <strong>in</strong>tellectual disabilities. Journal of Applied Research<br />

<strong>in</strong> Intellectual Disabilities, 13, 109–31.<br />

Sh<strong>in</strong>, J. Y. (2002). Social support for families of children with mental retardation: Comparison<br />

between Korea and the United States. Mental Retardation, 40, 103–118.<br />

Siklos, S., & Kerns, K. A. (2007). Assess<strong>in</strong>g the diagnostic experience of a small sample of parents<br />

of children with autism spectrum disorders. Research <strong>in</strong> Developmental Disabilities, 28, 9–22.<br />

S<strong>in</strong>ger, G. (2006). Meta-analysis of comparative studies of depression <strong>in</strong> mothers of children<br />

with and without developmental disabilities. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 11,<br />

155–169.<br />

Sloper, P. (1999). Models of service support for parents of disabled children. What do we<br />

know What do we need to know. Child: Care, Health and Development, 25, 85–99.<br />

Sloper, P., & Turner, S. (1993). Risk and resistance factors <strong>in</strong> the adaptation of parents of children<br />

with severe physical disability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 167–188.<br />

Snyder, R., & Lopez, S. J. (2002). Handbook of positive psychology. New York: Oxford<br />

University Press.<br />

Soresi, S., Nota, L., & Ferrari, L. (2007). Considerations on support that can <strong>in</strong>crease the<br />

quality of life of parents of children with disabilities. Journal of Policy and Practice <strong>in</strong><br />

Intellectual Disabilities, 4, 248–251.<br />

Sta<strong>in</strong>ton, T., & Besser, H. (1998). The positive impact of children with an <strong>in</strong>tellectual<br />

disability on the family. Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disability, 23, 56–70.<br />

Stallard, P., & Lenton, S. (1992). How satisfied are parents of pre-school children who have<br />

special needs with the services they have received A consumer survey. Child: Care,<br />

Health and Development, 18, 197–205.<br />

Summers, J. A., Marquis, J., Mannan, H., Turnbull, A., Flem<strong>in</strong>g, K., Poston, D., et al.<br />

(2007). Relationship of perceived adequacy of services, family-professional partnerships,<br />

and family quality of life <strong>in</strong> early childhood service programmes. International Journal of<br />

Disability, Development and Education, 54, 319–338.<br />

Taanila, A., Syrjälä, L., Kokkonen, J., & Järvel<strong>in</strong>, M. R. (2002). Cop<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> parents with<br />

physically and/or <strong>in</strong>tellectually disabled children. Child: Care, Health and Development, 28,<br />

73–86.<br />

Todd, S., & Sheam, J. (1996). Time and the person: The impact of support service on the<br />

lives of parents of adults with <strong>in</strong>tellectual disabilities. Journal of Applied Research <strong>in</strong> Intellectual<br />

Disabilities, 9, 40–60.<br />

Tunali, B., & Power, T. G. (1993). Creat<strong>in</strong>g satisfaction: A psychological perspective on<br />

stress and cop<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> families of handicapped children. Journal of Child Psychology and<br />

Psychiatry, 34, 945–957.<br />

Turnbull, A., & Ruef, M. (1996). Family perspectives on problem behaviour. Mental<br />

Retardation, 34, 280–293.<br />

Van Riper, M., Ryff, C., & Pridham, K. (1992). Parental and family well-be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> families of<br />

children with Down syndrome: A cooperative study. Research <strong>in</strong> Nurs<strong>in</strong>g and Health, 15,<br />

227–235.<br />

Veisson, M. (1999). Depression symptoms and emotional states <strong>in</strong> parents of disabled and<br />

non-disabled children. Social Behavior and Personality, 27, 87–98.<br />

Wallander, J. L., & Varni, J. W. (1998). Effects of pediatric chronic physical disorder on child<br />

and family adjustment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 29–46.


Author's personal copy<br />

Risk and Resilience 315<br />

Wallander, J. L., & Venters, T. L. (1995). Perceived role restriction and adjustment of<br />

mothers of children with chronic physical disability. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 20,<br />

619–632.<br />

Weiss, M. J. (2002). Hard<strong>in</strong>ess and social support as predictors of stress <strong>in</strong> mothers of typical<br />

children, children with autism, and children with mental retardation. Autism, 6, 115–130.<br />

White, N., & Hast<strong>in</strong>gs, R. P. (2004). Social and professional support for parents of adolescents<br />

with severe <strong>in</strong>tellectual disabilities. Journal of Applied Research <strong>in</strong> Intellectual Disability,<br />

17, 181–190.<br />

Wheeler, A., Hatton, D., Reichardt, A., & Bailey, D. (2007). Correlates of maternal<br />

behaviors <strong>in</strong> mothers of children with fragile X syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability<br />

Research, 51, 447–462.<br />

Wilgosh, L., & Scorgie, K. (2006). Theoretical model for conceptualiz<strong>in</strong>g cross-cultural<br />

applications and <strong>in</strong>tervention strategies for parents of children with disabilities. Journal of<br />

Policy and Practice <strong>in</strong> Intellectual Disabilities, 3, 211–218.<br />

Williams, K. R., & Wishart, J. G. (2003). The Son-Rise Program <strong>in</strong>tervention for autism.<br />

Journal of Intellectual Disability Reseach, 47, 291–299.<br />

Wyngaarden Krauss, M. (1993). Child-related and parent<strong>in</strong>g stress: Similarities and differences<br />

between mothers and fathers of children with disabilities. American Journal on Mental<br />

Retardation, 97, 393–404.<br />

Ylvén, R., Björck-Akesson, E., G, M., & Granlund, M. (2006). Literature review of positive<br />

function<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> families with children with a disability. Journal of Policy and Practice <strong>in</strong><br />

Intellectual Disabilities, 3, 253–270.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!