the moral reasoning of student athletes and athletic training students

the moral reasoning of student athletes and athletic training students the moral reasoning of student athletes and athletic training students

dissertations.wsu.edu
from dissertations.wsu.edu More from this publisher
25.11.2014 Views

comprised of 6 hypothetical moral issues unrelated to sport and as many as 30% of a sample can be lost due to consistency check violations. Hahm, Beller, & Stoll (1989) developed and validated the Hahm-Beller Values Choice Inventory (HBVCI) (Cronbach Alphas .77 - .88) an instrument based in deontological moral theory (Frankena, 1973), Rawl’s Theory of Justice (1972), and Kohlberg’s (1981) philosophy of cognitive moral development. The 21 scenarios (and in its revised form 12 scenarios) address commonly occurring sport moral dilemmas and challenge athletes to reason based on an ideal philosophy of sport. To date the instrument has been used with over 80,000 individuals within and outside all levels of sport from high school, college, Olympic, to professional sport (Beller, Stoll, & Hahm, 2006). To date, the majority of studies conducted with high school, collegiate, Olympic, and professional athletes have used the HBVCI. Hall (1986) found that college athletes scored below the norms of their college aged peers (Bredemeier & Shields, 1994). Bredemeier & Shields (1984b) found similar results with intercollegiate male basketball players, but also found no difference with intercollegiate swimmers. Other studies have found that athletes generally score significantly lower on moral reasoning tests than non-athlete peers including (Beller & Stoll, 1993; Beller & Stoll, 1995; Beller & Stoll, 1996; Beller, Stoll, Burwell, & Cole, 1996; Bredemeier & Shields, 1994; Hansen, Beller, & Stoll, 1998; Rudd, Stoll, & Beller, 1997). “Moral reasoning plays a critical role in the production of moral behavior. In fact, even if other factors influence moral choices to a similar (or even greater) degree, moral reasoning is critical because it produces the moral meaning that an intended action has for an individual.” (Bredemeier & Shields, 1994, p. 175). These studies give evidence that moral action could be related to the development of moral reasoning, however 30

it must be noted that only a low positive relationship exists between that of moral knowing (involving the moral reasoning process) and that of moral action (Kohlberg, 1981; Lumpkin, Stoll, & Beller, 2003). In other words, just because one might know what the right thing is to do, why it is the right thing to do, and the underlying socio-moral principles that guide what is right, an individual can choose to do wrong for a variety of competing reasons. However, in order for the chance of any consistent moral action to occur one must first know what is right and why it is right – the underlying moral development and moral reasoning process. According to Beller and Stoll (1995), differences between athletes and non-athletes may be explained in a few different ways. The first is that the, “…characterization of athletics with competition, contention of interest, physical skill and prowess” (Beller & Stoll, 1995, p. 358) can lead to athletes prioritizing “instrumental values” such as winning, fame and prestige over simply competing to their best abilities. Along the same lines, the possibility of great successes in the forms of money and celebrity status may make it difficult for athletes to distance themselves enough to make rational, moral judgments (Beller & Stoll, 1995). Other possibilities include a win-at-all-costs mentality in the athletic world, a tendency to objectify opponents and place the burden of responsibility for moral reasoning on coaches and officials, specialization at early ages, time spent on sport activity such as video, weights, rehab or practices, and a lack of outside social relationships (Beller & Stoll, 1995). Bredemeier (1985) has examined character development in sport, moral reasoning in sport and how levels of moral reasoning are correlated with such issues in sport as aggression. The purpose of this study is to find a correlation between levels of moral reasoning with respect to issues of doping. Rest and Narvaez (1994) stated that, “[F]or low scoring students, discussions of intermediate level concepts do not find lodging in bedrock of basic cognitive structure, but 31

it must be noted that only a low positive relationship exists between that <strong>of</strong> <strong>moral</strong> knowing<br />

(involving <strong>the</strong> <strong>moral</strong> <strong>reasoning</strong> process) <strong>and</strong> that <strong>of</strong> <strong>moral</strong> action (Kohlberg, 1981; Lumpkin,<br />

Stoll, & Beller, 2003). In o<strong>the</strong>r words, just because one might know what <strong>the</strong> right thing is to do,<br />

why it is <strong>the</strong> right thing to do, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> underlying socio-<strong>moral</strong> principles that guide what is right,<br />

an individual can choose to do wrong for a variety <strong>of</strong> competing reasons. However, in order for<br />

<strong>the</strong> chance <strong>of</strong> any consistent <strong>moral</strong> action to occur one must first know what is right <strong>and</strong> why it is<br />

right – <strong>the</strong> underlying <strong>moral</strong> development <strong>and</strong> <strong>moral</strong> <strong>reasoning</strong> process.<br />

According to Beller <strong>and</strong> Stoll (1995), differences between <strong>athletes</strong> <strong>and</strong> non-<strong>athletes</strong> may<br />

be explained in a few different ways. The first is that <strong>the</strong>, “…characterization <strong>of</strong> <strong>athletic</strong>s with<br />

competition, contention <strong>of</strong> interest, physical skill <strong>and</strong> prowess” (Beller & Stoll, 1995, p. 358) can<br />

lead to <strong>athletes</strong> prioritizing “instrumental values” such as winning, fame <strong>and</strong> prestige over simply<br />

competing to <strong>the</strong>ir best abilities. Along <strong>the</strong> same lines, <strong>the</strong> possibility <strong>of</strong> great successes in <strong>the</strong><br />

forms <strong>of</strong> money <strong>and</strong> celebrity status may make it difficult for <strong>athletes</strong> to distance <strong>the</strong>mselves<br />

enough to make rational, <strong>moral</strong> judgments (Beller & Stoll, 1995). O<strong>the</strong>r possibilities include a<br />

win-at-all-costs mentality in <strong>the</strong> <strong>athletic</strong> world, a tendency to objectify opponents <strong>and</strong> place <strong>the</strong><br />

burden <strong>of</strong> responsibility for <strong>moral</strong> <strong>reasoning</strong> on coaches <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong>ficials, specialization at early<br />

ages, time spent on sport activity such as video, weights, rehab or practices, <strong>and</strong> a lack <strong>of</strong> outside<br />

social relationships (Beller & Stoll, 1995).<br />

Bredemeier (1985) has examined character development in sport, <strong>moral</strong> <strong>reasoning</strong> in<br />

sport <strong>and</strong> how levels <strong>of</strong> <strong>moral</strong> <strong>reasoning</strong> are correlated with such issues in sport as aggression.<br />

The purpose <strong>of</strong> this study is to find a correlation between levels <strong>of</strong> <strong>moral</strong> <strong>reasoning</strong> with respect<br />

to issues <strong>of</strong> doping. Rest <strong>and</strong> Narvaez (1994) stated that, “[F]or low scoring <strong>student</strong>s, discussions<br />

<strong>of</strong> intermediate level concepts do not find lodging in bedrock <strong>of</strong> basic cognitive structure, but<br />

31

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!