07.11.2014 Views

International Giant Otter Studbook Husbandry and Management

International Giant Otter Studbook Husbandry and Management

International Giant Otter Studbook Husbandry and Management

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Inbreeding, physical, behavioral, <strong>and</strong> pair compatibility problems should be considered<br />

for repeated breeding or rearing failure, providing appropriate husb<strong>and</strong>ry <strong>and</strong><br />

management is used during cub-rearing <strong>and</strong> at all other times. Hagenbeck <strong>and</strong> Wünnemann<br />

(1992) suggest that giant otters “show individual preferences when accepting a mate”; hence,<br />

repairing mates should be considered when pairs show clear signs of extensive pair<br />

incompatibility. Although a pair should not be determined incompatible just on the<br />

observations of brief temporary quarrels, growling, etc. seen between group members during<br />

the normal day. For example, it is quite common that captive giant otters, even truly compatible<br />

pairs, will growl at each other <strong>and</strong> sometimes even briefly fight (not inflicting injury), when<br />

they are eating fish. They will often try to keep some distance apart when they are eating. One<br />

individual may beg or show the other that he/she wants the other’s fish <strong>and</strong> one or the other may<br />

growl or make defensive/offensive bodily movements in addition. [These behaviors are also<br />

seen among wild Pteronura family group members. At least among adults “each otter catches<br />

its own prey <strong>and</strong> consumes it immediately…<strong>and</strong> there is no food sharing” (Duplaix 1980).]<br />

Many other negative, aggressive, or domineering behaviors must be displayed <strong>and</strong> on a<br />

continuing basis to determine if the pair is incompatible. Note: the individual “personality type”<br />

(i.e. very dominant, submissive, patient, nervous etc.) of each otter can affect pairing success,<br />

therefore this should be considered when selecting possible mates <strong>and</strong> partners as well.<br />

It as well should not be assumed because of certain behaviors, that the presence of fathers<br />

during cub-rearing will have a negative impact on the success of cub-rearing. For example,<br />

it seems to be normal, both in captivity <strong>and</strong> in the wild (Staib 2002), that fathers will often take<br />

cubs out of the den/nestbox <strong>and</strong> the mothers will immediately take action to return the cubs to<br />

the den/nestbox. When this behavior is not abnormally frequent or excessive, it should not be<br />

determined as harmful. Parents at Dortmund Zoo (Sykes-Gatz pers. obs.), Hagenbeck Tierpark<br />

(Flügger pers. comm.) <strong>and</strong> Cali Zoo (Corredor pers. comm.) have all been observed to carry out<br />

these behaviors. In at least the first days after birth, mothers have also been seen to be a little<br />

protective over their cubs when the father tries to become involved with the cubs. This as well<br />

should not be seen as abnormal behavior. Soon afterwards, the father will become equally<br />

involved (<strong>and</strong> his involvement well accepted by his mate) in the care of the cubs. Providing<br />

multiple nestboxes or having multiple underground dens gives fathers a place to go <strong>and</strong><br />

mother’s a little free space during these times. As well, it is crucial that enclosures are properly<br />

designed <strong>and</strong> furnished to help occupy the father’s <strong>and</strong> mother’s attentions on something else<br />

rather than only the cubs.<br />

It may also be possible that when siblings from the same litter are reared <strong>and</strong> reach sexual<br />

maturity together, they will not breed if they are housed together as a potential breeding<br />

pair. This situation <strong>and</strong> failure to reproduce is known to have occurred in three cases, at three<br />

different zoos (Louzada da Silva, pers. comm. provided information about the pairs’ family<br />

history at Brasilia <strong>and</strong> Sorrocaba Zoos; Wünnemann 1991). For example, at Hagenbeck<br />

Tierpark, a female was kept with her brother, who she grew up with, until she was at least 3<br />

years old. This pair had rare <strong>and</strong> failed mating attempts <strong>and</strong> no pregnancies resulted.<br />

Wünnemann noted that this male was not well experienced at mating [maybe he did not<br />

penetrate the female]. This pair was then separated, the female was paired with a new partner<br />

<strong>and</strong> she had a litter. A litter was born at Cuiaba Zoo in 1994 <strong>and</strong> a pair from this litter was<br />

transferred to Sorrocaba Zoo <strong>and</strong> no litters were born from this pair during their lifetime,<br />

although they were kept together their entire lives (which was at least 6 years or more). (More<br />

detailed information regarding this pair is not known, so other explanations for breeding failure<br />

may also have been possible.) At Brasilia Zoo, a brother <strong>and</strong> sister that were reared together<br />

were eventually separated, but they were kept in visual-acoustic-olfactory contact with one<br />

167

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!