07.11.2014 Views

NMFS Biological Opinion on U.S. Navy training ... - Govsupport.us

NMFS Biological Opinion on U.S. Navy training ... - Govsupport.us

NMFS Biological Opinion on U.S. Navy training ... - Govsupport.us

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

FINAL PROGRAMMATIC BIOLOGICAL OPINION ON U.S. NAVY ACTIVITIES IN THE HAWAII RANGE COMPLEX 2008-2013<br />

Their inability to avoid further exposure, however, seems likely to produce stress resp<strong>on</strong>ses beca<strong>us</strong>e they would<br />

want to avoid the s<strong>on</strong>ar but would their circumstances would prevent them from doing so (see Box S of Figure 3).<br />

ATTENTIONAL CAPTURE. Attenti<strong>on</strong> is the cognitive process of selectively c<strong>on</strong>centrating <strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>e aspect of an animal’s<br />

envir<strong>on</strong>ment while ignoring other things (Posner 1994). Beca<strong>us</strong>e animals (including humans) have limited cognitive<br />

resources, there is a limit to how much sensory informati<strong>on</strong> they can process at any time. The phenomen<strong>on</strong> called<br />

“attenti<strong>on</strong>al capture” occurs when a stimul<strong>us</strong> (<strong>us</strong>ually a stimul<strong>us</strong> that an animal is not c<strong>on</strong>centrating <strong>on</strong> or attending<br />

to) “captures” an animal’s attenti<strong>on</strong>. This shift in attenti<strong>on</strong> can occur c<strong>on</strong>scio<strong>us</strong>ly or unc<strong>on</strong>scio<strong>us</strong>ly (for example,<br />

when an animal hears sounds that it associates with the approach of a predator) and the shift in attenti<strong>on</strong> can be<br />

sudden (Dukas 2002, van Rij 2007). Once a stimul<strong>us</strong> has captured an animal’s attenti<strong>on</strong>, the animal can resp<strong>on</strong>d by<br />

ignoring the stimul<strong>us</strong>, assuming a “watch and wait” posture, or treat the stimul<strong>us</strong> as a disturbance and resp<strong>on</strong>d<br />

accordingly, which includes scanning for the source of the stimul<strong>us</strong> or “vigilance” (Cowlishaw et al. 2004).<br />

Vigilance is normally an adaptive behavior that helps animals determine the presence or absence of predators, assess<br />

their distance from c<strong>on</strong>specifics, or to attend cues from prey (Bednekoff and Lima 1998, Treves 2000). Despite those<br />

benefits, however, vigilance has a cost of time: when animals foc<strong>us</strong> their attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> specific envir<strong>on</strong>mental cues, it<br />

is not attending to other activities such a foraging. These costs have been documented best in foraging animals,<br />

where vigilance has been shown to substantially reduce feeding rates (Saino 1994, Beauchamp and Livoreil 1997,<br />

Fritz et al. 2002).<br />

Animals will spend more time being vigilant, which translates to less time foraging or resting, when disturbance<br />

stimuli approach them more directly, remain at closer distances, have a greater group size (for example, multiple<br />

surface vessels), or when they co-occur with times that an animal perceives increased risk (for example, when they<br />

are giving birth or accompanied by a calf). Most of the published literature, however, suggests that direct approaches<br />

will increase the amount of time animals will dedicate to being vigilant. For example, bighorn sheep and Dall’s<br />

sheep dedicated more time being vigilant, and less time resting or foraging, when aircraft made direct approaches<br />

over them (Frid 2001, Stockwell et al. 1991).<br />

Several authors have established that l<strong>on</strong>g-term and intense disturbance stimuli can ca<strong>us</strong>e populati<strong>on</strong> declines by<br />

reducing the body c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> of individuals that have been disturbed, followed by reduced reproductive success,<br />

reduced survival, or both (Daan et al. 1996, Madsen 1994, White 1983). For example, Madsen (1994) reported that<br />

pink-footed geese (Anser brachyrhynch<strong>us</strong>) in undisturbed habitat gained body mass and had about a 46%<br />

reproductive success compared with geese in disturbed habitat (being c<strong>on</strong>sistently scared off the fields <strong>on</strong> which they<br />

were foraging) which did not gain mass and has a 17% reproductive success. Similar reducti<strong>on</strong>s in reproductive<br />

success have been reported for mule deer (Odocoile<strong>us</strong> hemi<strong>on</strong><strong>us</strong>) disturbed by all-terrain vehicles (Yarmoloy et al.<br />

1988), caribou disturbed by seismic explorati<strong>on</strong> blasts (Bradshaw et al. 1998), caribou disturbed by low-elevati<strong>on</strong><br />

military jet-fights (Luick et al. 1996), and caribou disturbed by low-elevati<strong>on</strong> jet flights (Harringt<strong>on</strong> and Veitch<br />

1992). Similarly, a study of elk (Cerv<strong>us</strong> elaph<strong>us</strong>) that were disturbed experimentally by pedestrians c<strong>on</strong>cluded that<br />

the ratio of young to mothers was inversely related to disturbance rate (Phillips and Alldredge 2000).<br />

The primary mechanism by which increased vigilance and disturbance appear to affect the fitness of individual<br />

animals is by disrupting an animal’s time budget and, as a result, reducing the time they might spend foraging and<br />

215

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!