01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp 01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

19.10.2014 Views

86 Research Report assistive technology in the workplace. The fourth study investigated the use of assistive technology to supplement adult remediation. There were different participants in each study. Those in the first study were a diverse group of fifty Ivy League and community college students; three had acquired learning disabilities through brain injury, while the others had been diagnosed as learning-disabled by criteria which varied from group to group. Those in the second study were a subgroup of 29 student participants from the first study. A research sample of eight learning-disabled participants in the third study, an intervention lasting for three months, was identified ‘with considerable effort’ through a variety of community agencies. A research sample of eight participants in the fourth study consisted of dyslexic adults in a remedial language arts tutorial programme. Outcomes were mixed. In summary, the investigators found that students for whom computer reader technology is helpful tend to be slow readers with poor comprehension and limited ability to sustain their concentration. However, they need good ability to integrate auditory and visual stimuli. With much ground to make up, these users have a high potential gain from the intervention. They nevertheless need to experience success when they first use the technology. They also need to be strongly motivated, with a substantial reading workload. Although they need sufficient time to use the system in a supportive environment, the time needed to scan reading matter must not be so great as to offset any benefits. Summary It will be clear that the studies in the preceding section bear no relationship to two of the theories described in Part two and none to any of the theoretical developments outlined in Part three, although there is no necessity for such a relationship (Sroufe, 1997). The first two theories in Part two and the first theory in Part three, are concerned with what students need to learn, whereas the other theories seek to explain why students experience difficulty in learning. Insofar as the evaluation studies have little to say to teachers of adults with learning difficulties, good practice in this field rests almost entirely on professional judgement informed by a background in linguistics and knowledge of the developmental course of normal reading acquisition. Interestingly, there appears to be no experimental evidence comparing group outcomes between adult dyslexics and ‘ordinary’ adult literacy learners. As earlier studies have concluded (e.g. Fowler & Scarborough, 1993), much of what has been learned from research on reading disabilities may be pertinent to the identification and the literacy development of adult learners generally—that is to say, to people who need to acquire knowledge and skills, but who have no specific difficulty in learning. However, the applicability of that research may itself be partly or largely a consequence of an over-extended reading disability concept. There is evidence from intervention studies with children that systematic and explicit teaching of phonics and exception words, with multisensory learning and intensive reinforcement, is generally effective, but that there is a minority of students whom it does not appear to help (Rack & Hatcher, 2002; Vellutino et al., 1996). Are these ‘treatment-resisters’ the true ‘dyslexics’? The logic of this interpretation is compelling and it is certainly easier to argue that ‘treatment-resisters’ are different from ‘normal’ readers in a way that ‘treatment-responders’ are not. Do ‘treatment-resisters’ have anything in common with one another, apart from their difficulty in learning to read? The

Developmental dyslexia in adults: a research review 87 research does not directly address this question, but it is unlikely that ‘treatment-resisters’ are a homogeneous group; for this reason, perhaps, the illusion of homogeneity should not be created by including them in the same classification. The best guidance for tutors might then be expected to come not from group studies but from a range of well-designed single-case studies, which might address various kinds of attentional difficulty and difficulties in drawing inferences. These studies have yet to be undertaken. General conclusions For the writer as well as for the reader, this has been a long and arduous review. It could easily have been more so. The scientific literature on reading and reading difficulties is vast and complex; the non-scientific literature is also extensive and highly problematic in its own way. When scientists of international distinction describe the situation in terms like those quoted in the opening paragraphs, they are not being alarmist. Although this review has tried to clarify the points at issue, the writer is painfully conscious of the loose ends and inconsistencies that remain; complexity can be simplified, but chaos remains chaos. Among members of the general public, there are several myths and misconceptions about dyslexia—namely that dyslexics are brighter than ordinary people, that they are especially gifted, that they are especially anti-social, that there is at least ‘one in every classroom’ and that, although there is ‘a gene for dyslexia’, its adverse effects can be dispelled by a course of literacy teaching which addresses their needs. Scientific support for these beliefs is lacking. But even systematic researchers can only stumble towards the truth. In what we thought we knew about dyslexia a generation ago, we now seem to have been largely mistaken. Month by month, new research findings make it necessary for us to update and perhaps modify our understanding of reading and reading difficulties. We are all revisionists now. The most urgent topic for revision is the concept of dyslexia itself, not least because it has in effect been commandeered in order to invest unsuccessful learners with responsibility for the shortcomings of their teachers. Worse even than that, it has led to an assumption that the skills essential for mainstream teaching are needed only by remedial specialists—an assumption which is guaranteed to put greater numbers of learners at risk. Every method of reading instruction appears to succeed with some learners (although success occurs with some methods only when the poverty of the stimulus is compensated by the ingenuity of the response). All methods fail with some learners (and possibly with some teachers, too). The lowest failure rate is achieved with systematic and explicit instruction in the alphabetic principle. However, even with this method, a few learners experience intractable difficulty. These are the learners whom it is appropriate to call ‘dyslexic’. It is not appropriate to think of them as the casualties of system failure. At the time of writing, they may find it more liberating to use alternative strategies than to persist in a struggle for literacy that can end only in a further experience of failure. However, describing all these learners as ‘dyslexic’ may suggest that they have more in common than is actually the case. It might also come, in time, to stigmatise them. After the first awareness campaign, over ten years ago, dyslexics appeared to become the butt of a spate of cruel jokes. We should not be surprised if the word ‘dyslexic’ eventually goes the way of ‘spastic’ and for much the same reason. Science and the world of literacy teaching and learning might be no poorer if it did.

Developmental dyslexia in adults: a research review 87<br />

research does not directly address this question, but it is unlikely that ‘treatment-resisters’ are<br />

a homogeneous group; for this reason, perhaps, the illusion of homogeneity should not be<br />

created by including them in the same classification. The best guidance for tutors might then be<br />

expected to come not from group studies but from a range of well-designed single-case studies,<br />

which might address various kinds of attentional difficulty and difficulties in drawing inferences.<br />

These studies have yet to be undertaken.<br />

General conclusions<br />

For the writer as well as for the reader, this has been a long and arduous review. It could easily<br />

have been more so. The scientific literature on reading and reading difficulties is vast and<br />

complex; the non-scientific literature is also extensive and highly problematic in its own way. When<br />

scientists of international distinction describe the situation in terms like those quoted in the<br />

opening paragraphs, they are not being alarmist. Although this review has tried to clarify the<br />

points at issue, the writer is painfully conscious of the loose ends and inconsistencies that remain;<br />

complexity can be simplified, but chaos remains chaos.<br />

Among members of the general public, there are several myths and misconceptions about<br />

dyslexia—namely that dyslexics are brighter than ordinary people, that they are especially gifted,<br />

that they are especially anti-social, that there is at least ‘one in every classroom’ and that,<br />

although there is ‘a gene for dyslexia’, its adverse effects can be dispelled by a course of literacy<br />

teaching which addresses their needs. Scientific support for these beliefs is lacking. But even<br />

systematic researchers can only stumble towards the truth. In what we thought we knew about<br />

dyslexia a generation ago, we now seem to have been largely mistaken. Month by month, new<br />

research findings make it necessary for us to <strong>update</strong> and perhaps modify our understanding of<br />

reading and reading difficulties. We are all revisionists now.<br />

The most urgent topic for revision is the concept of dyslexia itself, not least because it has in effect<br />

been commandeered in order to invest unsuccessful learners with responsibility for the<br />

shortcomings of their teachers. Worse even than that, it has led to an assumption that the skills<br />

essential for mainstream teaching are needed only by remedial specialists—an assumption which<br />

is guaranteed to put greater numbers of learners at risk.<br />

Every method of reading instruction appears to succeed with some learners (although success<br />

occurs with some methods only when the poverty of the stimulus is compensated by the ingenuity<br />

of the response). All methods fail with some learners (and possibly with some teachers, too). The<br />

lowest failure rate is achieved with systematic and explicit instruction in the alphabetic principle.<br />

However, even with this method, a few learners experience intractable difficulty. These are the<br />

learners whom it is appropriate to call ‘dyslexic’. It is not appropriate to think of them as the<br />

casualties of system failure. At the time of writing, they may find it more liberating to use<br />

alternative strategies than to persist in a struggle for literacy that can end only in a further<br />

experience of failure.<br />

However, describing all these learners as ‘dyslexic’ may suggest that they have more in common<br />

than is actually the case. It might also come, in time, to stigmatise them. After the first awareness<br />

campaign, over ten years ago, dyslexics appeared to become the butt of a spate of cruel jokes. We<br />

should not be surprised if the word ‘dyslexic’ eventually goes the way of ‘spastic’ and for much the<br />

same reason. Science and the world of literacy teaching and learning might be no poorer if it did.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!