19.10.2014 Views

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Developmental dyslexia in adults: a research review 55<br />

environmental factors can explain the observed differences in dyslexic behaviour better than<br />

differences in cognitive profiles might explain them (Sprenger-Charolles et al., 20<strong>01</strong>). Does<br />

this mean that, strictly speaking, the problems with phonological awareness represent a<br />

knowledge deficit rather than a learning disability? Or does it mean that one type of learning<br />

disability is activated only by ‘non-transparent’ orthographies and a second type of learning<br />

disability is activated by all orthographies? Genetic evidence appears to support the latter<br />

conjecture (Grigorenko et al., 20<strong>01</strong>) and thus to support the hypothesis that there are two<br />

independent sources of reading dysfunction, one related to phonological processing only and<br />

one related to lexical access, or naming speed only (Lovett, 1984)—the ‘double deficit’<br />

hypothesis (Bowers & Wolf, 1993; Wolf & Bowers, 1999; Wolf et al., 2000a). If this is so, then it<br />

might suggest an alternative to the phonological-core variable-difference model.<br />

The double-deficit model of reading disability is a further alternative to the phonological<br />

deficit model.<br />

<strong>Dyslexia</strong> and lexical access: the double-deficit hypothesis<br />

Introduction<br />

A combination of deficits in accuracy (or phonological deficits) and fluency (or naming-speed<br />

deficits) appears to characterise students with the most serious and pervasive impairments in<br />

reading across various languages (Wolf & Bowers, 1999). The double deficit hypothesis<br />

represents an evolving, alternative conceptualisation of reading disabilities in which, in<br />

addition to a core deficit in accuracy, there is a second core deficit in fluency (Wolf & Bowers,<br />

1999). Ultimately, the hypothesis could lead to a more differentiated view of reading failure<br />

and a more comprehensive approach to reading intervention (Wolf et al., 2000a).<br />

The double-deficit model conceptualises dyslexia as entailing core deficits in both accuracy<br />

and fluency.<br />

Is fluency distinct from phonological processing?<br />

Fluency is the most salient characteristic of skilled readers (Fuchs et al., 20<strong>01</strong>). It can be<br />

viewed as a performance indicator of overall reading competence, achieved when efficient<br />

low-level word recognition frees up memory capacity for higher-level comprehension (Fuchs<br />

et al., 20<strong>01</strong>). However, there is some uncertainty as to whether rapid automatised naming, the<br />

conventional test of lexical access, is simply a phonological subprocess or whether it is a way<br />

of ‘stressing’ the phonological system through the demand for retrieval of phonological codes<br />

(Cutting & Denckla, 20<strong>01</strong>).<br />

Evidence for two independent sources of reading difficulty might be provided by a double<br />

dissociation of accuracy and fluency, but not without the risk of circular argument: there is no<br />

theory-independent way to determine whether a given case is ‘pure’, since it requires in the<br />

first place a reliable theory of cognitive modules to guarantee that a ‘pure’ dissociation has<br />

been observed (Van Orden et al., 20<strong>01</strong>). From a behavioural perspective, oral reading fluency<br />

is a direct measure of phonological segmentation and phonological recoding (Fuchs et al.,<br />

20<strong>01</strong>). On the other hand, independent effects on reading achievement have been found for<br />

phonological awareness and rapid naming (de Jong & van der Leij, 1999). For separable<br />

effects, the evidence from psychometric research is supplemented by evidence from<br />

molecular genetics (Grigorenko et al., 20<strong>01</strong>).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!