19.10.2014 Views

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Developmental dyslexia in adults: a research review 53<br />

problem occurring in other typologies: an unbridgeable gap between ‘(h) those that are<br />

included in this classification’ and ‘(i) etcetera’ (Borges, 2000). The problem arises even in a<br />

univariate classification; in a multivariate classification, it leads to increasing fractionation<br />

(Van Orden et al., 20<strong>01</strong>).<br />

Is there a more persuasive way of classifying the observations? Need they be classified at all?<br />

Does it help to conceive of all the relevant distributions of reading-related cognitive skills as<br />

being continuously arrayed in a multidimensional space and not distributed in clusters (Ellis,<br />

1985; Stanovich, 19<strong>88</strong>)? Does it help to consider the question quantitatively, in order to avoid<br />

‘the inherent connotations of discreteness carried by many natural language terms’<br />

(Stanovich, 19<strong>88</strong>)? In so far as dyslexia (or ‘reading disability’) is conceptualised as a difficulty<br />

in learning how to decode print, the phonological-core variable-difference model (Stanovich,<br />

19<strong>88</strong>) not only recognises that phonological difficulties are common to both dyslexic and<br />

‘ordinary’ poor readers but, by conceptualising differences in terms of dimensions, it also<br />

avoids the problems of discreteness and residual or mixed categories in dyslexia subtype<br />

analyses.<br />

Although the phonological-core variable-difference model rests on a dimensional concept, it<br />

is claimed to be compatible with the categorical differences implied by subtypes (Morris et al.,<br />

1998). Nor are subtypes the only outstanding issue. The model proposes that the poor reader<br />

with dyslexia has an impairment localised at the phonological core whereas the ordinary poor<br />

reader has impairments extending into a variety of domains (Stanovich, 19<strong>88</strong>)—essentially the<br />

same distinction drawn between ‘specific reading retardation’ and general reading<br />

backwardness in the Isle of Wight study (Rutter et al., 1970). If the description ‘specific’ has<br />

any meaning at all, whether applied to ‘reading retardation’, learning difficulty, or dyslexia,<br />

things cannot be otherwise.<br />

However, a concept of specificity is less easy to reconcile with the pattern-of-difficulties<br />

concept of dyslexia, where severe and persistent difficulty in learning to read is but one<br />

characteristic among many (Miles, 1983), or with the finding that shared problems may be<br />

more numerous and diverse than allowed for by the model (Hoskyn & Swanson, 2000). The<br />

concept of specificity is correspondingly difficult to reconcile with some of the items listed in<br />

behavioural checklists for dyslexia screening (see Appendices 5 and 6). It is also difficult to<br />

reconcile with our current understanding of human development (Gottlieb & Halpern, 2002)<br />

and its implications for reading (Stanovich, 1986).<br />

Challenges to the categorical assumption of the phonological deficit theory are addressed by<br />

the multi-dimensional phonological-core, variable-difference model of reading disability.<br />

What does the phonological-core variable-difference model predict?<br />

The phonological-core variable-difference model leads to the prediction that the dyslexic’s<br />

impairment, while more localised, is more severe than that of the ‘ordinary’ poor reader and<br />

that because of its severity it will be more difficult to remediate (Stanovich, 19<strong>88</strong>). Consistent<br />

with this prediction is a recent finding, described by its authors as ‘new, and perhaps<br />

controversial’ (Rack & Hatcher, 2002), that dyslexics are actually less responsive to<br />

intervention than ordinary poor readers. Controversy? Almost certainly. Novelty? No. There is<br />

undoubtedly a conflict between robust findings that students with cognitive deficits in the<br />

domain of phonology are difficult to remediate (Vellutino et al., 1996), or even treatmentresistant<br />

(Chiappe et al., 20<strong>01</strong>) and claims that ‘the effects of dyslexia can be largely

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!