19.10.2014 Views

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

52<br />

Research Report<br />

the ‘normal’ readers on this task (Jorm et al., 1986a). If this is also the case with adults, then<br />

it might be that pseudoword repetition does have a discriminant function albeit, paradoxically,<br />

that errors in pseudoword repetition might identify not developmental dyslexics but ‘ordinary’<br />

poor readers—perhaps because the task loads on verbal ability at least as much as it loads<br />

on phonemic discrimination and verbal short-term memory.<br />

Comparable findings have been obtained in studies using other phonological awareness<br />

tasks. Here, too, previous experience of literacy appears to be an important condition for<br />

success, in that otherwise unimpaired but illiterate adults perform very poorly at the task of<br />

phoneme deletion (Adrián et al., 1995)—for example, saying ‘boat’ without the /b/. More<br />

specifically, as a predictor of success, previous experience of alphabetic literacy is better than<br />

previous experience of nonalphabetic literacy (Cheung et al., 20<strong>01</strong>).<br />

Thus, while phonological awareness tasks are essential to help the adult tutor to plan a<br />

programme of teaching, a student’s initial difficulty with these tasks need not indicate any<br />

biological abnormality since the ability to manipulate speech sounds is a taught skill, not an<br />

outcome of cognitive maturation or exposure to language (Read et al., 1987). Tasks such as<br />

phoneme deletion differentiate poor or beginning readers from accomplished readers; they do<br />

not differentiate dyslexic from non-dyslexic poor readers.<br />

This discussion leads to an important issue in the logic of diagnosis. It may be true —although<br />

the assumption has been queried (Miles et al., 2003)—that everyone with dyslexia experiences<br />

difficulty on assessments of phonological processing. However, the inference that everyone<br />

with impairments in phonological processing must then be dyslexic is not a logical corollary.<br />

The error is a classic one: the deductive fallacy of ‘affirming the consequent’. It is a fallacy<br />

which amateur (and professional) diagnosticians need to recognise and avoid—although that<br />

may prove difficult, since affirming the consequent is implicit in diagnostic materials<br />

published by the Prison Service (HM Prison Service, 1999) and the Basic Skills Agency (Klein,<br />

1993) in the past and it reappears in the Agency’s new Skills for Life Diagnostic Assessments<br />

in Literacy (Bradshaw et al., 2002).<br />

One way of avoiding this fallacy might be to ensure that only those impairments which are<br />

‘persistent’ after years of regular schooling qualify for consideration as ‘dyslexic’ (Herrington<br />

& Hunter-Carsch, 20<strong>01</strong>). How the tutor manages this situation needs to be guided by its<br />

ethical implications. Moreover, with respect to method and quality of early reading teaching<br />

strategies, the quest to identify whether or not there has been ‘regular schooling’ may be a<br />

fool’s errand.<br />

Phonological deficits do not permit dyslexics to be differentiated from other poor readers.<br />

Limitations of the phonological deficit theory: the phonological-core variable-difference<br />

model of reading disability<br />

The ability to detect and process speech sounds can be compromised for more than one<br />

reason and those reasons are likely to differ from one learner to another. In some cases, the<br />

dominant factor might appear to be experiential; in some, the dominant factor might appear<br />

to be biological; and, in the remaining cases, neither factor might predominate. The question<br />

then arises whether cases of reading disability form discrete clusters (or subtypes). From<br />

such an admittedly simplified account, a typology of reading-disability subtypes might be<br />

derived, with two questionably discrete categories and a residual category, thus illustrating a

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!