19.10.2014 Views

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Developmental dyslexia in adults: a research review 45<br />

Over time, too, the most appropriate measures of phonological awareness may change<br />

(Schatschneider et al., 1999). Learners begin to acquire phonological awareness when they<br />

learn to separate word units in the speech continuum, to hear the two parts of a compound<br />

word, to separate the syllables of a word, to select rhyme-words and to hear which words<br />

start with the same or different sounds (Goswami & Bryant, 1990; Sodoro et al., 2002). In its<br />

most complete sense, phonological awareness is neither innate nor acquired spontaneously<br />

in the course of cognitive development (Morais et al., 1979). ‘Discovering phonemic units is<br />

helped greatly by explicit instruction in how the system works’ (Ehri et al., 20<strong>01</strong>); moreover, it<br />

is optimally taught in the context of reading instruction (Hatcher et al., 1994).<br />

The importance of tuition becomes clear when we acknowledge that phonemic awareness—<br />

the insight that every spoken word can be conceived as a sequence of phonemes (the smallest<br />

units of language that distinguish one word from another) —differs from less fine-grained<br />

aspects of phonological awareness (such as syllable counting and rhyme recognition). To<br />

develop the phonemic awareness that reading and writing call for, students must learn to put<br />

their attention where it had never had to be and learn to attend to meaningless phonemes,<br />

not meaningful morphemes and words: ‘Speech does not require phonemic awareness for the<br />

same reason that it does not produce it’ (Liberman & Whalen, 2000). Our acknowledgement is<br />

underlined by the recognition that discovering phonemic units is helped by, or even<br />

conditional upon, explicit instruction in their isolation, identity, categorisation, blending,<br />

segmentation and deletion (Alegria et al., 1982; Cardoso-Martins, 20<strong>01</strong>; Ehri et al., 20<strong>01</strong>) and<br />

explicit instruction in letter-sound relationships.<br />

Phonemes themselves are abstractions: they do not exist as part of the acoustic speech<br />

signal (Baudouin de Courtenay, 1895; Liberman & Whalen, 2000; Nittrouer, 2002). While it is<br />

true that speech is a stream of vocal sounds, we learn to analyse this sound-stream as a<br />

sequence of linguistic units; in the normal course of events, we learn to recognise words and<br />

syllables before we first go to school and we learn to recognise phonemes as we are taught to<br />

read and spell. Each phoneme is a set of similar (but audibly different) sounds, or ‘phones’,<br />

like the sounds represented by the letter ‘p’ in ‘pot’ (where it is followed by a puff of air) and<br />

in ‘spot’ (where it isn’t), so that a phoneme is both a percept and a concept (Gleitman & Rozin,<br />

1977). For reading and spelling, it is less a noise in the ear than an idea in the mind (Baudouin<br />

de Courtenay, 1895).<br />

In English, especially, the correspondences between phonemes and graphemes (the letters or<br />

letter-groups used to transcribe phonemes) are complex (Jackson & Coltheart, 20<strong>01</strong>). For<br />

most people, learning to read offers the only opportunity to gain insight into the phonological<br />

structure of speech; if people are not taught, they may never intuit this structure (Liberman,<br />

1998; Morais et al., 1979). The method by which learners are taught to read may influence the<br />

development of phonemic awareness (Alegria et al., 1982), but the whole-word method is not<br />

incompatible with the development of phonemic awareness (Leybaert & Content, 1995) and<br />

neither do phonics methods guarantee that development (Vellutino et al., 1996).<br />

A useful distinction can be made between sensitivity to phonemes and the ability to analyse<br />

(or manipulate) them (Mann, 1987). Then, if phonemic awareness is defined as conscious<br />

access to the phonemic level of the speech stream (Stanovich, 1986), a lack of phonemic<br />

awareness need not imply any substantial inferiority in phonemic sensitivity (Adrián et al.,<br />

1995) and would not be sufficient evidence for a cognitive dysfunction. Indeed, the growth of<br />

phonemic sensitivity is a developmental imperative. Since the acoustic signal of speech lacks<br />

invariant physical correlates to phonetic segments and the ability to recognise segmental

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!